Skip to main content
International Journal of Epidemiology logoLink to International Journal of Epidemiology
. 2012 Dec 12;42(1):160–171. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys207

Cognition and behavioural development in early childhood: the role of birth weight and postnatal growth

Cheng Huang 1,*, Reynaldo Martorell 2, Aiguo Ren 3, Zhiwen Li 3
PMCID: PMC3600622  PMID: 23243117

Abstract

Background We evaluate the relative importance of birth weight and postnatal growth for cognition and behavioural development in 8389 Chinese children, 4–7 years of age.

Method Weight was the only size measure available at birth. Weight, height, head circumference and intelligence quotient (IQ) were measured between 4 and 7 years of age. Z-scores of birth weight and postnatal conditional weight gain to 4–7 years, as well as height and head circumference at 4–7 years of age, were the exposure variables. Z-scores of weight at 4–7 years were regressed on birth weight Z-scores, and the residual was used as the measure of postnatal conditional weight gain. The outcomes were child’s IQ, measured by the Chinese Wechsler Young Children Scale of Intelligence, as well as internalizing behavioural problems, externalizing behavioural problems and other behavioural problems, evaluated by the Child Behavior Checklist 4–18. Multivariate regressions were conducted to investigate the relationship of birth weight and postnatal growth variables with the outcomes, separately for preterm children and term children.

Results Both birth weight and postnatal weight gain were associated with IQ among term children; 1 unit increment in Z-score of birth weight (∼450 g) was associated with an increase of 1.60 [Confidence interval (CI): 1.18–2.02; P < 0.001] points in IQ, and 1 unit increment in conditional postnatal weight was associated with an increase of 0.46 (CI: 0.06–0.86; P = 0.02) points in IQ, after adjustment for confounders; similar patterns were observed when Z-scores of postnatal height and head circumference at age 4–7 years were used as alternative measurements of postnatal growth. Effect sizes of relationships with IQ were smaller than 0.1 of a standard deviation in all cases. Neither birth weight nor postnatal growth indicators were associated with behavioural outcomes among term children. In preterm children, neither birth weight nor postnatal growth measures were associated with IQ or behavioural outcomes.

Conclusions Both birth weight and postnatal growth were associated with IQ but not behavioural outcomes for Chinese term children aged 4–7 years, but the effect sizes were small. No relation between either birth weight or postnatal growth and cognition or behavioural outcomes was observed among preterm children aged 4–7 years.

Keywords: Cognition, internalizing behavioural problems, externalizing behavioural problems, birth weight, postnatal weight gain

Introduction

Nutrition is vital to brain development.1 The evidence suggests that undernutrition during ‘critical’ or ‘sensitive’ periods, predominantly during pregnancy and the early postnatal period, may have profound effects on cognition and behaviour throughout life.2,3 For example, as an indicator of prenatal malnutrition, low birth weight (birth weight <2500 g) is associated with lower cognition,4–6 poor school performance,5,7,8 and behavioural disorders.4,8–11 Positive relation of birth weight with intelligence quotient (IQ) was also observed across the normal range of birth weights,12–16 but inconsistently so at very high values of birth weight.16,17 The relations are shown to be robust to adjustment for confounders including family socioeconomic status (SES) and other familial confounding in sibship studies.14,18,19 Postnatal growth matters as well. Consistent evidence from many countries shows that early stunting, defined as height-for-age below −2 standard deviation (SD) of reference values, predicts lower cognition and educational achievement.20,21 Failure to thrive in infancy and childhood has been reported to be associated with behavioural abnormalities.22

What remains unclear is the relative importance of prenatal compared with postnatal growth on cognitive function and behavioural development, and evidence from the few relevant studies is inconclusive.22–29 For example, a study in the UK found an association of cognitive function with both birth weight and weight gain in infancy, birth weight having a relatively stronger impact.30 Another study in Guatemala found that early postnatal growth (0–2 years), but not prenatal or late postnatal growth, predicts women’s later educational achievement.24 Findings from the Newcastle Thousand Families Study suggested that height at the ages of 9 and 13 years, but not birth weight, predict childhood IQ at age 11 years.31 The inconsistency may be due to the use of different indicators of growth and human development across the studies, different time points when postnatal growth was measured, random error due to small sample size, and lack of control for some important confounders.24,28 In addition, major flaws existed in some previous studies. For example, including birth weight and postnatal weight directly in the same regression may confront problems of multicollinearity because growth is correlated across time.32,33 Only a few studies have taken into account the correlation between prenatal and postnatal growths, using appropriate analytic strategies including multi-stage least square estimation,23,24,34 and structural equation modeling.28

In the present study, we explored the relative importance of birth weight and postnatal growth in early childhood for a broad measure of cognitive and behavioural development, using a large sample (n = 8389) of Chinese children aged 4–7 years. We employed a two-stage least square estimation to address methodological issues caused by correlations among measures of weight at different ages. We were also interested in whether the findings were different between preterm children and term children.

Materials and Methods

Study sample

The data were from a China–US collaborative project designed to prevent neural tube birth defects with periconceptual folic acid supplementation during the period 1993–1996, which was approved and conducted by the U.S. Centers for Diseases Control and Prevention (CDC) and Peking University Health Science Center (PUHSC).35,36 The program enrolled more than 247 000 women who were preparing for marriage from Hebei, Zhejiang and Jiangsu provinces in China. Upon enrollment, extensive information from the participants, including basic demographics, education, occupation, other family, SES and general health conditions was obtained by well-trained medical staff, and the pregnancy/birth outcomes were tracked by the maternal and child health hospital-based monitoring system.35 In 2001, a random sample of 9100 children aged 4–7 years was selected from the children born to these women, and extensive data were collected concerning growth, cognition and behavioural development.37,38

We limited our sample to 8919 singleton live births, and we further excluded cases with missing data on child’s sex, birth order, birth weight, gestational age, mother’s age at delivery, maternal pre-pregnancy weight, mother taking folic acid during pregnancy, maternal height, maternal education, father’s occupation and mother’s IQ, as well as child’s height, weight, head circumference, IQ and behavioural problems at 4–7 years old. Our final sample included 8389 (94% of the 8919) children aged 4–7 years: 4424 boys and 3965 girls.

Measurements

Assessment of cognitive ability

The assessments of cognitive ability among the mothers and children were conducted at county maternal and children’s hospitals or designated township hospitals by well-trained neurological pediatricians, and interview appointments were arranged at the convenience of the mothers to administer the Chinese Wechsler Young Children Scale of Intelligence (C-WYCSI) for their children and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised in China for themselves (WAIS-RC).38

Maternal cognitive ability

The children’s biologic mothers’ IQ was assessed using the WAIS-RC, which is based on the WAIS with specific questions adapted to the Chinese setting. The WAIS-RC exhibited satisfactory reliability and validity similar to those of the WAIS.39,40

Child cognitive ability

The C-WYCSI was employed to measure intelligence of the children. C-WYCSI is based on the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI), but was adapted to the Chinese setting. Satisfactory validity and reliability of WPPSI have been confirmed by previous studies.41,42 The C-WYCSI retains five subsets of the original WPPSI, namely knowledge, arithmetic comprehension, picture completion mazes, block design and geometric design. The remaining three subsets (vocabulary, similarities and animal house) were substituted with picture vocabulary, picture summary and animal peg, respectively. C-WYCSI also includes an additional subset (pattern match). Like the WPPSI, the C-WYCSI adjusts for age and sex. Two standardized versions of the C-WYCSI, slightly differing in the numbers of items for each subset, were administered to children in rural and urban areas taking into account the marked cultural, educational and economic disparities between the areas.43 Studies show similar psychometric characteristics for the C-WYCSI in urban children and rural children, and the C-WYCSI could be validly and equally applied to the two groups.41 A raw score was first obtained on each subset and then converted to an age-scaled score of IQ according to a nationally standardized norm of China; a detailed description of the C-WYCSI is given elsewhere.41,44

Child behavioural problems

The assessment was based on the Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) 4–18, a widely used tool designed to evaluate children’s competencies and problem behaviours.45 The CBCL scale scores from societies with different cultures and languages were very comparable.46 CBCL was translated into Chinese in the 1980s and has been widely used since then, and the construct validity of the CBCL has been established.47–51 Comparison of children’s behavioural and emotional problems reported by parents in Chinese and American samples suggested that cross-cultural differences were generally modest in magnitude.52 In this survey, parents of the children were invited to administer the CBCL at county maternal and children’s hospitals or designated township hospitals. Under the instructions of two interviewers, who were well-trained neurological pediatricians, informants (in most cases, the children’s mothers) rated the children using a three-point scale (0 = not true; 1 = somewhat or sometimes true; and 2 = very true or often true) for each of 118 problem behaviour items.37 These items capture several broad scales classified as internalizing syndromes (withdrawal, somatic complaints and anxiety/depression), externalizing problems (delinquency and aggressive behaviour) and other problems (social problems, thought problems and attention problems).45

Birth weight and postnatal growth

Birth weight was measured within 24 h after delivery. Postnatal growth, including height, weight and head circumference, was measured in 2001, when the children were 4–7 years of age, at the same time as the neurodevelopment assessments, by well-trained medical professionals.37

Family SES

SES indicators include maternal education (high school and above, junior high school, elementary school and less), paternal occupation (agricultural worker, industrial worker, governmental employee/office worker and other).

Other variables

Other variables in the analysis were gestational age, sex and birth order of the child (firstborn vs higher-order births), mother’s age at childbirth (years) and pre-pregnancy maternal weight (kg) and height (cm). We also included whether the mother took a folic acid supplement during pregnancy for its observed effect on cognitive development,53 and urban residence to control for potential effects of urban/rural inequalities in socioeconomic, infrastructural and public health characteristics.43

Statistical analysis

Two-stage least square (2-SLS) modeling

We applied the 2-SLS approach to address potential problems that may be caused by the correlation between birth weight and postnatal weight at later time points. In the first stage, we created ‘conditional postnatal weight gain’ to substitute for the measured postnatal weight gain. This stage involves the prediction of postnatal weight from birth weight and the calculation of residual as ‘conditional postnatal weight gain’. In the second stage, birth weight and the residual (‘conditional postnatal weight gain’) were used as independent variables for assessing the relative importance of prenatal vs postnatal effects in regression models. The 2-SLS approach removes bias due to the correlation between growths at different time points; it also addresses the problem of multicollinearity and complicated interpretation of regression coefficients when compared with results from models in which initial size and subsequent size are simultaneously entered.32,33 We used Z-scores rather than raw scores in the weight variables, as weight-for-age Z-scores capture how far a weight deviates from the mean weight of children of the same age and sex, according to the World Health Organization’s 2006 standard.54 Z-scores are more appropriate than raw score in this analysis because the postnatal measures of weight were obtained from children at different ages, ranging from 4 to 7 years.

Preterm children and term children may follow different growth patterns in the early years of life,55 and they may also differ in cognitive and behavioural development. For example, preterm births may have higher risks for cognitive and behavioural problems as a result of the reduction in gestational duration or obstetric complications.56 We therefore analysed the data separately for preterm children and term children. For each sample, we tested the crude associations of birth weight (Z-score) and ‘postnatal conditional weight gain Z-scores’ with cognitive and behavioural outcomes: general IQ, internalizing behavioural problems, externalizing behavioural problems and other behavioural problems, with each outcome in a single model. In these crude analyses, we controlled for child’s age, in addition to child’s sex, to adjust for potential residual age effect on the age-standardized outcomes. We then tested whether these associations were robust to adjustment for a broad array of confounders including gestational age of the child, birth order of the child (firstborn vs higher-order births), mother’s age at childbirth, pre-pregnancy maternal weight and height, urban residence, father’s occupation (agricultural worker, industrial worker, government employee/office worker and other), mother’s education (high school and above, junior high school, and elementary school and below), whether mother took folic acid supplement during pregnancy and mother’s IQ. In all the models, we estimated cluster-robust standard error because the samples are clustered by residential areas.

We also conducted similar analysis with postnatal height and head circumference as alternative indicators of postnatal growth. We chose not to include postnatal weight and postnatal height simultaneously in the models because of their high correlation (r = 0.62), which may cause multicollinearity problems when including both in the regressions. All analyses were conducted using the Statistical Analysis System (SAS version 8.2).

Effect size analysis

To compare the relative magnitude of associations between growth variables (predictors) and the outcome variables, we also conducted effect-size analysis on any association with a P-value less than 0.05. The effect size is calculated as the absolute change in SD units in the outcome variable per 1-SD change in the predictor; effect sizes of magnitude 0.1, 0.3 and 0.5 are considered small, medium and large, respectively.57

Results

Descriptive information

Table 1 presents descriptive information by term status and sex of children. Approximately 8.5% of births were preterm (born before 37 weeks of gestation), and the average gestational age was 255 days (range: 175–265 days) for both preterm boys and preterm girls. Although the preterm children exhibited lower birth weight compared with term children (−0.67 vs 0.10 in Z-score of birth weight for boys; −0.57 vs 0.10 in Z-score of birth weight for girls), their postnatal growth, indicated by Z-scores of postnatal weight and height, was similar to that of term children. Term children and preterm children exhibited no difference in IQ or in behavioural problems. For both preterm children and term children, boys exhibited higher IQ than girls (Table 1). Boys performed better than girls on most of the subsets of the test, except for animal pegs and picture matching (two subsets measuring performance IQ) on which boys and girls obtained similar scores (results not shown). Similar boys’ advantages on IQ tests was found in the samples used for the standardization of the WISC-R in the US and Scotland, where boys outperformed girls on full-scale, verbal and performance IQ, and obtained higher scores on almost all subsets except for coding and digit span, which are considered to be tests of short-term memory.58 In addition, compared with girls, boys also had more externalizing behavioural problems but similar internalizing behavioural problems and other behavioural problems, which is similar to the gender patterns observed among preschool children across societies.59 All these differences were significant at the 0.05 level.

Table 1.

Characteristics of Chinese children by sex [mean (standard deviation) or percentage]

Characteristics Preterm children
Term children
Boys (N = 388) Girls (N = 266) Boys (N = 4036) Girls (N = 3699)
Birth weight (kg) 3.05 (0.48) 2.99 (0.47) 3.41 (0.39) 3.29 (0.38)
Birth weight<2.5 kg (%) 10.5 11.3 0.7 1.2
Birth weight for age Z-score −0.67 (1.06) −0.57 (1.09) 0.10 (0.78) 0.10 (0.82)
Postnatal weight for age Z-score −1.21 (0.81) −1.20 (0.86) −1.17 (0.85) −1.23 (0.79)
Underweight (%) 16.5 15.4 14.1 15.1
Postnatal height for age Z-score −2.00 (0.93) −1.93 (0.86) −1.95 (0.94) −1.93 (0.91)
Postnatal head circumference 51.1 (1.6) 50.3 (1.6) 50.8 (1.4) 50.1 (1.4)
Taking folic acid during pregnancy (%) 48.7 51.1 51.5 51.1
First born (%) 54.6 56.8 54.3 55.1
Age of child (months) 69.0 (7.4) 69.0 (7.0) 68.4 (7.4) 68.3 (7.4)
Mother’s age at child’s birth (years) 25.8 (3.6) 25.3 (3.3) 25.6 (3.1) 25.6 (3.1)
Gestational age (days) 254.9 (13.1) 254.9 (12.6) 281.4 (8.3) 282.3 (8.4)
Pre-pregnancy maternal weight (kg) 62.3 (7.6) 62.2 (6.6) 64.2 (7.3) 63.8 (7.1)
Maternal height (cm) 159.4 (4.3) 158.9 (4.5) 159.1 (4.5) 159.1 (4.3)
Mother’s education (%)
    High school and above 18.6 19.6 20.9 20.9
    Junior high school 63.7 63.2 61.4 60.2
    Elementary school and below 17.8 17.3 17.8 18.9
Maternal IQ 93.9 (17.0) 94.3 (15.8) 94.8 (16.4) 94.5 (16.5)
Father’s occupation (%)
    Agricultural worker 45.1 45.1 48.2 48.0
    Industrial worker 39.2 38.7 36.5 36.4
    Government employee/office worker 8.5 7.9 8.6 8.5
    Other 7.2 8.3 6.6 7.1
Urban residency (%) 41.2 33.1 38.2 38.3
Child’s general IQ 100.1 (15.6) 97.9 (15.6) 100.2 (16.3) 98.2 (16.4)
Externalizing behavioural problems 8.9 (6.1) 7.1 (5.6) 9.0 (6.4) 7.5 (5.7)
Internalizing behavioural problems 5.0 (4.9) 4.9 (4.5) 4.7 (4.8) 5.0 (4.9)
Other behavioural problems 9.2 (7.0) 8.5 (7.0) 8.9 (7.0) 8.6 (6.9)

Source: China-U.S. Collaborative Project for Neural Tube Defect Prevention.

Relationships of birth weight and postnatal weight gain with cognition and behavioural problems

Among preterm children (Table 2, top panel), birth weight was not associated with full-scale IQ, controlling for sex; postnatal weight gain was positively associated with full-scale IQ at 4–7 years of age (P < 0.01), and a 1-unit increment of Z-score in postnatal weight gain was associated with a 1.94-point (95% CI: 0.44–3.44) increase in full-scale IQ, with an effect size of 0.1. Adjustment for confounders including birth order, gestational age, pre-pregnancy maternal height and weight, mother taking folic acid supplement, mother’s education, father’s occupation, urban/rural residence and mother’s IQ substantially attenuated the association toward the null (0.90; 95% CI: −0.45 to −2.25; P = 0.18). In addition, the R-square values suggested that birth weight, postnatal weight gain and sex of child explained 12.5% of the variance in IQ; with the addition of potential confounders, a total of 32.0% of variance in IQ was explained.

Table 2.

Coefficient estimates (95% CI) of relationships of birth weight and postnatal weight gain with IQ and behavioural problems among Chinese children 4–7 years of age

IQ
Internalizing behavioural problems
Externalizing behavioural problems
Other behavioural problems
Sample Variable Adjusted for sex Adjusted for other factorsa Adjusted for sex Adjusted for other factorsa Adjusted for sex Adjusted for other factorsa Adjusted for sex Adjusted for other factorsa
Preterm children Z-score of birth weight 0.20 (−0.90, 1.29) 0.02 (−1.02,1.07) −0.11 (−0.45,0.23) 0.02 (−0.34,0.38) −0.06 (−0.49,0.35) −0.01 (−0.46, 0.43) −0.04 (−0.53,0.44) 0.04 (−0.48, 0.57)
Postnatal weight gain 1.94 (0.44,3.44) 0.90 (−0.45, 2.25) −0.48 (−0.94,−0.02) −0.45 (−0.92, 0.02) −0.19 (−0.76,0.38) −0.28 (−0.86, 0.29) −0.29 (−0.95, 0.38) −0.29 (−0.97, 0.39)
R-square 0.125 0.320 0.106 0.118 0.140 0.178 0.137 0.158
Term children Z-score of birth weight 1.91 (1.48,2.34) 1.60 (1.18, 2.02) −0.09 (−0.22,0.05) −0.03 (−0.17, 0.11) −0.10 (−0.26, 0.06) −0.14 (−0.31, 0.04) −0.15 (−0.34,0.03) −0.13 (−0.33, 0.07)
Postnatal weight gain 0.87 (0.43, 1.30) 0.46 (0.06, 0.86) −0.09 (−0.23, 0.04) −0.09 (−0.23, 0.04) 0.05 (−0.11, 0.22) −0.00 (−0.17, 0.17) 0.04 (−0.14, 0.23) 0.03 (−0.16, 0.22)
R-square 0.134 0.281 0.063 0.070 0.086 0.101 0.098 0.109

aOther factors include birth order and gestational age of child, pre-pregnancy maternal height and weight, mother taking folic acid supplement during pregnancy, mother’s education and IQ, father’s occupation and urban residency.

Source: China-U.S. Collaborative Project for Neural Tube Defect Prevention.

Regarding the behavioural outcomes, postnatal weight gain, rather than birth weight, is negatively associated with internalizing behavioural problems when controlling for sex of child, and an increment of 1 point in postnatal weight gain was associated with a 0.48-point (95% CI: 0.02–0.94; P = 0.04) decrease in child’s internalizing behavioural problems, but this association was no longer significant (P = 0.06) after adjusting for confounders. Neither birth weight nor postnatal weight gain was associated with externalizing behavioural problems or other behavioural problems.

The lower panel (Table 2) presents the results for term children. Both birth weight and postnatal weight gain were positively associated with full-scale IQ of the child when controlling for sex of the child. A 1-unit increment of Z-score in birth weight was associated with a 1.91-point (95% CI: 1.48–2.34; P < 0.001) increase in IQ, with an effect size of 0.09; and an increment of 1 point in postnatal weight gain was associated with a 0.87-point (95% CI: 0.43–1.30; p < 0.001) increase in IQ, with an effect size of 0.04. After adjusting for confounders, the magnitude of the association between birth weight and IQ was reduced to 1.60 (95% CI: 1.18–2.02; P < 0.001) and that between postnatal weight gain and IQ to 0.46 (95% CI: 0.06–0.86; P = 0.02).

Regarding the behavioural outcomes, none of internalizing behavioural problems, externalizing behavioural problems or other behavioural problems was associated with Z-score of birth weight or conditional postnatal weight gain among term children.

Relationships of birth weight and alternative postnatal growth indicators with cognition and behavioural problems

Table 3 presents the relative importance of birth weight and postnatal growth to cognition and behavioural problems when the Z-score of height at age 4–7 years was used as an indicator of postnatal growth.

Table 3.

Coefficient estimates (95% CI) of relationships of birth weight and postnatal height with IQ and behavioural problems among Chinese children 4–7 years of age

IQ
Internalizing behavioural problems
Externalizing behavioural problems
Other behavioural problems
Sample Variable Adjusted for sex Adjusted for other factorsa Adjusted for sex Adjusted for other factorsa Adjusted for sex Adjusted for other factorsa Adjusted for sex Adjusted for other factorsa
Preterm children Z-score of birth weight −0.05 (−1.16, 1.06) −0.08 (−1.13, 0.97) −0.06 (−0.40, 0.28) 0.07 (−0.30, 0.43) −0.05 (−0.47, 0.37) 0.03 (−0.41, 0.48) 0.01 (−0.48, 0.50) 0.13 (−0.40, 0.65)
Postnatal height 1.59 (0.18, 3.01) 0.68 (−0.59, 1.94) −0.34 (−0.77, 0.10) −0.31 (−0.75, 0.12) −0.11 (−0.65, 0.42) −0.16 (−0.70, 0.37) −0.36 (−0.98, 0.27) −0.35 (−0.98, 0.28)
R-square 0.123 0.319 0.103 0.115 0.140 0.166 0.138 0.151
Term children Z-score of birth weight 1.66 (1.23,2.09) 1.49 (1.07, 1.92) −0.08 (−0.21, 0.06) −0.02 (−0.16, 0.13) −0.11 (−0.27, 0.06) −0.12 (−0.30, 0.05) −0.17 (−0.35, 0.02) −0.13 (−0.33, 0.07)
Postnatal height 1.49 (1.10, 1.89) 0.82 (0.46, 1.17) −0.07 (−0.19, 0.05) −0.07 (−0.20, 0.05) 0.02 (−0.13, 0.17) −0.05 (−0.20, 0.10) 0.08 (−0.09, 0.25) 0.05 (−0.12, 0.22)
R-square 0.138 0.296 0.063 0.067 0.086 0.099 0.098 0.106

aOther factors include birth order and gestational age of child, pre-pregnancy maternal height and weight, mother taking folic acid supplement during pregnancy, mother’s education and IQ, father’s occupation and urban residency.

Source: China-U.S. Collaborative Project for Neural Tube Defect Prevention.

Among preterm children (upper panel, Table 3), postnatal height, rather than birth weight, was positively associated with IQ (P = 0.03), controlling for sex; a 1-unit increase in Z-score of postnatal height was associated with a 1.59-point (95% CI: 0.18–3.01) increase in child’s IQ, with an effect size of 0.1. After adjusting for confounders, the magnitude of the association was attenuated by about 60% to 0.68 (95% CI: −0.59 to 1.94; P = 0.29).

The lower panel (Table 3) presents results among term children. Both birth weight and postnatal height were positively associated with IQ of child, controlling for sex (P < 0.001). A 1-unit increment of Z-score in birth weight was associated with a 1.66-point (95% CI: 1.23–2.09) increase in IQ, with an effect size of 0.08; and a 1-unit increment of Z-score in postnatal height was associated with a 1.49-point (95% CI: 1.07–1.92) increase in IQ, with an effect size of 0.08. After adjusting for confounders, the association of IQ with postnatal height was reduced to 0.82 (95% CI: 0.46–1.17; P < 0.001) and that with birth weight was reduced slightly to 1.49 (95% CI: 1.07–1.92; P < 0.001).

Table 3 also shows that for both preterm children (upper panel) and term children (lower panel), neither birth weight nor postnatal height was associated with any of the behavioural outcomes including internalizing behavioural problems, externalizing behavioural problems or other behavioural problems.

As shown in Table 4, results using head circumference at age 4–7 years as an alternative indicator of postnatal growth were very similar to those using height at age 4–7 years as an indicator of postnatal growth (Table 3).

Table 4.

Coefficient estimates (95% CI) of relationships of birth weight and postnatal head circumference with IQ and behavioural problems among Chinese children 4–7 years of age

IQ
Internalizing behavioural problems
Externalizing behavioural problems
Other behavioural problems
Sample Variable Adjusted for sex Adjusted for other factorsa Adjusted for sex Adjusted for other factorsa Adjusted for sex Adjusted for other factorsa Adjusted for sex Adjusted for other factorsa
Preterm children Z-score of birth weight −0.12 (−1.23, 0.99) −0.05 (−1.08, 0.98) −0.10 (−0.45, 0.24) 0.01 (−0.35, 0.37) −0.12 (−0.54, 0.30) −0.03 (−0.47, 0.41) −0.05 (−0.54, 0.44) 0.09 (−0.42, 0.61)
Postnatal head circumference 1.10 (0.31, 1.89) 0.30 (−0.42, 1.02) 0.03 (−0.21, 0.28) 0.05 (−0.20, 0.29) 0.25 (−0.05, 0.55) 0.20 (−0.10, 0.50) 0.05 (−0.30, 0.40) 0.07 (−0.29, 0.43)
R-square 0.126 0.319 0.100 0.111 0.143 0.168 0.136 0.150
Term children Z-score of birth weight 1.46 (1.03, 1.90) 1.24 (0.82, 1.66) −0.09 (−0.22, 0.05) −0.03 (−0.17, 0.12) −0.12 (−0.29, 0.05) −0.13 (−0.31, 0.05) −0.13 (−0.32, 0.06) −0.09 (−0.29, 0.12)
Postnatal head circumference 1.49 (1.22, 1.76) 1.05 (0.80, 1.29) −0.00 (−0.08, 0.08) −0.01 (−0.09, 0.08) 0.07 (−0.03, 0.17) 0.02 (−0.08, 0.12) −0.08 (−0.19, 0.04) −0.10 (−0.22, 0.02)
R-square 0.146 0.301 0.063 0.067 0.086 0.099 0.098 0.107

aOther factors include birth order and gestational age of child, pre-pregnancy maternal height and weight, mother taking folic acid supplement during pregnancy, mother’s education and IQ, father’s occupation and urban residency.

Source: China-U.S. Collaborative Project for Neural Tube Defect Prevention.

Results from additional analyses

For easier interpretation of the results, linear regression models were employed in the main analysis above, which assume that every 1-unit change in growth variables results in the same change in outcome variables across the entire distribution of the exposure variables. The alternative analyses in which intelligence score is modeled as a nonlinear function of birth weight Z-score and the square of birth weight Z-score, plus other variables, showed slightly improved prediction compared with linear regression without square term of birth weight Z-score. Results from non-linear regression suggested that the effect of birth weight on intelligence score is greater when birth weight is lower than that at higher values of birth weight, until birth weight reaches approximately 4.1 kg (1.9 SD above the mean of the Z-score of birth weight in this sample), beyond which point birth weight starts to be negatively associated with intelligence. The pattern is consistent with the quadratic shape of the relation between birth weight and IQ observed in other studies.18,19

To test whether the impacts of birth weight and postnatal growth on IQ decline with age, further analyses were also conducted with subsamples divided by age. As shown in Figure 1, the relation between birth weight and IQ is consistent across ages, whereas postnatal weight has greater effect on IQ at younger age than older age. For children aged 48-60 months, a 1-unit increment in Z-score of postnatal weight is associated with a 1.57-point (95% CI: 0.11–3.02; P = 0.03) increment in IQ controlling for sex, whereas the association decreases to 0.94 (95% CI: 0.31–1.57; P = 0.003) for children aged 61–72 months, and toward the null (0.51; 95 CI: −0.14, 1.16; P = 0.12) for children aged 72 months and above. Similar patterns were observed with other postnatal growth variables.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Estimated changes of children's IQ for one unit change in Z-score of birth weight and conditional postnatal weight by age of children (a) controlling for sex; (b) controlling for confounders

In our sample, the distributions of both externalizing behavioural problems and other behavioural problems were right skewed, and internalizing behavioural problems were not normally distributed either. We also tested whether findings from the main analyses based on normal distribution assumption were robust. In alternative analyses with externalizing behavioural problems and other behavioural problems as outcome variables, we regressed the natural logarithm of the value of outcome variables (we added 0.1 to the value of outcome variables for all the cases to avoid taking the natural logarithm of zero) on birth weight and postnatal growth. In the alternative analysis with internalizing behavioural problems as the outcome variable, we first classified internalizing behavioural problems into five ordered categories by degrees of severity, and then used cumulative logit models to estimate the relative importance of birth weight and postnatal growth. These alternative analyses yielded similar findings regarding the relative importance of birth weight and postnatal growth on behavioural outcomes (results available upon request).

Finally, to test whether these results regarding the relative importance of birth weight and postnatal growth (Tables 2–4) were different between urban children and rural children, we added interaction terms between the growth indicators and urban/rural residence. All the P-values for these interactions were smaller than 0.05, which suggests that the relationships of prenatal growth and postnatal growth on cognition and behavioural problems were very similar for rural children and urban children.

Discussion

We investigated the relative importance of birth weight and postnatal growth in cognitive and behavioural outcomes in a large longitudinal cohort aged 4–7 years among both preterm children and term children in China.

Among preterm children, we found no association between birth weight and any outcome variable including IQ, internalizing behavioural problems, externalizing behavioural problems and other behavioural problems, whereas postnatal weight gain was positively associated with IQ and negatively associated with internalizing behavioural problems. Previous studies report similar findings and conclude that postnatal catch-up growth is crucial for development of pre-term children, especially for their cognitive functioning and neurodevelopment.60–64 However, this beneficial effect of catch-up growth was not robust to adjustment for confounders including family SES and mother’s IQ in our results, which may suggest an alternative mechanism underlying this association. For example, better family SES and higher mother’s IQ may improve both postnatal catch-up growth and cognitive development for preterm children. These findings regarding relative importance of birth weight and postnatal growth among preterm children were primarily based on ‘late preterm’ children who had an average gestational age of 36.4 weeks and showed successful postnatal catch-up growth in terms of weight, height and head circumference at ages 4–7 years; and whether these findings are applicable to children born very preterm merits further research. Previous studies found that the smaller the birth weight, the longer compensatory growth toward normal continues,55 and lack of ‘catch-up’ growth primarily occurs among small for gestational age (SGA) preterm children.65

Among term children, neither birth weight nor postnatal growth predicted any behavioural outcome, whereas both birth weight and postnatal growth were associated with child’s IQ at age 4–7 years, independently of a wide array of potential confounding variables. It is worth noting that all these associations were very small in terms of effect sizes. For example, the effect size for association with IQ is 0.09 for birth weight and 0.05 for postnatal weight gain, i.e. an increment of one SD in birth weight is associated with about 1.6 IQ points and an increment of one SD in postnatal weight is associated with about 0.8 IQ points. Such small effect sizes were also observed in other studies.11,13 For example, a study on 11899 Canadian children aged 6.5 years found that 1 SD in birth weight was associated with 0.82 IQ points (95% CI: 0.54–1.10) after adjusting for confounders, with an effect size of 0.05. For postnatal weight gain trajectories, 1 SD faster weight gain during 0–3 months, 3–12 months and 1–5 years was associated with an increase of 0.77 (95% CI: 0.42–1.11) IQ points (effect size = 0.05), 0.30 (95% CI: 0.02–0.58) IQ points (effect size = 0.02) and 0.40 (95% CI: 0.04–0.76) IQ points (effect size = 0.03), respectively, controlling for confounders and earlier growth. Sizes of the effects observed in studies that relied on neurobehavioural endpoints are often small.66 Such a small effect may not be clinically important for individual children, but should not be considered inconsequential at the population level,14,27 as ‘a large number of people at a small risk may give rise to more cases of disease than the small number who are at a high risk’.67 Therefore, nutritional interventions targeting underweight children in developing countries such as India, where 47% of children under 3 years old were underweight and 18% were severely underweight,68 may present a good strategy to avoid the considerable loss of IQ and developmental potentials in children in these countries.69

Several mechanisms may explain the significant, albeit weak, relationships between growth and cognition. First, proper nutrition during the critical first 1000 days (pregnancy and first 2 years) is vital for both normal growth and brain development, and nutritional deficits have long-term implications for cognitive function, school success and human capital.20,70,71 For example, iodine deficiency during pregnancy is associated with both impaired foetal growth and delayed brain development.72 A meta-analysis suggested that iodine deficiency was associated with a fall of 12 IQ points.73 Malnutrition during the first 1000 days may also reduce the numbers of neurons and synapses, dendritic arborization, and myelination, and consequently result in decreased brain size;74 it may cause changes in the structure or biochemistry of the brain and impair the functioning of the central nervous system.71

Another potential mechanism for the association between IQ and body size may be related to insulin-like growth factor (IGF) and growth hormone (GH).75 IGF and GH play a critical role in determining somatic growth and are also located in key regions of the brain responsible for learning and memory. Increasing evidence suggests IGF may impact on cognitive brain function.76 In children born small for gestational age, GH therapy led to catch-up growth as well as improvements in IQ scores.77

In addition, environmental factors, such as diet and childhood illness, may mediate the association between growth and cognitive development. More subtle mechanisms are also possible. Malnourished children are stunted and thus appear younger which may hamper positive interaction with others, particularly adults; stunted children may be less likely to be challenged to explore and expand their capability by parents and teachers, which may negatively impact on cognitive and behavioural development.33 ‘Functional isolation’ may also contribute, whereby malnourished infants are less active or attentive and seek less stimulation from their physical and social environments.78

Caution should be exercised in interpreting findings from this study. First, although we adjusted for a rich set of confounding factors, including maternal IQ, we cannot exclude the possibility that these observed associations were due to residual confounding by other factors. For example, households that provide better care to pregnant women may also provide more cognitively stimulating growth environments later for the newborn, which may improve children’s IQ.79 Previous sibling studies and twin studies that rule out genetic and shared environmental factors/confounders suggest that such familial confounding is less likely to be important.18,19,80 Second, postnatal growth in our study was measured at a single point at age 4–7 years, and the results cannot speak as to whether the earlier postnatal period may have a greater effect on cognition and behavioural development.81 There is evidence that adverse effects of postnatal growth failure on intellectual development may decline over time,25 which is consistent with our finding that postnatal growth has a larger association with IQ among younger children and that the association is toward the null among children aged 6 years and above. Third, because birth length and head circumference were measured very imprecisely, we used only birth weight as the proxy for prenatal growth,27 which prevented us from assessing associations with growth in height/head circumference during childhood independently of birth length/head circumference at birth. Fourth, the relatively small sample size of preterm children and the lesser statistical power may not allow detection of potential associations between birth weight and IQ and behavioural outcomes among preterm children, if these associations are small. Finally, our sample may not necessarily represent other developing countries. Although underweight and stunting are prevalent among Chinese children,82 China has a very low prevalence of low birth weight, lower than most developing countries and some developed countries,83 which is possibly due to its cultural practices and universal access to basic health and education.84

Despite these limitations, our study contributes to a better understanding of early growth and child development with several strengths. First, it benefited from its longitudinal nature and the large sample with careful measurements by well-trained professionals on key variables such as cognitive and behavioural development of a child. Second, we adjusted for gestational age, which makes birth weight a more accurate proxy of foetal growth. Third, we investigated both cognitive development and behavioural problems, whereas most previous studies focused on either cognition or behavioural dimensions. Finally, we applied rigorous methods to address methodological issues caused by the correlation between birth weight and postnatal weight.

Findings from our study and others confirm links between child growth and cognitive development, although mechanisms underlying these links remain unclear and merit further investigation. Future studies should improve measurements of growth including weight, height/length, head circumference at birth and multiple postnatal time points, and include populations with more severe malnutrition and prenatal and postnatal growth failure. When data become available, a sibling study or twins study design would be highly desirable.19

Funding

National Institutes of Health (1R03HD072104-01)

Conflict of interest: None declared.

References

  • 1.Georgieff MK. Nutrition and the developing brain: nutrient priorities and measurement. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85(Suppl 2):614–20S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/85.2.614S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Martorell R. Undernutrition during pregnancy and early childhood and its consequences for cognitive and behavioural development. In: Young ME, editor. Early Child Development: Investing in the Future. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier; 1997. pp. 39–83. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, et al. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369:145–57. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60076-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rickards A, Kelly E, Doyle L, Callanan C. Cognition, academic progress, behavior and self-concept at 14 years of very low birth weight children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2001;22:11–18. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200102000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Rickards A, Kitchen W, Doyle L, Ford G, Kelly E, Callanan C. Cognition, school performance, and behavior in very-low-birth-weight and normal birth-weight children at 8 years of age: a longitudinal study. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1993;14:363–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Strauss R. Adult functional outcome of those born small for gestational age: twenty-six-year follow-up of the 1970 British Birth Cohort. JAMA. 2000;283:625–32. doi: 10.1001/jama.283.5.625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Klebanov P, Brooksgunn J, Mccormick M. School achievement and failure in very-low-birth-weight children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1994;15:248–56. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.McCormick M, Gortmaker S, Sobol A. Very low birth weight children: behavior problems and school difficulty in a national sample. J Pediatr. 1990;117:687–93. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(05)83322-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Pharoah PO, Stevenson CJ, Cooke RW, Stevenson RC. Prevalence of behaviour disorders in low birthweight infants. Arch Dis Child. 1994;70:271–74. doi: 10.1136/adc.70.4.271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Thapar A, Langley K, Fowler T, et al. Catechol O-methyltransferase gene variant and birth weight predict early-onset antisocial behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:1275–78. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.11.1275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Keyes KM, Keyes MA, March D, Susser E. Levels of risk: maternal-, middle childhood-, and neighborhood-level predictors of adolescent disinhibitory behaviors from a longitudinal birth cohort in the United States. Ment Health Subst Use. 2011;4:22–37. doi: 10.1080/17523281.2011.533445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Jefferis BJ, Power C, Hertzman C. Birth weight, childhood socioeconomic environment, and cognitive development in the 1958 British birth cohort study. BMJ. 2002;325:305. doi: 10.1136/bmj.325.7359.305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lagerstrom M, Bremme K, Eneroth P, Janson CG. Long-term development for girls and boys at age 16-18 as related to birth weight and gestational age. Int J Psychophysiol. 1994;17:175–80. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(94)90033-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Matte TD, Bresnahan M, Begg MD, Susser E. Influence of variation in birth weight within normal range and within sibships on IQ at age 7 years: cohort study. BMJ. 2001;323:310–14. doi: 10.1136/bmj.323.7308.310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Richards M, Hardy R, Kuh D, Wadsworth MEJ. Birth weight and cognitive function in the British 1946 birth cohort: longitudinal population based study. BMJ. 2001;322:199–203. doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7280.199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Shenkin SD, Starr JM, Deary IJ. Birth weight and cognitive ability in childhood: a systematic review. Psycholl Bull. 2004;130:989–1013. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Richards M, Hardy R, Kuh D, Wadsworth MEJ. Birthweight, postnatal growth and cognitive function in a national UK birth cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2002;3:342–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Eriksen W, Sundet JM, Tambs K. Birth weight standardized to gestational age and intelligence in young adulthood: a register-based birth cohort study of male siblings. Am J Epidemiol. 2010;172:530–36. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwq199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Susser E, Eide MG, Begg M. Invited Commentary: The Use of Sibship Studies to Detect Familial Confounding. Am J Epidemiol. 2010;172:537–39. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwq196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Victora CG, Adair L, Fall C, et al. Maternal and child undernutrition: consequences for adult health and human capital. Lancet. 2008;371:340–57. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61692-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Walker SP, Wachs TD, Grantham-McGregor S, et al. Inequality in early childhood: risk and protective factors for early child development. Lancet. 2011;378:1325–38. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60555-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Liu JH, Raine A, Venables PH, Mednick SA. Malnutrition at age 3 years and externalizing behavior problems at ages 8, 11, and 17 years. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:2005–13. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.11.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Heinonen K, Räikkönen K, Pesonen AK, et al. Prenatal and postnatal growth and cognitive abilities at 56 months of age: a longitudinal study of infants born at term. Pediatrics. 2008;121:e1325–33. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-1172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Li HJ, DiGirolamo AM, Barnhart HX, Stein AD, Martorell R. Relative importance of birth size and postnatal growth for women's educational achievement. Early Hum Dev. 2004;76:1–16. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2003.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Mendez MA, Adair LS. Severity and timing of stunting in the first two years of life affect performance on cognitive tests in late childhood. J Nutr. 1999;129:1555–62. doi: 10.1093/jn/129.8.1555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Pongcharoen T, Ramakrishnan U, DiGirolamo AM, et al. Influence of prenatal and postnatal growth on intellectual functioning in school-aged children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166:411–16. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.1413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Richards M, Hardy R, Kuh D, Wadsworth MEJ. Birth weight, postnatal growth and cognitive function in a national UK birth cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2002;31:342–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Silva A, Metha Z, O'Callaghan FJ. The relative effect of size at birth, postnatal growth and social factors on cognitive function in late childhood. Ann Epidemiol. 2006;16:469–76. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2005.06.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Yang S, Tilling K, Martin R, Davies N, Ben-Shlomo Y, Kramer MS. Pre-natal and post-natal growth trajectories and childhood cognitive ability and mental health. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;40:1215–26. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyr094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Corbett SS, Drewett RF, Durham M, Tymms P, Wright CM. The relationship between birthweight, weight gain in infancy, and educational attainment in childhood. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2007;21:57–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2007.00783.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Pearce MS, Deary IJ, Young AH, Parker L. Growth in early life and childhood IQ at age 11 years: the Newcastle Thousand Families Study. Int J Epidemiol. 2005;34:673–77. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyi038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Esrey SA, Casella G, Habicht JP. The use of residuals for longitudinal data-analysis: the example of child growth. Am J Epidemiol. 1990;131:365–72. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Martorell R, Nguyen P. Interrelationship between growth and development in low and middle income countries. Nestle Nutr. 2010;65:99–121. doi: 10.1159/000281151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kuklina EV, Ramakrishnan U, Stein AD, Barnhart FH, Martorell R. Early childhood growth and development in rural Guatemala. Early Hum Dev. 2006;82:425–33. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2005.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Berry RJ, Li Z, Erickson JD, et al. Prevention of neural-tube defects with folic acid in China. Engl J Med. 1999;341:1485–90. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199911113412001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Huang C, Li Z, Wang M, Martore R. Early Life Exposure to the 1959-1961 Chinese Famine Has Long-Term Health Consequences. J Nutr. 2010;140:1874–78. doi: 10.3945/jn.110.121293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Li HT, Ye R, Achenbach TM, et al. Caesarean delivery on maternal request and childhood psychopathology: a retrospective cohort study in China. BJOG. 2011;118:42–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-0528.2010.02762.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Li HT, Ye RW, Pei LJ, Ren AG, Zheng XY, Liu JM. Caesarean delivery on maternal request and childhood intelligence: a cohort study. Chin Med J. 2011;124:3982–87. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ryan JJ, Dai X, Paolo AM. Intersubtest scatter on the mainland Chinese version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Psychol Assess. 1992;4:60. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Yao S, Chen H, Jiang L, Tam W. Replication of factor structure of Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Chinese version in Chinese mainland non-clinical and schizophrenia samples. Psychiatry Clinical Neurosc. 2007;61:379–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01672.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Guo B, Aveyard P, Dai X. The Chinese Intelligence Scale for Young Children: testing factor structure and measurement invariance using the framework of the Wechsler Intelligence Tests. Educ Psychol Meas. 2009;69:459–74. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Tang Q, Gong Y, Dai X, Chen M, Cai T. Chinese Intelligence Scale for Young Children (CISYC): validity study. Chin J Clin Psychol. 1998;6:76–80. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Zimmer Z, Wen M, Kaneda T. A multi-level analysis of urban/rural and socioeconomic differences in functional health status transition among older Chinese. Soc Sci Med. 2010;71:559–67. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.03.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Dai X, Gong Y, Tang Q, et al. The Chinese Intelligence Scale for Young Children: development strategy, item analysis, reliability, and national norms. Chin J Clin Psychol. 1998;6:203–06. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Achenbach T. Child Behavior Checklist 4-18. Burlington ON: University of Vermont; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Rescorla LA, Achenbach TM, Ivanova MY, et al. International comparisons of behavioral and emotional problems in preschool children: parents' reports from 24 societies. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2011;40:456–67. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2011.563472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Leung PWL, Kwong S, Tang C, et al. Test–retest reliability and criterion validity of the Chinese version of CBCL, TRF, and YSR. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:970–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01570.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Liu J, Cheng H, Leung PWL. The application of the preschool child behavior checklist and the caregiver - teacher report form to mainland Chinese children: syndrome structure, gender differences, country effects, and inter-informant agreement. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2011;39:251–64. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9452-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Liu X, Sun Z, Neiderhiser JM, Uchiyama M, Okawa M, Rogan W. Behavioral and emotional problems in Chinese adolescents: parent and teacher reports. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:828–36. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200107000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Tan TX, Dedrick RF, Marfo K. Factor structure and clinical implications of child behavior checklist/1.5–5 ratings in a sample of girls adopted from China. J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32:807–18. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsm025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Tepper P, Liu X, Guo C, Zhai J, Liu T, Li C. Depressive symptoms in Chinese children and adolescents: parent, teacher, and self reports. J Affect Disord. 2008;111:291–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Weine AM, Phillips JS, Achenbach TM. Behavioral and emotional problems among Chinese and American children: Parent and teacher reports for ages 6 to 13. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1995;23:619–39. doi: 10.1007/BF01447666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Roth C, Magnus P, Schjolberg S, et al. Folic Acid Supplements in Pregnancy and Severe Language Delay in Children. JAMA. 2011;306:1566–73. doi: 10.1001/jama.2011.1433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.World Health Organization. Child Growth Standards. Geneva: WHO; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Casey PH. Growth of low birth weight preterm children. Semin Perinatol. 2008;32:20–27. doi: 10.1053/j.semperi.2007.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Talge NM, Holzman C, Wang J, Lucia V, Gardiner J, Breslau N. Late-preterm birth and its association with cognitive and socioemotional outcomes at 6 years of age. Pediatrics. 2010;126:1124–31. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Lynn R, Mulhern G. A comparison of sex differences on the Scottish and American standardisation samples of the WISC-R. Pers Individ Diff. 1991;12:1179–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Egger HL, Angold A. Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: Presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:313–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Belfort MB, Rifas-Shiman SL, Sullivan T, et al. Infant Growth Before and After Term: Effects on Neurodevelopment in Preterm Infants. Pediatrics. 2011;128:e899–906. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-0282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Brandt I, Sticker EJ, Lentze MJ. Catch-up growth of head circumference of very low birth weight, small for gestational age preterm infants and mental development to adulthood. J Pediatr. 2003;142:463–68. doi: 10.1067/mpd.2003.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Casey PH, Whiteside-Mansell L, Barrett K, Bradley RH, Gargus R. Impact of prenatal and/or postnatal growth problems in low birth weight preterm infants on school-age outcomes: An 8-year longitudinal evaluation. Pediatrics. 2006;118:1078–86. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Franz AR, Pohlandt F, Bode H, et al. Intrauterine, Early Neonatal, and Postdischarge Growth and Neurodevelopmental Outcome at 5.4 Years in Extremely Preterm Infants After Intensive Neonatal Nutritional Support. Pediatrics. 2009;123:e101–09. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Yeung MY. Postnatal growth, neurodevelopment and altered adiposity after preterm birth from a clinical nutrition perspective. Acta Paediatr. 2006;95:909–17. doi: 10.1080/08035250600724507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Tudehope DI, Burns Y, Ocallaghan M, Mohay H, Silcock A. The relationship between intrauterine and postnatal-growth on the subsequent psychomotor development of very-low-birth-weight (VLBW) infants. Aust Paediatr J. 1983;19:3–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.1983.tb02041.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Bellinger DC. What is an adverse effect? A possible resolution of clinical and epidemiological perspectives on neurobehavioral toxicity. Environ Res. 2004;95:394–405. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2003.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Rose G. Sick individuals and sick populations. Int J Epidemiol. 1985;14:32–38. doi: 10.1093/ije/14.1.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Gragnolati M. India's Undernourished Children: A Call for Reform and Actio. Washington DC: World Bank Publications; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Engle PL, Black MM, Behrman JR, et al. Strategies to avoid the loss of developmental potential in more than 200 million children in the developing world. Lancet. 2007;369:229–42. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60112-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Prado E, Dewey K. Nutrition and brain development in early life. Insight. A&T Technical Brief 2012, issue 4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Levitsky DA, Strupp BJ. Malnutrition and the brain: changing concepts, changing concerns. J Nutr. 1995;125(Suppl 8):2212S–20S. doi: 10.1093/jn/125.suppl_8.2212S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Zimmermann MB, Jooste PL, Pandav CS. Iodine-deficiency disorders. Lancet. 2008;372:1251–62. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61005-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Qian M, Wang D, Watkins WE, et al. The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr. 2005;14:32–42. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Laus MF, Vales LDMF, Costa TMB, Almeida SS. Early postnatal protein-calorie malnutrition and cognition: a review of human and animal studies. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2011;8:590–612. doi: 10.3390/ijerph8020590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Berger A. Insulin-like growth factor and cognitive function. BMJ. 2001;322:203. doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7280.203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Aleman A, Torres-Alemán I. Circulating insulin-like growth factor I and cognitive function: neuromodulation throughout the lifespan. Prog Neurobiol. 2009;8:256–65. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Van Pareren YK, Duivenvoorden HJ, Slijper FSM, Koot HM, Hokken-Koelega ACS. Intelligence and psychosocial functioning during long-term growth hormone therapy in children born small for gestational age. J Clin Endocr Metab. 2004;89:5295–302. doi: 10.1210/jc.2003-031187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Lozoff B, Klein NK, Nelson EC, McClish DK, Manuel M, Chacon ME. Behavior of Infants with Iro-Deficiency Anemia. Child Dev. 1998;69:24–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Bradley RH. Children's home environments, healths, behavior, and intervention efforts: a review using the HOME inventory as a marker measure. Genet SocGen Psychol Monogr. 1993;119:439–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Newcombe R, Milne BJ, Caspi A, Poulton R, Moffitt TE. Birthweight predicts IQ: Fact or artefact? Twin Res Hum Genet. 2007;10:581–86. doi: 10.1375/twin.10.4.581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Caulfield R. Physical and cognitive development in the first two years. Early Childhood Education Journal. 1996;23:239–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Ke-You G, Da-Wei F. The magnitude and trends of under- and over-nutrition in Asian countries. Biomedical and environmental sciences : Biomed Environ Sci. 2001;14:53–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Xu B, Rantakallio P. Low birth weight in China and Finland. Scand J Soc Med Suppl. 1998;26:10–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Yip R, Li Z, Chong WH. Race and birth weight: the Chinese example. Pediatrics. 1991;87:688–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from International Journal of Epidemiology are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES