Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Subst Abuse Treat. 2013 Jan 8;44(5):541–547. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2012.12.005

Impulsivity is associated with treatment non-completion in cocaine- and methamphetamine-dependent patients but differs in nature as a function of stimulant-dependence diagnosis

Theresa Winhusen a,*, Daniel Lewis a, Bryon Adinoff b,c, Gregory Brigham a,d, Frankie Kropp a, Dennis M Donovan e, Cindy L Seamans f, Candace C Hodgkins g, Jessica C DiCenzo h, Christopher L Botero i, Davina R Jones j, Eugene Somoza a,k
PMCID: PMC3602401  NIHMSID: NIHMS428443  PMID: 23305820

Abstract

Greater impulsivity, assessed by the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) and Stroop interference scores, has been associated with treatment completion in cocaine-dependent patients. This study evaluated the relationships among impulsivity, stimulant-dependence diagnosis, and treatment completion. Six sites evaluating 12-step facilitation for stimulant abusers obtained the BIS-11 and Stroop from 182 methamphetamine- and/or cocaine-dependent participants. Methamphetamine-dependent, relative to cocaine-dependent, participants evidenced significantly greater BIS-11 Non-planning and Total scores. There was a trend for poorer response inhibition, measured by the Stroop, in cocaine-dependent, relative to methamphetamine-dependent, participants. Accounting for other factors related to treatment completion, BIS-11 Motor score, assessing the tendency to act without thinking, predicted treatment completion for both cocaine-dependent and methamphetamine-dependent patients. These results suggest that methamphetamine-dependent and cocaine-dependent patients may have different impulsivity profiles but that the BIS-11 may be useful in identifying both methamphetamine-dependent and cocaine-dependent patients who are at risk for treatment non-completion.

Keywords: impulsivity, stimulant dependence, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, Stroop

1. Introduction

Impulsivity is associated with stimulant-use disorders with evidence suggesting that impulsivity increases susceptibility to develop stimulant-use disorders (Adinoff et al., 2007; de Wit, 2009; Ersche et al., 2012) and that stimulant use increases impulsivity (Adinoff et al., 2007; de Wit, 2009; Moeller et al., 2001). Impulsivity is a multi-dimensional construct (Robbins et al., 2012) and measures of its various aspects typically do not correlate, possibly indicating that they reflect different brain processes (de Wit, 2009; Ersche et al., 2011). The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) is a self-report assessment designed to measure impulsiveness in three domains: Attentional Impulsiveness, Motor Impulsiveness, and Non-Planning Impulsiveness (Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995; Stanford et al., 2009). Attentional Impulsiveness assesses the inability to concentrate/focus attention, Motor Impulsiveness assesses the tendency to act without thinking and Non-Planning Impulsiveness assesses lack of forethought (Stanford et al., 2009). Another aspect of impulsivity that may be important in substance use disorders (SUD) is behavioral inhibition, which is the ability to inhibit a prepotent response (de Wit, 2009). A popular measure of response inhibition is the classical Stroop task (Stroop, 1935). Though numerous versions of the classical Stroop task are available (for review, see MacLeod, 1991), the test has generally been characterized as assessing selective attention and cognitive flexibility (Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006) and response inhibition (Archibald & Kerns, 1999; Strauss et al., 2006). Of primary importance is the interference trial, or the time it takes the participant to name the ink color of names of colors printed in incongruently colored inks. Abnormally slowed performance on the interference trial has been interpreted to reflect difficulty with response inhibition and has been associated with frontal lobe dysfunction (Golden, 1976; Mesulam, 1985; Milner, 1964).

Impulsivity, as measured by the BIS-11, has been shown to be significantly greater in cocaine-dependent (Coffey, Gudleski, Saladin, & Brady, 2003; Ersche et al., 2010; Kjome et al., 2010; Lane, Moeller, Steinberg, Buzby, & Kosten, 2007; Moeller et al., 2002; Moeller et al., 2005; Patkar, Murray, Mannelli, & Gottheil, 2004) and methamphetamine-dependent (Lee et al., 2009) patients relative to normal controls. Similarly, increased Stroop interference has been demonstrated in patients addicted to methamphetamine (Salo et al., 2007; Salo et al., 2002; Simon et al., 2000) and cocaine (Hester, Dixon, & Garavan, 2006; Jovanovski, Erb, Zakzanis, 2005; Rosselli, Ardila, Lubomski, Murray, & King, 2001; Strickland et al., 1993) relative to normal controls, although not all investigators have observed a difference (Bolla et al., 2004; Ersche et al., 2010; Gardini, Caffarra, & Venneri, 2009; Goldstein, Volkow, Wang, Fowler, & Rajaram, 2001). Of note, the relative importance of observed significant differences between normal controls and methamphetamine-dependent patients has been questioned since the scores of the dependent patients typically were within the normal range, and thus, were unlikely to be of clinical significance (Hart, Marvin, Silver, & Smith, 2012).

A potential indicator of clinical significance would be a significant association between test performance and treatment outcomes, such as the findings of Aharonovich and colleagues that cognitive deficits in cocaine-dependent patients are related to treatment completion (Aharonovich et al., 2006; Aharonovich, Nunes, & Hasin, 2003). Several studies have evaluated the relationship between the BIS-11/Stroop and treatment outcomes in cocaine-dependent participants. These studies have found that the BIS-11 (Patkar et al., 2004) and the Stroop (Brewer, Worhunsky, Carroll, Rounsaville, & Potenza, 2008; Patkar et al., 2004) did not predict stimulant use during treatment. Rather, studies have found that the BIS-11 (Moeller et al., 2002; Moeller et al., 2001; Patkar et al., 2004) and the Stroop (Brewer et al., 2008; Streeter et al., 2008) are predictive of treatment completion. While these findings are of interest, this prior research has consisted of single-site studies and, thus, the generalizability of the results is not clear. Also, the work has been conducted only with cocaine-dependent patients; while one might assume that these findings would generalize to methamphetamine-dependent participants, there is evidence to suggest that cocaine and methamphetamine abusers have different neurocognitive profiles (Simon et al., 2002) so this may not be the case. Still, a recent study evaluating the relationship between the BIS-11 and past substance use in cocaine- and methamphetamine-dependent patients did not find a significant difference in impulsivity based on stimulant-dependence diagnosis (Tziortzis, Mahoney, Kalechstein, Newton, & De la Garza, 2011), suggesting that impulsivity is similar across these diagnostic categories.

To evaluate the relationships among impulsivity, stimulant-dependence diagnosis, and treatment completion in a more diverse sample that included cocaine-dependent and methamphetamine-dependent patients and which included multiple sites, an ancillary study was added to a National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) Clinical Trials Network (CTN) trial on 12-step facilitation for stimulant abusers (STAGE-12). STAGE-12 was designed to evaluate the efficacy of a 12-Step facilitation intervention, relative to substance abuse treatment as usual, in improving outcomes in stimulant abusing individuals. It was predicted that BIS-11 and Stroop scores would differ significantly between cocaine-dependent and methamphetamine-dependent participants, based on the findings of Simon et al. (2002), and that performance on the BIS-11 and the Stroop would be predictive of treatment completion for both groups.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

Six participating substance abuse community treatment programs (CTPs), located in Columbus, Ohio, Dallas, Texas, Eugene, Oregon, Jacksonville, Florida, Portland, Oregon, and Seattle, Washington, recruited stimulant abusers participating in the STAGE-12 trial. Participants in the STAGE-12 trial were adults seeking outpatient substance use disorder treatment who had used stimulants in the prior 60 days, had a current diagnosis of stimulant abuse or dependence (past 6 months) based on the DSM-IV Checklist (Hudziak et al., 1993), and were medically and psychiatrically stable enough to participate in the trial. The 182 eligible participants for the present study were randomized into the NIDA CTN 12-step facilitation trial, endorsed methamphetamine or cocaine as the primary drug of choice, had a current diagnosis of cocaine-and/or methamphetamine-dependence, did not have a seizure disorder or a history of stroke, were able to correctly distinguish the colored stimuli on the Stroop task, and were able to provide written informed consent in English.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Baseline characteristics

Several baseline assessments were obtained including basic demographic information as well as factors that may be related to treatment completion including the presence of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and mood disorders. ADHD status was assessed with the Wender Utah Rating Scale (WURS), which is a 61-item self-report questionnaire with good validity and reliability in identifying adults with ADHD (McCann, Scheele, Ward, & Roy-Byrne, 2000; Rossini & O’Connor, 1995; Stein et al., 1995). The Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ; Spitzer, Kroenke, Williams, & the PHQ Primary Care Study Group, 1999) was used to assess for the PHQ diagnoses of Major Depressive Syndrome, other Depressive Syndrome, Panic Syndrome, and other Anxiety Syndrome. Studies have found good agreement between PHQ diagnoses and those of independent mental health professionals (Spitzer et al., 1999). The measures of stimulant use included self-report of use assessed using the Timeline Follow-Back procedure (Fals-Stewart, O'Farrell, Freitas, McFarlin, & Rutigliano, 2000; Sobell et al., 2001; Sobell & Sobell 1996;) and qualitative urine drug screen (UDS) results.

2.2.2. Impulsivity

Measures of impulsivity included the BIS-11 (Patton et al., 1995; Stanford et al., 2009) and the Comalli-Kaplan version of the Stroop Color Word test that allows for self-correction of errors (Comalli, Wapner, & Werner, 1962). The BIS-11 consists of 30 self-report items, with responses in a four-point Likert-type scale ranging from "Rarely/Never" to "Almost Always/Always" and comprises three domains: Attentional Impulsiveness, Motor Impulsiveness, and Non-Planning Impulsiveness; these three domains are summed to yield a total score. The Comalli-Kaplan version of the Stroop Color Word test utilizes timed trials in which three stimulus cards are presented in a standard order. Card 1 presents multiple blocks of color and asks the participant to name the color of each block (Trial 1). Card 2 involves asking the participant to read text of multiple color names that are printed in black and white (Trial 2). Card 3 is an interference task in which multiple color names are printed in incongruently-colored ink, and the participant is asked to name the ink color (Trial 3). Study staff recorded the time the participant took to complete each of the three trials. In addition, a derived interference time was calculated by taking the difference between the time taken to complete the color naming and interference trials. The interference and derived interference reaction times (RT) were the measures of interest.

2.2.3. Treatment completion

The sites participating in the STAGE-12 trial differed in the standard length of treatment offered. Five weeks was the minimum time to deliver the full STAGE-12 intervention (5 groups and 3 individual sessions). The treatment window was expanded to 8 weeks to accommodate participants/counselors schedules and to allow time for participants to make up missed sessions. To assess treatment completion, study staff used clinic records to record each participant’s treatment attendance during the first 8 weeks of the STAGE-12 trial, which provided information for each participant’s full intervention period. Completers were defined a priori as those who attended the first 5 weeks of treatment without missing two or more consecutive weeks; a participant who attended the first 4 weeks of treatment and missed the fifth week was considered a treatment completer if s/he attended treatment during the sixth week. STAGE-12 research visits were completed at screening/baseline, study weeks 2, 4, and 8, and at three and six months following randomization.

2.3. Procedures

See Donovan et al. (Donovan et al., 2012) for a description of the STAGE-12 study procedures. Briefly, participants who met full eligibility criteria were randomized to Stimulant Abuser Groups to Engage in 12-Step (STAGE-12) or treatment as usual (TAU). Participants assigned to STAGE-12 received a combination of five group and three individual sessions that replaced the five group and three individual sessions that would typically be provided. Participants randomized to TAU received treatment as ordinarily provided by the participating CTP, which included at least five group and three individual sessions. Participants in the present study completed a single session in which baseline characteristics were assessed and the BIS-11 and Stroop were completed. This ancillary testing session was typically completed within a week of randomization into the STAGE-12 trial.

2.4. Data analysis

All analyses were completed using SAS, Version 9. 3 (SAS Institute, 2010). Pearson correlations between the Stroop and BIS-11 subscales were all non-significant (all p-values >.05).

2.4.1. Impulsivity as a function of stimulant-dependence diagnosis

Ordinary least squares regressions were used to evaluate the relationship between impulsivity and stimulant-dependence diagnosis. To determine whether any differences in impulsivity were specific to stimulant-dependence diagnosis, characteristics on which the groups significantly differed (see Table 1) were evaluated, via the corrected Akaike Information Criteria (AICC), for inclusion as covariates in the ordinary least squares regressions testing the association between stimulant-dependence diagnosis (e.g., cocaine vs. methamphetamine) and impulsivity. This approach allowed an evaluation of the association of impulsivity with stimulant-dependence diagnosis when other variables related to stimulant-dependence diagnosis were controlled. However, site and race were not evaluated for inclusion since these variables were confounded with stimulant-dependence diagnosis (i.e., sites primarily had either cocaine- or methamphetamine-dependent patients, and methamphetamine-dependent patients were primarily Caucasian while cocaine-dependent patients were primarily African American). Thus, their inclusion tended to obscure all other relationships. Based on AICC, no participant characteristics were included in BIS-11 regressions except for route of stimulant administration and non-stimulant SUD diagnosis being included for Attentional Impulsiveness. For the Stroop, years of stimulant use was included for all scores except Color Naming time, court mandate status and years of non-stimulant use were additionally included for Word Reading time and Interference score, and age and stimulant UDS results on the day of testing were additionally included for Word Reading time.

Table 1.

Baseline sample characteristics as a function of stimulant-dependence diagnosis and treatment completion

Characteristic METHa
(N=47)
Cocaine
(N=125)
Stimulant Diagnosis
Analysis Test
Statistic
Non-
Completers
(N=51)
Completers
(N=131)
Completer Analysis
Test Statistic
Age (years) 34.8 (8.4) 40.4 (9.1) W= −3.6** 36.9 (8.4) 39.2 (9.6) W= −1.7
Education (years) 11.7 (1.4) 12.1 (1.6) W= −1.5 12.1 (1.4) 12.0 (1.7) W= −0.1
Male 17.0% 36.8% χ2 (1)= 6.2* 31.4% 31.3% χ2 (1) = 0.0
Race P=0.00** χ2 (2) = 1.1
    White 87.3% 25.8% 44.0% 42.7%
    Black 2.1% 66.9% 42.0% 48.1%
    Other / Mixed 10.6% 7.3% 14.0% 9.2%
Ethnicity-Hispanic 6.4% 2.4% P = 0.16 10.0% 3.8% P = 0.08
Court Mandate 29.8% 15.2% χ2 (1)= 4.7* 23.5% 16.8% χ2 (1) = 1.1
ADHDb (%) 27.7% 36.0% χ2 (1)= 1.1 45.1% 29.8% χ2 (1) = 3.8
Currently Smoking Cigarettes 80.9% 78.4% χ2 (1) = 0.1 80.4% 78.6% χ2 (1) = 0.1
Stimulant Positive UDSc 36.2% 16.1% χ2 (1)= 8.1** 35.3% 15.4% χ2 (1) = 8.8**
Stimulant use days in last 30 6.3 (8.0) 4.5 (5.6) T(63.6)= 1.4 7.6 (8.0) 3.9 (5.1) T(66.5)= 3.1**
Stimulant Route of Administration: χ2 (2)= 30.1** χ2 (2) = 2.6
      Smoking 54.4% 81.5% 65.3% 74.8%
      Oral/Nasal 6.5% 12.9% 16.3% 8.4%
      IV 39.1% 5.6% 18.4% 16.8%
Years of Stimulant Use 10.3 (6.8) 13.1 (7.6) W= −2.1* 11.4 (7.6) 12.5 (7.7) W= −0.9
Years of Non-Stimulant Use 7.3 (6.7) 18.0 (10.4) T (128.4)=−7.9** 13.2 (10.3) 15.4 (10.6) W= −1.3
Non-Stimulant SUDd Diagnosis 48.9% 79.2% χ2 (1)= 15.2** 76.5% 69.5% χ2 (1) = 0.9
Mood/Anxiety Disorder 52.3% 37.6% χ2 (1)= 2.8 52.2% 36.8% χ2 (1) = 3.3
Stage 12 treatment assignment: χ2 (1)= 0.2 χ2 (1) = 0.6
      12-step facilitation 42.6% 46.4% 41.2% 47.3%
      Treatment as usual 57.4% 53.6% 58.8% 52.7%
Dependence Diagnosis: P = 0.00*
      Methamphetamine 100% 0% 29.4% 24.4%
      Cocaine 0% 100% 58.8% 72.5%
     Both 0% 0% 11.8% 3.1%

Note: Where not specifically indicated, numbers represent means (standard deviations).

a

METH=methamphetamine;

b

ADHD=attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder;

c

UDS=Urine drug screen;

d

SUD=substance use disorder; W=Wilcoxon Rank Sum, χ2 (df)=Pearson's Chi Square, P=Fisher's Exact P statistic,

*

p < 0.05,

**

p<.01.

2.4.2. Treatment completion analyses All participants (N=182)

To determine whether impulsivity differed significantly between treatment completers and non-completers, the demographic and baseline characteristics of the completers and non-completers were first compared using either the Pearson Chi Square or the Fisher Exact for categorical variables (depending on expected category counts) and either the Wilcoxon or Student’s t for numeric variables (depending on whether or not values for completers and non-completers had similar variance). Characteristics on which the groups differed significantly (see Table 1) were evaluated, via AICC, for inclusion as covariates in the ordinary least squares regressions comparing the groups on impulsivity. This approach allowed an evaluation of the association of impulsivity with treatment completion status when other variables related to completion status were controlled. Covariates for the BIS-11 Total Impulsiveness analysis included site, stimulant UDS result on the day of testing, and stimulant-dependence diagnosis. Stimulant UDS result on the day of testing and reported stimulant use days in the 30 days before testing were included for Stroop Interference score and stimulant dependence diagnosis was included for the Derived Interference score.

Cocaine-dependent or Methamphetamine-dependent (N=172)

To determine whether impulsivity differed significantly between completers and non-completers in the stimulant-dependent subgroups, the analytic approach taken for the entire sample was repeated with the participants who were either cocaine-dependent or methamphetamine-dependent (i.e., the 10 participants meeting criteria for dependence on both cocaine and methamphetamine were excluded) but the model was expanded to include diagnostic group, and the interaction between diagnostic group and treatment completion status. Covariates for the BIS-11 Total Impulsiveness analysis included site and stimulant UDS result on the day of testing. Reported stimulant use days in the 30 days before testing was included for the Stroop Interference score analysis.

2.4.3. Cohen’s d

It has been recommended that effect sizes be reported along with p-values to provide information about the clinical significance of an effect in addition to its statistical significance (Nakagawa 2004). Consistent with this recommendation, we calculated the Cohen's d (Cohen, 1988) associated with the comparisons described in sections 2.4.1 and 2.4.2. Specifically, the Cohen's d was calculated from the regression coefficient and variance estimates in order to reflect the difference in the mean estimates from the regression.

3. Results

3.1. Sample characteristics

Characteristics of the 182 stimulant-dependent participants, as a function of stimulant-dependence diagnosis (e.g., cocaine vs. methamphetamine) and treatment completion status, are provided in Table 1. Participants averaged 38.5 years of age and had 12 years of education on average. Approximately 31% were male, and roughly 43% were non-Hispanic Caucasians.

3.2. Relationship between impulsivity and stimulant-dependence diagnosis

Table 2 provides impulsivity scores as a function of stimulant-dependence diagnosis (methamphetamine vs. cocaine). For BIS-11 scores, methamphetamine-dependent participants had significantly higher Non-Planning Impulsiveness (P=0.045) and Total Impulsiveness (P=0.010) scores indicating greater impulsivity on these factors in the methamphetamine-dependent, relative to the cocaine-dependent, participants. As can be seen in Table 2, the difference in Non-Planning Impulsiveness score was a small effect (d=0.35) while the difference in Total Impulsiveness score was a medium effect (d=0.45). For the Stroop, cocaine-dependent participants had significantly longer Color Naming times (P=0.018) and a trend towards significantly longer Interference (P=0.058) and Derived Interference (P=0.061) times.

Table 2.

Relationship of BIS-11 and Stroop scores to stimulant-dependence diagnosis

Impulsivity Measure Methamphetamine-
dependent (N=47)
Cocaine-
dependent (N=125)
X2 Cohen's d
BIS-11:
    Attentional 18.4 (2.7) 17.5 (3.7) 1.26 0.23
    Motor 26.1 (3.7 24.8 (4.5) 3.06 0.31
    Non-planning 25.6 (4.7) 23.9 (4.6) 4.03* 0.35
    Total 70.1 (7.2) 66.2 (9.4) 6.56* 0.45
Stroop:
    Color Naming 60.4 (9.6) 65.8 (14.0) 5.59* 0.41
    Word Reading 46.1 (7.4) 50.7 (11.1) 0.81 0.19
    Interference 107.9 (26.7) 122.0 (26.8) 3.59 0.37
    Derived Interference 47.6 (24.2) 56.1 (21.4) 3.51 0.33

Note: BIS-11=Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11; Values are Means (standard deviations).

*

p < 0.05.

3.3. Relationship between impulsivity and treatment completion

Table 3 displays impulsivity scores as a function of treatment completion status for the entire sample (N=182). Non-completers had significantly higher BIS-11 Motor Impulsiveness scores (P=0.002) and a trend toward significantly higher BIS-11 Total Impulsiveness scores (P=0.053). As can be seen in Table 3, the association between Motor Impulsiveness and treatment completion status had a medium effect size (d=0.53). The analyses evaluating the interaction of stimulant-dependence diagnosis and completion status revealed no significant interaction effects (data not shown) suggesting that the relationship between the BIS-11 and treatment completion did not differ significantly between the cocaine-dependent and methamphetamine-dependent participants. As can be seen in Table 3, none of the Stroop scores differed significantly between completers and non-completers for the total sample. The analyses evaluating the interaction of stimulant-dependence diagnosis and completion status revealed no significant interaction effects (data not shown) suggesting that the relationship between the Stroop and treatment completion did not differ significantly between the cocaine-dependent and methamphetamine-dependent participants.

Table 3.

BIS-11 and Stroop scores as a function of treatment completion status

Treatment Completion

Impulsivity Measure Completers
(N=131)
Non-Completers
(N=51)
X2
(d)
BIS-11:
    Attentional 17.6 (3.6) 18.0 (3.1) 0.64 (0.14)
    Motor 24.6 (4.4) 26.8 (3.7) 9.99** (0.53)
    Non-planning 24.5 (4.7) 24.5 (4.5) 0.01 (0.01)
    Total 66.7 (9.3) 69.4 (7.5) 3.74 (0.36)
Stroop:
    Color Naming 64.5 (13.0) 64.3 (12.7) 0.00 (0.01)
    Word Reading 49.4 (10.5) 49.1 (9.3) 0.00 (0.01)
    Interference 119.9 (27.6) 114.8 (27.0) 0.57 (0.13)
    Derived Interference 55.3 (23.0) 50.5 (21.0) 1.66 (0.22)

Note:

**

p<01. d=Cohen's d; Where not specifically indicated, numbers represent eans (standard deviations).

4. Discussion

The present study evaluated the relationships among impulsivity, stimulant-dependence diagnosis, and treatment completion in 182 methamphetamine- and/or cocaine-dependent participants in a clinical trial of a 12-step facilitation intervention. The present results suggest that methamphetamine-dependent, relative to cocaine-dependent, participants evidence greater impulsivity as measured by the BIS-11. In contrast, there was a trend for poorer response inhibition in the cocaine-dependent, relative to methamphetamine-dependent, participants as measured by the Stroop. The clinical significance of BIS-11/Stroop performance was assessed by evaluating the association between baseline BIS-11/Stroop scores and treatment completion. The BIS-11 Motor scale, which reflects the tendency to act without thinking, differed significantly between treatment completers and non-completers and had a medium-sized effect. For the Stroop, there were no significant differences between treatment completers and non-completers. The results indicated that the relationship between impulsivity and treatment completion did not differ significantly between the cocaine-dependent and methamphetamine-dependent participants.

Impulsivity represents a complex construct and measures of its varying aspects often fail to be significantly related to one another (Coffey et al., 2003; de Wit, 2009; Kjome et al., 2010); this was also the case in the present study in that there were no significant correlations between the subscales of the Stroop and the BIS-11. The present finding of differential impulsivity scores between the methamphetamine- and cocaine-dependent participants suggests that there may be functional differences between stimulant-dependence diagnostic groups and is consistent with the results of Simon et al. (2002) who found differences in neurocognitive functioning (e.g., perceptual speed, manipulation of information, verbal recall) between methamphetamine- and cocaine-dependent participants. While methamphetamine and cocaine are both stimulants, they have some key differences that could lead to different impulsivity profiles in their users. One key difference entails the relative length of subjective effects, which is considerably shorter for cocaine and can lead to bingeing behavior in cocaine abusers (Newton et al., 2005); this difference, as well as others, could lead to different brain adaptations which would manifest themselves in different behavior profiles. The present finding of significantly higher BIS-11 Impulsiveness scores in methamphetamine-dependent, relative to cocaine-dependent, participants is inconsistent with the finding of a recent study by Tziortzis et al. (2011). In the Tziortzis et al. (2011) study, BIS-11 Total Impulsiveness did not differ significantly between non-treatment-seeking methamphetamine- and/or cocaine-dependent participants being screened for participation in phase I clinical trials. The difference in findings may reflect the different patient samples being evaluated in the two studies; the present study included treatment-seeking patients entering treatment at CTPs while Tziortzis et al. (2011) was conducted with patients who were explicitly non-treatment seekers.

The finding of a significant association between impulsivity as measured by the BIS-11 and treatment completion is consistent with previous findings that the BIS-11 is predictive of treatment dropout (Moeller et al., 2001) and is inversely related to time in treatment in cocaine-dependent patients (Moeller et al., 2001; Patkar et al., 2004). There are no prior published studies evaluating the relationship between the BIS-11 and treatment completion in methamphetamine-dependent patients. The present results did not find a significant interaction effect between stimulant diagnosis and treatment completion, suggesting that the relationship between BIS-11 and treatment completion did not differ significantly between cocaine-dependent and methamphetamine-dependent participants. Taken together, the present findings suggest that impulsivity is associated with worse treatment outcome in stimulant-dependent patients and supports the suggestion that medication development for stimulant use disorders should include medications to improve cognitive performance including better impulse control (Sofuoglu, 2009). In contrast, the present study revealed no significant difference in Comalli-Kaplan Stroop interference scores between completers and non-completers for any of the analyses. This is inconsistent with Streeter et al.'s (2008) finding that performance on this task was predictive of treatment completion in cocaine-dependent participants. A potentially important difference between the Streeter et al. (2008) and the present study is the recruitment source for the study participants. Streeter et al. (2008) assessed individuals participating in cocaine clinical trials that primarily relied on advertising for study recruitment (Ciraulo personal communication August 2011) while those in the present study were recruited from clinic intakes at participating CTPs. A recent analysis by Winhusen, Winstanley, Somoza, & Brigham (2012) revealed that in a psychosocial trial for women with co-occurring substance use disorder and PTSD, participants recruited through advertising, relative to those from the clinic, had significantly higher levels of baseline drug use and higher rates of meeting DSM-IV-TR criteria for full PTSD. This finding suggests that substance-abusing participants recruited via advertising may be more severely impaired relative to participants recruited through the clinic. Indeed, the average Stroop interference scores in the Streeter et al. (2008) study were substantially higher than those in the present study. This suggests that the Streeter et al. (2008) sample was more impaired and this may account for the difference in findings. The present findings are also inconsistent with those of Brewer et al. (2008) who reported a significant correlation between Stroop interference, as measured by the computerized Stroop, and treatment retention in cocaine-dependent participants. It has been noted that different versions of the Stroop yield different performance results (Salo, Henik, & Robertson, 2001) and, thus, the Stroop scores from the present study, which utilized the Comalli-Kaplan version of the Stroop, cannot be directly compared with the Stroop scores from Brewer et al. (2008). There are no prior published studies evaluating the relationship between the Stroop and treatment completion in methamphetamine-dependent patients and, thus, no literature with which to compare the present results.

The present study has several strengths and a few limitations. A clear strength of the present study is that it was conducted at multiple sites, which enhances the generalizability of the results, and included a relative large sample of stimulant-dependent participants. A recent meta-analysis found that effect sizes are larger in single-site, compared to multi-site, trials (Dechartres, Boutron, Trinquart, Charles, & Ravaud, 2011). Thus the moderate effect size found for the relationship between BIS-11 Motor Impulsiveness score and treatment completion status is notable. Another study strength is the provision of preliminary data on the relationship between BIS-11/Stroop performance and treatment completion in methamphetamine-dependent patients; the present findings suggest that the significant association between BIS-11 and treatment completion that has been found for cocaine-dependent patients likely applies to methamphetamine-dependent patients as well. Another study strength is that it was conducted with individuals seeking SUD treatment at CTPs and, thus, the results are likely generalizable to individuals in treatment for stimulant-dependence disorders. A final strength was our accounting for other factors related to treatment completion (e.g., stimulant use status at baseline, ADHD, mood disorders, etc.) to determine the predictive validity of the BIS-11 and Stroop beyond that contributed by known factors. One limitation of this study was that, while we controlled for a number of the baseline characteristics on which the cocaine-dependent and methamphetamine-dependent participants differed, we could not control for race or site because these variables were highly correlated with stimulant-dependence diagnoses (i.e., sites primarily had either cocaine- or methamphetamine-dependent patients, and methamphetamine-dependent patients were primarily Caucasian while cocaine-dependent patients were primarily African American). Thus, the differences in impulsivity found between the methamphetamine and cocaine dependent participants may have been due in part to site- or race-related factors. The degree to which this might be the case is unknown since past research on both the Stroop (Mitrushina, Boone, Razani, & D’Elia, 2005) and the BIS-11 (Stanford et al., 2009) has not addressed the impact of race and geographic area on test performance. A final limitation was the relatively small sample of methamphetamine-dependent patients, which likely resulted in our analyses being underpowered for potential stimulant-dependence diagnosis by treatment completion interaction effects.

In summary, the present results suggest that the impulsivity profiles of treatment-seeking methamphetamine-dependent and cocaine-dependent patients might differ; should this finding be replicated it might shed light on some of the key differences between abusers of those substances. Consistent with past research with cocaine-dependent participants, the BIS-11 was significantly associated with treatment completion in this more diverse sample that included cocaine and methamphetamine-dependent patients. The present study, taken together with past research, suggests that it may be useful to obtain the BIS-11 at treatment intake to help identify stimulant-dependent patients at risk for treatment non-completion, with additional interventions applied to prevent premature termination.

Acknowledgments

Funding Source

This study was supported by the following grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) Clinical Trials Network: U10-DA013036 to Oregon Health and Science University (Dr. McCarty); U10-DA013732 to the University of Cincinnati (Drs. Somoza/Winhusen); U10-DA013720 to the University of Miami School of Medicine (Dr. Szapocznik); U10-DA020024 to the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center (Drs. Adinoff/Trivedi); and U10-DA013714 to the University of Washington (Dr. Donovan).

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributors

All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript. Jeff Theobald, B.S., assisted with manuscript preparation.

References

  1. Adinoff BA, Rilling LM, Williams MJ, Schreffler E, Schepis TS, Rosvall T, Rao U. Impulsivity, neural deficits and the addictions: The 'oops' factor in relapse. Journal of Addictive Diseases. 2007;25(S1):25–39. doi: 10.1300/J069v26S01_04. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aharonovich E, Hasin DS, Brooks AC, Liu X, Bisaga A, Nunes EV. Cognitive deficits predict low treatment retention in cocaine dependent patients. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2006;81:313–322. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aharonovich E, Nunes E, Hasin D. Cognitive impairment, retention and abstinence among cocaine abusers in cognitive-behavioral treatment. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2003;71:207–211. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(03)00092-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Archibald SJ, Kerns KA. Identification and description of new tests of executive functioning in children. Child Neuropsychology. 1999;5:115–125. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bolla K, Ernst M, Kiehl K, Mouratidis M, Eldreth D, Contoreggi C, Matochik J, Kurian V, Cadet J, Kimes A, Funderburk F, London E. Prefrontal cortical dysfunction in abstinent cocaine abusers. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2004;16:456–464. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16.4.456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brewer JA, Worhunsky PD, Carroll KM, Rounsaville BJ, Potenza MN. Pretreatment brain activation during Stroop task is associated with outcomes in cocaine-dependent patients. Biological Psychiatry. 2008;64:998–1004. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Coffey SF, Gudleski GD, Saladin ME, Brady KT. Impulsivity and rapid discounting of delayed hypothetical rewards in cocaine-dependent individuals. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2003;11:18–25. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.11.1.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  9. Comalli P, Jr, Wapner S, Werner H. Interference effects of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 1962;100:47–53. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1962.10533572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Dechartres A, Boutron I, Trinquart L, Charles P, Ravaud P. Single-center trials show larger treatment effects than multicenter trials: Evidence from a meta-epidemiologic study. Annals of Internal Medicine. 2011;155:39–51. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-155-1-201107050-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. de Wit H. Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes. Addiction Biology. 2009;14:22–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00129.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Donovan DM, Daley DC, Brigham GS, Hodgkins CC, Perl HI, Garrett SB, Doyle SR, Floyd AS, Knox PC, Botero C, Kelly TM, Killeen TK, Hayes C, Kau'iBaumhofer N, Seamans C, Zammarelli L. Stimulant abuser groups to engage in 12-Step: A multisite trial in the National Institute on Drug Abuse Clinical Trials Network. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2012.04.004. In Press: doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsat.2012.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ersche KD, Barnes A, Jones PS, Morein-Zamir S, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET. Abnormal structure of frontostriatal brain systems is associated with aspects of impulsivity and compulsivity in cocaine dependence. Brain. 2011;134:2013–2024. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Ersche KD, Bullmore ET, Craig KJ, Shabbir SS, Abbott S, Müller U, Ooi C, Suckling J, Barnes A, Sahakian BJ, Merlo-Pich EV, Robbins TW. Influence of compulsivity of drug abuse on dopaminergic modulation of attentional bias in stimulant dependence. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67:632–644. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ersche KD, Jones PS, Williams GB, Turton AJ, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET. Abnormal brain structure implicated in stimulant drug addiction. Science. 2012;335:601–604. doi: 10.1126/science.1214463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fals-Stewart W, O'Farrell TJ, Freitas TT, McFarlin SK, Rutigliano P. The timeline followback reports of psychoactive substance use by drug-abusing patients: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:134–144. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.68.1.134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gardini S, Caffarra P, Venneri A. Decreased drug-cue-induced attentional bias in individuals with treated and untreated drug dependence. Acta Neuropsychiatrica. 2009;21:179–185. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2009.00389.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Golden CJ. Identification of brain disorders by the Stroop Color and Word Test. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1976;32:654–658. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(197607)32:3<654::aid-jclp2270320336>3.0.co;2-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Rajaram S. Addiction changes orbitofrontal gyrus function: Involvement in response inhibition. Neuroreport. 2001;12:2595–2599. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200108080-00060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hart CL, Marvin CB, Silver R, Smith EE. Is cognitive functioning impaired in methamphetamine users? A critical review. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:586–608. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hester R, Dixon V, Garavan H. A consistent attentional bias for drug-related material in active cocaine users across word and picture versions of the emotional Stroop task. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2006;81:251–257. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hudziak J, Helzer JE, Wetzel MW, Kessel KB, McGee B, Janca A, Przybeck P. The use of the DSM-III-R checklist for initial diagnostic assessments. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 1993;34:375–383. doi: 10.1016/0010-440x(93)90061-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jovanovski D, Erb S, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive deficits in cocaine users: A quantitative review of the evidence. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2005;27:189–204. doi: 10.1080/13803390490515694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kjome KL, Lane SD, Schmitz JM, Green C, Ma L, Prasla I, Swann AC, Moeller FG. Relationship between impulsivity and decision making in cocaine dependence. Psychiatry Research. 2010;178:299–304. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.11.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lane SD, Moeller FG, Steinberg JL, Buzby M, Kosten TR. Performance of cocaine dependent individuals and controls on a response inhibition task with varying levels of difficulty. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2007;33:717–726. doi: 10.1080/00952990701522724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lee B, London ED, Poldrack RA, Farahi J, Nacca A, Monterosso JR, Mumford JA, Bokarius AV, Dahlbom M, Mukherjee J, Bilder RM, Brody AL, Mandelkern MA. Striatal dopamine d2/d3 receptor availability is reduced in methamphetamine dependence and is linked to impulsivity. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:14734–14740. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3765-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. MacLeod CM. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin. 1991;109:163–203. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. McCann BS, Scheele L, Ward N, Roy-Byrne P. Discriminant validity of the Wender Utah Rating Scale for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2000;12:240–245. doi: 10.1176/jnp.12.2.240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Mesulam MM. Principles of behavioral neurology. Philadelphia: F. A. Davis Company; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  30. Milner B. Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In: Warren JM, Akert K, editors. The frontal granular cortex and behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1964. pp. 313–334. [Google Scholar]
  31. Mitrushina M, Boone KB, Razani J, D'Elia LF. Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological assessment. 3rd edition. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  32. Moeller FG, Dougherty DM, Barratt ES, Oderinde V, Mathias CW, Harper RA, Swann AC. Increased impulsivity in cocaine dependent subjects independent of antisocial personality disorder and aggression. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2002;68:105–111. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(02)00106-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Moeller FG, Dougherty DM, Barratt ES, Schmitz JM, Swann AC, Grabowski J. The impact of impulsivity on cocaine use and retention in treatment. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2001;21:193–198. doi: 10.1016/s0740-5472(01)00202-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Moeller FG, Hasan KM, Steinberg JL, Kramer LA, Dougherty DM, Santos RM, Valdes I, Swann AC, Barratt ES, Narayana PA. Reduced anterior corpus callosum white matter integrity is related to increased impulsivity and reduced discriminability in cocaine-dependent subjects: Diffusion tensor imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:610–617. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Nakagawa S. A farewell to Bonferroni: The problems of low statistical power and publication bias. Behavioral Ecology. 2004;15:1044–1045. [Google Scholar]
  36. Newton TF, De la Garza R, 2nd, Kalechstein AD, Nestor L. Cocaine and methamphetamine produce different patterns of subjective and cardiovascular effects. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 2005;82:90–97. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Patkar AA, Murray HM, Mannelli P, Gottheil E. Pre-treatment measures of impulsivity, aggression and sensation seeking are associated with treatment outcome for African-American cocaine-dependent patients. Journal of Addictive Diseases. 2004;23:109–122. doi: 10.1300/J069v23n02_08. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1995;51:768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Rosselli M, Ardila A, Lubomski M, Murray S, King K. Personality profile and neuropsychological test performance in chronic cocaine-abusers. The International Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;110:55–72. doi: 10.3109/00207450108994221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Rossini ED, O’Connor MA. Retrospective self-reported symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: reliability of the Wender Utah Rating Scale. Psychological Reports. 1995;77:751–754. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1995.77.3.751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Salo R, Henik A, Robertson LC. Interpreting Stroop interference: An analysis of differences between task versions. Neuropsychology. 2001;15:462–471. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.15.4.462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Salo R, Nordahl T, Natsuaki Y, Leamon M, Galloway G, Waters C, Moore C, Buonocore M. Attentional control and brain metabolite levels in methamphetamine abusers. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;61:1272–1280. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Salo R, Nordahl TE, Possin K, Leamon M, Gibson DR, Galloway GP, Flynn NM, Henik A, Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV. Preliminary evidence of reduced cognitive inhibition in methamphetamine-dependent individuals. Psychiatry Research. 2002;111:65–74. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(02)00111-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. SAS Institute Inc. SAS/STAT software, Version 9. 3. Cary, NC, USA: SAS Institute Inc.; Copyright © [2002–2010] [Google Scholar]
  45. Simon SL, Domier C, Carnell J, Brethen P, Rawson R, Ling W. Cognitive impairment in individuals currently using methamphetamine. The American Journal on Addictions. 2000;9:222–231. doi: 10.1080/10550490050148053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Simon SL, Domier CP, Sim T, Richardson K, Rawson RA, Ling W. Cognitive performance of current methamphetamine and cocaine abusers. Journal of Addictive Diseases. 2002;21:61–74. doi: 10.1300/j069v21n01_06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sobell LC, Agrawal S, Annis H, Ayala-Velazquez H, Echeverria L, Leo GI, Rybakowski JK, Sandahl C, Saunders B, Thomas S, Zióikowski M. Cross-cultural evaluation of two drinking assessment instruments: Alcohol timeline followback and inventory of drinking situations. Substance Use and Misuse. 2001;36:313–331. doi: 10.1081/ja-100102628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Sobell LC, Sobell MB. Timeline followback: a calendar method for assessing alcohol and drug use. Toronto: Addiction Research Foundation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  49. Sofuoglu M. Cognitive enhancement as a pharmacotherapy target for stimulant addiction. Addiction. 2009;105:38–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02791.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JBW the Patient Health Questionnaire Primary Care Study Group. Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: The PHQ primary care study. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association. 1999;282:1737–1744. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.18.1737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Stanford MS, Mathias CW, Doughtery DM, Lake SL, Anderson NE, Patton JH. Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: An update and review. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009;47:385–395. [Google Scholar]
  52. Stein MA, Sandoval R, Szumowski E, Roizen N, Reinecke MA, Blondis TA, Klein Z. Psychometric characteristics of the Wender Utah Rating Scale: Reliability and factor structure for men and women. Psychopharmacology Bulletin. 1995;31:425–433. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Strauss E, Sherman EM, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. 3rd edition. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  54. Streeter CC, Terhune DB, Whitfield TH, Gruber S, Sarid-Segal O, Silveri MM, Tzilos G, Afshar M, Rouse ED, Tian H, Renshaw PF, Ciraulo DA, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Performance on the Stroop predicts treatment compliance in cocaine-dependent individuals. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:827–836. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Strickland TL, Mena I, Villanueva-Meyer J, Millar BL, Cummings J, Mehringer CM, Satz P, Myers H. Cerebral perfusion and neuropsychological consequences of chronic cocaine use. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 1993;5:419–427. doi: 10.1176/jnp.5.4.419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1935;18:643–662. [Google Scholar]
  57. Tziortzis D, Mahoney JJ, 3rd, Kalechstein AD, Newton TF, De la Garza R., 2nd The relationship between impulsivity and craving in cocaine- and methamphetamine-dependent volunteers. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 2011;98:196–202. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.12.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Winhusen T, Winstanley E, Somoza E, Brigham G. The impact of recruitment method on sample characteristics and treatment outcomes in a psychosocial trial for women with co-occurring substance use disorder and PTSD. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2012;120:225–228. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.06.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES