Abstract
We investigated psychophysically whether feature-based attention modulates the perception of figure–ground (F–G) segregation and, based on the results, we investigated computationally the neural mechanisms underlying attention modulation. In the psychophysical experiments, the attention of participants was drawn to a specific motion direction and they were then asked to judge the side of figure in an ambiguous figure with surfaces consisting of distinct motion directions. The results of these experiments showed that the surface consisting of the attended direction of motion was more frequently observed as figure, with a degree comparable to that of spatial attention (Wagatsuma et al., 2008). These experiments also showed that perception was dependent on the distribution of feature contrast, specifically the motion direction differences. These results led us to hypothesize that feature-based attention functions in a framework similar to that of spatial attention. We proposed a V1–V2 model in which feature-based attention modulates the contrast of low-level feature in V1, and this modulation of contrast changes directly the surround modulation of border-ownership-selective cells in V2; thus, perception of F–G is biased. The model exhibited good agreement with human perception in the magnitude of attention modulation and its invariance among stimuli. These results indicate that early-level features that are modified by feature-based attention alter subsequent processing along afferent pathway, and that such modification could even change the perception of object.
Keywords: feature-based attention, figure–ground segregation, border-ownership, early vision, psychophysical experiment, computational model
Introduction
Among the numerous objects that are projected onto the retina, attention selects objects that come into our perception (Posner, 1980; Itti and Koch, 2001). Such selection can be made based on space, feature, and object. In all cases, attention modulates the activity of neurons in the visual cortex (McAdams and Maunsell, 1999; Treue and Maunsell, 1999; Martinez-Trujillo and Treue, 2004; Gregoriou et al., 2009; Cohen and Maunsell, 2011; Wagatsuma et al., 2011), thus enhancing perception or even changing the perception (e.g., Palmer et al., 1993; Solmon et al., 1997; Hasson et al., 2001; Carrasco et al., 2004; Mitchell et al., 2004). For example, Tzvetanov et al. (2006) have shown psychophysically that motion direction acts as a feature and motion-discrimination is enhanced around an attended motion direction. Physiological studies have reported that a majority of cells in V2 are selective to border-ownership (BO; Zhou et al., 2000), which is a precursor of figure–ground (F–G) segregation, and that spatial attention modulates the activities of the cells (e.g., Qiu et al., 2007).
Although the neural mechanisms underlying the modulation of perception by attention have been studied extensively (e.g., Deco and Lee, 2004; Liu et al., 2007a; Ling et al., 2009; Reynolds and Heeger, 2009; Baluch and Itti, 2011), the manner via which the top-down signal mediating attention affects the bottom-up flow of F–G segregation has not been clarified. Top-down attention appears to modulate lower-level features, with modulation of contrasts in V1 at the lowest level (Lee et al., 1999; Paradiso, 2002; Carrasco, 2011). The afferent transmission beginning in the low-level features should gradually establish the perception as the signal progresses through the hierarchy of the visual pathway. Our previous computational study suggested that the luminance contrast in V1 within an attended location is modified by spatial attention, so that further processing in V2 is altered, to modulate the perception of F–G (Wagatsuma et al., 2008). Specifically, spatial attention strengthens the low-level feature contrast extracted in V1, followed by the feeding of this modified contrast to BO-selective cells in V2 via the surround modulation. As BO is determined based on the balance of surround low-level feature contrast between the sides with respect to its Classical Receptive Field (CRF) (Sakai and Nishimura, 2006), the response of a BO-selective cell is enhanced if spatial attention is directed to its preferred direction. Our model of spatial attention accounted for the mechanism of F–G switching and reproduced the human perception. It is natural to expect that a neural mechanism similar to this spatial attention underlies feature-based and object-based attention, as a common mechanism for which attention affects bottom-up flow. Recent studies support this expectation. A physiological study has implied that spatial- and feature-based attention affect local populations of cells in similar ways (Cohen and Maunsell, 2011). A large-scale simulation study has suggested that the differences between spatial and feature-based attention emerge from differential top-down influences on visual cortical networks rather than from the presence of different neural circuits specialized for the two types of attention (Wagatsuma et al., 2012).
We investigated whether and how feature-based attention modulates F–G segregation. First, we conducted psychophysical experiments to examine whether BO perception is modified by feature-based attention. In these experiments, the attention of participants was drawn to a specific motion direction via a motion-discrimination task similar to Tzvetanov’s et al. (2006) experiments (Figure 1), and participants were then asked to judge the side of figure in an ambiguous figure with surfaces consisting of distinct motion directions. The results of these experiments showed that feature-based attention alters perception, leading to the observation of the figure on the surface consisting of the attended direction of motion, with a degree comparable to that of spatial attention. Furthermore, these experiments showed that perception is dependent on the distribution of the motion direction difference along the border between figure and ground, and independent of the object size. Second, we examined computationally whether a neural mechanism similar to that of spatial attention underlies the perception. Specifically, based on the results of the psychophysical experiment, we proposed that feature-based attention modulates the feature contrast or edge, specifically motion direction differences, in early vision and that this modulation of the low-level feature contrast changes the activities of BO-selective cells directly via the surround modulation, thus modifying the perception of BO. We constructed a V1–V2 model and performed simulations of the model with ambiguous figures mimicking our psychophysical experiments. The model exhibited good agreement with human perception regarding the magnitude of attention modulation and reproduced its invariance among stimuli. These results suggest that early-level features that are modified by feature-based attention alter subsequent processing along the afferent pathway, and that such modification could even change the selection of an object during F–G segregation.
Materials and Methods
Psychophysical experiments
Stimuli
We used two types of stimuli consisting of a moving Random Dots Patterns (RDP): a cue stimulus for drawing attention to a specific motion direction (Figure 1A) and a test stimulus for examining the perception of Direction Of Figure (DOF, Sajda and Finkel, 1995) (Figure 1B). The cue stimulus aimed to lead the feature-based attention of participants to a specific motion direction (Figure 1A). Two circular surfaces of moving RDPs were transparently superimposed in space within a diameter of 8° (degree of the visual angle). One surface included an RDP moving toward the right and upward (35°, 45°, or 55° clockwise from the vertical) and the other surface included an RDP moving toward the left and upward (−35°, −45°, or −55°). We superimposed two surface with distinct directions of motion to effectively yield motion-based attention (Tzvetanov et al., 2006). All RDPs had a luminance of 3.84 cd/m2 and a speed of 4°/s (for Experiments 1 and 2) or 5°/s (for Experiment 3). We adopted faster speed for Experiment 3 in order to cancel out stimulus complexity that may lower the effect of attention. Diagonal bars (11° × 11°, 3.84 cd/m2) oriented either right and upward (45°) or left and upward (−45°) were chosen and drawn outside the circles, as references. Participants were instructed to fixate a small square (0.2° × 0.2°, 162.4 cd/m2) located at the center of the screen and judge whether the motion direction of RDP was more “counterclockwise” or “clockwise” with respect to the reference bars that were presented at the time (motion-discrimination task). This method for drawing attention to a specific direction of motion has been utilized by Tzvetanov et al. (2006). They showed that spatial interactions among sensory information were eliminated as far as attention was directed to a target direction of motion.
The DOF of a test stimulus was designed to be ambiguous along the vertical through a fixation point (Figure 1B). Ambiguous figures were generated from the combinations of pseudo white-noise random-block objects (see the Appendix for the generation algorithm; Sakai and Nishimura, 2006; Wagatsuma et al., 2008; Sakai et al., 2012). The shape of each object was generated from up to six square blocks that were placed within a 4 × 4 grid, for Experiments 1 and 2, or eight square blocks placed within a 6 × 6 grid, for Experiment 3. The RDP within one object moved toward +45° and the other toward −45°, surrounded by a RDP moving upward with a diameter of 12°. Differences in motion direction at borders evoked the segregation of regions and generated two surfaces in the shape of random-block objects against the background. The luminance and speed of RDPs were identical to those used for the cue stimulus. Participants were instructed to judge which side, the left or right, appeared in front of the other with respect to the fixation point, i.e., to report the DOF at the fixation point (DOF-discrimination task). To cancel any biases in the perception of DOF, we utilized mirror images with respect to the vertical and horizontal midlines and images with a polarity that was opposite to that of motion directions (Figure 1C; Wagatsuma et al., 2008). We prepared 40 test stimuli in total (five types of ambiguous figures × four mirror images × two motion polarities). The combination of four types of cue stimuli yielded 160 trials, which was considered as one set in this experiment. We performed two sets for each subject.
Experimental procedure
Figure 2 shows the procedure used to perform Experiments 1 and 2, which aimed to examine feature-based attention in the perception of DOF. The experiments started with the presentation of a fixation aid (0.2° × 0.2°) at the center of the screen for 470 ms. Participants were instructed to gaze at the fixation aid during the experiment. A pair of reference bars for motion-discrimination was presented along either the left/upward or right/upward diagonals for 360 ms. A cue stimulus appeared within a circle of 8° for 360 ms with the simultaneous presentation of the diagonal reference bars (Figure 1A). Participants were instructed to judge the direction of dot motion with respect to the reference bars (clockwise or counterclockwise) while ignoring the RDP moving orthogonally to the reference bars, and to report this judgment at the end of the trial. This task aimed to direct attention to a specific motion direction. Subsequently, a blank screen was presented for 235 ms and a test stimulus for DOF-discrimination (Figure 1B) was presented inside a circle of 12° for 360 ms. Participants were asked to report which of the left or right object was perceived as figure in the test stimulus by clicking mouse buttons sequentially. At the end of each trial, participants were told whether the response regarding motion-discrimination was correct or not; a blue square (5.7° × 5.7°) was displayed for 180 ms for a correct response and a red square was displayed for 180 ms for an incorrect response. This feedback aimed to enhance the attention of individuals to a specific motion direction. The order of presentation of each condition (the combination of the orientation of reference bars and the directions of moving dots in cue and test stimuli) was randomized. This method allowed us to examine the perception of DOF while attending to a specific direction of motion. This procedure was similar to our previous psychophysical experiments for testing whether the behaviors of the model for spatial attention agreed with human perception of attention modulation in BO determination (Wagatsuma et al., 2008).
Observers and apparatus
Six male and two female participants in their twenties with normal or corrected-normal vision participated in the experiments. They were familiar with visual psychophysics but not aware of the aim of the experiments. The experiment was conducted using a 21′′ CRT monitor (GDM8411; Silicon Graphics Inc.) at a refresh rate of 85 Hz, controlled by a PC (Precision 360-n; Dell). The monitor was placed at a distance of 57 cm in front of the participants in Experiments 1 and 2, and at 85.5 cm in Experiment 3. Shapes of random-block objects in Experiment 3 were more complex than those in Experiments 1 and 2 (see Difference in Motion Direction for the Attention Modulation of DOF – Experiment 3 and the Appendix for details) thus we drew them slightly larger to assure similar resolution on the monitor. For presenting their entire shapes within the same visual angle as in Experiments 1 and 2, the distance between the monitor and participants for Experiment 3 were set slightly longer than Experiments 1 and 2. Note that we did not compare directly the degree of attention modulation among the Experiments.
The model
Model architecture
The computational study (Sakai and Nishimura, 2006) proposed that the cortical mechanism underlying BO coding and F–G segregation involved the surrounding contrasts and surround modulation observed in early visual areas (Jones et al., 2001, 2002). Although this model was rather abstract model in the sense that BO was determined solely from contrast balance without biophysical details, the model not only reproduced the characteristics of BO-selective cells but also was supported by psychophysical experiments (Sugihara et al., 2007; Sakai et al., 2012). Based on this BO signaling mechanism depending on surrounding contrasts and edges, we proposed the model of spatial attention for BO modulation (Wagatsuma et al., 2008). In our previous model, spatial attention modulates luminance contrast in V1, which then alters the activities of BO-selective cells in V2. This spatial attention model explained the mechanism of the switch of F–G and reproduced qualitatively and quantitatively the human perception of DOF. It is expected that a common framework to this spatial attention model underlies the mechanism of different types of attention. Our proposed model for feature-based attention shared the framework used for spatial attention. We developed the present model to investigate the role of feature-based attention in the modulation of DOF that was observed in our psychophysical experiments. The proposed model was composed of two stages: V1 and V2, as illustrated in Figure 3A. In this proposed abstract model, top-down feature-based attention from higher visual areas, presumably MT, was directed to a specific feature such as the motion direction, which led to selective enhancement of the low-level feature contrasts or edges presented in V1 stage. This alternation resulted in the modulation of model BO-selective cells in V2 stage, because the responses of the cells were based on the surrounding modulation by the low-level feature contrast extracted in early vision (Figure 3B; Jones et al., 2001; Jones et al., 2002; Ozeki et al., 2009; Sakai and Nishimura, 2006; Sugihara et al., 2007; Sakai et al., 2012). Here, we consider local, component feature that appeared to be processed in V1, rather than complex pattern presentation apparent in MT. In the case of our experimental stimuli, edges or feature contrast are formed by the differences in motion directions. Note that there is no direct attention effect for the BO determination in V2 stage.
V1 stage
We consider local, component feature contrast that appeared to be processed in V1, rather than patterns apparent in MT. In our model, the low-level feature contrast in the V1 stage played a key role for the determination of the DOF, i.e., the responses of the V2 stage was independent of the distribution of the features itself. The V1 stage modeled the primary visual cortex, in which local edges or low-level feature contrasts, specifically the motion direction differences, are presented and are modulated by the top-down feature-based attention. The input to the V1 stage is provided by a stimulus map composed of edge detector Eθ(x, y), akin to the topographic representation in V1 cells (Craft et al., 2007; Mihalas et al., 2011). Indices x and y are spatial positions. Orientations, θ, were selected among 0°, 90°, 180°, and 270°. Input to the V1 stage, Eθ, had a resolution of 112 × 112 pixels. The activities of Eθ depended on the magnitude of the local contrast of the feature. In the case of our psychophysical stimuli, the activities of Eθ on the border between two objects are higher than that between objects and backgrounds.
Top-down feature-based attention was applied to the V1 stage and modulated the feature contrast or edge, specifically the motion direction differences, Eθ(x, y). The modulated edges, , are given by the following equation, as proposed by Lee et al. (Lee et al., 1999; Peters et al., 2005):
(1) |
where IAttn implies whether top-down feature-based attention is directed to this feature and represents the magnitude of attention. If Eθ(x, y) were the edges formed by motion direction differences with respect to the attended direction of motion, we used IAttn = 1.0. In the case of the attention to the opposite feature, we set IAttn = 0.0. γ and δ are constants. S is a semisaturation constant that prevents the denominator to be zero. The constant S is relatively sensitive to the simulation results. However, this semisaturation constant ranging between 2.0 and 3.5 showed marked attention modulation of model BO-selective cells. In our simulations, we used γ = 4.0, δ = 3.0, and S = 3.05. If the ratio of γ to δ is fixed, simulation results are almost the same even when these two constants are doubled. These constants were determined based on our previous model (Wagatsuma et al., 2008) and were fixed throughout all simulations. Equation 1 indicates that top-down feature-based attention in V1 stage enhances the edges such as the differences in the motion direction with respect to the specific feature.
V2 stage
A mathematical description of the surround suppression of a model BO-selective cell in V2 stage is given here. The activity of the model BO-selective cells is determined from the edge signals formed by motion direction differences transmitted from V1 stage (Figure 3B; Sakai and Nishimura, 2006; Sakai et al., 2012).
First, V2 stage pools the edge signals that are transmitted from V1 stage and have been modulated by top-down feature-based attention over space and orientation:
(2) |
where O1 represents the pooling of the modulated edge signals from V1 stage.
Second, the surrounding signal, , is given by a linear combination of edge signals from suppressive regions, which are defined by Gaussian functions, as illustrated in Figure 3B:
(3) |
where the index N represents the type of model BO-selective cells, which are distinguished by the size and the location of their surrounding suppressive regions; RN represents the suppressive regions of the model BO-selective cells. Physiological studies have reported a diversity of characteristics of BO-selective cells in V2 (Zhou et al., 2000; Qiu et al., 2007). The size and location of these suppressive regions determine the properties of and reproduce various BO selectivity. We implemented 10 types of suppressive regions from a pool of Gaussians generated randomly (Sakai and Nishimura, 2006; Sakai et al., 2012). These are common to a previous spatial attention work (Wagatsuma et al., 2008); kx and ky indicate the spatial extent of suppressive regions; and c is the connection strength.
Third, the response of model BO-selective cells, , was computed from the linear summation of the CRF signal, O1, and the surround signal, .
If ,
(4) |
otherwise
(5) |
For the determination of DOF, the activities of model BO-selective cells were pooled, for representing the population activities. For the sake of simplicity, we took the summation of all activities of BO-selective cells that prefer right side and those prefer left side, respectively. Based on the magnitude of the two values, the dominant population was considered to own the border. Note that feature-based attention did not act directly on the model BO-selective cells in V2 stage.
Results
Feature-based attention in the perception of DOF – Experiment 1
We investigated psychophysically the influence of feature-based attention on the determination of DOF. Participants judged DOF at the border between two adjacent regions (each consisting of an RDP moving toward a distinct direction) while directing their attention toward a particular direction of motion. All subjects reported the correct response in over 90% of the cue task. We expected that the region with the attended direction of motion would be chosen as figure more frequently. Figure 4A illustrates the shapes of the test stimuli (ambiguous figures comprising two objects) used in Experiment 1. Figure 4B shows the measured perceptual ratio of DOF, indicating the region that tended to be perceived as a figure. Note that we combined the data for each test stimulus from all mirror images and polarities of motion directions and from all participants who showed sensitivity to attention, to extract the effect of attention. To quantify the effect of feature-based attention on the perception of DOF, we carried out three-way ANOVA using two attention conditions (attend to the motion direction of left or right object), five ambiguous figures (types 1–1 through 1–5), and six participants (A, B, C, D, E, and F; five males and one female). There were significant main effects on attention (P < 0.001) and participants (P < 0.05) and significant interactions among the three factors (P < 0.005). The interactions led us to examine the simple main effects of each factor. Four participants (A, D, E, and F) showed significant differences in the perception of DOF with regard to two attention conditions (P < 0.001). The other two participants (B and C) did not exhibit significant differences (P = 0.48 and 0.12). We excluded the data of participants B and C from further analyses, because subsequent examinations focused on the nature of the modulation for the determination of DOF afforded by feature-based attention. The results obtained for all six participants, including individual data, are shown in Figure A1 in Appendix. Note that the tendency of the attention modulation of participants B and C was similar to other four participants although their magnitudes were small. Tzvetanov’s et al. (2006) experiment with RDP stimuli also reported the inter-subject variability while the averaged data of all subjects exhibited expected effects. The interaction between the attention condition and four participants (A, D, E, and F) was significant (two-way ANOVA, P < 0.001). The modulation afforded by attention was significant on all types of ambiguous figures (pairwise t-test, P < 0.005). These results indicate that the mean of all six participants exhibited a significant modulation by feature-based attention in the perception of DOF (with dependence on subject and stimulus type), and that four out of six participants exhibited a significant modulation that was independent of stimulus type (with dependence on subject). The amount of perceptual modulation by spatial attention in a similar experiment (Wagatsuma et al., 2008) was around 30–40%, whereas the amount of perceptual modulation by feature-based attention in the present experiment was around 45–65%. Although a direct comparison between the experiments is difficult, it appears that the modulation by feature-based attention is similar to (or maybe slightly larger than) the modulation by spatial attention.
The perception of DOF and the size of the object – Experiment 2
The result of Experiment 1 indicated that feature-based attention modulates the perception of DOF, so that participants tended to judge the region with an attended motion as figure. In this section, we investigated the features that are modulated by feature-based attention in the perception of DOF. Specifically, we examined whether the mount of the attended motion direction directly modulates the perception of DOF. The test stimuli used in Experiment 1 consisted of two random-block objects with the same area, aiming at canceling the imbalance of the object size. If the motion-selective cells directly underlie the modulation of DOF, larger objects will be perceived more frequently as figures and show more attention modulation. In this section, we examined whether the modulation of DOF depends on the imbalance of the size between the objects.
The test stimuli used in Experiment 2 are illustrated in Figure 5A: the ratio of the areas between the two objects ranged between 0.5 and 1.0. The shapes of the objects were identical to those used in the previous study of spatial attention (Wagatsuma et al., 2008; see Appendix). Six participants (all males) participated in the experiment. Figure 5B shows the measured perceptual ratio of DOF for all participants who indicated sensitivity to attention, using conventions that were identical to those shown in Figure 4B. Three out of six participants (A, B, and C) were excluded from further analyses and Figure 5B because they did not exhibit significant attention-based modulation (ANOVA, P = 0.34, 0.10, and 0.09, respectively). The results obtained for the six participants, including individual data, are presented in the Appendix (Figure A2 in Appendix). Note that the mean of all six participants exhibited a significant modulation by feature-based attention in the perception of DOF. The results suggest that the objects with attended motion are perceived more frequently as figure. To quantify the data, we performed three-way ANOVA using two attention conditions (attention to the left or right side motion direction), five types of ambiguous figures (type 2–1 through 2–5), and the three participants who exhibited significant attention modulation (D, E, and F). We observed a significant main effect of attention (P < 0.005), but no significant main effect on types of ambiguous figures (P = 0.439) and participants (P = 0.229). These results indicate that the significant modulation of the perception of DOF originates from feature-based attention and suggest the independence of modulation from stimulus type. Although we observed a significant interaction between participants and attention (P < 0.01), the tendency observed among the participants was identical (with different levels of magnitude). These results indicate that the modulation by feature-based attention is independent of the object size. To verify the modulation of the perception of DOF for each ambiguous figure and each subject, we carried out pairwise t-tests for each type of test stimulus and each subject. For all types of stimuli, all participants showed a significant difference in the perception of DOF between the attention conditions (all stimulus types, P < 0.01). This result further supports the irrelevance of the object size to attention modulation in the perception of DOF. The results of Experiment 2 indicate that the size of the object does not directly underlie the modulation of BO.
Difference in motion direction for the attention modulation of DOF – Experiment 3
In the previous section, we showed that the modulation of DOF by feature-based attention was independent of the object size. In this section, we examined the role of motion direction differences in the modulation afforded by feature-based attention regarding the perception of DOF. As described above, we defined motion direction difference as the difference in motion direction along the border between figure and ground. In our experiments, the difference in motion direction was set to 90°, thus the length of the border was proportional to the motion direction differences (Figures 6A,B). Recent accumulative data indicate that attention modulates the responses of early visual areas (Ito et al., 1998; Posner and Gilbert, 1999) and enhances feedforward processing (Wagatsuma et al., 2008; Zhang and Luck, 2009). Furthermore, computational models imply that the distribution of low-level feature contrast extracted in early vision is the basis of the determination of DOF (Sakai and Nishimura, 2006; Sakai et al., 2012). These works suggest that a factor extracted in the early vision, such as the edge or contrast of low-level features, plays an important role in attention modulation. We designed two sets of test stimuli: a balanced stimulus, with an equal amount of motion direction difference on the border between two objects (1:1; Figures 6A,C), and an unbalanced stimulus, with unequal amounts of motion direction difference on each side (1:4; Figures 6B,D). If feature-based attention had an effect on motion direction difference, the side with a higher amount of (or stronger) motion direction difference would be modulated effectively; thus, participants would tend to perceive this side as figure more frequently (Figure 6E). On the other hand, the side with a lower amount of (or weaker) motion direction difference would be modulated ineffectively; thus, participants would tend to perceive this side as a figure less frequently. The comparison between the two sets of stimuli aimed to clarify whether attention modulation of BO depends on difference in motion direction. See Appendix for the generation algorithm of balanced and unbalanced stimuli. The test stimuli used in Experiment 3 consisted of two random-block objects with the same size.
The two sets of test stimuli used in Experiment 3 are shown in Figures 6C,D. These stimuli have more complex shapes than those used in Experiments 1 and 2, because of greater diversity in the combination of block squares. Accordingly, we modified slightly the experimental procedure to yield perceptual clarity similar to that used in the preceding experiments. The modified procedure is illustrated in Figure 7. The secondary task of size-discrimination was added to achieve a greater level of concentration by the participants (Figure A3 in Appendix). After the cue stimulus, in half of the trials participants were asked to report on the object (right or left) that was bigger. This modification led participants to concentrate on the whole stimulus display. To cancel any biases in the perception of DOF, the “Task select” screen was presented just prior to “Response” screen. One single set of the experiment consisted of 160 trials: 80 trials for DOF- and 80 trials for size-discrimination tasks. The experiment comprised eight sets (160 × 8 = 1280 trials). Five participants with normal or corrected-normal vision (two females and three males) participated in this experiment.
We carried out this experiment to investigate the role of motion direction difference in attention modulation. If difference in motion direction underlies this modulation, as illustrated in Figure 6E, it is expected that the side with a higher amount of motion direction difference evokes more modulation and tends to be perceived as figure more frequently. Figure 8 shows the results obtained for three participants (D, E, and F). Because this experiment focused on the cause of attention modulation, we excluded two of the five participants, as they did not show significant modulation by attention (ANOVA, P = 0.52 and 0.49, respectively). The results obtained for all five participants, including individual data, are given in the Appendix (Figure A4 in Appendix). Note that the mean of all five participants exhibited a significant modulation by feature-based attention in the perception of DOF. To analyze whether difference in motion direction is essential for attention modulation, we carried out three-way ANOVA using the factors of attention (attention to the left or right), motion direction difference (balanced and unbalanced), and participants (D, E, and F). We observed a significant main effect of attention (P < 0.001), indicating the modulation by attention for these stimuli. We also observed the presence of significant interactions between attention and participants (P < 0.001) and between attention and motion direction difference (P < 0.001). The interaction between attention and subject led us to examine the simple main effects of attention and subject. This analysis indicated the presence of significant differences in the magnitude of attention modulation among participants, with all participants showing significance in attention modulation (P < 0.001). Similarly, we examined the interaction between attention and motion direction difference. The analysis of simple main effects showed that motion direction difference was a significant factor when attending to the side with a lower amount of motion direction difference, but not to the side with a higher amount of motion direction difference. This implies, based on the illustration presented in Figure 6E, that the red arrow was significant, whereas the blue arrow was not. This result indicates that the effect of attention is significantly smaller in the direction of a lower amount of motion direction difference (iii in Figure 8) compared with the effects of stimuli with balanced motion direction difference (i = ii). On the other hand, the magnitude of attention modulation was indistinguishable between the directions of a higher amount of motion direction difference and balanced stimuli (P = 0.086). These analyses show that a higher amount of motion direction difference (iv) is effective in attention modulation, whereas a lower amount of motion direction difference (iii) is significantly less effective. The cause of this asymmetry is discussed below (see Discussion). The results of Experiment 3 indicate that the feature contrast arising from the differences between two directions of motion plays a crucial role in the modulation of DOF by feature-based attention. These results do not conflict with the computational model that BO signaling is determined based on the balance of surround low-level feature contrast between both sides of its CRF, not the feature itself (Sakai and Nishimura, 2006).
Neural mechanisms underlying the modulation of BO by feature-based attention – the abstract V1–V2 network model
The results described in the previous section showed the crucial role of motion direction difference in the modulation of BO. This result led us to hypothesize that feature-based attention acts on low-level feature contrast extracted in early vision; thus, the modulated contrast changes the activities of BO-selective neurons in V2. Given that BO-selective cells signal the side of BO based on surround contrast (Sakai and Nishimura, 2006; Sakai et al., 2012), the modified contrast would modify directly the response of the cells. If this modification was strong, the side of BO could even be flipped, as observed for ambiguous figures. Essentially, this mechanism shares the framework of spatial attention for the determination of BO (Wagatsuma et al., 2008). Furthermore, if attention modulation of low-level feature contrast played a key role for the modulation of the responses of BO-selective cells and the switching of the perception of figure, it is expected that the magnitude of the perceptual modulation on the DOF is invariant among types of stimuli since attention in early vision might change low-level feature contrast prior to the binding to or perception of stimulus shapes. To investigate the plausibility of the mechanism of feature-based attention, we constructed an abstract V1–V2 network model (as shown in Figure 3A) and analyzed its behavior. In this model, feature-based attention presumably originates in higher visual areas such as MT. The top-down feature-based attention act on V1 cells, to increase edge or feature contrast, specifically the differences in motion direction. The increased feature contrast in V1 is fed to BO-selective cells in V2 and modifies their activities according to the modulation of contrast (Figure 3B). In short, dots with attended direction of motion have increased motion direction difference, and such “easy-to-see” dots tend to attract the direction of BO toward them. A detailed description of the model is given in the Section “Materials and Methods.” Parameters of this model were fixed through all simulations. The modulation of the responses of our model with a type of stimulus emerged from the attention conditions. We examined whether the model BO-selective cells reproduced the results of our psychophysical experiments, which were described in the preceding sections.
Feature-based attention in early vision for the modulation of BO-selective neurons
The results of the psychophysical experiments showed the significant modulation of DOF by feature-based attention. To test whether the model BO-selective cells behave in a manner that is similar to perception, we performed simulations of the model with the edge maps mimicking the shapes of test stimuli used in Experiments 1 and 2 and compared the magnitude of attention modulation observed in the model BO-selective cells with that obtained for human responses. The activities of the model BO-selective cells in response to individual stimuli are shown in Figure 9. The magnitude of attention modulation is defined as the difference in the proportion of the BO-left model cells between two attention conditions, which is presented by black arrows in Figure 9. We observed attention modulation that was similar to the human responses, as shown in Figures 4B and 5B and Figures A1 and A2 in Appendix. Note that there was no direct feature-based attention effect for the BO determination in V2 stage. These attention modulations of model BO-selective cells solely emerged from a change in the activities of model cells in V1 stage. There was no significant difference in the magnitudes of attention modulation between the model and human responses of six participants average (ANOVA, P = 0.500), suggesting the plausibility of the model as representative of the modulation of BO by feature-based attention. According to the model, no significant difference was observed in the magnitude of the attention modulation among stimuli (ANOVA, P = 0.137), indicating the shape invariance of the modulation. To quantify the shape variance in the psychophysical experiments, we carried out two-way ANOVA of the results of Experiments 1 and 2 using factors of stimulus type (n = 9) and participants (n = 6). The results of this analysis did not show significance for the main factors, indicating the invariance of shape and subject in attention modulation. The shape invariance shown for both simulations and human perceptions supports the hypothesis that feature contrast modulation in early vision is crucial for the modulation of the activities of BO-selective neurons. Shape invariance has also been reported in spatial attention (Wagatsuma et al., 2008). The agreement between the model and psychophysical results suggests that the modulation of low-level feature contrast in early vision underlies the modulation of BO-selective neurons in V2.
Feature contrast dependence in the modulation of BO
Experiment 3 showed the presence of motion direction difference dependence in the modulation of DOF by feature-based attention. We performed simulations of the model with edge maps mimicking the shapes of test stimuli used in Experiment 3, to investigate whether the activities of model BO-selective cells depend on the amount of edge formed by motion direction difference. Figure 10 shows the computed responses of model BO-selective cells, as categorized by difference in motion direction (Group 1:1 and 1:4). We observed attention-based modulation in the determination of BO. Two-way ANOVA using the factors of attention and motion direction difference (with the repetition of individual stimuli) was significant for the main factors (P < 0.001), but not for the interaction (P = 0.67), indicating the dependence of the model cell activities on attention and motion direction difference. These results suggest that the model determines BO on a specific side if that side includes a large amount of edge formed by motion direction difference compared with the other side, and if the side is attended.
Experiment 3 showed the asymmetry in the attention modulation with respect to the amount of motion direction difference: the modulation was less effective if attended to the side with a small amount of motion direction difference, but was similarly effective if attended to a large amount of motion direction difference compared with the equal motion direction difference in balanced stimuli. The model did not show this asymmetry. The analysis of the simple main effect of attention showed the significance independent of edge formed by motion direction difference (ANOVA, P = 0.66). This result indicates that the amount of the modulation is similarly independent of the strength of edge formed by motion direction difference to which attention is directed. After the description of Experiment 3, we speculated that a large amount of edge based on motion direction difference did not alter the modulation because the regular edge formed by motion direction difference in balanced stimuli reaches the level at which the modulation is saturated. As our model did not include the compressive non-linearity that establishes saturation, a large amount of edge formed by motion direction difference yielded enhancement in modulation. The issue of this asymmetry is discussed below (see Discussion). The simulations of the model with the stimuli used in the psychophysical experiments supported the hypothesis that the mechanism based on the modulation of low-level feature contrast by attention, presumably in early vision, plays a crucial role in the modulation of DOF by feature-based attention, similarly to spatial attention.
Discussion
We investigated whether and how feature-based attention modulates F–G segregation. Using ambiguous figures consisting of a combination of RDPs moving toward orthogonal directions, we conducted psychophysical experiments to examine whether the perception of BO is modified by directing feature-based attention to the direction of motion. Our results showed that feature-based attention alters the perception with a degree that is comparable to that of spatial attention. Furthermore, the modulation of perception was independent of stimulus types. These results support a crucial contribution of the early visual areas to the modulation of attention. Subsequent experiments showed that F–G perception is dependent on the distribution of difference in motion direction, but independent of the object size. Based on these results, we constructed a computational model for modulation by feature-based attention. In the model, top-down feature-based attention modulates the low-level feature contrast, specifically edge formed by motion direction difference, in V1, and this modulation of the feature contrast changes directly the surround contrast of BO-selective cells in V2. The simulations of the model showed good agreement with human perception regarding the magnitude of attention modulation and its invariance among stimuli. These results indicate that the contrast of early level features that are modified by feature-based attention alter subsequent processing along afferent pathways, and that such modification could even change the selection of an object during F–G segregation. This mechanism is essentially identical to that observed in spatial attention, with the exception that spatial attention modulates cells that share a retinotopic location, whereas feature-based attention modulates cells that share a feature.
The results of our psychophysical experiments indicated that the magnitude of feature-based attention for DOF perception is independent among the types of stimuli. This shape invariance suggests the influences of attention in early visual areas for the modulation of DOF perception (Wagatsuma et al., 2008). The attention effects of the proposed network model for BO-selective cells were limited to the V1 stage. However, this model not only showed the modulation of the activities of BO-selective cells and the flip of DOF perception, but also reproduced the shape invariance on the magnitude of attention modulation. The agreement between the psychophysics and the corresponding simulations supports the validity of the proposed model and suggests that attention modulation of low-level feature contrast in early visual area underlies the flip of BO determination. These results predict that attention modulation of DOF perception originates, at least in part, from modulation of low-level feature contrast in early vision sensitivity.
Our main result was that motion-based attention modulates the perception of DOF. Participants directed their attention toward a specific direction of motion through the motion-discrimination task utilized by Tzvetanov et al. (2006). Their study indicated the validity of this task for regulating participants’ motion-based attention. Followed by this cue stimulus, we presented the blank screen for 235 ms until the presentation of test stimulus (Figure 2). The presentation of such interval (blank screen) is crucial to assure the effect of feature-based attention. A physiological study reported that the effects of both spatial and feature-based attention were apparent with a randomized interval of 200–400 ms (Cohen and Maunsell, 2011). The recent psychophysical work reported that motion-based attention took 300–500 ms to exert its effect (Liu et al., 2007b). Other studies often used 200–3000 ms of ISI or SOA for studying feature-based attention (Lu and Itti, 2005; Katzner et al., 2009). These works suggest the behavioral latency of feature-based attention, supporting that motion-based attention underlies our observation of the modulation of DOF.
It is the main purpose of this work to understand how feature-based attention modulates the F–G segregation, so that we statistically analyzed the data of participants with sensitivity to attention. Some of participants did not exhibit significant effects of attention. Other psychophysical works also reported the inter-subject variability (Grunewald, 2004; Tzvetanov et al., 2006). However, the average of all participants showed significant attention modulation on DOF perception (Figures A1, A2, and A4 in Appendix), which suggested the validity of feature-based attention. We found that stable effects of attention were observed from the expert or well trained subjects (D, E, and F). The possible explanations for this difference among participants are the difficulty of the experiments and the training of participants. The procedure of the present experiments is similar to our previous experiments on spatial attention (Wagatsuma et al., 2008). In these previous experiments, a flashing dot directed the attention toward a specific location, without an additional task or a response to this cue stimulus. However, the present experiments consisted of dual tasks, motion- and DOF-discrimination tasks. The increase of the complexity and difficulty of the experiments might decrease the performance of naïve subjects.
The result of Experiment 2 showed the irrelevance of object size to attention modulation in the perception of DOF. This result led to the hypothesis that the modulation by feature-based attention is independent of motion energy that is considered a crucial, low-level feature processed in early visual areas (Adelson and Bergen, 1985; Landy and Bergen, 1991; DeAngelis et al., 1993). The amount of motion energy is proportional to the amount of area, because larger areas include a greater number of moving RDPs. However, the result of Experiment 2 indicated the independence of object size for the feature-based attention modulation, suggesting that motion energy does not underlie the modulation by feature-based attention. Although this prediction appears to be key for understanding the mechanism of visual attention, our present experimental methods appear to be not appropriate to discuss attention effects for the motion energy. A further study is necessary to understand the attention modulation of the motion energy.
Our Experiment 3 indicated that the modulation of DOF by attention was dependent on the distribution of differences in motion direction and that the modulation was asymmetric between balanced and unbalanced stimuli (Figure 8). Specifically, a large amount of motion direction differences was effective in attention modulation, whereas a small amount of motion direction difference was significantly less effective. It was not clarified whether this asymmetry is essential for attention modulation. The possible explanations for this asymmetry include the saturation of modulation. The modulation to the direction of a large amount of motion direction differences reached 80%; thus, further modulation may be difficult to measure. It could also be considered that a small amount of motion direction differences is less effective for the determination of DOF rather than the attention modulation. This alternative explanation could be examined by measuring the ratio of DOF perception for the present stimulus sets without directing attention. However, this measurement is sensitive to other cues, such as the shape and size of block stimuli, and would not help support this explanation. Our model did not reproduce this asymmetry, possibly because the model did not include either the mechanism of the saturation or the dependence of DOF on low-level feature contrast. The investigation on the quantitative behavior of the modulation may lead to further understanding of the mechanisms underlying the selection of surface by feature-based attention.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas “Shitsukan” (No.23135503) and No. 22300090 from MEXT, Japan.
Appendix
Generations of pseudo random-block objects and ambiguous figures
We carried out psychophysical experiments with pseudo white-noise random-block objects to investigate whether feature-based attention modulates the perception of F–G segregation. The pseudo white-noise random-block objects approximate all possible shapes under certain conditions. The method for the generation of pseudo white-noise random-block objects is given here.
A block object consisted of n square blocks placed on m × m grids, with center 2 × 2 grids were fixed with the two blocks placed on one side of the center so that contrast within the CRF was kept identical for all stimuli. The other blocks (n−2) were placed randomly adjacent to the existing blocks except for the CRF region. We call this set of stimuli as n-block stimuli. Stimulus shape is more complex with a larger number of blocks and grids. In this paper, the number of grid for Experiment 1 was fixed to 4 × 4 and that of blocks were 6. That of grid for Experiment 2 was 4 × 4 and that of blocks were 3, 4, 6, and 8. Stimulus shape used in Experiment 3 is more complex. The number of grid was 6 × 6 and that of blocks were 8.
We brought together two block stimuli to produce a stimulus with ambiguous BO at the center as illustrated in Figures 4A, 5A, and 6C,D. Ambiguous random-block stimuli were comprised of a combination of the block stimulus with BO left and that with right. All combinations of these stimuli were prepared, and if there was an overlap of blocks, we excluded such combination. For Experiment 1 and 2, because this method generated a number of stimuli, we selected five stimuli that appears most ambiguous from visual inspection of several people who did not participate in the experiments. For Experiment 3, we randomly selected 10 balanced (Group 1:1) and unbalanced (Group 1:4) stimuli from all generated combinations.
References
- Adelson E. H., Bergen J. R. (1985). Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2, 284–299 10.1364/JOSAA.2.000284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baluch F., Itti L. (2011). Mechanism of top-down attention. Trends Neurosci. 34, 210–224 10.1016/j.tins.2011.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrasco M. (2011). Visual attention: the past 25 years. Vision Res. 51, 1484–1525 10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrasco M., Ling S., Read S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 308–313 10.1038/nn1194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen M. R., Maunsell J. H. (2011). Using neuronal populations to study the mechanisms underlying spatial and feature attention. Neuron 70, 1192–1204 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.04.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craft E., Schütze H., Niebur E., von der Heydt R. (2007). A neural model of figure-ground organization. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 4310–4326 10.1152/jn.00203.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeAngelis G. C., Ohzawa I., Freeman R. D. (1993). Spatiotemporal organization of simple-cell receptive fields in the cat’s striate cortex. II. Linearity of temporal and spatial summation. J. Neurophysiol. 69, 1091–1117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deco G., Lee T. S. (2004). The role of early visual cortex in visual integration: a neural model of recurrent interaction. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 1089–1100 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03528.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregoriou G. G., Gotts S. J., Zhou H., Desimone R. (2009). High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science 324, 1207–1210 10.1126/science.1171402 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grunewald A. (2004). Motion repulsion is monocular. Vision Res. 44, 959–962 10.1016/j.visres.2003.10.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasson U., Hendler T., Bashat D. B., Malach R. (2001). Vase or face? A neural correlate of shape-selective grouping processes in the human brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 744–753 10.1162/08989290152541412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito M., Westheimer B., Gilbert C. D. (1998). Attention and perceptual learning modulate contextual influences on visual perception. Neuron 20, 1191–1197 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80499-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Itti L., Koch C. (2001). Computational modeling of visual attention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 194–203 10.1038/35058500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones H. E., Grieve K. L., Wang W., Sillito A. M. (2001). Surround suppression in primate V1. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 2011–2028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones H. E., Wang W., Sillito A. M. (2002). Spatial organization and magnitude of orientation contrast interactions in primate V1. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 2796–2808 10.1152/jn.00403.2001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katzner S., Busse L., Treue S. (2009). Attention to the color of a moving stimulus modulates motion-singnal processing in mcaque area MT: evidence for a unified attentional system. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 3:12. 10.3389/neuro.06.012.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landy M. S., Bergen R. (1991). Texture segregation and orientation gradient. Vision Res. 31, 679–691 10.1016/0042-6989(91)90153-V [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee K., Itti L., Koch C., Braun J. D. (1999). Attention activates winner-take-all competition among visual filters. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 375–381 10.1038/7286 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ling S., Liu T., Carrasco M. (2009). How spatial and feature-based attention affect the gain and tuning of population responses. Vision Res. 49, 1194–1204 10.1016/j.visres.2008.05.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu T., Larsson J., Carrasco M. (2007a). Feature-based attention modulates orientation-selective responses in human visual cortex. Neuron 55, 313–323 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu T., Stevens S. T., Carrasco M. (2007b). Comparing the time course and efficacy of spatial and feature-based attention. Vision Res. 47, 108–113 10.1016/j.visres.2007.04.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu J., Itti L. (2005). Perceptual consequences of feature-based attention. J. Vis. 5, 622–631 10.1167/5.8.1034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez-Trujillo J. C., Treue S. (2004). Feature-based attention increases the selectivity of population responses in primary visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 14, 744–751 10.1016/j.cub.2004.04.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAdams C. J., Maunsell J. H. (1999). Effects of attention on the reliability of individual neurons in monkey visual cortex. Neuron 23, 765–773 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)80034-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mihalas S., Dong Y., von der Heydt R., Niebur E. (2011). Mechanism of perceptual organization provide auto-zoom and auto-localization for attention to objects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 7583–7588 10.1073/pnas.1014655108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell J. F., Stoner G. R., Reynolds J. H. (2004). Object-based attention determines dominance in binocular rivalry. Nature 429, 410–413 10.1038/nature02584 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ozeki H., Finn I. M., Schaffer E. S., Miller K. D., Ferster D. (2009). Inhibitory stabilization of the cortical network underlies visual surround suppression. Neuron 62, 578–592 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer J., Ames C. T., Lindsey D. T. (1993). Measuring the effect of attention on simple visual search. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 19, 108–130 10.1037/0096-1523.19.1.108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paradiso M. A. (2002). Perceptual and neuronal correspondence in primary visual cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12, 155–161 10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00311-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters R. J., Iyer A., Itti L., Koch C. (2005). Components of bottom-up Gaza allocation in natural images. Vision Res. 45, 2397–2416 10.1016/j.visres.2005.03.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posner M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 32, 3–25 10.1080/00335558008248231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posner M. I., Gilbert C. D. (1999). Attention and primary visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 2585–2587 10.1073/pnas.96.6.2585 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qiu T., Sugihara T., von der Heydt R. (2007). Figure-ground mechanisms provide structure for selective attention. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1492–1499 10.1038/nn1853 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds J. H., Heeger D. J. (2009). The normalization model of attention. Neuron 61, 168–185 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sajda P., Finkel L. H. (1995). Intermediate-level vision representations and the construction of surface perception. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 7, 267–291 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.2.267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakai K., Nishimura H. (2006). Surrounding suppression and facilitation in the determination of border ownership. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 562–579 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.562 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakai K., Nishimura H., Shimizu R., Kondo K. (2012). Consistent and robust determination of border ownership based on asymmetric surrounding contrast. Neural Netw. 33, 257–274 10.1016/j.neunet.2012.05.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solmon J. A., Lavie N., Morgan M. J. (1997). Contrast discrimination function: spatial cueing effects. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 14, 2443–2448 10.1364/JOSAA.14.002443 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sugihara T., Tsuji Y., Sakai K. (2007). Border-ownership-dependent tilt after effect in incomplete figures. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A Opt. Image Sci. Vis. 24, 18–24 10.1364/JOSAA.24.000018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treue S., Maunsell J. H. (1999). Effects of attention on the processing of motion in macaque middle temportal and medial superior temporal visual cortical areas. J. Neurosci. 19, 7591–7602 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzvetanov T., Wolmelsdorf T., Niebergall R., Treue S. (2006). Feature-based attention influences contextual interactions during motion repulsion. Vision Res. 46, 3651–3658 10.1016/j.visres.2006.05.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagatsuma N., Potjans T. C., Diesmann M., Fukai T. (2011). Layer-dependent attentional processing by top-down signals in a visual cortical microcircuit model. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 5:31. 10.3389/fncom.2011.00031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagatsuma N., Potjans T. C., Diesmann M., Sakai K., Fukai T. (2012). Space-based and feature-based attention in a realistic layered-microcircuit model of visual cortex. J. Vis. 12, 662. 10.1167/12.9.662 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wagatsuma N., Shimizu R., Sakai K. (2008). Spatial attention in early vision for the perception of border ownership. J. Vis. 8, 22. 10.1167/8.3.22 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang W., Luck S. J. (2009). Feature-based attention modulates feedforward visual processing. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 24–25 10.1038/nn.2223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou H., Friedman H. S., von der Heydt R. (2000). Coding of border ownership in monkey visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 20, 6594–6611 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]