Abstract
Sensing the chemical warnings present in the environment is essential for species survival. In mammals, this form of danger communication occurs via the release of natural predator scents that can involuntarily warn the prey or by the production of alarm pheromones by the stressed prey alerting its conspecifics. Although we previously identified the olfactory Grueneberg ganglion as the sensory organ through which mammalian alarm pheromones signal a threatening situation, the chemical nature of these cues remains elusive. We here identify, through chemical analysis in combination with a series of physiological and behavioral tests, the chemical structure of a mouse alarm pheromone. To successfully recognize the volatile cues that signal danger, we based our selection on their activation of the mouse olfactory Grueneberg ganglion and the concomitant display of innate fear reactions. Interestingly, we found that the chemical structure of the identified mouse alarm pheromone has similar features as the sulfur-containing volatiles that are released by predating carnivores. Our findings thus not only reveal a chemical Leitmotiv that underlies signaling of fear, but also point to a double role for the olfactory Grueneberg ganglion in intraspecies as well as interspecies communication of danger.
Keywords: olfaction, animal communication, behavior, calcium imaging
In their continuous struggle for survival, predator and prey species interact to maximize the likelihood of finding food or avoiding being eaten. Preys have adapted to deal with dangerous encounters using specialized sensory systems. Detection of auditory signals, visual signs, and olfactory messages induces elaborate and innate survival behaviors in the prey, which can either hide, fight, or flee (1). Multiple hiding strategies (crypsis) coexist and are best when combined. Indeed, crypsis can be morphological when the prey uses its body shape/color to blend into the background (camouflage), it can be behavioral when the prey uses immobility or subtle steady movements to decrease the chance of being detected (freezing); or it can be chemical when the prey releases odorant molecules to mask its own odor (2, 3).
The olfactory messages communicating the presence of a danger to the prey are the scents naturally released in the environment by the predators, named kairomones (4, 5), and the alarm pheromones (APs) secreted by threatened or injured conspecifics (6–11). Intraspecies communication by APs is an evolutionarily widespread phenomenon, presumably occurring in all animal phyla. Social species of fish, insects, and mammals use AP secretion as an altruistic signal to protect their colony/group or family when in danger. These alert cues may derive from compounds that evolved to make the flesh unpalatable or toxic to predators. Their primary function could have been the control of skin pathogens (12, 13). In insects and fish, APs of variable chemical structures such as terpenes, hydrocarbons, ketones, or nitrogen/sulfated heterocyclic compounds have been identified (12–16). In mammals, the chemical structure of APs is still unknown but, to fulfill their sensory warning role, they should be volatile, hydrophilic, and short-lived molecules (17, 18). Their identification will be possible in rodents, for example, as they can be collected as a blend from the environment of a stressed animal and directly tested on a conspecific by physiological or behavioral experiments (17, 19–21).
Using CO2 euthanasia known to cause a major alarm condition (21, 22), we previously collected mouse APs as a blend and identified the sensory system involved in their detection (21). We showed that the blend of mouse APs induces calcium transients specifically in the neurons of the Grueneberg ganglion (GG), an olfactory subsystem present at the tip of the nose, close to the opening of the naris (21, 23–27). The mouse GG is an arrow-shaped neuronal structure 750–1,000 µm in length. It lines both sides of the nasal septum and comprises 300–500 cells. Each cell sends out a single axon, and axons fasciculate immediately as they project caudally along the dorsal roof of the nasal cavity to the necklace complex in the olfactory bulb (24–26, 28, 29). The GG displays multimodal properties (21, 30–33) and starts developing around embryonic day 16. Contrary to the other olfactory subsystems, the GG appears to be complete and functional at birth (24–28), ensuring immediate AP sensing and increasing chances of survival in the wild. An intact GG is necessary to observe the fearful behavior of the recipient animals in the presence of APs (21).
In this study, we collect and chemically identify volatile mouse pheromones by a solid-phase microextraction followed by a gas chromatography and mass spectrometry method. We further assess their alarm sensory function by calcium imaging and behavioral experiments leading to the identification of a mouse alarm pheromone, 2-sec-butyl-4,5-dihydrothiazole. This alarm cue produced by both male and female mice under different alarm conditions resembles the sulfur-containing volatiles present in predator scents. Interestingly, this shared chemical feature that signals danger is specifically deciphered by the olfactory Grueneberg ganglion.
Results
Chemical Extraction of Mouse Volatiles.
We used solid-phase microextraction (SPME) fibers to adsorb the volatile mouse APs released under a CO2 alarm condition. After extraction, these molecules were analyzed by gas chromatography coupled to mass spectrometry (GC/MS) (Fig. 1A and Fig. S1). A total of 44 mouse volatiles were detected (Fig. 1B) among which we found pheromones important for social interactions such as 2-heptanone (34) or α- and β-farnesene (35) (Fig. S2). Thirty-two of these 44 volatile chemical cues were also emitted by control mice (under a nonalarm condition). Of the 12 chemicals appearing only under alarm condition (Fig. 1B, in red), 8 were of transient nature, thereby fulfilling an important chemical criterion of volatile alarm cues. They could thus be considered as AP candidates. Among these eight AP candidates, we found five compounds listed in the chemical libraries—namely, 1,2-propanediol, butyric acid (36) (BA), pentanoic acid, dehydro-exo-brevicomin (37) (DHB) (Fig. S2), and 2-sec-butyl-4,5-dihydrothiazole (37) (SBT) (Fig. S3 A and B)—and three unlisted chemicals. The identified chemicals were then purchased if commercially available or synthesized in house (Fig. S3C) to test by calcium imaging whether they could activate mouse GG neurons.
Fig. 1.
Chemical identification of candidate mouse alarm pheromones. (A) Representative total ion chromatogram (TIC) of volatiles emitted by mice under alarm conditions obtained by SPME-GC/MS. Chemical formulas of a selection of volatiles emitted by mice (*) or background volatiles are indicated above peaks. A mix of five adult male and female (3:2) mice under lethal CO2 stress was used for this experiment. (B) Forty-four mouse volatiles were detected in our experimental conditions. The volatiles are listed according to their retention time (RT). Both the chemical nomenclature and the usual names (#) are mentioned. The names of the compounds detected but not found in the Wiley7N or the NIST08 libraries are listed as “Unidentified compound.” Red type indicates the 12 chemicals appearing only under alarm conditions. Short-lived molecules (compounds not generated 30 min after the death of the mice) are indicated by “†.”
Identification of a Mouse Alarm Pheromone.
We performed calcium imaging experiments on GG coronal slices (21) of a particular gene-targeted mouse strain called OMP-GFP (olfactory marker protein-green fluorescent protein) in which all olfactory neurons express GFP as a reporter gene (38, 39). Tissue slices were incubated in the calcium-sensitive dye Fura-2 acetoxymethyl ester. GG neurons were identified by the intrinsic green fluorescence of GFP in their cell bodies and by their specific morphology (Fig. 2A). The uptake of the dye was confirmed by fluorescence measurements (Fig. 2B). Candidate alarm cues were delivered in oxycarbonated artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) on the tissue slices in the imaging chamber (Fig. 2C). Four of five AP candidates did not activate GG neurons (1,2-propanediol, n = 0/28 neurons tested; BA, n = 0/19; pentanoic acid, n = 0/27; DHB, n = 0/53). Perfusion of SBT, on the other hand, induced in GG neurons, in both female and male mice tissue slice preparations, reversible and reproducible calcium transients (n = 36/50) (Fig. 2 C and D; Fig. S4 A–C). In a separate set of experiments, we could verify that SBT activated the same GG neurons as the blend of mouse APs (n = 9/9) (Fig. S4 D and E). Calcium responses were observed at different concentrations of SBT (Fig. 2E), allowing the estimation of an EC50 value in the micromolar range (n = 7) (Fig. 2F). Interestingly, SBT was produced by mice undergoing different alarm conditions: a lethal CO2 stress, a confinement stress, or a cold temperature stress (40–42) (Fig. S5 A–C). The estimated SBT concentration released during these different alarm conditions was also in the micromolar range (Fig. S5C). In addition, we find SBT to be released under alarm conditions by both male and female mice (Fig. S5 D–H). Together, these data suggested that SBT could be a candidate molecule for carrying the danger signal.
Fig. 2.
GG neurons are activated by alarm pheromones and predator kairomones that share a common chemical structure. (A) GG coronal slice from an OMP-GFP mouse where GG neurons can be observed with their intrinsic GFP fluorescence. (B) Uptake of Fura-2AM into GG neurons measured at 380 nm in color-coded map for unbound Fura before and at the peak of an intracellular calcium increase induced by a control pulse of KCl (20 mM). (C) SBT (10−4 M) induced a reversible calcium transient in GG neurons. The other identified volatile chemical cues emitted by mice 1,2-propanediol, BA, pentanoic acid, DHB, and octanoic acid (used as a long-lasting volatile control) perfused at the same concentration had no effect. (D) SBT emerged as the best candidate alarm pheromone from the normalized responses to KCl (20 mM). (E) SBT responses were observed over a broad range of concentrations (shown here from 10−4 to 10−7 M). (F) Dose–response representation of the neuronal percentage of activity for SBT; EC50 value for SBT calculated from the Hill equation: 3.0 ± 2.3 ⋅10−6 M (n = 7). (G) Exposure to SBT and a blend of APs and TMT, a component of fox feces, induces systemic stress responses measured by the elevation of the plasma corticosterone level normalized to the control ACSF solution (62.8 ± 7.3 ng/mL). BA, used here as an aversive control substance, did not induce any hormonal increase. Error bars: SEM of n = 6–15 observations; *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001; ns, not significant. (H) Representative calcium transients induced by five volatile predator kairomones (TMT; 2,6-DMP; 1,3-DT; TS; and 2-PT; here shown at 10−4 M) in mouse GG neurons from male and female mice. Corresponding chemical formulas are indicated below the names of the compounds. Fura-2 ratio is indicated with arbitrary units (a.u.). Perfusion times are indicated by horizontal bars. (Scale bars, 20 μm.)
We tested this hypothesis by exposing adult male and female mice to this putative alarm cue and found that it induced a significant elevation of their plasma corticosterone levels, indicating a systemic stress response (43) (Fig. 2G). Indeed, SBT alone could fully reproduce, in both genders (Fig. S6), the systemic stress response that was also generated by the blend of APs collected under the alarm condition (Fig. 1A). Thus, from these experiments, SBT emerged as our best candidate mouse alarm pheromone.
Alarm Pheromones and Predator Scents Sharing a Common Chemical Structure Activate Grueneberg Ganglion Neurons.
Interestingly, SBT shares its chemical structure with a different class of odorant molecules also involved in danger communication: the molecules found in rodent predator scents (44–46) (Fig. 2 C and H). These molecules are often the only warning a prey has that a predator is nearby. They include a class of heterocyclic sulfur or nitrogen-containing compounds generated by meat digestion (47). To these belongs 2,4,5-trimethylthiazoline (TMT), a component of fox (Vulpes vulpes) feces (48) that is well known for its fear-inducing properties (49, 50), (Fig. 2G).
Using calcium imaging, we found that TMT also induced an intracellular calcium increase in GG neurons of both female and male mice (n = 33/34; Fig. 2H). We further tested other representatives of this class of molecules at 10−3 to 10−9 M and found that each one activated GG neurons. Among these were dimethylpyrazines (DMP) [2,3-DMP (n = 39/39) or 2,6-DMP (n = 39/40)], dithiolanes [1,3-dithiolane (1,3-DT) (n = 18/19)], thietanes [trimethylene sulfide (TS) (n = 17/22)] and 2-propylthietane (2-PT; n = 30/30), which is secreted by the anal glands of a mouse predator, the stoat (Mustela erminea) (Fig. 2H). Thus, volatile danger cues, which share similar chemical structures released either by predators (kairomones) or by a conspecific (APs), activate GG neurons.
Olfactory Subsystem Specificity of Chemical Danger Cues.
Most of these GG ligands have, for humans, a strong and aversive smell. In the mouse, odorants as well as pheromones are often recognized by more than one olfactory subsystem (51) (Fig. 3A). To verify if this applied for SBT, TMT, and 2-PT, we perfused these substances on the neurons of the different subsystems of the mouse—the main olfactory epithelium (MOE) and the vomeronasal organ (VNO), in addition to neurons of the GG (Fig. 3 B–E). We found that SBT, TMT, and 2-PT could be detected by MOE and VNO neurons but that the GG comprised the highest proportion of responsive neurons. Indeed, over 70% of the GG neurons responded to these alert cues, suggesting that this olfactory subsystem might play a role not only in the detection of these odorant molecules but also, more importantly, in the perception of the danger quality of these substances (Fig. 3B).
Fig. 3.
Chemical danger cues show an olfactory subsystem specificity. (A) Schematic representation of a mouse head with the localization of the different olfactory subsystems (main olfactory epithelium, MOE; vomeronasal organ, VNO; and Grueneberg ganglion, GG). (B) Comparative table of neuronal activation showing strong olfactory subsystem dependence. SBT, TMT, and 2-PT (10−4 M) activated 72%, 97%, and 100% of the tested GG neurons, respectively (n = 114). The same cues activated 7%, 12%, and 4% of MOE neurons, respectively (n = 124) and 5%, 2%, and 1% of VNO neurons, respectively (n = 153). The aversive odorant BA (10−4 M), used here as a control, induced calcium transients in 15% of MOE neurons (n = 124) but did not activate GG neurons (n = 19) or VNO neurons (n = 78). Decreasing the concentration of SBT perfused to 10−6 M still activated 2% of MOE neurons and 57% of GG neurons. (C) Six representative GG neurons (colored and numbered circles), observed after the uptake of Fura-2 AM, were tested for activation. The different danger cues (SBT, TMT, and 2-PT) activated the majority of GG neurons tested whereas BA did not. Similar investigations as in C were performed on MOE (D) and VNO (E). Fura-2 ratio is indicated with arbitrary units (a.u.). Perfusion times are indicated by horizontal bars. (Scale bars, 20 μm.)
Danger Quality of a Chemical Cue Is Deciphered by a Functional Grueneberg Ganglion.
The axonal projection bundles of GG neurons reach the olfactory bulb of the brain in a phosphodiesterase (PDE2A)-positive necklace glomerular region (NG) (29) (Fig. S7 A and B). We found, by c-Fos labeling, that mice exposed to SBT or to the predator cue TMT (10−3 or 10−7 M) have their NG-associated mitral/tufted and granule cells activated (Fig. S7 C–E). Thus, these chemical cues might transfer their danger signal to the brain via GG axonal afferences. These GG projection bundles can be sectioned (axotomy), inducing the complete degeneration of the ganglion and the generation of axotomized (Axo) mice (21, 25). Typical freezing reactions as well as a decrease in walking distance have been observed previously in the presence of danger cues (APs and predator kairomones) (10, 20, 21, 50). To test the behavioral relevance of the specific GG recognition of chemical danger cues, we ran a set of experiments scoring the freezing time, the walking distance, and the risk assessment episodes performed by mice with or without a functional GG (control versus Axo mice) (Fig. 4A and Fig. S8).
Fig. 4.
Detection of chemical danger cues depends on a functional GG. (A) Behavioral experiments involving control (Ctrl; n = 6) and axotomized (Axo; n = 6) mice exposed successively for 5 min to a chemical cue (SBT, TMT, 2-PT, BA) and to ACSF deposited on a blotting paper (blue square). The percentage of freezing time (%) and the walking distance (m) show that SBT as well as TMT and 2-PT, but not BA, induced a stereotyped fear behavior abolished after axotomy. On the other hand, the scoring of risk assessment episodes was not affected by axotomy. (B) Mice phenotyping. Ventral whole-mount of the nasal cavities of a Ctrl (Left) and of an Axo (Right) OMP-GFP mouse (fluorescent view). The GG (Lower: white rectangles enlarged) and MOE regions are shown. (C) The general olfactory function was not affected by axotomy. No difference was observed between the two phenotypes in the latency to find a buried Oreo cookie in the bedding over 3 consecutive days. Ctrl mice (gray bars) and Axo mice (black bars). Mean ± SEM of n = 6, *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001; ns, not significant. (Scale bars, 0.25 mm.)
We found that the candidate alarm pheromone, SBT, as well as the predator kairomones, TMT and 2-PT, induced stereotypical fear reactions in control mice (Fig. 4A). SBT can therefore be considered as a mouse alarm pheromone. After GG axonal lesions (Axo mice) (Fig. 4B), the general odorant detection of the mice was not affected as no difference was observed in their ability to localize a hidden Oreo cookie (21) (Fig. 4C). Moreover, in these Axo mice, the presence of SBT, TMT, or 2-PT induced a high score of risk assessment episodes, confirming that the general detection of these odorants via the MOE and the VNO was indeed conserved (Fig. 3B). On the other hand, the fear-induced behaviors were replaced by exploratory activity (Fig. 4A). A functional GG is therefore necessary to perceive the danger-signaling properties of these chemically related substances.
Discussion
The olfactory system provides sensory information about the chemical composition of the external world. It fulfills a variety of tasks including locating and evaluating food, reproduction, social interactions, and danger avoidance (51). To escape predation, rodents are able to sense olfactory cues of very diverse chemical structures that are emitted by their predators. Related probably to this structural diversity and to the fundamental role of their detection for survival, these cues are recognized by morphologically and molecularly distinct subsystems within the nasal cavity. This multiple olfactory detection ability represents an evolutionary advantage for rodents. The volatile 2-phenylethylamine, a component of carnivore urine, is, for example, recognized in the mouse by the trace amine-associated receptor TAAR4 expressed in the neurons of the MOE (45). On the other hand, the nonvolatile major urinary protein family from predators is recognized by the neurons of the VNO (44). Here, we show that the class of volatile heterocyclic sulfur- or nitrogen-containing compounds generated by meat digestion is specifically recognized by the neurons of the Grueneberg ganglion. This result supports previously published observations reporting that TMT detection still occurred in mice where the main VNO-signaling element, the transient receptor potential channels type 2 (TRPC2) (44), or the key MOE canonical component, the cyclic nucleotide-gated channels A2 (CNGA2) (52), have been genetically deleted. Moreover, TMT detection was also reported to be unaffected by a zinc sulfate treatment inducing a transient anosmia (53).
The potential pathway(s) implicated in the detection of chemical danger cues by GG neurons remain(s) to be identified. Putative signaling elements have already been described (21, 31, 54–59), suggesting that GG neurons might use different pathways from those present in MOE or VNO neurons. Some proteins identified have a temporal pattern of expression and/or a restricted localization implying the presence of neuronal subpopulations in the GG. From our c-Fos labeling of the olfactory bulb, we can speculate that the GG neurons activated by alarm pheromones comprise the substantial subpopulation of PDE2A-expressing neurons. Interestingly, these PDE2A-positive neurons also express in their primary cilia the particulate guanylyl cyclase pGC-G and the CNGA3 ion channels (54, 57). These two membrane-bound proteins could play a role in danger cue detection. Further experiments will still be necessary to sort out and understand the GG transduction machinery, which might also involve different ions such as calcium or sodium.
The chemical danger cues activate a hard-wired circuitry to trigger innate fear behaviors (50, 60–62). Previous reports have shown that GG neurons send their axons to glomeruli in the dorsal zone of the olfactory bulb (24–26, 28, 29), a region also shown by genetic deletion experiments to trigger innate fear behaviors such as freezing (50). Freezing is one of the hiding strategies of the prey (behavioral crypsis) that can be considered as an innate survival behavior (1). Another form of antipredator adaptation also involves olfactory emissions (chemical crypsis) (3). We might here speculate that, in the presence of a predator, the production of alarm pheromones, such as SBT (63), not only signals danger to conspecifics but also induces freezing and might also modify the prey scent, which could fool the predator by mimicry of its own scent (2, 3).
In mammals, more than one chemical molecule probably acts as an alarm pheromone to communicate danger (64). For identifying mouse alarm pheromones, we chose a SPME fiber because of its high sensitivity to volatile chemicals. Unfortunately, probably not all volatile cues were trapped by this fiber due to their particular physico-chemical affinities. During our chemical procedure, we also found compounds not referenced in the chemical libraries. They deserve further investigations to verify whether they share similar chemical features and contribute to the danger message.
From our chemical, physiological, and behavioral screenings, SBT came out as a mouse alarm pheromone. It was produced under different alarm conditions by both female and male mice of different ages. It acted as a danger signal for mice of both genders, inducing intracellular calcium increases in APs sensing GG neurons. SBT activated the associated mitral/tufted and granule cells in the olfactory bulb. Its exposure led to an increase in plasma corticosterone and to the subsequent display of innate fear-related behaviors. SBT has been previously shown to induce intermale aggression in mice in association with the pheromone DHB (37). This behavior was observed only when the synthetic SBT was added to urine. SBT could therefore be released in the environment as a volatile chemical and as a compound linked to the mouse major urinary proteins (Mups). We showed that the volatile SBT signals a dangerous situation to a conspecific by initiating responses in GG neurons without association with mouse Mups whereas the Mups-linked SBT gives information as a long-lasting message in the urine for sex-related indications that will be detected by VNO neurons (65, 66).
The ability to detect the short-lived molecules encoding danger is essential for species survival. Molecules possessing the same chemical signature (heterocyclic sulfur or nitrogen-containing compounds) are emitted by predators (kairomones) and by stressed conspecifics (alarm pheromones); they induce similar innate fear reactions. In summary, our study shows that the chemical structure of danger cues refers to their specific coding quality and is deciphered by the Grueneberg ganglion neurons. It points to a double role for this olfactory subsystem in intraspecies as well as interspecies communication of danger. Our results provide further insight into how organisms chemically communicate and detect the presence of a danger to improve the general overall fitness of the species.
Materials and Methods
A detailed description of materials and methods is given in SI Materials and Methods. Briefly, male and female C57BL/6J and OMP-GFP gene-targeted mice were used for the experiments. SPME and GC/MS methods were used to collect and analyze volatile compounds. For calcium imaging, acute olfactory tissue slices were generated with a vibratome in cold oxygenated ACSF. Fura-2AM was used as a calcium indicator to record cellular activations after chemostimulation. For behavioral sessions, the risk assessment, the freezing duration, and the walking distance were evaluated as well as the general olfactory function of the mice. For statistical analyses (mean ± SEM), unilateral Student’s unpaired/paired t tests were performed for comparisons. Significance levels are indicated as follows: *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001; ns, not significant.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Ivan Rodriguez for the OMP-GFP mice; Roger Marti for the synthesis of mouse pheromonal ligands and organic chemistry advice; the Cellular Imaging Facility of the University of Lausanne and its coordinator, Jean-Yves Chatton; Pranvera Zenuni and Omar Alijevic for technical assistance; and Jacques Diezi, Kaethi Geering, Bernard Rossier, and Ron Stoop for fruitful discussions on the manuscript. This work is supported by the Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, University of Lausanne, and by Swiss National Foundation Grant FNS 3100A0-125192 (to M.-C.B.).
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
*This Direct Submission article had a prearranged editor.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1214249110/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- 1.Kavaliers M, Choleris E. Antipredator responses and defensive behavior: Ecological and ethological approaches for the neurosciences. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2001;25(7–8):577–586. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00042-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.De Moraes CM, Mescher MC. Biochemical crypsis in the avoidance of natural enemies by an insect herbivore. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101(24):8993–8997. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0403248101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Conover MR. Predator-Prey Dynamics: The Role of Olfaction. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kats LB. Chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey animals. Ecoscience. 1998;5(3):361–394. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Apfelbach R, Blanchard CD, Blanchard RJ, Hayes RA, McGregor IS. The effects of predator odors in mammalian prey species: A review of field and laboratory studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29(8):1123–1144. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Howe NR, Sheikh YM. Anthopleurine: A sea anemone alarm pheromone. Science. 1975;189(4200):386–388. doi: 10.1126/science.238286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Stowe MK, Turlings TC, Loughrin JH, Lewis WJ, Tumlinson JH. The chemistry of eavesdropping, alarm, and deceit. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1995;92(1):23–28. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.1.23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Beale MH, et al. Aphid alarm pheromone produced by transgenic plants affects aphid and parasitoid behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103(27):10509–10513. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0603998103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bowers WS, Nault LR, Webb RE, Dutky SR. Aphid alarm pheromone: Isolation, identification, synthesis. Science. 1972;177(4054):1121–1122. doi: 10.1126/science.177.4054.1121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gutiérrez-García AG, Contreras CM, Mendoza-López MR, García-Barradas O, Cruz-Sánchez JS. Urine from stressed rats increases immobility in receptor rats forced to swim: Role of 2-heptanone. Physiol Behav. 2007;91(1):166–172. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Boissy A, Terlouw C, Le Neindre P. Presence of cues from stressed conspecifics increases reactivity to aversive events in cattle: Evidence for the existence of alarm substances in urine. Physiol Behav. 1998;63(4):489–495. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00466-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Wyatt TD. Pheromones and Animal Behaviour: Communication by Smell and Taste. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Mathuru AS, et al. Chondroitin fragments are odorants that trigger fear behavior in fish. Curr Biol. 2012;22(6):538–544. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Brown GE, Adrian JC, Jr, Smyth E, Leet H, Brennan S. Ostariophysan alarm pheromones: Laboratory and field tests of the functional significance of nitrogen oxides. J Chem Ecol. 2000;26(1):139–154. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Chapman RF. 1998. The Insects: Structure and Function (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, UK), 4th Ed. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Vander Meer RK, Preston CA, Choi MY. Isolation of a pyrazine alarm pheromone component from the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta. J Chem Ecol. 2010;36(2):163–170. doi: 10.1007/s10886-010-9743-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kiyokawa Y, Kikusui T, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y. Alarm pheromone that aggravates stress-induced hyperthermia is soluble in water. Chem Senses. 2005;30(6):513–519. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bji044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Inagaki H, et al. The volatility of an alarm pheromone in male rats. Physiol Behav. 2009;96(4–5):749–752. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Rottman SJ, Snowdon CT. Demonstration and analysis of an alarm pheromone in mice. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1972;81(3):483–490. doi: 10.1037/h0033703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Zalaquett C, Thiessen D. The effects of odors from stressed mice on conspecific behavior. Physiol Behav. 1991;50(1):221–227. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(91)90524-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Brechbühl J, Klaey M, Broillet MC. Grueneberg ganglion cells mediate alarm pheromone detection in mice. Science. 2008;321(5892):1092–1095. doi: 10.1126/science.1160770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Leach MC, Bowell VA, Allan TF, Morton DB. Aversion to gaseous euthanasia agents in rats and mice. Comp Med. 2002;52(3):249–257. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Grüneberg H. A ganglion probably belonging to the N. terminalis system in the nasal mucosa of the mouse. Z Anat Entwicklungsgesch. 1973;140(1):39–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Koos DS, Fraser SE. The Grueneberg ganglion projects to the olfactory bulb. Neuroreport. 2005;16(17):1929–1932. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000186597.72081.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Roppolo D, Ribaud V, Jungo VP, Lüscher C, Rodriguez I. Projection of the Grüneberg ganglion to the mouse olfactory bulb. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;23(11):2887–2894. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04818.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Storan MJ, Key B. Septal organ of Grüneberg is part of the olfactory system. J Comp Neurol. 2006;494(5):834–844. doi: 10.1002/cne.20858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Fleischer J, Hass N, Schwarzenbacher K, Besser S, Breer H. A novel population of neuronal cells expressing the olfactory marker protein (OMP) in the anterior/dorsal region of the nasal cavity. Histochem Cell Biol. 2006;125(4):337–349. doi: 10.1007/s00418-005-0077-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Fuss SH, Omura M, Mombaerts P. The Grueneberg ganglion of the mouse projects axons to glomeruli in the olfactory bulb. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22(10):2649–2654. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04468.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Matsuo T, Rossier DA, Kan C, Rodriguez I. The wiring of Grueneberg ganglion axons is dependent on neuropilin 1. Development. 2012;139(15):2783–2791. doi: 10.1242/dev.077008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mamasuew K, Breer H, Fleischer J. Grueneberg ganglion neurons respond to cool ambient temperatures. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(9):1775–1785. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06465.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Schmid A, Pyrski M, Biel M, Leinders-Zufall T, Zufall F. Grueneberg ganglion neurons are finely tuned cold sensors. J Neurosci. 2010;30(22):7563–7568. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0608-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Mamasuew K, Hofmann N, Breer H, Fleischer J. Grueneberg ganglion neurons are activated by a defined set of odorants. Chem Senses. 2011;36(3):271–282. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjq124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Mamasuew K, et al. Chemo- and thermosensory responsiveness of Grueneberg ganglion neurons relies on cyclic guanosine monophosphate signaling elements. Neurosignals. 2011;19(4):198–209. doi: 10.1159/000329333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Jemiolo B, Andreolini F, Xie TM, Wiesler D, Novotny M. Puberty-affecting synthetic analogs of urinary chemosignals in the house mouse, Mus domesticus. Physiol Behav. 1989;46(2):293–298. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90270-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Novotny M, Harvey S, Jemiolo B. Chemistry of male dominance in the house mouse, Mus domesticus. Experientia. 1990;46(1):109–113. doi: 10.1007/BF01955433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Imai A, Morishita K. Relationships of volatile components and bacterial flora in mouse caecum. Lab Anim. 1983;17(1):45–49. doi: 10.1258/002367783781070911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Novotny M, Harvey S, Jemiolo B, Alberts J. Synthetic pheromones that promote inter-male aggression in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1985;82(7):2059–2061. doi: 10.1073/pnas.82.7.2059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Mombaerts P, et al. Visualizing an olfactory sensory map. Cell. 1996;87(4):675–686. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81387-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Potter SM, et al. Structure and emergence of specific olfactory glomeruli in the mouse. J Neurosci. 2001;21(24):9713–9723. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-24-09713.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Retana-Márquez S, Bonilla-Jaime H, Vázquez-Palacios G, Martínez-García R, Velázquez-Moctezuma J. Changes in masculine sexual behavior, corticosterone and testosterone in response to acute and chronic stress in male rats. Horm Behav. 2003;44(4):327–337. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Krishnamurthy S, Garabadu D, Reddy NR, Joy KP. Risperidone in ultra low dose protects against stress in the rodent cold restraint model by modulating stress pathways. Neurochem Res. 2011;36(10):1750–1758. doi: 10.1007/s11064-011-0490-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Grauer E, Alkalai D, Kapon J, Cohen G, Raveh L. Stress does not enable pyridostigmine to inhibit brain cholinesterase after parenteral administration. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 2000;164(3):301–304. doi: 10.1006/taap.2000.8906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Thomas RM, Urban JH, Peterson DA. Acute exposure to predator odor elicits a robust increase in corticosterone and a decrease in activity without altering proliferation in the adult rat hippocampus. Exp Neurol. 2006;201(2):308–315. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2006.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Papes F, Logan DW, Stowers L. The vomeronasal organ mediates interspecies defensive behaviors through detection of protein pheromone homologs. Cell. 2010;141(4):692–703. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2010.03.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ferrero DM, et al. Detection and avoidance of a carnivore odor by prey. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(27):11235–11240. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1103317108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Isogai Y, et al. Molecular organization of vomeronasal chemoreception. Nature. 2011;478(7368):241–245. doi: 10.1038/nature10437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Nolte DL, Mason JR, Epple G, Aronov E, Campbell DL. Why are predator urines aversive to prey? J Chem Ecol. 1994;20(7):1505–1516. doi: 10.1007/BF02059876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Vernet-Maury E, Polak E, Demael A. Structure-activity relationship of stress-inducing odorants in the rat. J Chem Ecol. 1984;10(7):1007–1018. doi: 10.1007/BF00987509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Wallace KJ, Rosen JB. Predator odor as an unconditioned fear stimulus in rats: Elicitation of freezing by trimethylthiazoline, a component of fox feces. Behav Neurosci. 2000;114(5):912–922. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.5.912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kobayakawa K, et al. Innate versus learned odour processing in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nature. 2007;450(7169):503–508. doi: 10.1038/nature06281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Munger SD, Leinders-Zufall T, Zufall F. Subsystem organization of the mammalian sense of smell. Annu Rev Physiol. 2009;71:115–140. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.70.113006.100608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Bepari AK, Watanabe K, Yamaguchi M, Tamamaki N, Hirohide T. Visualization of odor-induced neuronal activity by immediate early gene expression. BMC Neurosci. 2012;13(1):140. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Galliot E, Laurent L, Hacquemand R, Pourié G, Millot JL. Fear-like behavioral responses in mice in different odorant environments: Trigeminal versus olfactory mediation under low doses. Behav Processes. 2012;90(2):161–166. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2012.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Fleischer J, Mamasuew K, Breer H. Expression of cGMP signaling elements in the Grueneberg ganglion. Histochem Cell Biol. 2009;131(1):75–88. doi: 10.1007/s00418-008-0514-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Fleischer J, Schwarzenbacher K, Besser S, Hass N, Breer H. Olfactory receptors and signalling elements in the Grueneberg ganglion. J Neurochem. 2006;98(2):543–554. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.03894.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Fleischer J, Schwarzenbacher K, Breer H. Expression of trace amine-associated receptors in the Grueneberg ganglion. Chem Senses. 2007;32(6):623–631. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjm032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Liu CY, Fraser SE, Koos DS. Grueneberg ganglion olfactory subsystem employs a cGMP signaling pathway. J Comp Neurol. 2009;516(1):36–48. doi: 10.1002/cne.22096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Pyrski M, et al. Sodium/calcium exchanger expression in the mouse and rat olfactory systems. J Comp Neurol. 2007;501(6):944–958. doi: 10.1002/cne.21290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Liu CY, Xiao C, Fraser SE, Lester HA, Koos DS. Electrophysiological characterization of Grueneberg ganglion olfactory neurons: Spontaneous firing, sodium conductance, and hyperpolarization-activated currents. J Neurophysiol. 2012;108(5):1318–1334. doi: 10.1152/jn.00907.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Matsumoto H, et al. Spatial arrangement of glomerular molecular-feature clusters in the odorant-receptor class domains of the mouse olfactory bulb. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103(6):3490–3500. doi: 10.1152/jn.00035.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Johnson MA, et al. Neurons expressing trace amine-associated receptors project to discrete glomeruli and constitute an olfactory subsystem. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109(33):13410–13415. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206724109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Pacifico R, Dewan A, Cawley D, Guo C, Bozza T. 2012. An olfactory subsystem that mediates high-sensitivity detection of volatile amines. Cell Rep 2(1):76–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 63.Zhang J, Sun L, Bruce K, Novotny M. Chronic exposure of cat odor enhances aggression, urinary attractiveness and sex pheromones of mice. J Ethol. 2007;26(2):279–286. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Kiyokawa Y, Kikusui T, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y. Alarm pheromones with different functions are released from different regions of the body surface of male rats. Chem Senses. 2004;29(1):35–40. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjh004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Cavaggioni A, Mucignat-Caretta C, Zagotto G. Absolute configuration of 2-sec-butyl-4,5-dihydrothiazole in male mouse urine. Chem Senses. 2003;28(9):791–797. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjg073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Cavaggioni A, Mucignat-Caretta C, Redaelli M, Zagotto G. The scent of urine spots of male mice, Mus musculus: Changes in chemical composition over time. Rapid Commun Mass Spectrom. 2006;20(24):3741–3746. doi: 10.1002/rcm.2789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.