Abstract
Autistic individuals typically excel on spatial tests that measure abstract reasoning, such as the Block Design subtest on intelligence test batteries and the Raven’s Progressive Matrices nonverbal test of intelligence. Such well-replicated findings suggest that abstract spatial processing is a relative and perhaps absolute strength of autistic individuals. However, previous studies have not systematically varied reasoning level – concrete vs. abstract – and test domain – spatial vs. numerical vs. verbal, which the current study did. Autistic participants (N = 72) and non-autistic participants (N = 72) completed a battery of 12 tests that varied by reasoning level (concrete vs. abstract) and domain (spatial vs. numerical vs. verbal). Autistic participants outperformed non-autistic participants on abstract spatial tests. Non-autistic participants did not outperform autistic participants on any of the three domains (spatial, numerical, and verbal) or at either of the two reasoning levels (concrete and abstract), suggesting similarity in abilities between autistic and non-autistic individuals, with abstract spatial reasoning as an autistic strength.
Introduction
Enhanced spatial perception is a signature characteristic of autistic individuals (e.g., [1]; see Sinclair’s (1999) essay, “Why I dislike person first language” [51], for why we have chosen to use the term “autistic person(s)” rather than “person(s) with autism”). The most replicated autistic strength is on the embedded figures task: Autistic individuals locate hidden figures in a visual display more rapidly than non-autistic individuals do [2], [3], [4], [5], [6]. In fact, the speed with which autistic participants correctly locate hidden figures distinguishes them from non-autistic participants more powerfully (d = 2.8) than do measures of theory of mind (d = 1.0) or executive function (d = 0.3–1.1) [7]. Even non-autistic adults who have more autistic traits than other non-autistic adults locate hidden figures more rapidly and accurately [8].
Strengths can be identified in either absolute or relative terms. A relative strength, or a personal strength, is an area in which an individual excels compared with other areas in which the individual performs less well. An absolute strength, such as a population strength, is an area in which a population or group of individuals excel, compared with another group of individuals who perform less well. Autistic individuals’ strength on the embedded figures test is a highly replicated absolute strength.
A highly replicated relative strength for many autistic individuals is their performance on Block Design subtests, which occur on various intelligence tests [9], [10], [11]. To complete a Block Design subtest, the participant arranges small blocks to replicate a target pattern. Although some studies report Block Design performance as an autistic absolute strength (i.e., autistic individuals perform Block Design better than non-autistic individuals [7], [12], [13]), and some non-autistic individuals who have more autistic traits perform Block Design better than other non-autistic individuals who have fewer autistic traits [14], [15], most studies report Block Design as a relative autistic strength (i.e., autistic individuals perform Block Design subtests better than other subtests).
In fact, autistic individuals usually show a marked peak on the Block Design subtest [16], [17], [18], [19], [20]. Table 1 summarizes the results of nearly 40 studies of autistic participants’ performance on Wechsler intelligence test batteries. On average, autistic participants perform two-thirds of a standard deviation higher on the Block Design subtest than they do on other subtests illustrating a relative autistic strength.
Table 1. Studies Reporting Wechsler Subtest Scores for Autistic Children and Adults.
First Author, Year [Citation] | N | M | Range | Best Subtest | Worst Subtest |
Allen, 1991 [16] | 20 | 5.28 | 9.90 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Asarnow, 1987 [52] | 23 | 8.71 | 8.30 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Bartak, 1975 [53] | 9 | 6.90 | 9.70 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Bölte, 2002 [54] | 20 | 7.69 | 3.80 | Object Assembly | Picture Arrangement |
Bölte, 2004 [55] | 59 | 7.11 | 2.94 | Similarities | Picture Arrangement |
Charman, 2011 [29] | 127 | 6.33 | 3.70 | Picture Arrangement | Comprehension |
Dawson, 2011 [31] | 57 | 10.02 | 4.92 | Information | Coding/Digit Symbol |
de Bruin, 2006 [56] | 100 | 8.54 | 2.34 | Picture Arrangement | Coding/Digit Symbol |
Dennis, 1999 [57] | 8 | 9.26 | 4.37 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Ehlers, 1997 [58] | 80 | 8.70 | 3.60 | Similarities | Coding/Digit Symbol |
Freeman, 1985 [59] | 21 | 9.07 | 5.60 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Gilchrist, 2001 [60] | 33 | 7.96 | 4.23 | Block Design | Coding/Digit Symbol |
Goldstein, 2002 [61] | 31 | 9.34 | 6.16 | Information | Coding/Digit Symbol |
Happé, 1994 [17] | 51 | 4.67 | 4.33 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Holdnack, 2011 [62] | 43 | 8.26 | 3.21 | Similarities | Coding/Digit Symbol |
Koyama, 2006 [63] | 27 | 9.44 | 5.50 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Koyama, 2007 [64] | 73 | 9.73 | 4.17 | Digit Span | Comprehension |
Koyama, 2008 [18] | 106 | 10.32 | 3.00 | Digit Span | Comprehension |
Koyama, 2009 [65] | 142 | 9.65 | 3.37 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Lincoln, 1988 [66] | 46 | 6.01 | 7.40 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Lockyer, 1970 [67] | varies | 5.09 | 5.44 | Block Design | Coding/Digit Symbol |
Mayes, 2003 [68] | 63 | 9.22 | 1.83 | Similarities | Coding/Digit Symbol |
Mayes, 2004 [69] | 93 | 10.25 | 4.70 | Similarities | Coding/Digit Symbol |
Mayes, 2008 [70] | 54 | 10.05 | 6.30 | Similarities | Coding/Digit Symbol |
Merchán-Naranjo, 2011 [71] | 29 | 9.47 | 5.83 | Information | Coding/Digit Symbol |
Minshew, 2005 [72] | 215 | 9.66 | 3.61 | Information | Coding/Digit Symbol |
Narita, 1987 [73] | 45 | 5.23 | 8.98 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Noterdaeme, 2010 [74] | 112 | 9.79 | 4.05 | Information | Picture Arrangement |
Nyden, 2001 [75] | 20 | 10.55 | 7.00 | Vocabulary | Digit Span |
Ohta, 1987 [76] | 16 | 5.85 | 9.20 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Rumsey, 1988 [77] | 10 | 10.55 | 7.65 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Shah, 1988a [17] | 18 | 5.94 | 9.60 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Siegel, 1996 [19] | 81 | 8.98 | 3.07 | Block Design | Coding/Digit Symbol |
Spek, 2008 [78] | 43 | 11.44 | 2.72 | Comprehension | Coding/Digit Symbol |
Szatmari, 1990 [24] | 43 | 7.77 | 2.18 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Tymchuk, 1977 [79] | 20 | 8.16 | 4.37 | Digit Span | Comprehension |
Venter, 1992 [20] | 52 | 6.41 | 4.35 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Williams, 2006 [80] | 38 | 10.50 | 5.60 | Information | Comprehension |
Average (weighted equally) | 38 | 8.36 | 4.02 | Block Design | Comprehension |
Average (weighted by N) | 2028 | 8.74 | 3.05 | Block Design | Comprehension |
N = number of participants; M = average score; Range = range of scores.
Unpublished data as reported in Happé (1994).
Autistic individuals’ strength on Block Design tests is often interpreted, not as an area of cognitive strength, but instead as an area of diagnostic weakness. For example, a popular theory proposes that autistic individuals excel on Block Design because they actually suffer from weak central coherence – a reduced ability to ‘see the big picture.’ According to this theory, autistic individuals excel at decomposing the target pattern because their perception is too piecemeal [13]. However, even when the typical Block Design task is reversed – participants have to match a pre-segmented target to an intact pattern – autistic participants still excel [21].
When autistic individuals’ strength on Block Design is not being re-interpreted as a weakness, it is often dismissed as reflecting merely low-level [22] or concrete processing [23], [24]. However, according to psychometricians, Block Design must draw on complex problem solving because it is so highly correlated with general intelligence [25], [26]. Tests that correlate highly with general intelligence, as Block Design does, are considered more general, and, therefore, more abstract cognitive tests. Their high correlation with general intelligence suggests that they draw on more components of general intelligence [25], [26].
In contrast to Block Design, the Coding/Digit Symbol subtest, for which autistic individuals typically do not excel, is only moderately correlated with general intelligence [25]. Tests that correlate only moderately with general intelligence are considered more specific, and, therefore, more concrete cognitive tests. Note that abstractness and concreteness, when applied to cognitive tests, are not synonymous with more and less difficult. Rather, more general, abstract tests are those that correlate more highly with general intelligence, whereas less general, concrete tests are those that correlate only moderately with general intelligence [25], [26]. Block Design is a more abstract cognitive test, because it draws on more components of intelligence, whereas Coding/Digit Symbol is considered more concrete, because it draws on fewer components of general intelligence [25], [26]. Therefore, contrary to some autism researchers’ assumptions, autistic individuals’ strength on Block Design is a strength in abstract spatial reasoning.
Block Design is not the only abstract spatial test on which autistic individuals excel. The Raven’s Progressive Matrices [27] is an even more abstract test [25], [26], and autistic individuals show a relative strength on Raven’s Progressive Matrices. Autistic children and adults score higher on Raven’s Progressive Standard Matrices than they score on Wechsler tests [28], [29], [30], [31]; in contrast, non-autistic children and adults score the same across the two types of tests. When autistic and non-autistic children are matched on their performance on Wechsler tests, autistic children are more accurate than non-autistic children on the Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices [32]. When autistic and non-autistic adults are equal in their accuracy on the Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices, autistic individuals are faster [33]. Even for non-autistic adults, their degree of autistic traits predicts their successful completion of Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices items that are considered more visual-spatial [34]. Thus, autistic individuals’ performance on the Raven’s Progressive Matrices, the most agreed upon test of abstract spatial reasoning, suggests a strength in abstract spatial reasoning.
Further evidence for autistic individuals’ strength in abstract spatial reasoning can be approximated from their performance profile on the Wechsler intelligence scales. The subtests on Wechsler scales vary in both reasoning level (concrete or abstract) [25], [26] and test domain (spatial, numerical, or verbal). As shown in Table 2, autistic participants perform better on Wechsler subtests that tap the spatial domain (e.g., Block Design, Picture Completion) than they perform on subtests that tap the numerical domain (e.g., Arithmetic, Digit Symbol/Coding) or verbal domain (e.g., Vocabulary, Comprehension). Autistic participants also perform better on Wechsler subtests that assess abstract reasoning (e.g., Vocabulary, Arithmetic, Block Design) than on subtests that assess concrete reasoning (e.g., Comprehension, Digit Symbol/Coding, Picture Completion). Thus, Figure 1 suggests that autistic participants have strength in abstract spatial processing.
Table 2. Effects of Test Domain and Reasoning Level in Wechsler Subtest Scores for Autistic Children and Adults.
First Author, Year [Citation] | Concrete Spatial | AbstractSpatial | Concrete Numerical | Abstract Numerical | Concrete Verbal | Abstract Verbal |
Allen, 1991 [16] | 87.58 | 106.00 | 73.38 | 71.75 | 62.38 | 64.13 |
Asarnow, 1987 [52] | 98.67 | 117.50 | 87.50 | 91.00 | 88.00 | 86.75 |
Bartak, 1975 [53] | 96.00 | 112.00 | 87.25 | 78.00 | 66.25 | 72.25 |
Bölte, 2002 [54] | 87.00 | 93.00 | 86.50 | 87.50 | 88.50 | 90.75 |
Bölte, 2004 [55] | 83.56 | 89.37 | 84.07 | 86.15 | 84.47 | 88.75 |
Charman, 2011 [29] | 86.67 | 80.00 | 80.00 | 80.50 | 78.25 | 79.75 |
Dawson, 2011 [31] | 97.00 | 100.80 | 85.50 | 102.80 | 101.10 | 107.80 |
de Bruin, 2006 [56] | 93.68 | 92.90 | 87.70 | 90.75 | 94.05 | 95.73 |
Dennis, 1999 [57] | 96.27 | 108.75 | 95.90 | 95.65 | 90.03 | 97.20 |
Ehlers, 1997 [58] | 90.92 | 99.00 | 89.38 | 87.75 | 97.50 | 97.63 |
Freeman, 1985 [59] | 99.17 | 110.50 | 91.25 | 90.50 | 92.25 | 91.75 |
Gilchrist, 2001 [60] | 87.83 | 102.00 | 89.33 | 88.30 | 88.53 | 89.10 |
Goldstein, 2002 [61] | 97.92 | 103.40 | 86.85 | 99.70 | 95.65 | 100.90 |
Happé, 1994 [17] | 76.37 | 86.35 | 76.60 | 67.15 | 68.73 | 66.68 |
Holdnack, 2011 [62] | n/a | 93.85 | 86.90 | 90.90 | 92.80 | 93.10 |
Koyama, 2006 [63] | 98.67 | 110.00 | 100.75 | 94.50 | 89.25 | 94.25 |
Koyama, 2007 [64] | 96.40 | 108.75 | 100.53 | 101.25 | 94.08 | 98.30 |
Koyama, 2008 [18] | 100.07 | 108.40 | 103.18 | 100.95 | 97.85 | 102.93 |
Koyama, 2009 [65] | 97.15 | 106.10 | 100.43 | 97.90 | 94.30 | 97.93 |
Lincoln, 1988 [66] | 87.23 | 101.20 | 79.88 | 76.60 | 69.33 | 71.20 |
Lockyer, 1970 [67] | 75.68 | 91.40 | 74.70 | 72.05 | 70.90 | 72.60 |
Mayes, 2003 [68] | 96.50 | 98.50 | 92.42 | 92.17 | 96.75 | 99.33 |
Mayes, 2004 [69] | 102.33 | 108.00 | 91.00 | 95.00 | 103.00 | 108.00 |
Mayes, 2008 [70] | n/a | 108.00 | 87.50 | 50.00 | 105.50 | 107.50 |
Merchán-Naranjo, 2011 [71] | 102.53 | 93.30 | 82.60 | 89.45 | 98.10 | 104.75 |
Minshew, 2005 [72] | 95.95 | 104.30 | 94.40 | 101.00 | 96.38 | 103.28 |
Narita, 1987 [73] | 73.78 | 102.00 | 91.63 | 68.35 | 59.55 | 71.90 |
Noterdaeme, 2010 [74] | 93.78 | 104.60 | 89.05 | 102.85 | 99.25 | 106.63 |
Nyden, 2001 [75] | 95.00 | 110.00 | 90.75 | 95.50 | 109.25 | 120.00 |
Ohta, 1987 [76] | 85.33 | 103.00 | 81.75 | 72.50 | 64.50 | 73.75 |
Rumsey, 1988 [77] | 98.00 | 124.50 | 104.50 | 102.50 | 96.88 | 103.13 |
Shah, 1988a [17] | 87.50 | 111.00 | 75.50 | 72.50 | 71.75 | 68.00 |
Siegel, 1996 [19] | 94.88 | 101.85 | 91.85 | 95.25 | 92.58 | 96.60 |
Spek, 2008 [78] | 105.95 | 110.10 | 101.33 | 108.85 | 109.88 | 110.13 |
Szatmari, 1990 [24] | 89.45 | 93.60 | 88.03 | 88.40 | 85.88 | 89.53 |
Tymchuk, 1977 [79] | 89.93 | 99.00 | 90.68 | 89.10 | 88.20 | 91.55 |
Venter, 1992 [20] | 83.62 | 91.25 | 85.35 | 77.50 | 78.00 | 77.35 |
Williams, 2006 [80] | 100.13 | 108.30 | 96.90 | 105.65 | 98.83 | 110.98 |
Average (weighted equally) | 92.46 | 102.44 | 89.02 | 88.37 | 88.38 | 92.15 |
Average (weighted by N) | 89.20 | 101.01 | 90.56 | 91.21 | 91.47 | 95.40 |
Concrete Spatial tests comprise Picture Completion, Picture Arrangement, and Object Assembly; Abstract Spatial comprises Block Design; Concrete Numerical comprises Digit Symbol/Coding and Digit Span; Abstract Numerical comprises Arithmetic; Concrete Verbal comprises Comprehension and Similarities; and Abstract Verbal comprises Vocabulary and Information. Composite scores were normalized to have a M = 100 and SD = 15. An ANOVA on these data indicated significant main effects of domain (F(2, 70) = 20.31, p<0.001) and reasoning level (F(1, 35) = 88.73, p<0.001) and a significant interaction between domain and reasoning level (F(2, 70) = 17.07, p<0.001).
Unpublished data were reported in Happé (1994).
However, this analysis of Wechsler subtest performance does not systematically vary reasoning level, abstract versus concrete, and domain, spatial versus non-spatial. In fact, no previous study has systematically varied reasoning level and test domain. That was the purpose of the present study, which examined relative and absolute strengths in autistic and non-autistic individuals’ performance on 12 tests that varied by reasoning level (concrete vs. abstract) and domain (spatial vs. numerical vs. verbal).
Methods
Ethics Statement
Participants gave written consent, approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Wisconsin–Madison to ensure the study was ethical. In addition, the Gateway Council (a committee of academic researchers and autistic adults) ensured that the study was inclusive, respectful, accessible, and relevant.
Participants
Participants were recruited through the Gateway Project (http://thegatewayproject.org), which is an Internet-based research platform for inclusive, respectful, accessible, and relevant studies involving autistic and non-autistic adults. Internet-based studies, including studies that administer cognitive tests, provide results comparable to in-person studies [35], [36]. For example, Internet-based administration of reading and math tests provide results that are highly correlated with in-person administration of the same tests on the same participants [37].
Participants in the Gateway Project complete a 30-minute enrollment survey (i.e., the Gateway Survey) that collects demographic data, such as age, personal and parental education level, and includes the Autism-Spectrum Quotient [38]. For the current study, autistic participants were defined as adults who met criteria for the autism spectrum on the Autism-Spectrum Quotient and self-identified as autistic. Non-autistic participants were adults who did not meet criteria for the autism spectrum on the Autism-Spectrum Quotient and did not self-identify as either being autistic or as having any other disability.
All participants were native English speakers who did not report being blind or having significant vision loss lasting at least 6 months. Data were analyzed from 72 autistic adults (36 males, 36 females) and 72 non-autistic adults (36 males, 36 females) who completed all 12 cognitive tests. Autistic and non-autistic participants were matched on sex, age, personal education, and parental education (all t(142) <1, all ps >.01). A summary of the participants’ demographic characteristics is provided in Table 3.
Table 3. Demographic Characteristics for Autistic and Non-Autistic Participants.
Autistic | Non-Autistic | p | |
N | 72 | 72 | |
AQ Percent: M (SD) | 78.26 (9.68) | 33.56 (11.09) | <.001 |
Formal Diagnosis: No/Yes | 23/49 | 72/0 | <.001 |
Age at Session 1: M (SD) | 41.64 (12.49) | 41.47 (12.22) | .94 |
Age at Session 2: M (SD) | 41.68 (12.50) | 41.51 (12.22) | .94 |
Age at Session 3: M (SD) | 41.69 (12.51) | 41.53 (12.22) | .94 |
Sex: Male/Female | 36/36 | 36/36 | 1.00 |
Personal Education: M (SD) | 15.76 (2.09) | 15.67 (1.91) | .77 |
Parent Education: M (SD) | 15.44 (2.82) | 15.47 (2.89) | .95 |
Latino or Hispanic: No/Yes | 70/2 | 70/2 | 1.00 |
Racial Category: White/Other | 68/4 | 68/4 | 1.00 |
Country: US/Other | 59/13 | 68/4 | .02 |
Materials
The 12 cognitive tests varied by reasoning level (concrete or abstract) and domain (spatial, numerical, or verbal). All 12 tests were time-limited, and all but the two concrete numerical tests were multiple-choice. Nine of the 12 cognitive tests were taken from the Educational Testing Service (ETS) Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests [39], with the other three cognitive tests taken from the Cognitive Abilities Test [40]. Table 4 summarizes the reasoning level, test domain, number of items, time limit, and correlation with general intelligence for each test.
Table 4. Description of Cognitive Tests.
Cognitive Test | Reasoning Level | Domain | Number of Items | Time Limit (in minutes) | Correlation with General Intelligence [25], [26] |
Card Rotations | Concrete | Spatial | 160 | 6 | r = .3 |
Cube Comparisons | Concrete | Spatial | 42 | 6 | r = .5 |
Paper Folding | Abstract | Spatial | 20 | 6 | r = .7 |
Figure Analogies | Abstract | Spatial | 20 | 10 | r = .7a |
Addition | Concrete | Numerical | 120 | 4 | r = .3 |
Subtraction and Multiplication | Concrete | Numerical | 120 | 4 | r = .3 |
Necessary Arithmetic Operations | Abstract | Numerical | 30 | 10 | r = .7 |
Number Analogies | Abstract | Numerical | 24 | 12 | r = .7a |
Vocabulary I | Concrete | Verbal | 36 | 8 | r = .3a |
Extended Range Vocabulary | Concrete | Verbal | 48 | 12 | r = .3a |
Letter Sets | Abstract | Verbal | 30 | 14 | r = .7a |
Verbal Analogies | Abstract | Verbal | 30 | 10 | r = .8 |
Correlation from comparable test (e.g., Pattern Analogies for Figure Analogies).
The four spatial tests were Card Rotations, Cube Comparisons, Paper Folding, and Figure Analogies. In Card Rotations, participants determine which of eight options represents a two-dimensional target when rotated or flipped. In Cube Comparisons, participants determine whether two three-dimensional cubes are the same or different, allowing for each to be rotated. In Paper Folding, participants determine which of five drawings represent how a sheet of folded paper would appear when unfolded. In Figure Analogies, participants select among five geometric figures the geometric figure that forms a pair analogous to another pair of geometric figures. Card Rotations and Cube Comparisons served as the two concrete spatial tests, while Paper Folding and Figure Analogies served as the two abstract spatial tests.
The four numerical tests were Addition, Subtraction and Multiplication, Necessary Arithmetic Operations, and Number Analogies. On the Addition test, participants add three one- or two-digit numbers (e.g., 80+78+15). On the Subtraction and Multiplication test, participants alternate between subtracting two-digit numbers from two-digit numbers (e.g., 98–75) and multiplying two-digit numbers by one-digit numbers (e.g., 86×6). On the Necessary Arithmetic Operations test, participants identify the numerical operations required to solve arithmetic word problems (e.g., If a man earns $5.75 an hour, how many hours should he work each day in order to make an average of $46.00 per day? Subtract, Divide, Add, or Multiply). On the Number Analogies test, participants select among five numbers the number that forms a relation analogous to two previous sets of relations between numbers (e.g., [5 → 4 ] [8 → 7 ] [3 → ? ] 1, 2, 3, 5, or 6). The Addition Test and the Subtraction and Multiplication Test served as the two concrete numerical tests, while Necessary Arithmetic Operations and Number Analogies served as the two abstract numerical tests.
The four verbal tests were Vocabulary I, Extended Range Vocabulary, Letter Sets, and Verbal Analogies. On the Vocabulary I and Extended Range Vocabulary tests, participants select among four words the synonym of a target word (e.g., jovial: refreshing, scarce, thickset, wise, or jolly). On Letter Sets, participants select among five sets of letters the letter set that obviates the pattern established by the other four letter sets (e.g., NOPQ, DEFL, ABCD, HIJK, or UVWX). On Verbal Analogies, participants select among five words the word that forms a relation analogous to a previous relation between two words (e.g., [cow → milk : chicken → ?] feather, dinner, egg, hen, or bird). Vocabulary I and Extended Range Vocabulary served as the two concrete verbal tests, while Letter Sets and Verbal Analogies served as the two abstract verbal tests.
Procedure
Participants, who met eligibility requirements, were notified of the study via email. They were administered the 12 cognitive tests over three 45-minute sessions. Each session assessed a single test domain (i.e., spatial, numerical, or verbal, in that order). During each session, participants completed two concrete tests followed by two abstract tests. As is standard practice, the tests were administered in the same order for all participants. At the conclusion of each session, participants were able to enter a drawing with a 1 in 10 chance of receiving a $25 gift card. Participants who completed all three sessions received an additional $10 gift card.
Each session began with a set of unrelated warm-up items, which is a procedure demonstrated to reduce participant drop-out in Internet-based studies [41], [42]. Before each test, participants read detailed instructions and completed practice items. At the conclusion of each session, participants stated whether they performed the tests to the best of their ability.
Data Analysis
A conservative α = .01 was used for all analyses because of the multiple comparisons conducted in this study. A raw score for each participant on each test was computed according to scoring criteria provided by the ETS Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests and the Cognitive Abilities Test. A participant’s raw score was excluded from further analysis if the participant took more than 10 more seconds beyond the time limit for that test; the participant failed to respond correctly to any item on that test; or if there was high accuracy in a very short time on that test. Less than 5% of the raw scores for each cognitive test were excluded (M = 2.12%, SD = 0.86, range = 0.95%–4.36%), and the likelihood of exclusion did not differ between autistic and non-autistic participants (all χ2s >0.05 and <3.0, all ps >.01).
Before further analysis, the raw score distributions for all 12 tests were inspected for normality (kurtosis and skewness within ±2). The raw scores for 2 of the tests, Letter Sets and Verbal Analogies, were cube-transformed because their distributions were leptokurtic, indicating a higher probability of extreme scores. The distribution of Verbal Analogies raw scores was also negatively skewed. After cube-transformation, the raw score distributions for Letter Sets and Verbal Analogies were relatively normal (Letter Sets before cube-transformation: kurtosis = 2.18; Letter Sets after cube-transformation: kurtosis = −0.24; Verbal Analogies before cube-transformation: kurtosis = 7.35 and skew = −2.51; Verbal Analogies after cube-transformation: kurtois = 0.68 and skew = −0.85).
Participants’ raw scores fell within 1 SD of a sample of 11th and 12th graders, on Cube Comparisons, Paper Folding, and Letter Sets, and within 2 SD on Extended Range Vocabulary [39]. Furthermore, Spearman-Brown split-half correlations between odd and even items on each test ranged from r(70) = .81 to r(70) = .98 in autistic participants and r(70) = .75 to r(70) = .99 in non-autistic participants, suggesting good internal consistency.
For comparisons among tests, raw scores for each test were normalized to have a M = 100 and SD = 15 because standard scores are not provided for the ETS Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests. For comparisons among domains and reasoning levels, six composite scores, concrete spatial, abstract spatial, concrete numerical, abstract numerical, concrete verbal, and abstract verbal, were computed by averaging participants’ normalized scores from each composite’s two source tests.
Results
Participants’ raw and normalized scores on the 12 tests are presented in Table 5, and all raw data are available by request from the first author. A group (2) × domain (3) × reasoning level (2) mixed-design ANOVA indicated a significant three-way interaction (F(2, 284) = 8.99, p<.001). This three-way interaction was also significant when the sample was expanded to all participants who completed the study, 103 autistic and 148 non-autistic, although this larger sample was unmatched for sex, age, personal education, and parent education (F(2, 498) = 16.79, p<.001). The three-way interaction between group, domain, and reasoning level was also significant when the sample was reduced to only those of autistic participants with a formal diagnosis (49 autistic; 72 non-autistic; F(2, 238) = 11.39, p<.001). No other interactions or main effects were significant (all ps >.01).
Table 5. Means (and Standard Deviations) for Raw and Normalized Test Scores.
Raw Test Scores | Normalized Test Scores | ||||
Cognitive Test | Autistic | Non-Autistic | Autistic | Non-Autistic | p |
Card Rotations (Concrete – Spatial) | 93.38 (25.79) | 104.57 (26.69) | 96.86 (14.46) | 103.14 (14.97) | .01 |
Cube Comparisons (Concrete – Spatial) | 21.03 (9.76) | 18.57 (8.94) | 101.96 (15.56) | 98.04 (14.25) | .12 |
Paper Folding (Abstract – Spatial) | 12.90 (4.01) | 10.52 (3.55) | 104.50 (15.19) | 95.50 (13.46) | <.001 |
Figure Analogies (Abstract – Spatial) | 14.82 (4.38) | 13.60 (3.94) | 102.18 (15.67) | 97.82 (14.07) | .08 |
Addition (Concrete – Numerical) | 21.69 (8.41) | 24.68 (7.74) | 97.27 (15.62) | 102.73 (14.18) | .03 |
Subtraction and Multiplication (Concrete – Numerical) | 33.88 (12.55) | 36.33 (11.49) | 98.47 (15.40) | 101.53 (14.30) | .22 |
Necessary Arithmetic Operations (Abstract – Numerical) | 18.72 (5.89) | 18.64 (4.57) | 100.12 (16.82) | 99.88 (13.04) | .93 |
Number Analogies (Abstract – Numerical) | 13.31 (5.16) | 13.04 (4.57) | 100.41 (15.94) | 99.59 (14.10) | .75 |
Vocabulary I (Concrete – Verbal) | 31.33 (5.01) | 30.46 (4.26) | 101.40 (16.14) | 98.60 (13.74) | .26 |
Extended Range Vocabulary (Concrete – Verbal) | 33.35 (8.90) | 30.98 (7.73) | 102.12 (15.90) | 97.88 (13.83) | .09 |
Letter Setsa (Abstract – Verbal) | 21.12 (4.98) | 21.13 (5.17) | 99.83 (14.83) | 100.17 (15.27) | .89 |
Verbal Analogiesa (Abstract – Verbal) | 25.42 (3.36) | 25.40 (3.46) | 100.05 (15.64) | 99.95 (14.44) | .97 |
t-test conducted on cubed transformed raw scores.
To explore further the significant three-way interaction between group, test domain, and reasoning level, post-hoc domain x reasoning level repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted within each group to assess relative strengths, and post-hoc group x reasoning level mixed-design ANOVAs were conducted across the three domains to assess absolute strengths. Domain and reasoning level significantly interacted for both autistic (F(2, 142) = 4.60, p = .01) and non-autistic participants (F(2, 142) = 4.40, p = .01). As shown in Figure 2, autistic participants exhibited a relative advantage for the abstract reasoning composite in the spatial domain (F(1, 71) = 12.64, p = .001). Non-autistic participants exhibited the reverse pattern of performance with a relative advantage for the concrete reasoning composite in the spatial domain (F(1, 71) = 8.88, p = .004). Reasoning level did not significantly affect performance in the numerical or verbal domains for either autistic (numerical: F(1, 71) = 2.28, p = .14; verbal: F(1, 71) = 1.28, p = .26) or non-autistic participants (numerical: F(1, 71) = 1.74, p = .19; verbal: (F(1, 71) = 1.49, p = .23). Furthermore, the interaction between group and reasoning level was significant in the spatial domain (F(1, 142) = 20.86, p<.001), with autistic participants exhibiting an absolute strength over non-autistic participants on abstract spatial tests (F(1, 142) = 9.11, p = .003), but not on concrete spatial tests (F(1, 142) = 0.28, p = .60).
A sex × group × domain × reasoning level mixed-design ANOVA examined sex differences in performance on the cognitive tests. Sex did not interact significantly with either domain (F(2, 280) = 3.03, p = .05) or group (autistic vs. non-autistic, F(1, 140) = 2.95, p = .09). Figure 3 shows performance of the autistic group broken down by sex.
Discussion
The present study varied reasoning level (concrete and abstract) and test domain (spatial, numerical, and verbal) to examine more systematically previous suggestions of a relative and absolute autistic strength on tests of abstract spatial reasoning. Autistic participants performed significantly better on abstract spatial tests than concrete spatial tests, suggesting spatial abstract reasoning is a relative autistic strength. Furthermore, autistic participants performed significantly better than non-autistic participants on abstract spatial tests, suggesting that spatial abstract reasoning is also an absolute autistic strength. Autistic participants’ superior performance on the abstract spatial tests used in this study rebuffs the assumption that their previously documented strengths on Block Design [17] and Raven’s Progressive Matrices [30] arise from rote memory or low-level concrete processing or that autistic individuals are impaired on tests that require abstract reasoning [43], [44].
From where does autistics’ abstract spatial strength arise? We don’t know, but we do know that very early in life autistic children are considerably more receptive to abstract spatial stimuli than are non-autistic children. In a recent study, autistic toddlers split their time evenly examining abstract spatial stimuli (on one side of a computer monitor) and videos of unknown children (on the other side of the monitor) [45]. Typically developing toddlers, and toddlers with other developmental disabilities, glanced only occasionally at the abstract spatial stimuli. Only a few non-autistic toddlers split their viewing time equally between the abstract spatial stimuli and the videos of unknown children, despite the abstract spatial stimuli extending across half the computer monitor placed before them. Later in life, many autistic adults also express a preference for spatial representations compared with verbal representations [46].
With regard to the verbal domain, in the current study, autistic participants performed as well as the non-autistic participants on both concrete verbal tests and abstract verbal tests. Such a finding might appear to contradict the well-established weakness autistic participants display on some of the Wechsler subtests that tap the verbal domain, most notably the Wechsler Comprehension subtest. However, the verbal tests in the present study differ from the verbal Wechsler subtests in several ways. The verbal tests in the present study offered multiple-choice responses to written stimuli, whereas the Wechsler verbal subtests require overt language production to spoken questions. The verbal subtests on the Wechsler scales also require face-to-face interaction with an administrator, whereas all the tests in the present study avoid this potential confound.
The data in present study resemble the pattern of sex differences in cognitive abilities sometimes found in other studies, with a slight, but not always significant, male advantage on the spatial [47] and numerical domains [48] and a slight, but not always significant, female advantage on the verbal domain [49]. In the present study, this pattern of slight, but not significant, sex differences occurred for both autistic and non-autistic participants. Such a finding seems at odds with a recently popularized theory about autism: the extreme male brain theory [50], which claims that male and female autistic individuals not only mirror but exaggerate non-autistic male strengths (i.e., an advantage on spatial and numerical tests and a disadvantage on verbal tests). However, autistic females resembled the pattern usually found with non-autistic females, and autistic males resembled the pattern usually found with non-autistic males. Thus, the present results argue against autistic strengths as simply an extreme version of male strengths.
The present study is the first large-scale Internet-based examination of autistic and non-autistic cognition. Internet-based research platforms minimize the social and communication barriers often present in more traditional laboratory settings. The current study’s success supports the use of the Internet as a research medium for investigating other cognitive domains. Most importantly, the results further our understanding of the nature of autistic cognition and its strengths.
Acknowledgments
We thank C. Boisclair, M. Dawson, and S. Dern for conversations that influenced the design of this study. We thank E. Ashkenazy, C. Boisclair, and S. Dern for pilot testing the study materials to ensure they were accessible to autistic individuals and for recommending minor changes to the instructions for some of the tests to ensure that the instructions were as clear as possible, S. Gardner and P. Ramraj for technical support, and D.M. Goldsmith and N. Whaley for their help in developing the images that were used for stimuli. Finally, we thank E. Ashkenazy, C. Boisclair, S. Dern, P. Devine, A.J.B. Fugard, H.H. Goldsmith, K. McDonald, Y. Miyamoto, C. Nicolaidis, and M. Triana for feedback on a previous version of this manuscript.
Funding Statement
The Vilas Research Trust from the University of Wisconsin-Madison to MAG supported this research. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Mottron L, Dawson M, Soulieres I, Hubert B, Burack J (2006) Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight principles of autistic perception. J Autism Dev Disord 36: 27–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Edgin JO, Pennington BF (2005) Spatial cognition in autism spectrum disorders: superior, impaired, or just intact? J Autism Dev Disord 35: 729–745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Jolliffe T, Baron-Cohen S (1997) Are people with autism and Asperger syndrome faster than normal on the Embedded Figures Test? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 38: 527–534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Keehn B, Brenner LA, Ramos AI, Lincoln AJ, Marshall SP, et al. (2009) Brief report: eye-movement patterns during an embedded figures test in children with ASD. J Autism Dev Disord 39: 383–387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Pellicano E, Gibson L, Maybery M, Durkin K, Badcock DR (2005) Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: a possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia 43: 1044–1053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Shah A, Frith U (1983) An islet of ability in autistic children: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 24: 613–620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Pellicano E, Maybery M, Durkin K, Maley A (2006) Multiple cognitive capabilities/deficits in children with an autism spectrum disorder: “weak” central coherence and its relationship to theory of mind and executive control. Dev Psychopathol 18: 77–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Grinter EJ, Maybery MT, Van Beek PL, Pellicano E, Badcock JC, et al. (2009) Global visual processing and self-rated autistic-like traits. J Autism Dev Disord 39: 1278–1290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Wechsler D (1997) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.
- 10.Wechsler D (2002) Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence - Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.
- 11.Wechsler D (2003) Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Fourth Edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.
- 12. Morgan B, Maybery M, Durkin K (2003) Weak central coherence, poor joint attention, and low verbal ability: independent deficits in early autism. Dev Psychol 39: 646–656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Shah A, Frith U (1993) Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 34: 1351–1364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Grinter EJ, Van Beek PL, Maybery MT, Badcock DR (2009) Brief report: visuospatial analysis and self-rated autistic-like traits. J Autism Dev Disord 39: 670–677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Stewart ME, Watson J, Allcock AJ, Yaqoob T (2009) Autistic traits predict performance on the block design. Autism 13: 133–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Allen MH, Lincoln AJ, Kaufman AS (1991) Sequential and simultaneous processing abilities of high-functioning autistic and language-impaired children. J Autism Dev Disord 21: 483–502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Happe FG (1994) Wechsler IQ profile and theory of mind in autism: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 35: 1461–1471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Koyama T, Kurita H (2008) Cognitive profile difference between normally intelligent children with Asperger’s disorder and those with pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 62: 691–696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Siegel DJ, Minshew NJ, Goldstein G (1996) Wechsler IQ profiles in diagnosis of high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord 26: 389–406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Venter A, Lord C, Schopler E (1992) A follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 33: 489–507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Caron MJ, Mottron L, Berthiaume C, Dawson M (2006) Cognitive mechanisms, specificity and neural underpinnings of visuospatial peaks in autism. Brain 129: 1789–1802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Bolte S, Hubl D, Dierks T, Holtmann M, Poustka F (2008) An fMRI-study of locally oriented perception in autism: altered early visual processing of the block design test. J Neural Transm 115: 545–552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Ropar D, Peebles D (2007) Sorting preference in children with autism: The dominance of concrete features. J Autism Dev Disord 37: 270–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Szatmari P, Tuff L, Finlayson MA, Bartolucci G (1990) Asperger’s syndrome and autism: neurocognitive aspects. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 29: 130–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Marshalek B, Lohman DF, Snow RE (1983) The complexity continuum in the radex and hierarchical models of intelligence. Intelligence 7: 107–127. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Snow RE, Kyllonen PC, Marshalek B (1984) The topography of ability and learning correlations. In: Sternberg RJ, editor. Advances in the psychology of human intelligence. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 47–102.
- 27.Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH (1998) Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.
- 28. Bolte S, Dziobek I, Poustka F (2009) Brief report: The level and nature of autistic intelligence revisited. J Autism Dev Disord 39: 678–682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Charman T, Pickles A, Simonoff E, Chandler S, Loucas T, et al. (2011) IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders: data from the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Psychol Med 41: 619–627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Dawson M, Soulieres I, Gernsbacher MA, Mottron L (2007) The level and nature of autistic intelligence. Psychol Sci 18: 657–662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dawson M, Soulieres I, Gernsbacher MA, Mottron L (2011) Autistic spectrum intelligence: What about Asperger Syndrome? PLoS One. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 32. Hayashi M, Kato M, Igarashi K, Kashima H (2008) Superior fluid intelligence in children with Asperger’s disorder. Brain Cogn 66: 306–310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Soulieres I, Dawson M, Samson F, Barbeau EB, Sahyoun CP, et al. (2009) Enhanced visual processing contributes to matrix reasoning in autism. Hum Brain Mapp 30: 4082–4107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Fugard AJ, Stewart ME, Stenning K (2011) Visual/verbal-analytic reasoning bias as a function of self-reported autistic-like traits: A study of typically developing individuals solving Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices. Autism 15: 327–340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Carlbring P, Bruntb S, Bohmanb S, Austinc D, Richardsc J, et al. (2007) Internet vs. paper and pencil administration of questionnaires commonly used in panic/agoraphobia research. Computers in Human Behavior 23: 1421–1434. [Google Scholar]
- 36. Vallejo MA, Jordan CM, Diaz MI, Comeche MI, Ortega J (2007) Psychological assessment via the internet: a reliability and validity study of online (vs paper-and-pencil) versions of the General Health Questionnaire-28 (GHQ-28) and the Symptoms Check-List-90-Revised (SCL-90-R). J Med Internet Res 9: e2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Haworth CM, Harlaar N, Kovas Y, Davis OS, Oliver BR, et al. (2007) Internet cognitive testing of large samples needed in genetic research. Twin Res Hum Genet 10: 554–563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E (2001) The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord 31: 5–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ekstrom RB, French JW, Harman H, Derman D (1976) Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
- 40.Lohman DF, Thorndike RL, Hagen EP, Smith P, Fernandes C, et al.. (2001) Cognitive Abilities Test - Third Edition. London: GL Assessment.
- 41. Reips UD (2001) The Web Experimental Psychology Lab: five years of data collection on the Internet. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 33: 201–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Reips UD (2002) Standards for Internet-based experimenting. Exp Psychol 49: 243–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Minshew NJ, Siegel DJ, Goldstein G, Weldy S (1994) Verbal problem solving in high functioning autistic individuals. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 9: 31–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Minshew NJ, Meyer J, Goldstein G (2002) Abstract reasoning in autism: a dissociation between concept formation and concept identification. Neuropsychology 16: 327–334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Pierce K, Conant D, Hazin R, Stoner R, Desmond J (2011) Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 68: 101–109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Grandin T (2009) How does visual thinking work in the mind of a person with autism? A personal account. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364: 1437–1442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Voyer D, Voyer S, Bryden MP (1995) Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychol Bull 117: 250–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Else-Quest NM, Hyde JS, Linn MC (2010) Cross-national patterns of gender differences in mathematics: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 136: 103–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Hyde JS, Linn MC (1988) Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 104: 53–69. [Google Scholar]
- 50. Baron-Cohen S (2002) The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci 6: 248–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Sinclair J (1999) Why I dislike person first language.
- 52. Asarnow RF, Tanguay PE, Bott L, Freeman BJ (1987) Patterns of intellectual functioning in non-retarded autistic and schizophrenic children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 28: 273–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Bartak L, Rutter M, Cox A (1975) A comparative study of infantile autism and specific development receptive language disorder. I. The children. Br J Psychiatry 126: 127–145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Bolte S, Rudolf L, Poustka F (2002) The cognitive structure of higher functioning autism and schizophrenia: a comparative study. Compr Psychiatry 43: 325–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Bölte S, Poustka F (2004) Comparing the intelligence profiles of savant and nonsavant individuals with autistic disorder. Intelligence 32: 121–131. [Google Scholar]
- 56. de Bruin EI, Verheij F, Ferdinand RF (2006) WISC-R subtest but no overall VIQ-PIQ difference in Dutch children with PDD-NOS. J Abnorm Child Psychol 34: 263–271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Dennis M, Lockyer L, Lazenby AL, Donnelly RE, Wilkinson M, et al. (1999) Intelligence patterns among children with high-functioning autism, phenylketonuria, and childhood head injury. J Autism Dev Disord 29: 5–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Ehlers S, Nyden A, Gillberg C, Sandberg AD, Dahlgren SO, et al. (1997) Asperger syndrome, autism and attention disorders: a comparative study of the cognitive profiles of 120 children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 38: 207–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Freeman BJ, Lucas JC, Forness SR, Ritvo ER (1985) Cognitive processing of high-functioning autistic children: Comparing the K-ABC and the WISC-R. J Psychoedu Assess 3: 357–362. [Google Scholar]
- 60. Gilchrist A, Green J, Cox A, Burton D, Rutter M, et al. (2001) Development and current functioning in adolescents with Asperger syndrome: a comparative study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 42: 227–240. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Goldstein G, Minshew NJ, Allen DN, Seaton BE (2002) High-functioning autism and schizophrenia: a comparison of an early and late onset neurodevelopmental disorder. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 17: 461–475. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Holdnack J, Goldstein G, Drozdick L (2011) Social Perception and WAIS-IV Performance in Adolescents and Adults Diagnosed With Asperger’s Syndrome and Autism. Assessment 18: 192–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Koyama T, Tachimori H, Osada H, Kurita H (2006) Cognitive and symptom profiles in high-functioning pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 36: 373–380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Koyama T, Tachimori H, Osada H, Takeda T, Kurita H (2007) Cognitive and symptom profiles in Asperger’s syndrome and high-functioning autism. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 61: 99–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Koyama T, Kamio Y, Inada N, Kurita H (2009) Sex differences in WISC-III profiles of children with high-functioning pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 39: 135–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Lincoln AJ, Courchesne E, Kilman BA, Elmasian R, Allen M (1988) A study of intellectual abilities in high-functioning people with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 18: 505–524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Lockyer L, Rutter M (1970) A five- to fifteen-year follow-up study of infantile psychosis: IV. Patterns of cognitive ability. Br J Soc Clin Psychol 9: 152–163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL (2003) Analysis of WISC-III, Stanford-Binet:IV, and academic achievement test scores in children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 33: 329–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL (2004) Similarities and differences in Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Third Edition (WISC-III) profiles: support for subtest analysis in clinical referrals. Clin Neuropsychol 18: 559–572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL (2008) WISC-IV and WIAT-II profiles in children with high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord 38: 428–439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Merchan-Naranjo J, Mayoral M, Rapado-Castro M, Llorente C, Boada L, et al.. (2011) Estimation of the Intelligence Quotient Using Wechsler Intelligence Scales in Children and Adolescents with Asperger Syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 72. Minshew NJ, Turner CA, Goldstein G (2005) The application of short forms of the Wechsler Intelligence scales in adults and children with high functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord 35: 45–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Narita T, Koga Y (1987) Neuropsychological assessment of childhood autism. Adv Biol Psychiatry 16: 156–170. [Google Scholar]
- 74. Noterdaeme M, Wriedt E, Hohne C (2010) Asperger’s syndrome and high-functioning autism: language, motor and cognitive profiles. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 19: 475–481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Nyden A, Billstedt E, Hjelmquist E, Gillberg C (2001) Neurocognitive stability in Asperger syndrome, ADHD, and reading and writing disorder: a pilot study. Dev Med Child Neurol 43: 165–171. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Ohta M (1987) Cognitive disorders of infantile autism: a study employing the WISC, spatial relationship conceptualization, and gesture imitations. J Autism Dev Disord 17: 45–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Rumsey JM, Hamburger SD (1988) Neuropsychological findings in high-functioning men with infantile autism, residual state. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 10: 201–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Spek AA, Scholte EM, van Berckelaer-Onnes IA (2008) Brief report: The use of WAIS-III in adults with HFA and Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 38: 782–787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Tymchuk AJ, Simmons JQ, Neafsey S (1977) Intellectual characteristics of adolescent childhood psychotics with high verbal ability. J Ment Defic Res 21: 133–138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Williams DL, Goldstein G, Minshew NJ (2006) The profile of memory function in children with autism. Neuropsychology 20: 21–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]