Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Adolesc. 2012 Oct 13;36(1):31–43. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.09.002

Norms governing urban African American adolescents’ sexual and substance-using behavior

M Margaret Dolcini a,*, Joseph A Catania a, Gary W Harper b, Susan E Watson c, Jonathan M Ellen d, Senna L Towner a
PMCID: PMC3604793  NIHMSID: NIHMS414751  PMID: 23072891

Abstract

Using a probability-based neighborhood sample of urban African American youth and a sample of their close friends (N = 202), we conducted a one-year longitudinal study to examine key questions regarding sexual and drug using norms. The results provide validation of social norms governing sexual behavior, condom use, and substance use among friendship groups. These norms had strong to moderate homogeneity; and both normative strength and homogeneity were relatively stable over a one-year period independent of changes in group membership. The data further suggest that sex and substance using norms may operate as a normative set. Similar to studies of adults, we identified three distinct “norm-based” social strata in our sample. Together, our findings suggest that the norms investigated are valid targets for health promotion efforts, and such efforts may benefit from tailoring programs to the normative sets that make up the different social strata in a given adolescent community.

Keywords: Adolescents, Friendships, African American, HIV risk behavior, Norm validity, Norm stability


Urban African American youth are at high risk for STIs (e.g., (Ellen et al., 2005; Valleroy, MacKellar, Karon, Janssen, & Hayman, 1998) and are more likely to be represented among adolescent HIV/AIDS cases (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012). To date, the impact of HIV/STI public health programs in these communities has been modest. A potential weakness of past programs and the theories that underlie them is a lack of conceptual work on the complexities of social norms within the context of social development and community social structure.

Social norms are important to adolescent health (e.g., Benda & Diblasio, 1994; Brechwald & Prinstein, 2011; Romer et al., 1994; Valente, 2010) and the modification of social norms is central to community-level health promotion (Catania, Coates, & Kegeles, 1994; Crosnoe & McNeely, 2008; Lapinski & Rimal, 2005; Latkin et al., 2009). HIV/STI interventions frequently focus on changing social norms that govern sexual risk and related behaviors (Dolcini, Harper, Boyer, & Pollack, 2010; Feldman, O’Hara, Baboo, Chitalu, & Lu, 1997; Fisher, Kimble, Misovich, & Weinstein, 1998; Kirby, 2001; Rothenberg & Holmes, 2001;Stanton et al., 2002). Consequently, it is important to gain a better understanding of the norms that influence health risk behaviors. The present study examined the validity, interconnectedness, homogeneity, strength, stability, and community variation in friendship-based norms governing risk behaviors for African American adolescents living in a poor urban neighborhood. The long-term goal of this work is to develop tailored interventions that modify norms and reduce risk behavior.

Defining and measuring norms

Although norm-related concepts are integral to most health promotion models, the complexity of norms is infrequently considered (see Donnelly, Vaske, Whittaker, & Shelby, 2000; Etzioni, 2000; Hechter & Opp, 2001a, 2001b; Rimal, 2008;Risjord, 1998; Verkooijen, de Vries, & Nielsen, 2007), and there is little empirical work to guide the selection of concepts and measurement strategies. Norms may operate on multiple social psychological levels (i.e., expectations, values, social rules;Horne, 2001; Lapinski & Rimal, 2005) and with respect to different levels of social connectedness (e.g., reference group norms at the friendship, neighborhood, or societal levels). Moreover, norm assessments may include either descriptive (i.e., perceptions of what is) or injunctive (i.e., what ought-to-be) approaches, and may be conducted at individual or network levels. Finally, there is some uncertainty about the extent to which social norms govern behaviors that occur almost entirely under private circumstances (e.g., sexual behavior).

Typically, studies that address sensitive or private behaviors assess individual-level perceptions of an assumed reference group norm (e.g., Arrow & Burns, 2004; Borsari & Carey, 2001; Fishbein et al., 2001; Lapinski & Rimal, 2005; Norris & Ford, 1998; Richard, Bell, & Montoya, 2000; Stanton et al., 2002). However, perceived norms may reflect a false consensus; that is, perceptions of group activity may simply be products of one’s own behavior, rather than reflecting the social norm (Ross, Greene, & House, 1977). Alternately, an individual may fail to perceive an existing norm (Rimal, 2008), such that a group member does not consciously recognize a norm, but his or her behavior is affected by norm-related social rewards or sanctions. These shortcomings may be addressed by obtaining norm data from multiple members of a given reference-group (Latkin et al., 2009; Rothenberg & Holmes, 2001).

Research on reference group norms should focus on norms associated with meaningful relationships (Campo et al., 2003;Dolcini & Adler, 1994; Dolcini et al., 2010). Friendship groups are important during adolescence (Brown, Dolcini, & Leventhal, 1997; Savin-Williams & Berndt, 1990) and urban African American youth report close longstanding friendships (Dolcini, Harper, Watson, Catania & Ellen, 2005). Thus, the current examination of norms is focused on friendship groups.

Norm properties and health promotion

Norm relatedness/sets

Building on earlier theorists, Rahav (1984) hypothesized that sets of inter-related norms converge to pattern behavior. For example, a set of related norms for acceptable public behavior is seen in how North Americans queue in public; individuals form a single line, do not push or cut in, and save places for individuals who have to temporarily exit. From a health promotion perspective, it is important to understand whether targeted behavior is governed by sets of related norms. Interventions may have a diminished impact if they are directed at only part of the relevant normative set. Norms governing adolescent sexual behavior maybe independent or may reflect an inter-related normative set that includes substance use norms. A set perspective is suggested by the frequent co-occurrence of adolescent sex and substance use. For instance, adolescent substance use may occur in conjunction with sex to enhance sexual pleasure or cope with life stressors (He, Kramer, Houser, Chomitz, & Hacker, 2004). Thus, norms governing sexual behavior and substance use are likely to be correlated and associated with similar outcomes. However, not all drug or alcohol use occurs in the context of sex. Thus, we anticipated that drug and sexual norms would be more strongly associated with their domain specific outcomes than with the co-occurring behavior.

Norm homogeneity and strength

Norm homogeneity reflects the extent to which reference group members hold similar normative expectations. Norm strength reflects the intensity with which members adhere to normative positions (e.g., a strongly pro-drug norm). Greater norm homogeneity and strength should be related to greater behavioral conformity among friends. For instance, when group members uniformly support a strongly pro-condom norm, it is likely that members use condoms regularly. Such norms would support sexual health promotion programs. Conversely, condom norms that are highly homogeneous and negative would pose barriers to condom use interventions.

Community-level norms

Community-level norms are relevant because sexual health programs often target community-level change through diffusion (Latkin et al., 2009; Rogers, 1995). Information about community-level norms can be obtained by aggregating friendship group norms. High levels of norm homogeneity across friendship groups suggest a strong community-level norm, whereas high variability reflects a weaker community norm. Understanding homogeneity across reference groups is important because adolescents may have multiple reference groups (Brown, 1990; Dolcini & Adler, 1994; Verkooijen et al., 2007) and the norms of all these groups may be relevant. Additionally, adolescents are in a period of exploration that may heighten the impact of “out-group” norms. Taken as a whole, the degree of homogeneity in norms across friendship networks is viewed as representing the strength of community-level normative influences.

Norm stability

Norm stability reflects the extent to which norm homogeneity and strength remain constant over time. Changes in friendship group membership may influence norm stability, but prior research has not directly addressed this issue. Highly fluid groups may experience norm change, thus reducing norm stability. Alternatively, norms may be resistant to changes in composition. For instance, incoming group members may be subject to the same expectations as existing members. In addition, new group members may be selected by or attracted to groups on the basis of shared values (e.g., Kandel, 1978;Matsueda & Anderson, 1998).

Thus, the fluidity observed in adolescent friendship networks (Savin-Williams & Berndt, 1990) may impact health promotion efforts that rely on norm change. For instance, difficulties in sustaining gains in condom use (e.g., Fisher, Fisher, Bryan, & Misovich, 2002; Johnson, Carey, Marsh, Levin, & Scott-Sheldon, 2003) may reflect changes in adolescent reference groups, and subsequent diminishment of group-level pro-condom norms. In their longitudinal investigation, Fang, Stanton, Li, Feigelman, and Baldwin (1998) found lower stability in expectations around use of condoms at follow-up, suggesting an erosion of pro-condom norms. However, they did not examine friendship group mobility, so it is unclear if group change led to norm instability.

Norms and social stratification

Finally, social stratification of norms may be relevant to health promotion. Sociologists describe three societal stratifications regarding sexual behavior and values, that they label traditional (e.g, conservative), relational, and recreational (Lauman, Gagnon, Michaels, & Michael, 1994). And, research on African American youth found that sex related norms vary in strength across different social strata (Reed et al., 2012). Thus, interventions may need to be tailored to community segments (Brown et al., 1997; Latkin et al., 2009). For example, youth who regularly attend school and church represent a different social segment than youth who are truant, non-church attendees; their risk behaviors differ (McCree, Wingood, DiClemente, Davies, & Harrington, 2003; Wallace & Williams, 1997) and they probably hold differing norms.

The current study

The current investigation examined friendship-based reference group norms that govern sexual health and substance use (i.e., sexual recreation, condom, and drug norms) and are relevant to HIV/STI transmission. Specifically, the current study examined perceptions of norms by friendship group members, addressed the validity of network norms (i.e., Do they predict the behavior of individuals in the group?), and describes properties of norms relevant to sexual health promotion (e.g., inter-relatedness, stability). In addition, we examined the extension of reference group norms to community-level norms.

Based on prior work and parallel to larger societal divisions in sexual behavior and values (Laumann, Gagnon, Michael, & Michaels, 1994) we expected to observe two large strata, reflecting (1) low and (2) moderate endorsement of sex and drugs, and (3) a smaller strata with high endorsement of sex and drug use. We hypothesized that the three behavioral strata would reflect traditional, moderate, and highly positive (i.e., recreational) sex and drug use norms, respectively. Further, we hypothesized that condom norms would be strong only among groups moderate on the recreation norm scale. That is, traditional groups would not use condoms because, in general, they are more sex negative, while the highly positive sex groups might view condoms as counter to their focus on recreation and pleasure (Arnold, Fletcher, & Farrow, 2002; Fisher, Byrne, White, & Kelley, 1988; Roberti, 2004).

In summary, the present study aims to address relevant issues concerning norms and norm properties in a longitudinal sample of urban African American friendship groups. To the best of our knowledge, despite considerable attention to the issues of norms in health promotion, no studies to date have examined sexual and substance use norm properties among adolescents.

Method

Recruitment and procedure

The present investigation was part of a longitudinal study examining social development and risk behavior among urban African American youth. The institutional review board (IRB) at the sponsoring institution approved study procedures. Participants were recruited through random digit dialing from a specific zip code defining an urban neighborhood in San Francisco to obtain a representative sample of African Americans aged 14–18. African American adolescents in this neighborhood have the highest rates of sexually transmitted infections in the city (Ellen et al., 2005; STD Control Section, 2009).

In the initial screening, 70% of contacted households agreed to cooperate (see Dolcini et al., 2005). This cooperation rate is in line with studies of ethnic minority urban households using similar techniques (e.g., Duncan, Strycker, Duncan, He, & Stark, 2002; Ellen et al., 2005). Participants over age 18 and parents, for those under 18, provided informed consent, and all youth under 18 assented. Youth received remuneration for participation in the study. Of the 119 individuals recruited via telephone from the initial probability sample, 91 (76%) subsequently completed an interview with a gender-matched interviewer. These friendship-network seeds identified 333 close friends,111 of whom were successfully recruited for the study. Friends qualified for participation if they were aged 13–21 years and lived in or near the study neighborhood. Initial eligibility was determined by the seed, and confirmed by recruited friends. The 91 seeds and 111 friends completed an in person baseline interview and 81% participated in a 1-year follow-up interview (163/202).

Sample characteristics

Descriptive data on the initial probability sample of youth (seeds), the enumerated friendship networks, and of the close friends sample (subsample of enumerated friends) have been reported previously (Dolcini et al., 2005). In general, the close friend sample parallels the enumerated list of friends in terms of age, place of residence, gender, friendship length and quality, and proportion of friends spending time together on weekends. On average, friendship groups included about four youth and over 90% were same gender, as is typical for adolescents (Ennett & Bauman, 1996; Urberg, Değirmencioğlu, Tolson, & Halliday-Scher, 1995).

For the present analyses, we limited the sample to friendship groups for which we had a seed and corresponding data on at least one friend. This reduced the baseline sample from 202 to 167 respondents, representing 57 friendship clusters. The respondents in the 57 friendship groups did not significantly differ from the original sample on gender, employment status, neighborhood residence, mean age, relationship length, the proportion of friends youth hang out with on weekends, network size, or friendship quality (see Table 1). The demographic, norm, and behavioral measures were embedded in a larger interview on health and sexual health (available from the authors).

Table 1.

Demographic and relationship variables for study sample.

Variable Total (n = 167) Seeds (n = 57) Close friends (n = 110)
%
Male 55.7 56.1 55.5
African American 95.1 100 92.5
Currently in school 87.4 89.5 86.4
Employed 45.5 42.1 47.3
Reside in study neighborhood 83.5 100 75.0
M (SD)
Age 16.7 (1.5) 16.5 (1.4) 16.8 (1.5)
Length of relationship (years) 6.4 (2.8) 6.1 (2.8) 6.5 (2.8)
Weekend associationa 72 (31) 74 (30) 68 (31)
Friendship quality score 6.2 (0.9) 6.2 (0.8) 6.2 (1.0)
Network sizeb 4.0 (1.5) 3.9 (1.5) 4.0 (1.5)

Note. First six items are demographic variables; last four items are relationship variables.

a

Average percent of friends within group that the seed/friends hang out with on the weekends.

b

Number of enumerated friends.

Norm scales

The items for the norm measures were scored or reverse scored so that higher scores reflected supporting more risky practices and then summed to produce a total score. Table 2 presents psychometric data, which support the reliability and validity of the three scales reflecting recreation norms, condom norms, and drug norms. The recreation scale was so named to reflect a greater normative emphasis on personal pleasure to the exclusion of sexual health. Items for all scales are also included in the table.

Table 2.

Norm scales: items, reliability, and factor structure.

Scale Factor loading
Scale
Cronbach’s alpha
1 2 3 M (SD)
Recreation norma 1.95 (0.61) 0.82
 that it is okay to have sex with someone they just met. (R) 0.79
 that it is okay to say “no” when they do not want to have sex. 0.59
 that it is okay to have sex with lots of people. (R) 0.74
 that it is not okay to have sex with someone else when they have a
 main boyfriend or girlfriend.
0.52
 that it is okay to for teenagers to get pregnant. (R) 0.30
 that it is not cool to be going out with someone and not have sex. (R) 0.55
 that it is okay to pressure someone to have sex with them. (R) 0.65
Condom norma,b,c 1.53 (0.51) 0.78
 that people should always use a condom when having sex with a new person.a 0.60
 that people should use condoms whenever they have sex, even when they
 have sex with a main girlfriend or boyfriend.a
0.72
 use condoms when they have sex with a new person.b 0.67
 use condoms whenever they have sex, even when they have sex with a main
 boyfriend or girlfriend.b
0.77 0.34
Drug normd 2.08 (0.87) 0.82
 are high on alcohol or drugs when they have sex. 0.66
 get drunk or high at least once a week. 0.79
 that it is cool to get drunk or high. (R) 0.72

Note. Factor Analysis, Eigen values: Factor 1 = 4.54, Factor 2 = 2.15, Factor 3 = 1.65 with Oblimin Rotation; loadings ≥ 0.30 shown in table, total % variance accounted for by 3 factors = 60%. Mean scale scores range from 1 to 4. (R) = Item reverse scored.

a

Item(s) stem = “Most of your close friends think.”

b

Item(s) stem = “Most of your close friends.”

c

Condom items based on both stems and were examined with and without z-transformation which, as expected, had no effect on correlations

d

First two items stem = “How many of your close friends are.”; third item stem = “Most of your closest friends think.”

Friendship characteristics

Friendship characteristics included network size, average length of friendships, and days of the week that friends spent time together. Friendship quality was assessed with a 7-item measure (e.g., Do you trust [friend’s name]? Would you ever talk to [friend’s name] about family problems or really personal problems? Would [friend’s name] ever back you up?” (Dolcini et al., 2005) (Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.76).

Substance use

We examined cigarette, alcohol, and other drug use. We dichotomized substance use items because a relatively small proportion of the sample reported high levels of use.

Cigarette smoking was categorized into regular use (weekly/daily smoking, 12%) vs. irregular/nonuse (1/month or less, 88%) to distinguish smokers from experimenters and nonusers.

The drug and alcohol use variable was categorized to reflect regular use and problematic use patterns vs. experimentation/nonuse (alcohol use with friends and alcohol use alone 1/month to almost every day vs. a few times in lifetime to nonuse; smoked marijuana/weed 1/month to almost every day vs. a few times in lifetime to nonuse). For potentially more dangerous drugs (e.g., crack/cocaine, speed, LSD), any use was considered problematic (almost every day to at least once vs. never). We aggregated across drug/alcohol use; users were scored as 1 (44% of respondents) while the reference category was zero drug use or very low experimental marijuana or alcohol use (56% of respondents). Self-report measures of substance use are valid and reliable assessments of adolescent behavior when confidentiality is assured, the instrument is well constructed, and privacy is adequate, as was the case here (see Fisher et al., 2002; Johnston & O’Malley, 1985).

Sexual behaviors

For the present analyses we selected the most broadly relevant indicators of sexual transition (virginity status), and sexual risk for HIV/STIs (condom use during vaginal or anal intercourse, numbers of sexual partners).

Virginity status was defined as nonvirgin (67%) vs. virgin (33%, reference group = 0) reflecting experience with vaginal or anal intercourse.

We assessed frequency of condom use during vaginal and anal intercourse, and frequency of vaginal and anal intercourse in the past year for each of participants’ three most recent sexual partners. We aggregated across partners for sexually active participants and computed the overall percent condom use (Catania et al., 1992, 2005). For analytic purposes, we categorized condom use into inconsistent/nonuse (31% of sexually active participants) vs. consistent use (all the time, reference group = 0; 69% of sexually active participants) during vaginal and/or anal intercourse.

Multiple sexual partners were defined as having two or more sexual partners (involving anal or vaginal intercourse) in the past year; 35% reported multiple partners. We limited these analyses to sexually active respondents and categorized them as those with multiple vs. only one sexual partner (reference group = 0). Prior work has shown, that within the limits of self-reported behavior, measures of adolescent sexual behavior are reliable (Catania, Gibson, Marin, Coates, & Greenblatt, 1990).

Results

Norm relatedness/sets

Scale characteristics

We conducted exploratory factor analyses using Oblimin rotation to allow for correlated factors to examine the extent to which the three norm measures reflected independent norms at the item level. The results (Table 2) indicated that items hypothesized to be indicators for each norm loaded on three distinct factors. One minor exception was an item from the condom norm measure (loading > 0.30 on the drug norm factor). However, this item has a substantially larger loading on the condom norm factor. The factor analyses suggest that the three norm scales measured correlated, but independent constructs.

Table 2 also shows the scale means, standard deviations, and the Cronbach’s Alphas for each scale at baseline. All three scales evidence good internal reliability at baseline and 12 months, (Cronbach’s Alphas at follow-up; recreation norm = 0.83, condom norm = 0.76, and drug norm = 0.84, suggesting they are psychometrically stable over one year.

Next we examined the inter-norm correlations. The drug and sexual recreation norm scales were moderately correlated (r = 0.51, p = 0.001), while more modest correlations were found for the condom and recreation norm scales (r = 0.29, p = 0.01) and the condom and drug norm scales (r = 0.26, p = 0.01). These results suggest that, as anticipated, the three norms form a moderately inter-related normative set.

Scale validity

To examine the validity of the norm scales, we investigated the relationship between group-level norms and individual-level behavior. We computed the relationship of the friendship group norms to sexual and substance using behavior of each group’s seed (i.e., index case). We selected the seeds’ baseline behavior as the methodologically appropriate target for analytic purposes because these respondents were selected from a probability sample and therefore, are most likely to be unbiased representatives of the community. We examined smoking, drug/alcohol use, virginity status, multiple sex partners, and inconsistent condom use (see Table 3). The mean norm score for each friendship group was used to represent the group norm. An Empirical Bayes estimation was used to correct for differences in group size in computing group means for recreation and drug norms. In contrast, group mean values for the condom norm were extremely homogeneous and did not require adjustment.

Table 3.

Friendship group norms as a correlate of seed behavior (n = 57 groups).

Seed behaviora Group norm
Recreation
Condom
Drug
OR [95% CI] p-Value OR [95% CI] p-Value OR [95% CI] p-Value
Sexual
 Nonvirgin 1.4 [1.1, 1.7] ** 1.2 [1.0, 1.5] * 1.2 [1.0, 1.3] **
 Multiple partners/year 1.5 [1.2, 2.0] *** 1.1 [1.0, 1.4] ns 1.1 [1.0, 1.3] *
 Inconsistent condom use 1.1 [0.9, 1.2] ns 1.3 [1.1, 1.7] ** 1.1 [1.0, 1.3] ns
Drugs/alcohol
 Smoking 1.0 [0.8, 1.3] ns 1.3 [1.0, 1.9] ns 1.3 [1.1, 1.7] **
 Drug/alcohol 1.2 [1.0, 1.4] ns 1.4 [1.1, 1.8] ** 1.6 [1.3, 2.0] ***

Note. To correct for cluster size, recreation and drug norms utilize an Empirical Bayes estimation of the group mean. Condom norms are based on the observed group mean because cluster values are extremely homogeneous obviating the need for the Empirical Bayes estimation method. OR: Odds ratio. The norm scores are adjusted to a unit value of 0.1 for all logit models; CI: confidence interval. p < 0.05*, 0.01**, 0.001***; ns = p > 0.06.

a

Seed reported behavior at baseline interview and coded as Yes = 1, No = 0.

Univariate logistic models

At the univariate level, sexual recreation, condom, and drug norms all had significant behavioral outcomes in common, as well as unique relationships to other outcomes (see Table 3). For instance, all three norms were correlated with virginity status: more positive recreation, drug, and condom norms were associated with being sexually experienced. In contrast, only the drug norm was related to cigarette smoking, with more positive drug norms associated with higher use. And, only the condom norm was associated with condom use; higher anti-condom norms were correlated with inconsistent condom use. At least two of the norms were significantly associated with one of the other behavioral outcomes (i.e., drug and alcohol use, multiple sexual partners).

Multivariate logistic models

In instances where multiple norms were significantly correlated with an outcome (i.e., virginity status, multiple partners, and substance use), we examined their relative contributions using multivariate logistic regression. Condom use was not examined in a multivariate model because only the condom norm was related to that outcome, and a multivariate model was unwarranted.

In the multivariate models, independent variables were entered with more theoretically proximal causes entered last in order to control for the less proximal variables (e.g., the drug norm measure was entered last in the substance use model). All models included all three norm measures. For virginity status and multiple sexual partners, only the recreation norm was statistically significant (ORs [95% CI], respectively = 1.3 [1.05, 1.64], p = 0.01; 1.5 [1.1,1.9], p = 0.002) relative to other norms in the models (all p values > 0.10). In the substance use model, only the drug norm was significant (OR [95% CI] = 1.7 [1.29, 2.27], p = 0.001).

Norm homogeneity and strength (level)

Table 4 provides data on the homogeneity (Intra Class Correlations [ICC]) and strength (mean level) of the three norms for the friendship groups (n = 57 groups).

Table 4.

Stability and homogeneity of norm scales.

All friendship groups
Longitudinal friendship groups
ICC [95% CI], p-value M (SD) ICC [95% CI], p-value M (SD)
Baseline
 Recreation norm 0.49 [0.33, 0.65], <0.001 1.95 (0.61) 0.56 [0.39, 0.73], <0.001 1.98 (0.62)
 Condom norm 0.04 [0.00, 0.20], >0.10 1.53 (0.51) 0.05 [0.00, 0.23], 0.291 1.49 (0.49)
 Drug norm 0.48 [0.32, 0.64], <0.001 2.08 (0.87) 0.41 [0.21, 0.60], <0.001 2.07 (0.83)
12 Months
 Recreation norm 0.47 [0.28, 0.66], <0.001 1.96 (0.59) 0.54 [0.35, 0.73], <0.001 1.98 (0.62)
 Condom norm 0.03 [0.00, 0.25], >0.10 1.64 (0.49) 0.10 [0.00, 0.31], >0.10 1.62 (0.51)
 Drug norm 0.46 [0.27, 0.65], <0.001 2.15 (0.85) 0.49 [0.29, 0.69], <0.001 2.14 (0.85)

Note. ICC values range from 0 to 1, and reflect the percent of total variation due to between-cluster variation. ICC values of zero reflect high homogeneity with heterogeneity increasing to a maximum at ICC value of 1. Groups = 2 or more close friends including seed. All friendship groups = 167 respondents (57 groups) at baseline and 127 (55 groups) at 12 months. Longitudinal friendship groups (models effect of change in group membership over time) = 123 respondents at baseline and 105 respondents at 12 months (38 friendship groups at both time points). A small proportion of the friendship groups (10%) in our sample included both males and females. We examined ICC values for groups with and without mixed gender members and the results were extremely similar. Unless otherwise noted, data for all groups are reported.

We were interested in the homogeneity of mean scores for norms between the clusters in our sample, as opposed to the more typical assessment of variability in scores among members within clusters. Since ICC values, which vary from 0 to 1, represent the within group variation over the total variation (McGraw & Wong, 1996), in this study they reflect the degree of homogeneity between friendship groups (e.g., a value of 0 = complete homogeneity). The ICC values and corresponding 95% confidence intervals were computed using STATA (StataCorp, 2001). P-values were obtained by dividing the ICC by the asymptotic standard error to obtain large sample Z statistics that test whether the ICC is zero in the population from which the sample was drawn.

Although we can test ICC values for statistical significance, there are not guidelines for interpreting whether the ICC values reflect high, moderate, or low homogeneity. To compensate for the lack of guidelines for categorizing ICC values, we constructed four equal interval ICC categories that provide a working metric: very homogenous (ICC range 0.00–0.25), moderately homogenous (ICC range 0.26–0.50), low homogeneity (ICC range 0.51–0.75), and very low homogeneity (ICC range 0.76–1.00).

Condom norm

The condom norm ICC values were not statistically significant from zero, reflecting a strong degree of homogeneity. That is, friendship groups held similar beliefs regarding condom use. In terms of normative strength, friendship groups were strongly pro-condom. Overall, scores fell within one standard deviation (SD) of the mean and ranged from a very pro-condom value of 1.02 to a moderately pro-condom value of 2.04. Thus, friendship groups reflected consistently homogeneous norms that were generally pro-condom, indicating a high degree of conformity at the community level.

Recreation norm and drug norm

In contrast to the condom norms, the ICC values for the recreation and drug norms were both significant (Table 4), reflecting moderate homogeneity. In terms of norm strength, mean drug norm scores within 1 SD of the grand mean (across groups) range from a strongly anti-drug mean score of 1.21 to a weaker anti-drug mean score of 2.95. Mean recreation norm scores within 1 SD of the grand mean range from a strong anti-recreation value of 1.34 to a moderate anti-recreation value of 2.26. Thus, the recreation and drug norms have somewhat greater variability in normative strength and homogeneity than the condom norm (see Fig. 1). In total, these findings suggest that, at the community level, there is less conformity on sexual recreation and drug using norms compared to the condom norm.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Friendship group measures for recreation, condom, and drug norms.

Gender differences in group norms

We also examined norm homogeneity separately for male and female friendship groups, excluding mixed gender groups (10% of friendship groups were mixed gender). Males and females were similar in terms of norm homogeneity on the condom and drug norm (data available from authors), but appeared to differ on the recreation norm. Female friendship groups evidenced a very homogeneous sexual recreation norm (ICC = 0.17, p > 0.10) and male friendship groups showed more moderate homogeneity (ICC = 0.40, p < 0.05). There was also a significant gender difference on mean values for the sexual recreation norm (t = 5.84, p < 0.0001; M’s [SD] respectively = 2.29 [0.49], and 1.52 [0.46]), with males expressing a stronger sexual recreation norm.

Norm stability

The longitudinal nature of the study allowed us to examine ICC values for all three norm measures at baseline and 12 months later. The values showed either minimal or no change, evidencing stability (see Table 4). This stability is confirmed by comparing mean norm levels; all mean differences were non-significant (p > 0.10).

We also conducted an exploratory analysis to examine stability in norm homogeneity over time as function of group continuity. First, we identified; 1) a restricted longitudinal sample including only intact friendship groups across the 12 month period, and 2) an all groups longitudinal sample that included all friendship groups regardless of whether they were intact or had changed over time. This comparison does not directly contrast intact vs. changed groups, but addresses the broader question of whether changes in group membership influence overall community norms. We then contrasted the ICC values over time for the two longitudinal sample constructions. The results indicated that the inclusion of changed friendship groups had little impact on the ICC values. ICC values varied by 0.07 or less from baseline to follow-up in both samples (Table 4). Our results suggest that movement of friends into and out of the close friend sample did not substantially influence norm stability.

Norm structure

Finally, we examined whether the friendships formed meaningful clusters based on the patterns observed among the three norm measures. Using cluster analysis, we examined how norms clustered by friendship group (i.e., the cluster units were the 57 friendship groups). We hypothesized three general clusters based on norms: conservative, moderate, and recreation clusters. We hypothesized that these three groups would reflect, respectively, increasingly greater conformity to pro-drug and recreation norms, and that condom norms would be strong only among groups moderate on the sexual recreation norm (see scale validity section for information on use of Empirical Bayes estimation).

Cluster analysis was conducted using three different clustering methods (Ward’s Method; Average Linkage Within Group and K-Means Methods). We followed the guidance of other investigators and used Ward’s method as an exploratory approach to identify clusters and assign cases to subgroups, and the other two methods were used as a confirmatory approach to validate typology (Anderberg, 1973; Burns & Burns, 2008). Thus, the confirmatory work lines up with the initial results using Ward’s method and gives us more confidence in our findings. All three methods consistently identified 3 clusters in our sample; recreation seekers, moderates, and conservatives. Unexpectedly, but consistent with our findings on homogeneity of the condom norm at the community level, we found that for all three clusters the condom scales reflected pro-condom use norms (see Table 5). Thus, although the drug and recreation norms produced clusters as expected, the condom norm did not differentiate the clusters.

Table 5.

Norm cluster analysis: profile summary (Ward’s method).

Cluster Norm
Recreation Ma(SD) Condom M(SD) Drug Ma(SD)
Recreationists 2.55 (0.22) 1.67 (0.31) 3.22 (0.39)
Moderates 1.98 (0.39) 1.62 (0.30) 2.45 (0.25)
Conservatives 1.71 (0.41) 1.45 (0.30) 1.50 (0.34)
a

Bayes estimators used for recreation and drug norm.

Groups identified as recreation seekers had higher scores on all three norms. All three clustering methods identified the same nine friendship groups in this cluster (16% of the groups). Moderates composed a cluster of groups with moderate scores on recreation and drug norms. Ward’s Method identified 15 groups as a moderate cluster (26% of the groups). Of those groups identified as moderate by Ward’s Method, 87% were subsequently identified by the two confirmatory methods as moderate (>70% is considered adequate confirmation). The final cluster of friendship groups, conservatives, had lower scores on recreation and drug norms. Ward’s Method identified 33 groups in this cluster (58% of the groups), and of these groups, 94% were identified by the two confirmatory methods as a conservative cluster.

Discussion

This study sought to investigate social norms at the group and community levels for sexual behavior and substance use among African American adolescent friendship groups living in an urban neighborhood. Additionally, we examined properties of norms that have relevance to HIV/STI prevention among adolescents (homogeneity, strength, stability, inter-relatedness, stratification).

Norm validity

Our findings support the validity of the norms examined. All three reference group norms (i.e., recreation, condom use, drug use) were significantly associated with individual-level behavior in friendship groups; relevant reference group norms predicted condom use, multiple partnerships, and substance use by individuals. These results support findings from prior work using individual level perceived norm measures (Borsari & Carey, 2001; Fishbein et al., 2001). Thus, our findings do not support the false consensus hypothesis.

Our focus on norms that impact behaviors that are typically private (e.g., sex) enabled us to address validity of group norms on such behaviors. Although sex is usually unobserved by friends, our findings suggest that group norms generalize to sexual activities. Sexual norms, as measured at the friendship group level, predicted individual-level sexual behavior, confirming the proposition that social sanctions impact private sexual acts. Findings also suggest that research addressing the social elements of reference group norm processes, such as reputation-based sanctions (e.g., snubbing, avoiding a person; Brown, 1990; Eder, 1985), may require investigation at both group and individual levels.

Although our findings contribute to the literature on adolescent norms, there are aspects of norm validity that we were unable to address. We were unable to examine how norms develop and how their impact may change across adolescence. Prior work shows that perceived condom use norms significantly impacted new sexual relationships among young adults, but had little effect on condom use in more established relationships (Catania et al., 1994). Additional work is needed to examine the extent to which norms govern sexual behaviors of adolescents over the course of their romantic and sexual relationships.

Norm properties and social stratification

This investigation offers an initial foray into the concept of normative sets as applied to adolescent sexual behavior and substance use. We found that the three norms examined are inter-related, but do not have a simple additive effect on individual-level behavior (i.e., only one norm was significant in each multivariate model testing multiple norms). As anticipated, each norm had primarily unique effects, but norms may converge at the situational level. For instance, friends who use drugs together may find themselves in situations that increase the chances that they will have sex (e.g., as part of the “party scene” Bachanas et al., 2002). However, the choice to have sex is more directly determined by expectations regarding appropriate sexual behavior, as opposed to drug use.

Despite the inter-relatedness of substance use and sexual behavior norms, we do not assume that they reflect one overarching norm (e.g., a deviance norm), in part because the norms each have some unique behavioral relationships. At the same time, it may be important to direct interventions at the entire norm set. Positive outcomes in HIV/STI interventions for urban ethnic minority youth may be related in part to the attention given to the role of substance use in unsafe sexual behavior (Dolcini et al., 2010; Jemmott, Jemmott, Braverman, & Fong, 2005).

In terms of norm homogeneity and strength, our finding of a strong pro-condom norm across friendship groups suggests a community-level norm. Other work with African Americans has shown that adults have relatively positive condom attitudes and high condom use (Anderson, 1999; Catania et al., 2001; Thorburn, Harvey, & Ryan, 2005), and youth have relatively high condom use and perceive their friends to be using condoms (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2010; Romer et al., 1994; Stanton et al., 2002). Thus, African American adolescents may be embedded in a pro-condom social environment. The extensive HIV/STI prevention conducted in African American communities in California, including the community in our study, has led to high levels of safe-sex awareness. However, along with procondom norms, we also found small clusters of youth with generally pleasure-focused or recreational norms. Thus, there may continue to be small, but highly sexually active subpopulations of youth (e.g., with high levels of sexual mixing and sexual partner concurrency) that continue to contribute disproportionately to STI spread (Fichtenberg, Jennings, Glass, & Ellen, 2010). Nevertheless, strong and consistent condom norms provide a useful base for further behavior change.

In contrast to the condom norm, we did not observe norm strength and homogeneity for the drug and recreation norms. We found larger differences between friendship groups on both these norms, and observed gender differences on the recreation norm. The present data suggest that, with regard to sex, females conform to a less pleasure-focused norm and have greater homogeneity in their normative expectations regarding sexual recreation than do males. Although the results indicate that males are more “recreational” in their sexual values than females, their sexual values are more diverse than stereotypes would suggest (e.g., Anderson, 1999). In fact, the majority of male friendship groups were sexually traditional. These findings support qualitative studies indicating that African American adolescent males are not uniformly accepting of recreational sexual behavior (Giordano, Longmore, Manning, & Northcutt, 2009; Harper, Gannon, Watson, Catania, & Dolcini, 2004; Reed et al., 2012; Towner, Dolcini, & Harper, in press).

Community-level norm stability was another focus of the present study. In general, norm stability is important to consider in the context of health promotion because stable norms may be resistant to change efforts, while transient norms may lack the power to maintain programmatic changes over time. In this study, all three norms were found to be highly to moderately stable over time at the community level, and there was no observable impact on homogeneity (across groups) due to changes in group membership over time. This suggests that friendship groups either remain sufficiently intact to preserve norms or that new friends already share or quickly acquire similar values. Both selection and influence contribute to similarities in behavior among adolescent friends (Crosnoe & McNeely, 2008; Kandel, 1978; Savin-Williams & Berndt, 1990) and future work should further explore these processes with regard to norms.

We also examined how norms may form more “macro-level” structures at the community level. Past work confirms that, among adolescents, social stratification is evident and has consequences for health behaviors (e.g., crowds; Brown, 1990; Brown, Von Bank, & Steinberg, 2008; Dolcini & Adler, 1994; La Greca, Prinstein, & Fetter, 2001). Social stratification was also evident in the present study. The cluster analysis partially supported the hypothesis that we would find three social strata with differing norms regarding substance use, sexual recreation, and condoms. The largest of the three strata is relatively traditional, followed by a more moderate group, and a smaller but still substantial stratum of more recreationally focused friendship groups (16% of groups). However, contrary to prediction, all three social strata had similar normative values around condom use, reflecting the broad pro-condom attitudes noted earlier. Given the variation in norms across friendship groups, these findings suggest that tailoring adolescent interventions to specific population segments or social groups will provide productive avenues for HIV/STI prevention. One approach includes friendship based interventions that are designed to incorporate social and cultural factors into the structure and the content of programs and to influence behavior through the engagement of the social group in the intervention process (Dolcini et al., 2010; Stanton et al., 1996). Another approach might involve programs tailored to different school-based crowds where norms and behavior show substantial variation (see Brown et al., 1997).

Limitations and summary

Our study is based on a single neighborhood, and generalization may be limited. The present study is limited by our reliance on self-reports; sexual behavior and substance use are misreported to different degrees. However, our goal is not to estimate the prevalence of behaviors, but is focused on the relationships between norms and behavior.

Our research focused on close friends because one would expect norms to be highly salient in friendship networks. Nevertheless, it will be relevant to recruit a broader sampling of friendship networks in future studies. Studies based on wider network coverage are also needed to address how variations in reference group and community-level factors might impact group norms over longer time periods.

Some friendship groups in our study were relatively small. Although we took steps to statistically correct for variations in group size when computing mean norm values, means based on two values may not be sufficient indicators of the total group. However, the correlation between norm scores for dyads was on average greater than 0.80 indicating a strong correspondence between dyad members. Strengths of the study include the use of a household probability sample in a low-income community, obtaining network-level data on close friends, and maintaining high retention across time. These strengths add to the generalizability and relevance of the current investigation.

In brief, the present results suggest that group norms governing condom use, sexual recreation, and drug use may need to be considered in conducting HIV/STI prevention that seeks to change community-level norms. Furthermore, sexual health promotion programs may need to vary depending on the normative values of friendship groups. The durability of the norms over time suggests that they may be relatively resistant to change, at least in the short term. However, once norms have been modified, existing social structures may be sufficient to maintain the changes over time. Extending this work to other communities and ethnic groups of adolescents is warranted.

Acknowledgments

This work was primarily supported by MH57234 (PI: Dolcini) and funding from the Centers for Disease Control grant R30/CCR-915467-03, the NIH/NIMH grants MH54320-06A1 and P50MH42459-14, and the State Office of AIDS grant 01-16085. Manuscript preparation was supported in part by NICHD R01HD061027 (PI: Dolcini). Thanks to Lance Pollack, Tor Neilands, Richard Brand, Jason Chang, and Lei Han of the University of California San Francisco for contributions to statistical and data analyses, and to Gail Bolan and the NACS Advisory Panel for contributions to the main project. Portions of this work were presented previously at the Society for Research on Adolescence Biennial Meeting, San Francisco, 2006. We thank Marcia Macomber for assistance in manuscript preparation.

References

  1. Anderberg MR. Cluster analysis for applications. Academic Press; New York: 1973. [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson E. Code of the street: Decency, violence, and the moral life of the inner city. W.W. Norton & Company, Inc.; New York: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  3. Arnold P, Fletcher S, Farrow R. Condom use and psychological sensation seeking by college students. Sexual and Relationship Therapy. 2002;17(4):355–365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1468199021000017209. [Google Scholar]
  4. Arrow H, Burns KL. The psychological foundations of culture. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, NJ: 2004. Self-organizing culture: how norms emerge in small groups; pp. 171–199. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bachanas PJ, Morris MK, Lewis-Gess JK, Sarett-Cuasay EJ, Sirl K, Ries JK, et al. Predictors of risky sexual behavior in African American adolescent girls: implications for prevention interventions. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2002;27(6):519–530. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/27.6.519. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/27.6.519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benda BB, Diblasio FA. An integration of theory: adolescent sexual contacts. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1994;23(3):403–420. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01536727. [Google Scholar]
  7. Borsari B, Carey KB. Peer influences on college drinking: a review of the research. Journal of Substance Abuse. 2001;13(4):391–424. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(01)00098-0. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0899-3289(01)00098-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brechwald WA, Prinstein MJ. Beyond homophily: a decade of advances in understanding peer influence processes. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2011;21(1):166–179. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00721.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00721.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brown BB. Peer groups and peer cultures. In: Feldman SS, Elliott GR, editors. At the threshold: The developing adolescent. Harvard University Press; Cambridge: 1990. pp. 171–196. [Google Scholar]
  10. Brown BB, Dolcini MM, Leventhal A. Transformations in peer relationships at adolescence: implications for health-related behavior. In: Schulenberg J, Maggs JL, Hurrelmann K, editors. Health risks and developmental transitions during adolescence. Cambridge University Press; London: 1997. pp. 161–189. [Google Scholar]
  11. Brown BB, Von Bank H, Steinberg L. Smoke in the looking glass: effects of discordance between self- and peer rated crowd affiliation on adolescent anxiety, depression and self-feelings. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2008;37(10):1163–1177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-007-9198-y. [Google Scholar]
  12. Burns RB, Burns RA. Cluster analysis business research methods and statistics using SPSS. Sage; Los Angeles: 2008. pp. 552–566. Retrieved from. http://www.uk.sagepub.com/burns/chapters.htm. [Google Scholar]
  13. Campo S, Brossard D, Frazer MS, Marchell T, Lewis D, Talbot J. Are social norms campaigns really magic bullets? Assessing the effects of students’ misperceptions on drinking behavior. Health Communication. 2003;15(4):481–497. doi: 10.1207/S15327027HC1504_06. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327027hc1504_06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Catania JA, Canchola J, Binson D, Dolcini MM, Paul JP, Fisher L, et al. National trends in condom use among at-risk heterosexuals in the United States. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes. 2001;27(2) doi: 10.1097/00126334-200106010-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Catania JA, Coates TJ, Kegeles S. A Test of the AIDS risk reduction model: psychosocial correlates of condom use in the AMEN cohort survey. Health Psychology. 1994;13:548–555. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.13.6.548. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.13.6.548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Catania J, Coates T, Stall R, Turner H, Peterson J, Hearst N, et al. Prevalence of AIDS-related risk factors and condom use in the United States. Science. 1992;258:1101–1106. doi: 10.1126/science.1439818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Catania JA, Gibson D, Marin B, Coates TJ, Greenblatt R. Response bias in assessing sexual behaviors relevant to HIV transmission. Evaluation and Program Planning. 1990;13(1):19–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0149-7189(90)90005-H. [Google Scholar]
  18. Catania J, Osmond D, Neilands T, Canchola J, Gregorich S, Shiboski S, et al. Commentary on Schroder. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2005;29(2):86–95. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm2902_2. (2003a, 2003b) http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15324796abm2902_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention . Youth risk behavior surveillance, surveillance summaries – United States. SS-5. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Morbidity and mortality weekly report; Atlanta, Georgia: 2010. 2009. p. 59. Retrieved from. http://www.cdc.gov/Healthy/Youth/yrbs/index.htm. [Google Scholar]
  20. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention . HIV surveillance report, 2010: Diagnoses for HIV infection and AIDS in the United States and Dependant areas. Vol. 22. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; Atlanta: 2012. Retrieved from. http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/surveillance/resources/reports/2010report/index.htm. [Google Scholar]
  21. Crosnoe R, McNeely C. Peer relations, adolescent behavior, and public health research and practice. Family and Community Health. 2008;31(Suppl. 1):S71–S80. doi: 10.1097/01.FCH.0000304020.05632.e8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.FCH.0000304020.05632.e8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dolcini MM, Adler NE. Perceived competencies, peer group affiliation, and risk behavior among early adolescents. Health Psychology. 1994;13(6):496–506. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.13.6.496. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.13.6.496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dolcini MM, Harper GW, Watson SE, Catania JA, Ellen JM. Friends in the ‘hood’: should peer-based promotion target out-of-school networks? Journal of Adolescent Health. 2005:26. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.10.003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.10.003, 267.e267-267.e216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dolcini MM, Harper GW, Boyer CB, Pollack LM. Project ÒRÉ: a friendship-based intervention to prevent HIV/STI in urban African American adolescent females. Health Education & Behavior. 2010;37(1):115–132. doi: 10.1177/1090198109333280. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1090198109333280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Donnelly MP, Vaske JJ, Whittaker D, Shelby B. Toward an understanding of norm prevalence: a comparative analysis of 20 years of research. Environmental Management. 2000;25(4):403–414. doi: 10.1007/s002679910032. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s002679910032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Duncan SC, Strycker LA, Duncan TE, He H, Stark MJ. Telephone recruitment of a random stratified African American and white family study sample. Journal of Ethnicity in Substance Abuse. 2002;1(3):57–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J233v01n03_05. [Google Scholar]
  27. Eder D. The cycle of popularity: interpersonal relations among female adolescents. Sociology of Education. 1985;58(3):154–165. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2112416. [Google Scholar]
  28. Ellen JM, Brown BA, Chung SE, Potterat JJ, Muth SQ, Valente TW, et al. Impact of sexual networks on risk for gonorrhea and chlamydia among low-income urban African American adolescents. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2005;146(4):518–522. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2004.11.023. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2004.11.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Ennett ST, Bauman KE. Adolescent social networks: school, demographic, and longitudinal considerations. Journal of Adolescent Research. 1996;11(2):194–215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0743554896112003. [Google Scholar]
  30. Etzioni A. Social norms: internalization, persuasion, and history. Law & Society Review. 2000;34(1):157. [Google Scholar]
  31. Fang X, Stanton B, Li X, Feigelman S, Baldwin R. Similarities in sexual activity and condom use among friends within groups before and after a risk-reduction intervention. Youth & Society. 1998;29(4):431–450. doi: 10.1177/0044118x98029004002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0044118X98029004002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Feldman DA, O’Hara P, Baboo KS, Chitalu NW, Lu Y. HIV prevention among Zambian adolescents: developing a value utilization/norm change model. Social Science and Medicine. 1997;44(4):455–468. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(96)00164-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0277-9536(96)00164-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fichtenberg C, Jennings J, Glass T, Ellen J. Neighborhood socioeconomic environment and sexual network position. Journal of Urban Health. 2010;87(2):225–235. doi: 10.1007/s11524-009-9425-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11524-009-9425-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Fishbein M, Hennessy M, Kamb M, Bolan GA, Hoxworth T, Iatesta M, et al. Using intervention theory to model factors influencing behavior change: project RESPECT. Evaluation & the Health Professions. 2001;24(4):363–384. doi: 10.1177/01632780122034966. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/01632780122034966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Fisher WA, Byrne D, White LA, Kelley K. Erotophobia-erotophilia as a dimension of personality. Journal of Sex Research. 1988;25(1):123–151. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224498809551448. [Google Scholar]
  36. Fisher JD, Fisher WA, Bryan AD, Misovich SJ. Information-motivation-behavioral skills model-based HIV risk behavior change intervention for inner-city high school youth. Health Psychology. 2002;21(2):177–186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.21.2.177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fisher JD, Kimble D, Misovich SJ, Weinstein E. Dynamics of HIV risk behavior in HIV-infected men who have sex with men. AIDS and Behavior. 1998;2:101–113. [Google Scholar]
  38. Giordano PC, Longmore MA, Manning WD, Northcutt MJ. Adolescent identities and sexual behavior: an examination of Anderson’s ‘player’ hypothesis. Social Forces. 2009;87(4):1813–1843. doi: 10.1353/sof.0.0186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/sof.0.0186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Harper GW, Gannon C, Watson SE, Catania JA, Dolcini MM. The role of close friends in African-American adolescents’ dating and sexual behavior. Journal of Sex Research. 2004;41(4):351–362. doi: 10.1080/00224490409552242. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224490409552242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hechter M, Opp K-D. Introduction. Social norms. Russell Sage Foundation; New York: 2001a. pp. xi–xx. [Google Scholar]
  41. Hechter M, Opp K-D. Social norms. Russell Sage Foundation; New York: 2001b. [Google Scholar]
  42. He K, Kramer E, Houser RF, Chomitz VR, Hacker KA. Defining and understanding healthy lifestyles choices for adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2004;35(1):26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2003.09.004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2003.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Horne C. Sociological perspectives on the emergence of norms. Social norms. Russell Sage Foundation; New York: 2001. pp. 3–34. [Google Scholar]
  44. Jemmott JB, 3rd, Jemmott LS, Braverman PK, Fong GT. HIV/STD risk reduction interventions for African American and Latino adolescent girls at an adolescent medicine clinic: a randomized controlled trial. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2005;159(5):440–449. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.5.440. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.159.5.440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Johnson BT, Carey MP, Marsh KL, Levin KD, Scott-Sheldon LA. Interventions to reduce sexual risk for the human immunodeficiency virus in adolescents, 1985–2000: a research synthesis. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 2003;157(4):381–388. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.157.4.381. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.157.4.381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Johnston L, O’Malley P. Issues of validity and population coverage in student surveys of drug use. In: Rouse B, Kozel N, Richards L, editors. Self-report methods of estimating drug use: Meeting current challenges to validity. Vol. 57. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research; Washington D.C: 1985. pp. 31–54. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kandel DB. Homophily, selection, and socialization in adolescent friendships. American Journal of Sociology. 1978;84(2):427–436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/226792. [Google Scholar]
  48. Kirby D. Understanding what works and what doesn’t in reducing adolescent sexual risk-taking. Family Planning Perspectives. 2001;33(6):276–281. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. La Greca AM, Prinstein MJ, Fetter MD. Adolescent peer crowd affiliation: linkages with health-risk behaviors and close friendships. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2001;26(3):131–143. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/26.3.131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/26.3.131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lapinski MK, Rimal RN. An explication of social norms. Communication Theory. 2005;15(2):127–147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2005.tb00329.x. [Google Scholar]
  51. Latkin C, Donnell D, Celentano DD, Aramrattna A, Liu T-Y, Vongchak T, et al. Relationships between social norms, social network characteristics, and HIV risk behaviors in Thailand and the United States. Health Psychology. 2009;28(3):323–329. doi: 10.1037/a0014707. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Laumann E, Gagnon J, Michael R, Michaels S. The social organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  53. Matsueda RL, Anderson K. The dynamics of delinquent peers and delinquent behavior. Criminology. 1998;36(2):269–308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.1998.tb01249.x. [Google Scholar]
  54. McCree DH, Wingood GM, DiClemente R, Davies S, Harrington KF. Religiosity and risky sexual behavior in African-American adolescent females. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2003;33(1):2–8. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00460-3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00460-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. McGraw KO, Wong SP. Forming inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychological Methods. 1996;1(1):30–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989x.1.1.30. [Google Scholar]
  56. Norris AE, Ford K. Moderating influence of peer norms on gender differences in condom use. Applied Developmental Science. 1998;2(4):174–181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s1532480xads0204_1. [Google Scholar]
  57. Rahav M. Norm set and deviant behavior: the case of age-sex norms. Deviant Behavior. 1984;5(1-4):151–179. [Google Scholar]
  58. Reed SJ, Bangi A, Sheon N, Harper GW, Catania JA, Richards KAM, et al. Influences on sexual partnering among African American adolescents with concurrent sexual relationships. Research in Human Development. 2012;9(1):78–101. doi: 10.1080/15427609.2012.654435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15427609.2012.654435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Richard AJ, Bell DC, Montoya ID. Normative influence on condom use in the personal networks of female cocaine smokers. AIDS Education and Prevention. 2000;12(4):357–374. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Rimal RN. Modeling the relationship between descriptive norms and behaviors: a test and extension of the theory of normative social behavior (TNSB) Health Communication. 2008;23(2):103–116. doi: 10.1080/10410230801967791. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10410230801967791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Risjord M. Norms and explanation in the social sciences. Studies in History & Philosophy of Science Part A. 1998;29(2):223–237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0039-3681(98)00005-3. [Google Scholar]
  62. Roberti JW. A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking. Journal of Research in Personality. 2004;38(3):256–279. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0092-6566(03)00067-9. [Google Scholar]
  63. Rogers EM. Diffusion of innovations. 4th ed. The Free Press; New York: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  64. Romer D, Black M, Ricardo I, Feigleman S, Kaljee L, Galbraith J, et al. Social influences on the sexual behavior of youth at risk for HIV exposure. American Journal of Public Health. 1994;84:977–985. doi: 10.2105/ajph.84.6.977. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.84.6.977. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Ross L, Greene D, House P. The false consensus effect: an egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1977;13(3):279–301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(77)90049-x. [Google Scholar]
  66. Rothenberg RB, Holmes KK. Workshop summary: connecting the dots; Paper presented at the Center on AIDS and Other Medical Consequences of Drug Abuse (CAMCODA), National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA), the network paradigm in research on drug abuse, HIV, and other blood-borne and sexually transmitted infections: new perspectives, approaches, and applications; Rockville, MD. 2001.May, [Google Scholar]
  67. Savin-Williams RC, Berndt TJ. Friendship and peer relations. At the threshold: The developing adolescent. Harvard University Press; Cambridge: 1990. pp. 277–307. [Google Scholar]
  68. Stanton B, Li X, Pack R, Cottrell L, Harris C, Burns JM. Longitudinal influence of perceptions of peer and parental factors on African American adolescent risk involvement. Journal of Urban Health. 2002;79(4):536–548. doi: 10.1093/jurban/79.4.536. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jurban/79.4.536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Stanton BF, Li X, Ricardo I, Galbraith J, Feigelman S, Kaljee L. A randomized, controlled effectiveness trial of an AIDS prevention program for low-income African-American youths. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 1996;150(4):363–372. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1996.02170290029004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. StataCorp . Stata statistical software: Release 7.0. Stata Corporation; College Station, TX: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  71. STD Control Section . San Francisco sexually transmitted disease annual summary. San Francisco; California: 2009. [Google Scholar]
  72. Thorburn S, Harvey SM, Ryan EA. HIV prevention heuristics and condom use among African-Americans at risk for HIV. AIDS Care. 2005;17(3):335–344. doi: 10.1080/09540120412331299762. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09540120412331299762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Towner SL, Dolcini MM, Harper GW. Romantic relationship dynamics of urban African American adolescents: monogamy, multiple partnerships, commitment, and trust. Youth and Society. doi: 10.1177/0044118X12462591. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Urberg KA, Değirmencioğlu SM, Tolson JM, Halliday-Scher K. The structure of adolescent peer networks. Developmental Psychology. 1995;31(4):540–547. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.31.4.540. [Google Scholar]
  75. Valente TW. Social networks and health: Models, methods, and applications. Oxford University Press; New York: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  76. Valleroy LA, MacKellar DA, Karon JM, Janssen RS, Hayman CR. HIV infection in disadvantaged out-of-school youth: prevalence for U.S. Job Corps entrants, 1990 through 1996. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes Human Retrovirology. 1998;19(1):67–73. doi: 10.1097/00042560-199809010-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Verkooijen KT, de Vries NK, Nielsen GA. Youth crowds and substance use: the impact of perceived group norm and multiple group identification. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2007;21(1):55–61. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.1.55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-164x.21.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Wallace JM, Jr., Williams DR. Health risks and developmental transitions during adolescence. Cambridge University Press; New York, NY, US: 1997. Religion and adolescent health-compromising behavior; pp. 444–468. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES