Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Menopause. 2013 Mar;20(3):269–273. doi: 10.1097/GME.0b013e31826e7574

Racial Differences in Perception of Healthy Body Weight in Mid-Life Women: Results from Do Stage Transitions Result in Detectable Effects (STRIDE) Study

Semara Thomas 1, Roberta B Ness 2, Rebecca C Thurston 3, Karen Matthews 3, Chung-Chou Chang 4, Rachel Hess 4
PMCID: PMC3593962  NIHMSID: NIHMS407015  PMID: 23435023

Abstract

Objectives

Perception of a healthy body weight may influence health behaviors including physical activity level, nutritional habits, and health outcomes, and these perceptions may vary importantly by race. Midlife is a critical period for women, which typically includes weight gain. We assessed the associations between perception of healthy body weight and body mass index (BMI) and whether they vary by race.

Methods

In the Do Stage Transitions Result in Detectable Effects (STRIDE) study, body mass index (BMI) and perception of body weight (healthy, underweight, or overweight) were measured at the baseline examination. Multinomial logistic regression models examined the associations, with race (White vs. Black) as a moderator variable.

Results

Of 729 women enrolled, 689 women (95%, N=145 Black, N=544 White) were included in these analyses. Even though the average BMI was higher for Black women compared to White women (33.1 vs. 29.2, respectively, p<.0001), Black women were less likely to report that they weighed too much (RRR (Relative Risk Ratio) [95% CI]: 0.4 [0.2, 0.9], p 0.022) and more likely to think that they did not weigh enough (RRR [95% CI]: 14.2 [1.8, 110], p 0.011).

Conclusion

Although Black women in general face a greater threat of morbidity from weight-related chronic diseases, they are more likely to be accepting of their weight at higher BMI’s, relative to Whites. Weight-loss interventions and counseling about healthy body size may influence healthy behavior and reduce chronic disease risk.

Keywords: Midlife, Body Image, Weight-Perception, Race, Health, Behavior

INTRODUCTION

Body image refers to an individual’s opinion of their external appearance. Understanding body image is important because it potentially can influence an individual’s behavior. Given that Blacks are more likely to suffer from obesity-related medical conditions such as hypertension and noninsulin-dependent diabetes, understanding ways to help curtail unhealthy behavior that leads to these conditions are essential.1,2 Body image has been studied extensively in relation to disordered eating (although typically a restrictive eating pattern) in teenage and young adult women.3, 4 Results from these studies show that Black women have a more positive body-image when compared to their white counterparts, that persists with weight loss and weight regain.5 In contrast, qualitative work suggests that premenopausal obese Black women are dissatisfied with their body size.6 This study used a more in-depth analysis of body image by conducting interviews that were often 40–60 minutes in length, and noted that for these participants, body image satisfaction often fluctuated depending on social factors such as community resources, opinion of loved ones, ability to find clothes that fit, etc.

Body image, or body weight perception, is important because of its relationship to weight change. For example, in younger women (aged 25–37 at baseline) women, Lynch and colleagues found that body size perception influenced weight change over 13years—obese women who perceived themselves as obese lost weight while those who perceived themselves as over- or normal-weight gained weight over this period.7 Understanding body image and the potential impact it has on the behavior of adult women is especially important given that the peri- to postmenopausal period is a time when a woman is most likely to gain weight,8 experience a loss in fat-free body mass, and an increase in central adiposity.9, 10 These changes increase the risk for obesity-related diseases by increasing one’s risk for insulin resistance, atherosclerosis and hypertension.11 Although women gain weight during their mid-life period, personal choices such as regular physical activity and diet can prevent or decrease this change.12, 13 Body image may influence these choices.14

This study aims to expand the knowledge of body weight perception and its association with obesity in women, specifically in mid-life and beyond. We sought to understand the associations between actual body size (body mass index (BMI) calculated as weight (kg)/height (m2)) and weight perception among mid-life women. We additionally examine whether the relation between BMI and weight perception differs between Black and White women during the mid-life period. We hypothesized that at higher BMI’s, mid-life Black women would be more likely to perceive their current weight as healthy when compared to White women.

METHODS

Data Source and Study Population

Do Stage Transitions Result in Detectable Effects (STRIDE) is an ongoing cohort study of 732 women ages 40 to 65 years enrolled between January and December 2005 from a single university-based general internal medicine practice. Briefly, all women ages 40–65 who presented to the practice were asked to participate in a study examining the impact of menopause on health-related quality of life. All women who were in this age range, spoke English, and consented to participate were enrolled in the study. Recruitment details have been described previously.15 This study was approved by the University Institutional Review Board and registered with clinicaltrials.gov (#NCT00097994). Of the 732 original STRIDE participants, 724 women, aged 40–65 years, had complete data.

Measures

Weight and Weight Perception

To calculate BMI, the participants’ weight and height were abstracted from the office visit that coincided with STRIDE enrollment. Based on BMI, participants were classified as underweight (BMI < 18), normal (BMI ≥ 18, and <25), overweight (BMI ≥ 25, and < 30), or obese (BMI ≥ 30).16 Because of the small numbers, the four participants (out of 724) that were underweight were excluded from further analyses.

Weight perception was assessed using a single question, “Do you think that your current weight is healthy?”. Response options included: “Yes”; “No, I think I weigh too much”; and “No, I don’t think I weigh enough”.

Race

Participants were asked to self-report race. They were able to choose more than one racial category and were able to report race as other. Of the 720 women with complete data and that were either normal weight, overweight or obese, 544 were White, and 145 were Black. These 689 women were included in these analyses. Of the 31 participants excluded, 7 identified as Native-Americans, 14 Asian, and 7 “Other;” 3 had missing race data.

Stage of Menopause

Participants’ menopausal status was defined using self-reported bleeding patterns. Menopausal status was categorized based on a previously defined modification of the Stages of Reproductive Aging Workshop criteria.17 Participants with regular bleeding patterns considered premenopausal. Perimenopausal participants had irregular bleeding patterns but their last menstrual period (LMP) occurred <12 months ago. Postmenopausal participants had their LMP ≥12 months ago. Because of small numbers and the inability to identify menopausal status based on bleeding patterns, women taking oral contraceptive pills (OCPs) were grouped with premenopausal women, while women with a hysterectomy were grouped with postmenopausal women.

Other Variables

Age was calculated as date of survey completion minus date of birth. Participants self-reported their marital status and educational attainment. They were asked to report the presence or absence of nine comorbid medical conditions including: heart disease, heart failure, depression, diabetes, emphysema or lung disease, stroke, arthritis, cancer (other than skin cancer), and high blood pressure. Women were placed into comorbidity categories of: “none,” “1–2 conditions,” or “≥3 conditions.”

Social support was measured using the 12-item Interpersonal Support Evaluation List (ISEL).18 The emotional well-being scale of the RAND-36 was used as a proxy for depressive symptoms.

Analysis

Participant characteristics were summarized using frequencies and measures of central tendency. We used t-tests for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables to compare differences in study variables by race. We also used univariable-multinomial logistic regression models to examine the associations of race, BMI, and the other covariables (age, educational attainment, marital status, menopause status, emotional wellbeing and social support) with weight perception. We entered covariables into subsequent multivariable multinomial logistic models examining the primary outcome of weight perception (categorized as Healthy, Too Much or Not Enough). Secondary analysis included categorical weight (normal, overweight and obese) and tested the interactions between race and BMI (as a continuous and categorical variable). All analyses were conducted using STATA 10.0 (Stata Corp., College Station, TX, USA) and alpha was set to 0.05.

RESULTS

Sample Characteristics

A total of 689 of 732 (94%) STRIDE participants had complete data, were not underweight, and are either White or Black; 79% were White, while 21% were Black. The mean BMI of the participants was at the usual cutoff for obesity, i.e. 30.0. 16 Table 1 shows the sociodemographic and health characteristics of the STRIDE study participants according race.

Table 1.

Sociodemographic and Health Characteristics of Participants in the STRIDE Study (n (%))

Race

Characteristic Overall White Black p-value
(n= 689) (n= 544) (n= 145)

Age (mean (sd)) 50.9 ( 6.4) 50.8 (6.4) 51.5 (6.4) 0.220

BMIa (mean (sd)) 30.0 (7.6) 29.2 (7.1) 33.1 (8.8) < 0.0001

Social Support (Interpersonal 3.4 (0.6) 3.4 (0.6) 3.3 (0.6) 0.011
Support Eval List) (mean (sd))

Emotional Well-Being (mean (sd)) 45.5 (10.6) 46.0(10.6) 43.5(10.3) 0.009

Marital Status (Married)(n(%)) 367 (53.3) 328 (60.3) 39 (26.9) <0.0001

Education(n(%)) <0.0001
   ≤ High school 108 (15.7) 52 (9.6) 56 (38.6)
   Some College 182 (26.4) 131 (24.1) 51 (35.2)
   Completed College 162 (23.5) 146 (26.8) 16 (11.0)
   Graduate Degree 237 (34.4) 215 (39.5) 22 (15.2)

Weight Perception (n(%)) <0.0001
   Healthy 181 (26.3) 148 (27.2) 33 (22.8)
   Too Much 493 (71.5) 390 (71.7) 103 (71.0)
   Not Enough 15 (2.2) 6 (1.1) 9 (6.2)

Menopause Status(n(%)) <0.0001
   OCP/Premenopause 186 (27) 158 (29.0) 28 (19.3)
   Perimenopause 145 (21.0) 125 (23.0) 20 (13.8)
   Postmenopause/Hysterectectomy 358 (52) 261 (48.0) 97 (66.9)

Comorbidities(n(%)) <0.0001
   None 223 (32.4) 195 (35.8) 28 (19.3)
   1–2 352 (51.1) 284 (52.2) 68 (46.9)
   ≥ 3 114 (16.5) 65 (11.9) 49 (33.8)
a

BMI: Body Mass Index = Weight (kilograms)/height (meters2)

OCP: Oral Contraceptive

Greater proportions of White as compared to Black participants were married, had a graduate education, were perimenopausal and had no medical comorbidities. Most of the Black participants were postmenopausal or had had a hysterectomy, had three or more comorbidities and had some college or a high school education. On average, Black participants had a higher BMI (33.1) than their White counterparts (29.2).

Models Examining Association of Sociodemographic Characteristics and Weight Perception

In unvariable models, a higher BMI was associated with perceptions of weighing too much (Relative Risk Ratio (RRR) [95% confidence interval (CI)]: 1.5 [1.4, 1.6]) (Table 2). Compared to White women, Black women thought they did not weigh enough (RRR [95% CI]: 6.7 [2.2, 20.2]). Additionally, reporting ≥ 3 comorbidities was associated with being more likely to have an abnormal weight perception (too much or not enough): (RRR [95% CI]: 2.2 [1.3, 3.8], and (7.2 [1.7, 31.1), respectively.

Table 2.

Univariable Models examining association of sociodemographic characteristics and weight perception*

Weight Perception*, RRR (95% CI)

Characteristic Healthy Too Much Not Enough
(N = 181) (N = 493) (N= 15)

Black ref 1.2 (0.8, 1.8) 6.7 (2.2, 20.2)c

Age (years) ref 1.0 (.98, 1.0) 1.0 (0.9, 1.1)

BMI ** ref 1.5 (1.4, 1.6)c 0.7 (0.6, 0.9)b

Married ref 1.1 (0.8, 1.5) 0.2 (0.06, 0.8)a

Social Support ref 0.7 (0.5, 1.0)a 0.2 (0.1, 0.5)c

Emotional Well-Being ref 1.0 (.97–1.0)b 0.9 (.86–.95)c

Education
   ≤ High school ref
   Some College 1.8 (1.1, 3.2)a 0.4 (0.09, 1.6)
   Completed College 1.5 (0.8, 2.6) 0.4 (0.08, 1.5)
   Graduate Degree .98 (0.6, 1.6) 0.1 (0.02, 0.6)b

Menopause Status
   OCP/Premenopause ref
   Perimenopause 1.5 (0.9, 2.6) 2.7 (0.4, 16.9)
   Postmenopause/Hysterectomy 1.4 (0.9, 2.0) 3.3 (0.7, 15.7)

Comorbidities
   None ref
   1–2 2.2 (1.5, 3.2)c 2.2 (0.5, 9.2)
   ≥ 3 2.2 (1.3, 3.8)b 7.2 (1.7, 31.1)b
*

Response to the question “Do you think your current weight is healthy?” yes (referent); no, I weigh too much; or no I don’t weigh enough.

**

BMI: Body Mass Index = Weight (kilograms)/height (meters2)

a

p≤ 0.05,

b

p≤ 0.01,

c

p≤ 0.001

In models adjusted for BMI and covariates outlined in Table 1, compared to White women, Black women were less likely to report that they weighed too much, and more likely to report that they did not weigh enough (RRR [95% CI]: 0.4 [0.2, 0.9] and 14.2 [1.8, 110], respectively) (Table 3). In addition, higher BMI (continuous) and higher educational attainment remained associated with perceptions of weighing too much, and lower BMI and poor emotional well-being remained associated with perceptions of not weighing enough (Table 3). As expected, analyzing BMI as a categorical variable yielded a similar pattern of results, and was significant (p=0.0001).

Table 3.

Multivariable Models examining association of sociodemographic characteristics and weight perception*

Weight Perception, RRR (95% CI)

Characteristic Healthy Too Much Not Enough
(N=181) (N=493) (N=15)

Black ref 0.4 (0.2, 0.9)a 14.2 (1.8, 110)b

Age (years) ref 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) 1.1 (0.9, 1.3)

BMI** ref 1.6 (1.4, 1.7)c 0.6 (0.4, 0.8)c

Married ref 1.4 (0.9, 2.3) 0.7 (0.1, 4.4)

Social Support ref 0.8 (0.5, 1.3) 0.4 (0.1, 1.5)

Emotional Well-Being ref 1.0 (0.9, 1.0) 0.9 (0.8, 0.9)b

Education
   ≤ High school ref
   Some College 2.2 (0.9, 5.1) 2.2 (0.3, 17.6)
   Completed College 3.5 (1.5, 8.4)b 0.6 (0.05, 8.2)
   Graduate Degree 2.6 (1.1, 5.8)a 0.3 (0.03, 4.0)

Menopause Status
   OCP/Premenopause ref
   Perimenopause 1.3 (0.6, 2.6) 7.3 (0.5, 106)
   Postmenopause/Hysterectomy 1.1 (0.5, 2.4) 1.4 (.08, 23.2)

Comorbidities
   None ref
   1–2 1.5 (0.9, 2.4) 0.2 (0.2, 1.5)
   ≥ 3 0.4 (0.2, 1.1) 0.9 (0.08, 10.6)
*

Response to the question “Do you think your current weight is healthy?” yes (referent); no, I weigh too much; or no I don’t weigh enough.

**

BMI: Body Mass Index = Weight (kilograms)/height (meters2)

a

p≤ 0.05,

b

p≤ 0.01,

c

p≤ 0.001

The interaction between race and BMI (continuous) was significant (RRR [95% CI]: 0.8 [0.7, 0.9]), such that at higher BMIs, Black women were less likely to perceive themselves as weighing too much than were White women. Including the interaction, Black women with higher BMIs were not more likely to report that they did not weigh enough than were White women (RRR [95% CI]: 0.9 [0.5, 1.6]). With BMI as a categorical variable, the interaction model results were not significant.

DISCUSSION

This study examined the association between weight perception and actual body weight in mid-life women. Not surprisingly, a higher BMI was associated with reporting that one weighed too much, while lower BMI’s were associated with reporting that one didn’t weigh enough. However, we found differences between White and Black women, such that Black women were less likely to perceive their body weight as too great, irrespective of actual body size. This is consistent with the analysis by Wolfe, which examined the “cultural tolerance of fatness” in African-American women.19 Wolfe hypothesized that acceptance of heavier weights in African-American women could be secondary to decreased pre-occupation with thinness as an ideal body weight and the interaction of perceived ideal body weight by socioeconomic position. Others have postulated similar theories. 2022 Our study did not focus on how perceptions of body weight differed according to socioeconomic position specifically. We did see significant gradients in body weight perceptions with educational attainment, one measure of socioeconomic position. However, our findings persisted controlling for educational attainment. It is important to note that there is some evidence that suggests that African-American women are dissatisfied at higher weights, which seems to be impacted by other factors such as social class. 21, 23 The perception of a body weight that is ideal or healthy varies considerably by racial group.

According to the Center for Disease Control (CDC), during the past 20 years, there has been a dramatic increase in obesity in the United States. Even more troubling is that Blacks had 51 percent higher prevalence of obesity compared with Whites in the 2006–2008 Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System (BRFSS) data. The perception of not weighing enough despite having a mean BMI that was classified as obese in Black women may result in a lack of weight loss attempts.2426 This may contribute to Black women’s higher prevalence of obesity-related medical conditions including diabetes and cardiovascular disease.

This study has several limitations that deserve mention. It is a cross-sectional study of women, who are already participating in an ongoing research project, and as such causality cannot be inferred and the population may not generalize to other settings. Additionally, the cross-sectional design limits the temporality of the association studied. Due to missing data and small sample sizes, analysis of certain variables were limited. The sample sizes were small for women who perceived themselves as “not weighing enough,” resulting in large confidence intervals particularly around race. This should be reexamined in a larger study. Our classification of menopausal status is based on bleeding patterns. Because women taking OCPs and those who have had hysterectomies cannot be classified based on bleeding patterns, and the small numbers of women in these groups, we considered women on OCPs to be pre-menopausal and women with a hysterectomy to be post-menopausal. We only used a single question to assess weight perception, and, therefore, cannot comment in detail on the more general concept of body image. Finally, the single measure for adiposity (BMI) limits the analysis. Future work could include other measures, such as waist-to-hip ratio, which may be a better predictor of obesity and its associated health risks.

CONCLUSION

The results of this study have direct implications for clinical care. Any physician should have a discussion with their patients with a BMI greater than or equal to 26 (thus including overweight and obese patients) about how to lose weight, the psychosocial factors that fuel their weight gain, or lack of weight loss, and the health effects of this weight. Similar to the 5 A’s approach established for smoking cessation (a behavior and counseling model used by healthcare providers to help patients quit smoking), we propose the creation and testing of a model for weight loss (in congruence with the United States Preventative Services Task Force (USPSTF) guidelines that advocate screening for and intervening on obesity) to be used by healthcare providers. 27, 28 Instituting such interventions not only to emphasizes the importance of addressing obesity in clinical care, but also to raise awareness that a patient’s perception of what constitutes overweight/obese may vary from established clinical categories, particularly among certain racial groups. They also allow us to place discussions of obesity in the appropriate clinical context, with attention to the psychosocial overlay.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We would like to thank the STRIDE participants, without whom this work would not be possible.

Dr. Hess and STRIDE were supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health’s National Institute on Aging (K23AG024254)

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Conflict of Interest: The authors have no financial or personal relationships that could inappropriately influence (or bias) the author’s decisions, work, or manuscript.

References

  • 1.Harris MI. Noninsulin-dependent diabetes mellitus in black and white Americans. Diabetes Metab Rev. 1990 Mar;6(2):71–90. doi: 10.1002/dmr.5610060202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hertz RP, Unger AN, Cornell JA, Saunders E. Racial disparities in hypertension prevalence, awareness, and management. Arch Intern Med. 2005 Oct 10;165(18):2098–2104. doi: 10.1001/archinte.165.18.2098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Altabe M. Ethnicity and body image: quantitative and qualitative analysis. Int J Eat Disord. 1998 Mar;23(2):153–159. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199803)23:2<153::aid-eat5>3.0.co;2-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Thompson JK, van den Berg P, Roehrig M, Guarda AS, Heinberg LJ. The sociocultural attitudes towards appearance scale-3 (SATAQ-3): development and validation. Int J Eat Disord. 2004 Apr;35(3):293–304. doi: 10.1002/eat.10257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Chandler-Laney PC, Hunter GR, Bush NC, et al. Associations among body size dissatisfaction, perceived dietary control, and diet history in African American and European American women. Eat Behav. 2009 Dec;10(4):202–208. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2009.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Baturka N, Hornsby PP, Schorling JB. Clinical implications of body image among rural African-American women. J Gen Intern Med. 2000 Apr;15(4):235–241. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2000.06479.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lynch E, Liu K, Wei GS, Spring B, Kiefe C, Greenland P. The relation between body size perception and change in body mass index over 13 years: the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) study. Am J Epidemiol. 2009 Apr 1;169(7):857–866. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Macdonald HM, New SA, et al. Longtitudinal Changes in weight gain in perimenopausal and early postmenopausal women: effects of dietary energy intake, energy expenditure, dietary calcium intake and hormone replacement therapy. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2003;27(26) doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802283. 669 0676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Wang Q, Hassager C, Ravn P, Wang S, Christiansen C. Total and regional body-composition changes in early postmenopausal women: age-related or menopause-related? Am J Clin Nutr. 1994 Dec;60(6):843–848. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/60.6.843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ritland LM, Alekel DL, Matvienko OA, et al. Centrally located body fat is related to appetitive hormones in healthy postmenopausal women. Eur J Endocrinol. 2008 Jun;158(6):889–897. doi: 10.1530/EJE-07-0836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wing RR, Matthews KA, Kuller LH, Meilahn EN, Plantinga PL. Weight gain at the time of menopause. Arch Intern Med. 1991 Jan;151(1):97–102. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sternfeld B, Wang H, Quesenberry CP, Jr, et al. Physical activity and changes in weight and waist circumference in midlife women: findings from the Study of Women's Health Across the Nation. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;160(9):912–922. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwh299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Matthews KA, Abrams B, Crawford S, et al. Body mass index in mid-life women: relative influence of menopause, hormone use, ethnicity. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2001 Jun;25(6):863–873. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kruger J, Lee CD, Ainsworth BE, Macera CA. Body size satisfaction and physical activity levels among men and women. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2008 Aug;16(8):1976–1979. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hess R, Olshansky E, Ness R, et al. Pregnancy and birth history influence women's experience of menopause. Menopause. 2008 May-Jun;15(3):435–441. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e3181598301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ogden CL, Carroll MD, McDowell MA, Flegal KM. Obesity among adults in the United States--no statistically significant chance since 2003–2004. NCHS Data Brief. 2007 Nov;(1):1–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hess R, Conroy MB, Ness R, et al. Association of lifestyle and relationship factors with sexual functioning of women during midlife. J Sex Med. 2009 May;6(5):1358–1368. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2009.01225.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Cohen S, Wills TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol Bull. 1985 Sep;98(2):310–357. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wolfe WA. Obesity and the African-American woman: a cultural tolerance of fatness or other neglected factors. Ethn Dis. 2000 Autumn;10(3):446–453. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bowen DJ, Tomoyasu N, Cauce AM. The triple threat: a discussion of gender, class, and race differences in weight. Women Health. 1991;17(4):123–143. doi: 10.1300/j013v17n04_06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Caldwell MB, Brownell KD, Wilfley DE. Relationship of weight, body dissatisfaction, and self-esteem in African American and white female dieters. Int J Eat Disord. 1997 Sep;22(2):127–130. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199709)22:2<127::aid-eat2>3.0.co;2-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Davis EM, Clark JM, Carrese JA, Gary TL, Cooper LA. Racial and socioeconomic differences in the weight-loss experiences of obese women. Am J Public Health. 2005 Sep;95(9):1539–1543. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.047050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Buck BH, Black SE, Behrmann M, Caldwell C, Bronskill MJ. Spatial- and object-based attentional deficits in Alzheimer's disease. Relationship to HMPAO-SPECT measures of parietal perfusion. Brain. 1997 Jul;120(Pt 7):1229–1244. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.7.1229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kumanyika SK, Obarzanek E, Stevens VJ, Hebert PR, Whelton PK. Weight-loss experience of black and white participants in NHLBI-sponsored clinical trials. Am J Clin Nutr. 1991 Jun;53(6 Suppl):1631S–1638S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/53.6.1631S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Tyler DO, Allan JD, Alcozer FR. Weight loss methods used by African American and Euro-American women. Res Nurs Health. 1997 Oct;20(5):413–423. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-240x(199710)20:5<413::aid-nur5>3.0.co;2-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Levy AS, Heaton AW. Weight control practices of U.S. adults trying to lose weight. Ann Intern Med. 1993 Oct 1;119(7 Pt 2):661–666. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-119-7_part_2-199310011-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Fiore MC. US public health service clinical practice guideline: treating tobacco use and dependence. Respir Care. 2000 Oct;45(10):1200–1262. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Screening for obesity in adults: recommendations and rationale. Ann Intern Med. 2003 Dec 2;139(11):930–932. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-139-11-200312020-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES