Abstract
Background
Although many studies indicate that maternal smoking during pregnancy (SDP) is correlated with later offspring antisocial behavior (ASB), recent quasi-experimental studies suggest that background familial factors confound the association. The present study sought to test alternative etiological hypotheses using multiple indices of adolescent ASB, comparing differentially exposed siblings, and testing assumptions in the sibling-comparison design.
Methods
The study examined the association between maternal SDP and adolescent-reported ASB, criminal convictions, and membership in a group of individuals with early-starting and chronic ASB among 6,066 offspring of women from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth, a representative sample of women in the United States. The analyses controlled for statistical covariates and examined associations while comparing differentially exposed siblings.
Results
At the population-level, each additional pack of cigarettes/day predicted greater mean adolescent-reported ASB symptoms (ratio of means=1.15, 95% CIs=1.08–1.22), odds of being in the top 10% of ASB (OR=1.34, 95% CIs=1.10–1.65), hazard of a criminal conviction (HR=1.51, 95% CIs=1.34–1.68), and odds of chronic ASB (OR=1.57, 95% CIs=1.25–1.99). SDP robustly predicted most assessments of ASB while controlling for measured covariates. When siblings exposed to differing levels of SDP were compared, however, all of the associations were attenuated and were not statistically significant: adolescent-reported mean ASB (ratio of means=0.86, 95% CIs=0.74–1.01), High ASB (OR=0.67, 95% CIs=0.41–1.12), criminal conviction (HR=0.98, 95% CIs=0.66–1.44), and Chronic ASB (OR=0.80, 95% CIs=0.46–1.38).
Conclusions
The results strongly suggest that familial factors account for the correlation between SDP and offspring adolescent ASB, rather than a putative causal environmental influence of SDP.
Most research on the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy (SDP) and offspring antisocial behavior (ASB) has been consistent with a causal inference (Ernst, Moolchan, & Robinson, 2001; Olds, 1997; Wakschlag, Pickett, Cook, Benowitz, & Leventhal, 2002). The causal inference has been supported recently by human research, including studies conducted across several countries (Brion, et al., 2010), controlling for numerous confounds (Paradis, Fitzmaurice, Koenen, & Buka, 2010), and that have explored differences in brain structure and functional processes in exposed and unexposed offspring (review in Derauf, Kekatpure, Neyzi, Lester, & Kosofsky, 2009). The results also are consistent with recent basic animal research on brain changes associated with prenatal nicotine exposure, including alterations in nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (e.g., Gold, Keller, & Perry, 2009).
Recent quasi-experimental studies in humans, however, have suggested that family background characteristics account for the statistical association between SDP and offspring ASB (review in Knopik, 2009). These studies, which use design features to rule out alternative hypotheses (Rutter, 2007; Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002), suggest that unmeasured risk factors that covary with maternal SDP likely increase risk for ASB in the offspring instead of a direct prenatal environmental influence of SDP on risk for ASB.
Several studies on ASB have compared differentially exposed siblings by looking at risk for ASB among offspring where the mother changed her smoking across pregnancies. This design controls genetic and shared environmental factors that are confounded with SDP as alternative explanations for the apparent effect of SDP (Donovan & Susser, 2011; Lahey & D’Onofrio, 2010; Rutter, 2007). If the sibling who was exposed to greater SDP had higher levels of ASB (a within-family association), a causal association would be supported. In contrast, if both siblings had the same level of ASB, the results would suggest SDP does not cause ASB. Gilman, Gardner, & Buka (2008) found that differentially exposed siblings did not vary in their rates of childhood conduct problems. And, D'Onofrio et al. (2008) found that maternal SDP was not associated with increased risk for childhood conduct problems or oppositional problems when comparing differentially exposed siblings. A recent study found comparable results when exploring the association between SDP and offspring criminal convictions during adolescence and early adulthood (D'Onofrio, Singh, Iliadou, Lambe, Hultman, Grann, et al., 2010). These findings were confirmed in a novel in vitro fertilization study of maternal SDP and childhood conduct problems (Rice, et al., 2009), which controls genetic confounds by examining risk factors in women who were not genetically related to their children (Thapar, et al., 2007).
Certainly converging evidence across numerous studies and across various designs is necessary to support a causal inference (e.g., Rutter, Pickles, Murray, & Eaves, 2001). It is important to note that the existing quasi-experimental studies on ASB are limited by several problems. First, many of the studies are limited by measurement problems. Many studies relied on the same reporter (the mother) for the assessment of both SDP and ASB (Rutter, 2007). And, one study (D'Onofrio, Singh, Iliadou, Lambe, Hultman, Grann, et al., 2010) has been criticized for using extreme measures of ASB (criminal convictions), that may make the interpretation of findings difficult, if not impossible (Talati & Weissman, 2010). Second, a great majority of the studies are only based on childhood conduct problems, not ASB during adolescence when the societal costs and suffering associated with ASB are greater (e.g., R. Loeber, D. P. Farrington, M. Stouthamer-Loeber, & W. Van Kammen, 1998).
Third, the lack of measures of adolescent ASB also is problematic because SDP may differentially influence specific developmental trajectories of ASB. One study suggests that SDP may be more strongly associated with early-starting and chronic forms of ASB (Brennan, Grekin, & Mednick, 1999), which is consistent with the current theories suggesting that the trajectory of ASB is associated with early neurocognitive deficits (e.g., Moffitt, 2006). Fourth, few of the quasi-experimental studies have been able to test the assumptions inherent in their designs. For example, the sibling-comparison design has a number of limitations, including the inability account for whether exposure to the risk in one pregnancy carries over to other pregnancies, historical changes, and factors that influence why women change their smoking behavior over time (Donovan & Susser, 2011; Lahey & D’Onofrio, 2010).
The current study sought to address these issues by: (a) examining the associations between SDP and multiple measures of ASB in a large study of offspring of a representative sample of women in the United States, (b) investigating these associations while comparing differentially exposed siblings with the same biological mothers, and (c) testing assumptions inherent in the sibling-comparison design.
Method
Sample
Mothers
The mother-generation for the current study was drawn from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979 (NLSY79). The NLSY79 was a nationally-representative household sample of 14–22-year-old male and female youth selected using a stratified and clustered design, with an oversample of African American and Hispanic youth (Baker & Mott, 1989). The NLSY79 sample consisted of the 4,926 females (1,472 African American, 977 Hispanic, and 2,477 non-Hispanic white and other groups) who have given birth to children.
Offspring
Biennial assessments of all biological offspring of all NLSY79 women began in 1986 (Chase-Lansdale, Mott, Brooks-Gunn, & Phillips, 1991). Participation in the 12 assessments through 2008 averaged >90%. Mothers answered questions about each of their children’s behavior and environments in every assessment. Youth aged 10 years and over also reported on their behavior and environments in each assessment. A total of 11,506 offspring have been born to all of the women in the NLSY79 cohort through 2008. Observations were dropped from the current analyses if the observations were missing information about (a) the mother's identification number (n=11); maternal smoking during pregnancy (n=1,255), which was mostly due to an invalid skip pattern during a wave of assessment (D’Onofrio, et al., 2008); and adolescent ASB (n=5,010), which consisted mostly of offspring who were too young to be assessed for adolescent ASB. The analyses, therefore, were based on 6,066 offspring born to 2,694 mothers.
Measures
Maternal smoking during pregnancy
In every wave since 1986, CNLSY mothers were asked about their frequency of smoking during pregnancy for each pregnancy using a four-point scale. Information was obtained from the mother on birth outcomes and smoking and drinking during pregnancy in the first assessment following the child’s birth. The frequency of maternal SDP is presented in Table 1. Using plasma biomarkers as the criterion variables, the validity of self-reported tobacco use during pregnancy has been demonstrated to be high in other studies (George, Granath, Johansson, & Cnattingius, 2006). Nonetheless, there is some systematic inaccuracy of false negatives (George, et al., 2006) and underestimating the amount of smoking (Post, Gilljam, Bremberg, & Galanti, 2008). The resulting imperfect sensitivity of self-reported smoking measures results in conservative tests of maternal smoking effects. The strongest evidence for the validity of maternal reports, however, is that they are consistently correlated with objective measures of the offspring’s physical growth retardation (Day, et al., 1991). Previous CNLSY studies have found SDP to be highly correlated with offspring birth weight (D'Onofrio et al., 2008).
Table 1.
Distribution of Maternal Smoking during Pregnancy and offspring Adolescent Antisocial Behavior
Variable | N | Percentage |
---|---|---|
Smoking during Pregnancy | ||
No packs/day | 4431 | 73.1 |
Half packs/day | 1205 | 19.9 |
One and a half packs/day | 394 | 6.5 |
Two and a half packs/day | 36 | 0.6 |
Adolescent ASB (rounded average number activities in past 12 months over ages 14—17 years) | ||
Zero | 1562 | 25.8 |
One | 2027 | 33.4 |
Two | 1173 | 19.3 |
Three | 675 | 11.1 |
Foura | 365 | 6.0 |
Fivea | 168 | 2.8 |
Sixa | 70 | 1.2 |
Sevena | 26 | 0.4 |
Note.
Included in identifying a group of teenagers with High ASB.
Offspring ASB
Adolescent-Reported ASB and High ASB
Across ages 14–17 years, youth completed a self-report questionnaire of ASB in private. They reported on 7 delinquent behaviors during the past 12 months from the Self-Reported Delinquency scale (SRD; Elliott & Huizinga, 1983): hurt someone badly enough to need bandages or a doctor; lied to parent about something important; took something from a store without paying for it; intentionally damaged or destroyed property that didn’t belong to you; had to bring your parent(s) to school because of something you did wrong; skipped a day of school without permission; and ran away from home overnight. The SRD is the benchmark measure in contemporary delinquency research (R. Loeber, D. P. Farrington, M. Stouthamer-Loeber, & W. B. Van Kammen, 1998; Moffitt, Caspi, Rutter, & Silva, 2001). The 7 SRD delinquency items were selected for the CNLSY because they tap high-prevalence acts that are highly correlated with more serious delinquent behaviors. In three waves of the CNLSY, the young adult SRD delinquency scale also included 10 additional items. The 7-item scale delinquency correlated highly with the sum of the remaining more serious delinquency items, had good internal consistency, and showed excellent criterion validity in both sexes (Lahey, Van Hulle, D'Onofrio, Rodgers, & Waldman, 2008).
To create a measure of ASB we calculated a rounded average of the reported items across assessments from 14 to 17 years of age. The distribution of the self-reported ASB from 14–17 years of age is presented in Table 1. The distribution indicates that a majority of offspring report at least one item during the past year. In addition to of number of items reported in the previous year we also created an binary indicator of High ASB, which was based the highest 10% of the distribution of the SRD..
Criminal Convictions
Offspring also reported on their convictions for numerous offenses, including assault; robbery (using weapon or force); theft; fencing stolen goods; vandalism; other property offenses; marijuana possession; sale or distribution of marijuana; illicit drug possession; manufacturing, sale or distribution of illicit drugs; underage drinking; major traffic offense; or other crime. The offspring also reported the first age at conviction, which ranged from 10 to 30 years old. We predicted risk for ever being convicted, based on the first age of conviction. Kaplan-Meier estimates of the prevalence of criminal convictions revealed that 25.2% of offspring were convicted by the age of 30, with a higher prevalence in males (35.4%) than females (15.3%), which are consistent with national estimates (FBI, 2011).
Chronic ASB
To create an index of early-starting and Chronic ASB, we used a combination of mother-rated childhood conduct problems from 4 to 9 years old and adolescent self-reported ASB. Mothers rated childhood conduct problems using the Behavior Problems Index (BPI), a measure that selected items from the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1978). The mean of the 7 BPI items indexed childhood conduct problems, and the items overlap substantially with those used to define child conduct problems in longitudinal studies (Fergusson & Horwood, 2002; Moffitt, et al., 2001). Previous studies have documented the factor structure and stability of the measure in the CNLSY, as well as the relation with maternal SDP (D’Onofrio, et al., 2008). To create a group of offspring with early-starting and chronic ASB we selected individuals who were in the top 20% on both measures. The measure was restricted to individuals with assessments at both time points (n=3,793), with 243 individuals (6.4%) meeting the criteria for Chronic ASB, which is generally consistent with recent epidemiological estimates (e.g., Barker & Maughan, 2009; Odgers, et al., 2008).
The measures of offspring ASB were highly associated with each other. Each additional item on the adolescent-reported ASB was associated with greater risk for criminal convictions (HR=1.56, p<0.001) and for membership in the Chronic ASB group (OR=2.94, p<0.001). High ASB (the top 10% of the self-reported ASB) was associated with more reported criminal convictions (HR=4.18, p<0.001) and with membership in the Chronic ASB group (OR=19.83, p<0.001).
Offspring-Specific Covariates
Table 2 describes the distribution of the covariates used in the study. To account for missing values in all of the covariates, we created dummy codes to compare any individuals with missing values to those observations with low risk. The study included a number of covariates that vary within families, including gender, offspring birth order, and maternal age at childbearing. Maternal age at childbearing compared teenage childbearing to non-teenage childbearing. High maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy was also assessed, which was measured by reported drinking more than 3–4 days/month compared to those with less frequent consumption (D’Onofrio, et al., 2007).
Table 2.
Distribution of Offspring-Specific and Familial Covariates
Variable | N | Percentage |
---|---|---|
Offspring-Specific Covariatesa | ||
Female | 2988 | 49.3 |
Birth Order | ||
One | 2691 | 44.4 |
Two | 1978 | 32.6 |
Three | 931 | 15.4 |
Four plus | 466 | 7.7 |
Maternal Teenage Childbearing | ||
Noc | 3514 | 57.9 |
Yes | 2461 | 40.6 |
Missing | 91 | 1.5 |
Maternal Alcohol Consumption during Pregnancy | ||
Lowc | 5540 | 91.3 |
High | 516 | 8.51 |
Missing | 10 | 0.2 |
Maternal/Familial Covariatesb | ||
Mother Adolescent ASB | ||
Lowc | 2327 | 86.4 |
High | 248 | 9.6 |
Missing | 119 | 4.4 |
Maternal Intellectual Abilities | ||
Highc | 2077 | 77.1 |
Low | 522 | 19.4 |
Missing | 95 | 3.5 |
Maternal Educational Attainment | ||
Highc | 2614 | 97.0 |
Low | 80 | 3.0 |
Maternal Income | ||
Highc | 2247 | 90.1 |
Low | 247 | 9.9 |
History of Binge Drinking | ||
Noc | 2253 | 83.6 |
Yes | 441 | 16.4 |
History of Alcohol Abuse/Dependence | ||
Noc | 2213 | 82.2 |
Yes | 436 | 16.2 |
Missing | 45 | 1.7 |
Maternal Adolescent Substance Use | ||
Noc | 2076 | 77.1 |
Yes | 578 | 21.5 |
Missing | 40 | 1.5 |
Family Race/Ethnicity | ||
Caucasianc | 1335 | 49.6 |
African American | 832 | 30.9 |
Hispanic | 527 | 19.6 |
Note.
Based on 6,066 offspring.
Based on 2,694 unique mothers.
Used as the reference group in the analyses. Missing presents offspring or families with missing values for the variable.
Maternal/Familial Covariates
The analyses controlled for maternal reports of her history of adolescent delinquency (Rodgers, Rowe, & Li, 1994), which was indexed by comparing women in the top 10% to those in rest of the distribution. Low maternal intellectual abilities was based on the bottom 20% of the distribution of a composite score from the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery, which was given in 1980. Low maternal educational attainment was assessed by fewer than 12 years of completed education. Low income was measured as familial income at the age of 30 below $5,795 in 1986 dollars (the bottom 10% of the distribution). The 1994 assessment included a detailed assessment of lifetime history of alcohol problems. The mothers were asked the number of binge episodes and a 25 item assessment of alcohol abuse and dependence items. If women ever reported binge drinking or any abuse of dependence items, they were considered to be at high risk for each measure. Maternal adolescent substance use was indexed by self report of any of the following substances during adolescence: cocaine, amphetamines, barbiturates, tranquilizers, psychedelic drugs, or heroin. Finally, we included family race/ethnicity, as measured by maternal self-report in the NLSY79 study, which included Caucasian, African American, and Hispanic groups.
Sampling weights
Based on clustered, unequal selection probability design, the NLSY79 provides weights indicating the inverse of the probability of each participant being selected into the sample. In family-based analyses, these weights apply equally to all CNLSY offspring to a given mother.
Analyses
Regression-Based Analyses
We used three models to examine the relation between maternal SDP and each measure of ASB. We fit the models in Mplus 6.11 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2010). Each model used robust standard errors at the original NLSY79 household level to account for the non-independence of the observations in extended-families, and each model used the sampling weights.
Model 1 predicted each measure of ASB by maternal SDP, offspring gender, and birth order. The parameter estimate associated with maternal SDP represents what we will refer to as the unadjusted association. Model 2 added all of the measured offspring-specific and maternal/familial covariates. The parameter associated with SDP quantifies the association of ASB to SDP while statistically controlling for the measured covariates. Model 3 predicted offspring ASB in the context of a fixed effects model at the mother level, which holds constant all factors that siblings share (Allison, 2009). This model therefore compared differentially exposed siblings and thereby provided an estimate of the association between SDP and ASB while controlling for all familial factors—both genetic and environmental—that make siblings similar. The models were based on 1,088 offspring of 353 mothers who varied their smoking across pregnancies.
Because the distributions of the measures for ASB were different, the analyses used several analytical models. For offspring-reported adolescent ASB behavior counts we used negative binomial models to predict the mean number of ASB behaviors per person, a model that allows for over-dispersion of the response distribution relative to the Poisson (Long, 1997). We used logistic regression models to predict the dichotomous variables of High ASB and Chronic ASB. The negative binomial and logistic regression models were each based on a two level analysis with a random intercept at the second level to account for the clustering of siblings within a family. Finally because the reports of convictions were right-censored (the offspring had not lived through the entire risk period), we used Cox proportional hazards survival analysis models to predict first offspring criminal conviction. A sandwich estimator was used to account for the familial clustering in the survival analysis models.
Sensitivity Analyses
We ran additional analyses to test a number of moderators, rule out alternative explanation for the results, and examine some of the assumptions in the sibling-comparison design. First, we examined whether offspring gender and maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy moderated the association between maternal SDP and offspring ASB. Second, we reran the analyses predicting each measure of ASB using a categorical assessment of maternal SDP (smoked or not during pregnancy) to examine whether any possible inaccuracy in reporting the amount of cigarette smoking could account for the results. Third, we ran the models without the sampling weights to determine the extent to which the use of sampling weights influenced the results.
Fourth, we tested a number of the assumptions in the sibling-comparison design, especially regarding the stable unit treatment value assumption—the assumption that exposure to the risk does not influence other unexposed participants (Rubin, 2006), which is particularly relevant for sibling-comparison designs (Lahey & D’Onofrio, 2010). If smoking in one pregnancy influenced all subsequent pregnancies, then a sibling-comparison approach would provide the wrong answer. Women who decrease their smoking over time may be quite different than those that increase their smoking during pregnancy. Furthermore, if the validity of the assessment of SDP has changed over time, the sibling-comparison approach could provide misleading results. To address these concerns, we ran a bi-directional case-crossover study (e.g., Meyer, Williams, Hernandez-Diaz, & Cnattingius, 2004) predicting adolescent-reported ASB in which we compared differentially exposed siblings among the first two children of women who either: (a) increased their smoking over time or (b) decreased their smoking over time. If the sibling-comparison parameters in both groups revealed no association, the results would suggest the findings from the overall sibling-comparison approach are robust to these assumptions.
Results
Regression-Based Analyses
Adolescent-Reported ASB
The parameter estimates and standard errors associated with each independent variable in the negative binomial models predicting adolescent-reported ASB are presented in Table 3. The SDP parameter estimates are reproduced in Table 4 as ratios of mean ASB behavior counts per person, with associated confidence intervals. In Model 1, each additional pack of cigarettes a mother smoked per day during pregnancy was associated with a 15% relative difference in mean ASB behaviors. When controlling for all measured covariates, the results from Model 2 indicated that the association between SDP and ASB was attenuated (a 6% difference in ASB behaviors) but was still statistically significant. After fitting Model 3, the fixed effects (sibling-comparison) model that compared differentially exposed siblings, we found that the association went in the opposite direction (a negative 14% difference in ASB behaviors). Although the parameter was not statistically significant, the confidence interval excludes all ratios of means over 1.01, strongly suggesting that SDP does not lead to an increase in ASB. Taken as a whole, these analyses suggest that siblings have similar levels of ASB, regardless of level of exposure to maternal SDP.
Table 3.
Regression Parameter Estimates (and Standard Errors) of Negative Binomial Models Exploring the Association between Maternal Smoking during Pregnancy and Adolescent-Reported Antisocial Behavior
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3a | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Independent Variable | b | SE | b | SE | b | SE |
Pack/day | 0.14* | 0.03 | 0.06* | 0.03 | −0.15 | 0.08 |
Female | −0.24* | 0.03 | −.25* | 0.03 | −.24* | 0.03 |
Birth Order | 0.01 | 0.02 | −0.01 | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.02 |
Maternal Teenage Childbearing | ||||||
Nob | - | - | ||||
Yes | 0.25* | 0.04 | ||||
Missing | 0.20 | 0.14 | ||||
Maternal Alcohol Consumption during Pregnancy | ||||||
Lowb | - | - | ||||
High | 0.00 | 0.05 | ||||
Missing | 0.32 | 0.26 | ||||
Mother Adolescent ASB | ||||||
Lowb | - | - | ||||
High | 0.08 | 0.05 | ||||
Missing | 0.11 | 0.09 | ||||
Maternal Intellectual Abilities | ||||||
Highb | - | - | ||||
Low | −0.01 | 0.05 | ||||
Missing | −0.05 | 0.09 | ||||
Maternal Educational Attainment | ||||||
Highb | - | - | ||||
Low | 0.08 | 0.08 | ||||
Maternal Income | ||||||
Highb | - | - | ||||
Low | 0.10 | 0.06 | ||||
History of Binge Drinking | ||||||
Nob | - | - | ||||
Yes | 0.08 | 0.04 | ||||
History of Alcohol Abuse/Dependence | ||||||
Nob | - | - | ||||
Yes | 0.08 | 0.05 | ||||
Missing | 0.09 | 0.12 | ||||
Maternal Adolescent Substance Use | ||||||
Nob | - | - | ||||
Yes | 0.25* | 0.04 | ||||
Missing | −0.10 | 0.16 | ||||
Family Race/Ethnicity | ||||||
Caucasianb | - | - | ||||
African American | 0.21* | 0.06 | ||||
Hispanic | 0.26* | 0.04 |
Note. All parameters are presented as log ratios of means.
p<.05.
Based on sibling-comparison model.
Used as the reference group in the analyses. Parameters associated with the missing variables compared offspring with missing values on the covariates to those in the reference group.
Table 4.
Relative mean ASB behaviors, odds of high and chronic ASB, and hazard of criminal conviction (and Confidence Intervals) for Offspring Antisocial Behavior associated with Maternal Smoking during Pregnancy for Three Analytical Models
Measure of ASB | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 |
---|---|---|---|
Adolescent Reported ASBa | 1.15* (1.08 – 1.22) | 1.06* (1.00 – 1.13) | 0.86 (0.74 – 1.01) |
High ASBb | 1.34* (1.10 – 1.65) | 1.08 (0.85 – 1.37) | 0.67 (0.41 – 1.12) |
First Criminal Convictionc | 1.51* (1.34 – 1.68) | 1.32* (1.16 – 1.49) | 0.98 (0.66 – 1.44) |
Chronic ASBb | 1.57* (1.25 – 1.99) | 1.31* (1.01 – 1.72) | 0.80 (0.46 – 1.38) |
Note. Parameters represent the relative increase in mean number of behaviors (for ASB), in odds of prevalent ASB (high or chronic), or in hazard of first criminal conviction associated with a mother smoking an additional pack per day.
Based on a multi-level negative binomial model.
Based on multilevel logistic regression model.
Based on a multi-level survival analysis.
High ASB
The parameter estimates associated with SDP for the other measures of ASB are presented in Table 4 (full results are available upon request). In Model 1, maternal SDP predicted increased odds (OR=1.34) of being in the highest 10% of the adolescent-reported ASB. In Model 2, which included statistical covariates, maternal SDP was not statistically associated with increased odds of High ASB (OR=1.08). In Model 3 (the sibling-comparison model), maternal SDP was not associated with High ASB (OR=0.67). Again, the estimate suggested decreased odds of High ASB, but was not statistically significant.
Criminal Convictions
In Model 1 a Cox proportional hazards model indicated that maternal SDP predicted increased risk (hazard) for being convicted of a criminal offence (HR=1.51). The inclusion of measured covariates in Model 2, attenuated the parameter estimate (HR=1.32), but the association was still statistically significant. The results of Model 3, the fixed effects (sibling-comparison) model, however, indicated no increased risk (HR=0.98) as a function of SDP. The results are consistent with the hypothesis that differentially exposed siblings have similar rates of criminal convictions.
Chronic ASB
The results of Model 1 indicated that maternal SDP predicted increased risk (OR=1.57) for having Chronic ASB. Maternal SDP robustly predicted Chronic ASB in Model 2 when statistical covariates were included the model (OR=1.31). Model 3 results indicated that the association between SDP and Chronic ASB went in the opposite direction but the parameter was not statistically significant (OR=0.80). Again, the results are consistent with the notion that maternal SDP is not associated with Chronic ASB when comparing differentially exposed siblings.
Sensitivity Analyses
First, the results indicated that neither offspring gender nor maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy moderated the association between maternal SDP and offspring ASB, as measured by adolescent-reported ASB, while controlling for the measured covariates (full results available upon request).
Second, analyses predicting each measure of ASB using a dichotomous measure of SDP revealed comparable results. In the entire sample maternal SDP predicted adolescent-reported each measure of ASB The associations were somewhat attenuated but remained statistically significant when including the measured covariates. However, when comparing differentially exposed siblings the associations were greatly attenuated and were not statistically significant. Third, rerunning the analyses without the sampling weights provided comparable findings, indicating that the use of the sampling weights did not skew the results.
Fourth, we used a bi-directional, case-crossover approach that compared differentially exposed siblings in (a) women (n=185) who increased their smoking across their first two pregnancies (OR=0.83, CI=0.69–1.02) and (b) women (n=108) who decreased their smoking across their first two pregnancies (OR=0.98, CI=0.70–1.37). Thus, SDP did not predict ASB in either group, and the difference (blogit=.17, SE=.20, p=.40) was not statistically significant.
Discussion
The current study found converging evidence across all measures of ASB, including adolescent-reported counts of antisocial activities, membership in the highest 10% of ASB, criminal convictions, and membership in a group of individuals with chronic ASB across childhood and adolescence. Maternal SDP was robustly correlated with ASB in offspring during adolescence when statistically controlling for measured covariates, but when genetic and environmental factors that are correlated with SDP across families were controlled in sibling comparisons, no evidence was found for a casual influence of SDP on offspring ASB during adolescence.
The results and conclusions are consistent with previous quasi-experimental studies of child conduct problems (D’Onofrio, et al., 2008; Gilman, et al., 2008; Rice, et al., 2009) and provides converging evidence with previous research predicting offspring criminal convictions (D'Onofrio, Singh, Iliadou, Lambe, Hultman, Grann, et al., 2010). The results are also consistent with recent quasi-experimental studies of related constructs, including intellectual abilities and academic achievement (D'Onofrio, Singh, Iliadou, Lambe, Hultman, Neiderhiser, et al., 2010; Lambe, Hultman, Torrang, MacCabe, & Cnattingius, 2006; Lundberg, et al., 2010), different indices of ADHD (Lindblad & Hjern, 2010; Obel, et al., 2011; Thapar, et al., 2009), and psychiatric assessments of adolescent functioning (D'Onofrio, Singh, Iliadou, Lambe, Hultman, Grann, et al., 2010) and stress coping (Kuja-Halkola, D'Onofrio, Iliadou, Langstrom, & Lichtenstein, 2010). The conclusion also is consistent with recent studies that have found the associations between SDP and offspring cognitive and behavioral problems are confounded by measured familial risks (Batty, Der, & Deary, 2006; Boutwell & Beaver, 2010; Lavigne, et al., 2011; Roza, et al., 2009) when using extensive covariates and/or propensity score matching.
One strength of the current study was the opportunity to test some of the assumptions in the sibling-comparison design (Donovan & Susser, 2011; Lahey & D’Onofrio, 2010). The results of the bi-directional case-crossover approach suggest that carry-over effects of SDP from one pregnancy to another, possible changes in the validity of self-report SDP over time, or other historical changes do not unduly influence the interpretation of the sibling comparison results (also see Lundberg, et al., 2010).
Limitations
All quasi-experimental designs have threats to their internal and external validity (Shadish, et al., 2002). For example, the current study was not able to test every assumption in the sibling-comparison design, such as the specific generalizability of the findings from women who varied their smoking behavior across pregnancies to women without variability. We also did not explore the role of measurement error (review in Heath, et al., 1993), although the assessments of SDP and ASB have been shown to have excellent reliability.
Another statistical limitation of sibling-comparison designs is the reduction in power to detect associations because the estimates can only be based on the comparison of offspring of women who varied their smoking (Lahey & D’Onofrio, 2010), as is the case with all fixed-effects models (Allison, 2009). But, the lack of finding statistically significant associations with maternal SDP was not due solely to low statistical power. The confidence intervals around the SDP parameter predicting adolescent-reported ASB in the sibling-comparison models indicated the data is not consistent with the hypothesis that SDP causes ASB. And, previous sibling-comparison analyses in the CNLSY have found that SDP is independently associated with offspring birth weight (D'Onofrio et al., 2008), which indicates that the use of the design with this dataset can detect small effects for some traits. Because the confidence intervals around the estimates from the sibling-comparison models predicting criminal convictions and chronic ASB are larger, however, the findings will need to be replicated in larger samples.
The current study also did not include all offspring of the NLSY79, as many of the offspring are not yet adolescents, but the analyses tried to account for this limitation by utilizing sampling weights. And, as is true for all regression analyses, there is the possibility that confounding variables masked a causal influence. In the sibling-comparison analyses such variables must (a) vary within families, (b) be positively associated with SDP, and (c) be negatively associated with ASB. Finally, the analyses do not identify the true causes of the association between SDP and offspring ASB because sibling comparison studies by themselves cannot identify the source of the familial confounding (Donovan & Susser, 2011; Lahey & D’Onofrio, 2010).
Given the limitations in the current study, research using other approaches, such as an in vitro fertilization design (Rice, et al., 2009), adoption studies (Leve, Neiderhiser, Scaramella, & Reiss, 2010), offspring of siblings and twins (D’Onofrio, et al., 2008), and larger sibling-comparison studies are required to test the causal inference regarding the association between SDP and offspring ASB and identify the confounding factors.
Implications and Directions for Future Research
Overall, the results of the current study failed to find evidence consistent with a causal prenatal environmental effect of SDP on offspring ASB. The current study addressed concerns that previous quasi-experimental studies relied on maternal reports for both SDP and ASB (Rutter, 2007) and predicted extreme measures of ASB that do not generalize (Talati & Weissman, 2010); in the current sibling-comparison analyses SDP did not predict either common forms of adolescent-reported ASB or rare indices of high ASB. In fact, we know of no quasi-experimental study of SDP that has shown an independent association between SDP and any measure of psychosocial, behavioral, or cognitive development (review Knopik, 2009). This suggests that previous human studies overestimated the causal influence of maternal SDP on ASB. It is important to stress that these findings are in contrast to the results of quasi-experimental studies of pregnancy related risks for which there is clear evidence consistent with a causal environmental effect (e.g., Cnattingius, 2004; Johansson, Dickman, Kramer, & Cnattingius, 2009).
The current results and the growing quasi-experimental research have important implications for many fields (D'Onofrio, Rathouz, & Lahey, 2011). More translational research is needed to understand the discrepant findings from quasi-experimental human research and experimental animal studies. Perhaps, biological differences in human and animal pregnancies (Huizink, 2009), such as mechanisms responsible for parturition (Mitchell & Taggert, 2009), account for some of the differences. Gene-environment interaction studies also must consider that SDP may not be an environmental causal influence. And, prevention efforts must seriously consider familial confounding--interventions may need to target the reduction in SDP and the familial risk factors that frequently co-occur with SDP.
Acknowledgement
The work was supported by a grant from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (5 R01 HD061384).
Footnotes
There were no financial conflicts of interest for any of the authors.
References
- Achenbach TM. The Child Behavior Profile: I. Boys aged 6–11. [Journal; Peer Reviewed Journal] Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1978;46:478–488. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.46.3.478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allison PD. Fixed effects regression models. Washington DC: Sage; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Baker P, Mott FL. NLSY child handbook. Columbus, OH: Center for Human Resource Research; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Barker ED, Maughan B. Differentiating early-onset versus childhood-limited conduct problem youth. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166:900–908. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08121770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Batty GD, Der G, Deary IJ. Effect of maternal smoking during pregnancy on offspring's cognitive ability: Empirical evidence for complete confounding in the US National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. Pediatrics. 2006;118(3):943–950. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boutwell BB, Beaver KM. Maternal cigarette smoking during pregnancy and offspring externalizing behavioral problems: A propensity score matching analysis. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2010;7:146–163. doi: 10.3390/ijerph7010146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brennan PA, Grekin ER, Mednick SA. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and adult male criminal outcomes. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56:215–219. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.3.215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brion MJ, Victora C, Matijasevich A, Horta B, Anselmi L, Steer C, et al. Maternal smoking and child psychological problems: Disentangling causal and noncausal effects. Pediatrics. 2010;126(1):e57–e65. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-2754. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chase-Lansdale PL, Mott FL, Brooks-Gunn J, Phillips DA. Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth: A unique research opportunity. Developmental Psychology. 1991;27:918–931. [Google Scholar]
- Cnattingius S. The epidemiology of smoking during pregnancy: Smoking prevalence, maternal characteristics, and pregnancy outcomes. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2004;6:S125–S140. doi: 10.1080/14622200410001669187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D'Onofrio BM, Rathouz PJ, Lahey BB. The importance of understanding gene-environment correlations in the development of antisocial behavior. In: Kendler KS, Jaffee SR, Romer D, editors. The Dynamic Genome and Mental Health: The Role of Genes and Environments in Youth Development. New York: Oxford University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- D'Onofrio BM, Singh AL, Iliadou A, Lambe M, Hultman C, Grann M, et al. Familial confounding of the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring criminality: A population-based study in Sweden. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67:529–538. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D'Onofrio BM, Singh AL, Iliadou A, Lambe M, Hultman C, Neiderhiser JM, et al. A quasi-experimental study of maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring academic achievement. Child Development. 2010;81:80–100. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01382.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Onofrio BM, Van Hulle CA, Waldman ID, Rodgers JL, Harden KP, Rathouz PJ, et al. Smoking during pregnancy and offspring externalizing problems: An exploration of genetic and environmental confounds. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:139–164. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Onofrio BM, Van Hulle CA, Waldman ID, Rodgers JL, Rathouz PJ, Lahey BB. Causal inferences regarding prenatal alcohol exposure and childhood externalizing problems. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:1296–1304. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Day NL, Goldschmid L, Robles N, Richardson G, Cornelius M, Taylor P, et al. Prenatal alcohol exposure and offspring growth at 18 months of age: the predictive validity of two measures of drinking. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1991;15:914–918. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1991.tb05187.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derauf C, Kekatpure M, Neyzi N, Lester B, Kosofsky B. Neuroimaging of children following prenatal drug exposure. Seminars in Cell and Developmental BIology. 2009;20(4):441–454. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2009.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donovan SJ, Susser E. Commentary: Advent of sibling designs. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2011;40(2):345–349. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyr057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott DS, Huizinga D. Social class and delinquent behavior in a national youth panel. [Journal; Peer Reviewed Journal] Criminology: An Interdisciplinary Journal. 1983;21:149–177. [Google Scholar]
- Ernst M, Moolchan ET, Robinson ML. Behavioral and neural consequences of prenatal exposure to nicotine. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40(6):630–641. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200106000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- FBI. Uniform Crime Reports as prepared by the National Archive of Criminal Justice Data. 2011 Retrieved August 12, 2011, from http://www.fbi.gov/about-us/cjis/ucr/
- Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ. Male and female offending trajectories. [Journal; Peer Reviewed Journal] Development and Psychopathology. 2002;14:159–177. doi: 10.1017/s0954579402001098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- George L, Granath F, Johansson ALV, Cnattingius S. Self-reported nicotine exposure and plasma levels of cotinine in early and late pregnancy. Acta Obstetrica et Gynecologia Scandinavica. 2006;85:1331–1337. doi: 10.1080/00016340600935433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilman SE, Gardener H, Buka SL. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and children's cognitive and physical development: A causal risk factor? American Journal of Epidemiology. 2008;168:522–531. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold AB, Keller AB, Perry DC. Prenatal exposure of rats to nicotine causes persistent alterations of nicotinic cholinergic receptors. Brain Research. 2009;1250:88–100. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.10.076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heath AC, Kessler RC, Neale MC, Hewitt JK, Eaves LJ, Kendler KS. Testing hypotheses about direction of causation using cross-sectional data. Behavior Genetics. 1993;23:29–50. doi: 10.1007/BF01067552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huizink AC. Moderate use of alcohol, tobacco and cannabis during pregnancy: New approaches and update on research findings. Reproductive Toxicology. 2009;28:143–151. doi: 10.1016/j.reprotox.2009.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansson ALV, Dickman PW, Kramer MS, Cnattingius S. Maternal smoking and infant mortality: Does quitting smoking reduce the risk of infant death? Epidemiology. 2009;20:1–8. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e31819dcc6a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knopik VS. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and child outcomes: Real or spurious effect? Developmental Neuropsychology. 2009;34:1–36. doi: 10.1080/87565640802564366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuja-Halkola R, D'Onofrio BM, Iliadou AN, Langstrom N, Lichtenstein P. Prenatal smoking exposure and offspring stress coping in late adolescence: No causal link. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2010;39(6):1531–1540. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahey BB, D’Onofrio BM. All in the family: Comparing siblings to test causal hypotheses regarding environmental influences on behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2010;19:319–323. doi: 10.1177/0963721410383977. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahey BB, Van Hulle CA, D'Onofrio BM, Rodgers JL, Waldman ID. Is Parental Knowledge of Their Adolescent Offspring’s Whereabouts and Peer Associations Spuriously Associated with Offspring Delinquency? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36:807–823. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9214-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lambe M, Hultman C, Torrang A, MacCabe J, Cnattingius S. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and school performance at age 15. Epidemiology. 2006;17:524–530. doi: 10.1097/01.ede.0000231561.49208.be. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavigne JV, Hopkins J, Gouze KR, Bryant FB, LeBailly SA, Binns HJ, et al. Is smoking during pregnancy a risk factor for psychopathology in young children? A methodological caveat and report on preschoolers. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2011;36(1):10–24. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsq044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leve LD, Neiderhiser JM, Scaramella LV, Reiss D. The early growth and development study: Using the prospective adoption design to examine genotype-environment interplay. Behavior Genetics. 2010;40:306–314. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9353-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindblad F, Hjern A. ADHD after fetal exposure to maternal smoking. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2010;12(4):408–415. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntq017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loeber R, Farrington DP, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Van Kammen W. Antisocial behavior and mental health problems: Explanatory factors in childhood and adolescence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Loeber R, Farrington DP, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Van Kammen WB. Antisocial behavior and mental health problems: Explanatory factors in childhood and adolescence. viii. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 1998. p. 330. [Google Scholar]
- Long JS. Regression models for categorical and limited dependent variables. New York: Sage; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Lundberg F, Cnattingius S, D’Onofrio B, Altman D, Lambe M, Hultman C, et al. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and intellectual performance in young adult Swedish male offspring. Pediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology. 2010;24:79–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2009.01073.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer KA, Williams P, Hernandez-Diaz S, Cnattingius S. Smoking and risk of oral clefts: Exploring the impact of study designs. Epidemiology. 2004;15:671–678. doi: 10.1097/01.ede.0000142148.51230.60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell BF, Taggert MJ. Are animal models relevant to key aspects of human parturition? American Journal of Physiology - Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology. 2009;297:R525–R545. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00153.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt TE. Life-course persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial behavior. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen D, editors. Developmental Psychopathology. 2nd Edition. New York: Wiley; 2006. pp. 570–598. [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M, Silva PA. Sex differences in antisocial behaviour: Conduct disorder, delinquency, and violence in the Dunedin Longitudinal Study. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s Guide. Sixth Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén and Muthén; 1998–2010. [Google Scholar]
- Obel C, Olsen J, Henriksen TB, Rodriguez A, Järvelin MR, Moilanen I, et al. Is maternal smoking during pregnancy a risk factor for Hyperkinetic disorder?—findings from a sibling design. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2011;40(2):338–345. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Odgers CL, Moffitt TE, Broadbent JM, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington HL, et al. Female and male antisocial trajectories: From childhood origins to adult outcomes. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:673–716. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olds D. Tobacco exposure and impaired development: A review of the evidence. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 1997;3(3):257–269. [Google Scholar]
- Paradis AD, Fitzmaurice GM, Koenen KC, Buka SL. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and criminal offending among adult offspring. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2010 doi: 10.1136/jech.2009.095802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Post A, Gilljam H, Bremberg S, Galanti MR. Maternal smoking during pregnancy: a comparison between concurrent and retrospective self-reports. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology. 2008;22:155–161. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2007.00917.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice F, Harold GT, Boivin J, Hay DF, Van den Bree M, Thapar A. Disentangling prenatal and inherited influences in humans with an experimental design. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Early Edition Online. 2009 doi: 10.1073/pnas.0808798106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodgers JL, Rowe DC, Li C. Beyond nature vs. nurture: DF analyses of nonshared influences on problem behaviors. Developmental Psychology. 1994;30:374–384. [Google Scholar]
- Roza SJ, Verhulst FC, Jaddoe VWV, Steegers EAP, Mackenbach JP, Hofman A, et al. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and child behaviour problems: The Generation R Study. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2009;38(3):680–689. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyn163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin DB. Matched Sampling for Causal Effects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M. Proceeding from observed correlation to causal inference: The use of natural experiments. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2007;2:377–395. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00050.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M, Pickles A, Murray R, Eaves LJ. Testing hypotheses on specific environmental causal effects on behavior. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:291–324. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.3.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadish WR, Cook TD, Campbell DT. Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. New York: Houghton Mifflin; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Talati A, Weissman MM. In utero smoking exposure warrants further investigation. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67:1094. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, Harold G, Rice F, Ge X, Boivin J, Hay D, et al. Do intrauterine or genetic influences explain the foetal origins of chronic disease? A novel experimental method for disentangling effects. BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2007;7:25. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-7-25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, Rice F, Hay D, Boivin J, Langley K, Van den Bree M, et al. Prenatal smoking might not cause Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Evidence from a novel design. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66:722–727. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wakschlag LS, Pickett KE, Cook E, Benowitz NL, Leventhal BL. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and severe antisocial behavior in offspring: A review. American Journal of Public Health. 2002;92:966–974. doi: 10.2105/ajph.92.6.966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]