Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Mar 26.
Published in final edited form as: J Cogn Dev. 2012 Feb 9;13(1):82–110. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2011.573516

Sources of Cognitive Inflexibility in Set-Shifting Tasks: Insights Into Developmental Theories From Adult Data

Anthony Steven Dick 1
PMCID: PMC3608105  NIHMSID: NIHMS362031  PMID: 23539267

Abstract

Two experiments examined processes underlying cognitive inflexibility in set-shifting tasks typically used to assess the development of executive function in children. Adult participants performed a Flexible Item Selection Task (FIST) that requires shifting from categorizing by one dimension (e.g., color) to categorizing by a second orthogonal dimension (e.g., shape). The experiments showed performance of the FIST involves suppression of the representation of the ignored dimension; response times for selecting a target object in an immediately-following oddity task were slower when the oddity target was the previously-ignored stimulus of the FIST. However, proactive interference from the previously relevant stimulus dimension also impaired responding. The results are discussed with respect to two prominent theories of the source of difficulty for children and adults on dimensional shifting tasks: attentional inertia and negative priming. In contrast to prior work emphasizing one over the other process, the findings indicate that difficulty in the FIST, and by extension other set-shifting tasks, can be attributed to both the need to shift away from the previously attended representation (attentional inertia), and the need to shift to the previously ignored representation (negative priming). Results are discussed in relation to theoretical explanations for cognitive inflexibility in adults and children.


Set-shifting tasks are commonly used to index the development of cognitive flexibility in preschoolers (Cragg & Chevalier, 2009 for review). In one variant, the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS), children are explicitly told to sort test cards (e.g., blue bowls and red baskets) to target cards (e.g., blue baskets and red bowls) by one dimension (e.g., color) and then after a few trials switch to sort these same cards by another dimension (e.g., shape; Frye, Zelazo, & Palfai, 1995; Zelazo, 2006; Zelazo, Müller, Frye, & Marcovitch, 2003). Importantly, there is a high degree of conflict such that each test card can be matched to the target cards by two opposing dimensions. Even though preschoolers correctly answer questions about the rules of the task, and sort correctly by the initial rule, they err by continuing to sort on the initial dimension after the rule-change. Older preschoolers and adults are able to shift successfully to sort by the new rule. Another set-shifting task variant, the Flexible Item Selection Task (FIST; Jacques & Zelazo, 2001), is also a challenge for preschoolers and younger school-aged children (Dick, 2006; Jacques & Zelazo, 2001). This task is similar to the DCCS, but has the additional challenge in that children are not explicitly told the sorting rule but instead must generate it from the visual display. As opposed to the DCCS, which demands explicit rule following, the FIST further demands inductive inferences (Blaye & Jacques, 2009; Jacques & Zelazo, 2005). Yet, both tasks demand cognitive flexibility, which is the focus of the present study. Specifically, of issue in the present investigation are two theoretical accounts, attentional inertia and negative priming, for the source of difficulty on these seemingly simple set-shifting tasks.

The present study uses as a point of departure a finding by Diamond and Kirkham (2005) reporting that adults show a switch cost (i.e., slowed response time, RT; Monsell, 2003; Wylie & Allport, 2000) on a simple variant of the DCCS. They argued that both adults and children demonstrate “attentional inertia” in set-shifting tasks. On this account, people have difficulty shifting away from, or redirecting, pre-instantiated mind-sets. That is, adults and children lack the ability to redirect focus on previously relevant features of the object (e.g., the redness of the bowl) in cases when these become irrelevant (e.g., thinking of the shape of the bowl). Further, it is the development of top-down processes of inhibition that allow successful task switching (Diamond, Carlson, & Beck, 2005; Diamond & Kirkham, 2005; Kirkham, Cruess, & Diamond, 2003).

There is, however, a different explanation: adults and children may not be demonstrating a tendency to continue what they were doing (inertia), but instead may have difficulty returning to a dimension that was previously suppressed, a possibility which forms part of a negative priming interpretation (Müller, Dick, Gela, Overton, & Zelazo, 2006; Zelazo et al., 2003). In the visual attention literature, negative priming refers to the finding that when a distractor object is ignored on one trial (the prime trial), responses to this object on the subsequent trial (the probe trial) are impaired relative to a neutral control stimulus (Fox, 1995; May, Kane, & Hasher, 1995; Neill, 1977; Tipper, 1985). When applied to set-shifting tasks such as the DCCS or the FIST, a negative priming interpretation suggests a bottom-up process of interference in which the problem is one of engaging attention to the formerly irrelevant dimension or stimulus value. Thus, children and adults show a switch cost because they have difficulty returning to what had previously had been the distractor (e.g., the basket or bowl when color is relevant) when it becomes the target on the switch trial (e.g., the basket or bowl when shape is relevant). This interpretation is quite different from the inertia account since it argues that the source of difficulty is not in suppressing a now-irrelevant rule or association, but in returning to (or re-instantiating) a previously ignored rule or association. The implication, then, is that past representations are actively ignored rather than simply discarded and as a result directly affect the present problem-solving situation.

Developmental studies of the DCCS have been designed to adjudicate between these two theories. For example, in support of attentional inertia, Diamond and colleagues (Diamond et al., 2005; Kirkham et al., 2003) showed that manipulations that increase the saliency of the previously relevant stimulus features impair performance (e.g., leaving the test cards face up in the sorting box), while those that decrease the saliency of the previously relevant dimension improve performance (e.g., allowing the child to verbally label the relevant dimension following the rule switch, removing the target cards or replacing them with target puppets; hiding test cards in a sleeve, or physically separating the features of the test card; Kloo & Perner, 2005; Kray, Kipp, & Karbach, 2009; Perner & Lang, 2002; Towse, Redbond, Houston-Price, & Cook, 2000; see Müller, Zelazo, Lurye, & Lieberman, 2008 and Yerys & Munakata, 2006 for findings where labeling either fails to improve or negatively impacts performance). Three-year-old children also have difficulty on a Partial Change version of the DCCS, in which only relevant values of the preswitch phase are retained during the postswitch, but the majority of three-year-olds pass a Total Change version, in which the values of both pre-switch dimensions are changed (Zelazo et al., 2003). These findings together provide support for the idea that difficulty in the DCCS stems from the need to shift away from previously relevant aspects of the task situation. However, there is also evidence in support of negative priming at the stimulus level. Zelazo, Müller and colleagues (Müller et al., 2006; Zelazo et al., 2003) found that children still have difficulty even when the previously relevant values are removed in the postswitch so that it is impossible to direct attention to them (i.e., in this Negative Priming version children sort red and blue boats and rabbits by color in the preswitch phase, and then green and yellow boats and rabbits by shape in the postswitch phase). Notably, this only occurs in cases where conflict between the stimulus values is present in the preswitch or prime phase (Malley & Strayer, 1995; Müller et al., 2006; Experiment 2). Similar experiments by Chevalier and Blaye (2008) using a novel task-switch paradigm with three-year-olds have also pointed to difficulty in activating the previously ignored representation.

Although compelling, these developmental findings are theoretically inconclusinve: the findings are consistent with both attentional inertia and negative priming, and they are consistent with a third possibility that accepts both explanations, on which difficulty in these tasks might in some cases be codetermined by the need to both (a) shift away from previously relevant representations and (b) shift to previously ignored representations. Indeed, it is hard to find convincing evidence that one or the other process is at work because in all aforementioned studies the task design confounds both possibilities. In the standard task switching manipulations, children almost always either succeed or fail to shift dimensions, and thus it is impossible to simultaneously investigate the contribution of the two opposing processes in cases where both processes could be taking place. To illustrate, the fact that children fail the Negative Priming version of the DCCS supports negative priming, but does not preclude the possibility that attention to the previously relevant dimension is also a source of difficulty when the standard version is used. Similarly, verbal labeling of the relevant dimension, in addition to reducing inertia, could also help in shifting to the previously irrelevant dimension.

To summarize, the central question, for the two experiments to be reported here, is to what extent the difficulty in cognitive flexibility tasks can be attributed to the failure to suppress the previously relevant stimulus, or to failed activation of previously ignored information, or to both. We also assessed a corollary issue, whether negative priming occurs at the level of the stimulus values or at the level of the dimension. It is necessary to understand the sources of cognitive inflexibility, but absent in the literature is a developmentally relevant paradigm that can reveal the cognitive processes occurring during task switching. In this article, we developed a novel paradigm to overcome the limitations of prior developmental investigations. This choice reaction time paradigm measures response latency in a more sensitive parametric manner, and it includes a neutral control condition as a comparison. We start here with adults because they represent the end state of the target competence, and we can take advantage of the sensitivity of RT measures to investigate this competence. We also aimed to investigate other task-switching methodologies, as the majority of work in this area has concentrated on DCCS performance, and this task is only effective for assessing executive function development in the preschool period. As a consequence, we focused on the FIST because it has been shown to be valuable for assessing the development of cognitive flexibility across a more expansive age range using a single task methodology, from preschool (Jacques & Zelazo, 2001) through early-to-middle childhood (Dick, 2006). However, it is somewhat limited in explanatory value because no research to date has investigated factors that make the FIST difficult for children. The findings we present provide insight into the cognitive processes necessary for success or failure on these tasks, and at the same time establish a novel method for investigating sources of cognitive inflexibility in set-shifting tasks appropriate for use in children.

Paradigm and Summary of Experiments

To investigate processes underlying cognitive flexibility in dimensional shifting tasks, we chose to develop and validate a novel task paradigm beginning first with adult participants. Adults were tested on two tasks: the FIST (Jacques & Zelazo, 2001) and a dimension abstracted oddity task (Chalmers & Halford, 2003). Recall that in the FIST, participants typically make one selection about which objects match (i.e., they choose from three pictures those two matching on a dimension, for example, color), and then shift to make a second selection, which applies to the same stimuli but while focusing on a different dimension. In the present study, participants were only required to make the first selection. We then assessed the effect of the task by immediately following the FIST selection with an oddity task. In the oddity task, participants were presented with four objects, and determined the “odd” object that did not match the other three objects on a dimension. In this oddity trial a single stimulus unequivocally served as the target on the switch trial, negating the possibility of making the same selection on the FIST trial if that trial had been repeated.

The principle, then, was to use the RT measure in the oddity task as an index for processes occurring during FIST selection. The key manipulation was how the target in the oddity trial related to the preceding FIST trial features in terms of the selected dimension and stimulus values. Thus, referring to Figure 1A, if on the FIST trial the participant selects the two stimuli as matching based on color rather than shape, they are assumed to be “set” to color, and to have suppressed shape. We assume the constant or non-variable dimension (e.g., size) is neither attended nor suppressed.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Illustration of the paradigm. A. On a computer screen, a FIST trial was presented requiring participants choose two objects that match in a way that is not like the third object. Immediately following the selection, an oddity trial was presented requiring participants select the odd object (three objects match on a single dimension, while the fourth “odd” object does not match on that dimension). Asterisks denote selections. Participants responded using buttons attached to a standard keyboard that corresponded spatially to the objects on the screen. B. Yoked prime-probe trials were presented with a 500 ms response-stimulus interval.

Several reasons motivated the use of the FIST and oddity tasks in lieu of a task such as the DCCS, which was successfully used with adults by Diamond and Kirkham (2005): (1) to eliminate any biasing effects of overt language, and to eliminate any memory load to remember the cued dimension, the participant is free to choose the matching dimension; (2) both tasks share several properties with the DCCS and other set-shifting tasks commonly used to assess the development of cognitive flexibility—in particular a high degree of conflict exists between the dimension deemed to be relevant and that deemed to be irrelevant; (3) finally, as noted above, very little developmental research has investigated the sources of cognitive inflexibility on set-shifting tasks other than the DCCS.

The same general design was followed for both Experiments 1 and 2. Adult participants were presented with a FIST trial (i.e., the prime), followed by an oddity problem (i.e., the probe). This yoked FIST-oddity pairing repeated for 175 trials (Figure 1B). Five FIST-oddity conditions were presented in which the primary manipulation was the relation between each yoked FIST-oddity pair. In what follows “dimension” denotes the category (e.g., color, shape, size) and “stimulus value” denotes the specific value (e.g., blue, basket, small). Prime target refers to the selected category of the prime, and prime distractor to the ignored category. Probe target refers to the category of the odd object of the oddity trial.

Experiment 1

Experiment 1 used five conditions to investigate processes contributing to difficulty in task switching. All of the specific examples reference Figure 2. The conditions were:

Figure 2.

Figure 2

A. Sample stimuli are given for all conditions. To facilitate comparison across conditions, Screen 2 examples are all related to the same example FIST trial in which color (blue) is the selected dimension or target, and shape (basket) is the irrelevant dimension or distractor. The odd dimensions for the oddity trials are: Neutral Control (NC; size), Attended Repetition (AR; color), Ignored Repetition (IR; shape), Total Change (TC; shape), Negative Priming (NP; shape),Selected Neutral Control (SNC; size), andSelected Ignored (SI; shape).

  • Neutral Control (NC): the prime target (color) was unrelated to the probe target (size), and none of the prime stimulus values were used during the probe.

  • Attended Repetition (AR): the prime target (color) was also the probe target (color), and all stimulus values from the prime were used in the probe.

  • Ignored Repetition (IR): the prime distractor (shape) was the probe target (shape), and all stimulus values from the prime were used in the probe.

  • Total Change (TC): As in NC, none of the prime stimulus values were used during the probe, but both dimensions (color and shape) that varied on the prime also varied during the probe. As in IR, the prime distractor was the probe target.

  • 5. Negative Priming (NP): the prime target stimulus values (red and blue) were removed during the probe, but the stimulus values for the prime distractor (basket and bowl) were repeated in the probe. As in both IR and TC, the prime distractor (shape) was the probe target. The TC and NP conditions were based on two DCCS manipulations of the same name (Zelazo et al., 2003).

Support for attentional inertia as an alternative to negative priming would be provided if the AR condition revealed a facilitation effect. For AR, the oddity target is consistent with the FIST “mind set”. Thus, although the attentional inertia theory as currently developed in the literature only specifies conditions that impair responding, a prediction that RT will be faster relative to a neutral stimulus (AR < NC) would be consistent with the theory.

Alternatively, support for negative priming would be provided if the oddity response in the NP condition was slower than in the TC condition. This is because specific values of the distractor dimension are maintained in NP but not TC, these values will be negatively primed, and RT will be slowed (NP > TC). This prediction is inconsistent with attentional inertia because relevant values of the pre-shift dimension (e.g., red and blue) cannot interfere with responding if they are removed in the NP oddity trial. Thus, attentional inertia predicts NP = TC.

Finally, two additional comparisons provide information about processes at work during FIST selection. The first assesses whether interference during the probe might stem from both the prime distractor and prime target values. In IR, both the prime distractor and prime target values are repeated, while NP only repeats the prime distractor stimulus values. If both the previously relevant and previously irrelevant stimulus values contribute to impaired responding, then IR should be slower than NP (IR > NP). If there is no difference, this would suggest that shifting attention to the prime distractor values (negative priming) contributes to impaired responding more than shifting attention away from the prime target values (attentional inertia).

The final comparison speaks to an issue that has been difficult to assess in previous developmental investigations. The issue is whether negative priming occurs at the level of task set or dimension (e.g., color) or at the level of individual stimulus values (e.g., red). Comparing the NC and TC conditions can assess this. If there is negative priming of the distractor task set (e.g., suppression of “categorize by shape” when “categorize by color” is the relevant task set), then when all stimulus values are removed during the probe, it should be easier to shift to a neutral dimension (as in NC) than to a dimension in which the prime distractor dimension is the probe target dimension (as in TC; i.e., NC < TC). If there were no difference (NC = TC), this would provide some suggestion that negative priming, if it occurs at all, does not occur at the level of the task set.

Method and Design

Participants

Participants were twenty-four Temple University adult undergraduate students (9 males, M = 227.58 months, SD = 15.91). Two additional participants were not analyzed due to equipment error. The Temple University Institutional Review Board approved the study. All participants provided informed consent and were debriefed.

Materials and Task Design

Stimuli were presented on a PC using Presentation® (Neurobehavioral Systems). Response buttons were attached to a standard keyboard and corresponded spatially to the objects on screen (inset Figure 1A). FIST-oddity trials were constructed using the following stimulus attributes: (a) shape: bag, basket, dog bowl, light bulb, clover, coffee mug, hammer, pig, pin, and scissors; (b) color: red, orange, yellow, green, blue, and purple; (c) size: large (1602 pixels), medium (802 pixels), and small (402 pixels). Pilot testing revealed that sizes were easily distinguishable, but orange and red were difficult to distinguish quickly and thus were never paired together. Although NP and TC utilized the same stimuli as the other conditions, because size only had three stimulus values and could not be used as a varying stimulus on the TC condition. Thus, for NP and TC only color and shape, but not size, were potential matching dimensions.

There were 175 paired prime-probe trials. The first 25 trials (five trials for each condition) were designated practice trials to allow participants to perform at a steady state level, leaving 30 test trials available for analysis from each condition. The probe (oddity) followed the prime (FIST) with a response-stimulus interval (RSI) of 500 ms (Figure 1B). Prime-probe pairs were pre-specified for all possible selections, but the presentation of the probe stimulus was determined conjointly by both the participant's prime response and by the condition. For example, in a pre-randomized fashion, the program determined the order of the presentation of conditions. Then, if the trial was an IR trial with shape and color as possible FIST selections, the program prepared two oddity trials, but determined which one of these two oddity trials to present based on the participant's FIST selection. The order of conditions and pairing of dimensions (shape, color, size) were pre-randomized across trials, and order of presentation was identical across participants.

Procedure

The design was a within subjects design. All participants were given a demonstration in which they were told that there were two screens to be presented consecutively, and that the response buttons corresponded to each of these screens. For FIST trials, participants were instructed to choose two objects that match in a way that is not like the third object, and it was explained that there were only two possible ways for this to occur. They were also shown that there was an incorrect way to respond (i.e., objects could be matched on the constant dimension—e.g., size, when all objects are the same size—but this violated the matching rule). The program provided feedback (“Correct!”, or “Incorrect!” when subjects matched on the constant dimension), and asterisks were placed on screen next to the selected objects when the buttons were pressed. The oddity trial only appeared following correct FIST responses. Incorrect FIST responses occurred very infrequently, but when they did, the screen reset and participants were allowed to respond correctly before moving onto the oddity trial.

In the demonstration, the experimenter used the FIST response buttons to indicate one of the two possible choices, at which point the oddity task appeared. For oddity, the participant saw four objects and was told that three of the objects shared some attribute that the other did not share. This fourth object was the odd object to be selected. The demonstration continued for five trials, and participants were told to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible, that mistakes were okay and to continue on to the next trial. Because participants were to respond quickly, they kept their hands above the keyboard throughout the experiment. Participants responded to the prime trial pressing buttons sequentially (to select the two matching objects) with their finger of choice. No feedback was given for the oddity task. In summary, a complete test trial was as follows: (a) for the prime, a FIST trial was presented, and the participant selected two matching objects, (b) 500 ms RSI, (c) for the probe, an oddity trial was presented, and the participant selected the odd object, (d) 500 ms RSI until the next trial (Figure 1B).

Results and Discussion

After removing practice trials, there were 30 trials available for analysis in each condition. We first analyzed error rates. Incorrect responses (i.e., participants picked a non-odd object during the probe) accounted for about 4% of test trials. There were significant differences in error rates among the conditions (Friedman's χ2df= 4 = 23.7, p < .001). Post-hoc Wilcoxon signed-rank tests revealed significantly more errors occurred in the NC condition compared to all other conditions (corrected for multiple comparisons using the False Discovery Rate (FDR) procedure, i = 5, q* = .05; Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995, largest value of corrected results p < .007). Error rates were low. Means (and standard deviations) for each condition were as follows: NC = 2.04(1.12); AR = 1.13(0.99); IR = 1.04(1.08); TC = 0.92(1.01); NP = 0.67(0.96). The effect sizes were small (out of thirty trials, the NC condition elicited, on average, one more error than the other conditions; probability of superiority values for significant differences ranged from .58 to .70, with .50 indicating no difference; Sheskin, 2007). Because the NC condition elicited fast RTs, this possibly reflects a speed-accuracy trade-off (i.e., the phenomenon that faster responses tend to produce more errors; Liu & Smith, 2009; Miller, Sproesser, & Ulrich, 2008; Thorpe, Fize, & Marlot, 1996). Notably, though, errors on the TC condition were not different from the other conditions, and this condition was comparable to the NC condition in that it also elicited fast RTs. Therefore, evidence for a speed-accuracy-trade-off is equivocal. When errors are infrequent, as they are here, they are often removed from the analysis of RT data (Ratcliff, 1993). Thus, in the subsequent analysis we focus on correct responses and delay discussion of the results of the analysis of error rates, and potential explanations for the condition differences, for the General Discussion.

For the valid RTs (i.e., correct responses), all responses with latency more than 2.5 standard deviations from the condition mean (about 3% of test trials) were excluded. The remaining oddity RTs were averaged for each condition, and these served as the dependent variable. We first assessed interactions with repeated measures Condition (5) × Sex (2) Analyses of Variance (ANOVAs), which revealed no significant effect of sex (p = .37, ηp2 = .04), and no significant Condition × Sex interaction (p = .13, ηp2 = .08 ). We next proceeded to examine four planned non-orthogonal repeated measures contrasts (Rosenthal, Rosnow, & Rubin, 2000) focused on specific hypotheses: (1) AR < NC predicted by attentional inertia; (2) NP > TC predicted by negative priming; (3) IR > NP assessing the contribution of both negative priming and attentional inertia; and (4) TCNC, assessing whether negative priming occurs at the level of stimulus dimension. A fifth comparison, IR > NC, was also tested, but the results are not informative with respect to adjudicating between the two competing theories (i.e., due to the similarity of IR to the standard FIST and DCCS tasks, both negative priming and attentional inertia would predict longer RT during the IR condition). The results of this comparison are, however, generally informative, and we report them as a validation of the method. All significant contrasts survived a correction for multiple comparisons using the FDR procedure (i = 5; q* = .05). With the exception of TCNC, hypothesis tests were directional.

Results of the comparisons are reported in Figure 3A. First, two findings supported the negative priming hypothesis; RTs were slower for the conditions in which the distractor became the target: (a) IR > NC, t(22) = 5.12, p < .001, d = .591; (b) NP > TC, t(22) = 4.04, p < .001, d = .64. Most importantly, the result NP > TC would not be predicted by attentional inertia because the prime target stimulus values are removed, yet negative priming would predict this result because the prime distractor becomes the probe target.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Mean response time to select the odd object for each condition for both Experiment 1 (top) and Experiment 2 (bottom). Non-significant comparisons are reported to the right of each figure. n.s. = non-significant. * p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001. Error bars represent +/− one standard error.

Second, the results of the contrast comparing AR < NC failed to support attentional inertia as an alternative to negative priming. There was no facilitation effect; participants were actually slightly slower in AR than in the NC, although this difference was not significant, t(22) = −1.63, p > .05 (two-tailed, opposite the expected direction), d = .17. This is an unexpected finding, according to attentional inertia, because repetition of the prime target dimension—i.e., the maintenance of the previously relevant “mind set” during the probe trial—should have facilitated performance relative to a neutral condition.

The comparison IR > NP was significant, t(22) = 2.33, p < .05, d = .20, suggesting that both the previously relevant and previously irrelevant stimulus values contribute to impaired responding during the probe trial. That is, while NP only contributes interference from the prime distractor, IR contributes interference from both the prime distractor and the prime target, rendering the IR condition more difficult. This result actually suggests both attentional inertia and negative priming contribute to impaired responding.

Finally, we found TC was not significantly different from NC, t(22) = −1.15, p > .05, d = .20. In both cases, the prime target and distractor values are removed, but in TC the prime distractor dimension becomes the probe target. This was, however, not sufficient to impair responding relative to a neutral stimulus, suggesting that negative priming does not occur at the level of the dimension.

Experiment 2

The absence of facilitation in the AR condition was surprising, and might suggest that attentional inertia does not play a role in task-switching during the FIST. This finding, however, is open to an alternative explanation. Evidence from the adult task switching literature suggests that stimulus values that form an association with one task might then cause interference when these stimulus values are used with another task (Allport, Styles, & Hsieh, 1994; Allport & Wylie, 1999, 2000). In this case, it is possible that repeating the stimulus values during the probe could have helped maintain activation of the prime distractor, causing interference during the probe task, and leading to slowed responding when the same stimulus values are re-presented. In the same fashion, repeated stimulus values could have maintained activation of the prime target dimension leading to facilitation (as predicted by attentional inertia), which might offset the interference from the distractor. Thus, one might predict no difference (AR = NC) due to the offsetting contributions of the target and distractor task pairings. This would indicate that both attentional inertia and negative priming contribute to difficulty in task switching. Another finding from Experiment 1 also supports this possibility; IR (which contains both the prime target and distractor values) was more difficult than NP (which contains only the distractor values).

In Experiment 2, we had two goals. First we wanted to rule out the possibility that the slightly slower responding in the AR condition relative to NC was a spurious finding, and thus we conducted a replication of the NC, AR, and IR conditions on a new sample of participants. Second, we wanted to investigate further the role of negative priming, and the conditions under which negative priming occurs. In Experiment 1, we found no negative priming at the level of the task set (or dimension), and we wanted to determine if varying just one of the prime stimulus dimensions (in this case the prime distractor dimension) is sufficient to instantiate negative priming. To this end, we created two new conditions to assess the effect of holding constant the selected stimulus dimension of the prime on probe target selection when the probe target is (a) neutral with respect to the prime, or (b) when the probe target is the distractor dimension of the prime. The two new conditions replaced TC and NP (from Exp. 1), and were:

  • Selected Neutral Control (SNC): the prime target dimension (color) did not vary during the probe, and, as in NC, the prime target (color) was unrelated to the probe target (size). The prime constant dimension (size) was the probe target.

  • Selected Ignored (SI): the prime target dimension (color) did not vary during the probe, and, as in IR, the prime distractor (shape) was the probe target (shape). The prime constant dimension (size) was the probe distractor.

In both conditions, the stimuli of the oddity trial do not vary on the prime target dimension (i.e., on the previous “mind set”, reds and blues). Attentional inertia would predict the conditions should not differ. Whereas attentional inertia is silent with respect to the effect of the previously irrelevant dimension and stimulus values on current selection, negative priming predicts that RT is slower when the distractor becomes the target. Negative priming, therefore, would predict a slowed response in the SI condition (SI > SNC).

It is also informative to investigate the difference between IR and SI. Similar to the comparison of IR and NP reported earlier, the comparison of IR > SI tells us whether there is additional interference from the previously relevant stimuli. Again, in IR, interference during the probe potentially stems from both the prime distractor and prime target stimulus values. SI only varies the prime distractor stimulus values. Thus, if both the previously relevant and previously irrelevant stimulus values contribute to impaired responding, then IR should be slower than SI (IR > SI). As with the comparison IR > NP, if there were no difference, this would suggest greater interference from the prime distractor values (negative priming) than from the prime target values (attentional inertia).

Method and Design

Participants

Participants were thirty Temple University adult undergraduate students (17 males, M = 229.13 months, SD = 13.35). Three additional participants were removed due to equipment error. The Temple University Institutional Review Board approved the study. All participants provided informed consent and were debriefed.

Materials and Task Design

Participants were tested in the same manner as in Experiment 1, except that in Experiment 2 the SI and SNC conditions replaced the NP and TC conditions. The NC, AR, and IR conditions were presented as in Experiment 1.

Results and Discussion

There were 30 trials available for analysis in each condition. Incorrect responses only accounted for about 6% of test trials. As in Experiment 1, there were significant differences in error rates among the conditions (Friedman's χ2df= 4 = 21.8, p < .001). Post-hoc Wilcoxon signed-rank tests revealed significantly more errors occurred in the SNC condition compared to AR, IR, and SI conditions, and in the NC condition compared to AR (FDR corrected i = 10; q* = .05, largest corrected p < .009). Error rates were again low. Means (and standard deviations) for each condition were as follows: NC = 1.96(1.38); AR = 1.26(0.83); IR = 1.44(1.43); SNC = 2.97(2.68); SI = 1.97(1.38). The effect sizes were small (out of thirty trials, the SNC and NC conditions elicited, on average, one more error than the other conditions; probability of superiority values for significant differences ranged from .53 to .63, with values of .50 indicating an equal proportion or no difference). In this case the conditions with the fastest RTs were also those with the most errors, which is consistent with a speed-accuracy-trade-off. Because errors were infrequent, as in Experiment 1, errors were removed from subsequent analysis, and we delay the error discussion for the General Discussion.

For the valid RTs (i.e., correct responses), all responses with latency more than 2.5 standard deviations from the condition mean (about 3% of test trials) were excluded. The remaining oddity RTs were averaged for each condition, and these served as the dependent variable. We first assessed interactions with repeated measures Condition (5)×Sex (2) ANOVAs, which revealed no significant effect of sex (p = .39, ηp2 = .03), and no significant Condition×Sex interaction (p = .68, ηp2 = .02). We next proceeded to examine planned non-orthogonal repeated measures contrasts (Rosenthal et al., 2000) focused on specific hypotheses. The first two contrasts were the same as that reported in Experiment 1, comparing (1) AR < NC predicted by attentional inertia; and (2) IR > NC, validating the method. The final contrasts compared (3) SI > SNC, assessing whether negative priming affects responding when the prime target stimulus values do not vary during the probe; (4) IR > SI, assessing the contribution of both negative priming and attentional inertia. All significant contrasts survived a correction for multiple comparisons using the FDR procedure (i = 4; q* = .05). All hypothesis tests were directional.

Results of the comparisons are reported in Figure 3B. The pattern of results for NC, AR, and IR replicated Experiment 1. Two findings supported the negative priming hypothesis; RTs were slower for conditions in which the distractor became the target: (a) IR > NC, t(28) = 9.45, p < .001, d = .75, replicating Experiment 1; and (b) the novel finding, SI > SNC, t(28) = 2.43, p < .01, d = .27. Here, the response was slower when the distractor became the target, even though the prime target dimension (e.g., color) did not vary during the probe, and thus it was not possible to identify the odd object based on that dimension. We also replicated the absence of a facilitation effect in the AR condition. Again, AR response was slightly slower than NC, although again this difference was non-significant, t(28) = 1.71, p = .10 (two-tailed), d = .20. Finally, the comparison IR > SI was significant, t(28) = 5.73, p < .001, d = .45. In Experiment 1, we found the same result for the comparison IR > NP; in both cases it was more difficult to switch to a stimulus in which both the prime target and distractor stimulus values were potential probe targets. We interpret both results as suggesting that the previously relevant and previously irrelevant stimulus values of the prime contribute to impaired responding during the probe trial.

General Discussion

Summary of Results

The present study contributes to ongoing investigations of the development of cognitive flexibility in two principal ways. First, it investigates an understudied task (i.e., the FIST) which has been shown to be useful for exploring, using a single task methodology, the development of cognitive flexibility during both preschool (Jacques & Zelazo, 2001) and during early-to-middle childhood (Dick, 2006). In addition, it validates a method for investigating cognitive processes that contribute to performance on this task. Second, the results we report here provide evidence with adults that difficulty on cognitive flexibility tasks such as the FIST cannot be attributed to a single factor of attentional inertia or negative priming. According to attentional inertia, the key problem is the need for a conceptual shift away from a certain property of a stimulus (e.g., blueness and redness) to focusing on another property of the same stimulus (e.g., baskets and bowls). In contrast, the negative priming theory asserts that the competing distractor (e.g., baskets and bowls when focusing on color) is suppressed during initial selection of the target, resulting in difficulty shifting to the distractor representation when this representation becomes the target. The central findings of the two experiments reported here provide broad support that (a) the source of difficulty on the FIST can be attributed to both attentional inertia and negative priming—that is, neither explanation alone is sufficient; (b) negative priming in this task occurs at the level of individual stimulus values, and not at the level of dimension or task-set.

Although the findings presented here were acquired with adult participants, they are consistent with the predictions made by theories of the development of cognitive flexibility, and with empirical findings in children. This could be interpreted to suggest that the cognitive processes involved in navigating problem solving situations in childhood are similar if not identical to those we use as adults. Indeed, Diamond and Kirkham (2005) found that, when assessed with sufficiently sensitive measures (i.e., RT), adults show performance deficits on the DCCS, a task that is successfully managed by most 5-year-olds. In another example, using eye tracking to supplement a behavioral measure, Keysar and colleagues (Keysar, Lin, & Barr, 2003) showed that adults fail to routinely apply theory of mind knowledge that they acquired by 6 years. Thus, under the right circumstances, some of the same problem solving difficulties with children can be observed with adults, and it may be the case that the findings from developmental and adult studies can be brought together under the same theoretic umbrella—for example, for task-switching, under a model incorporating both attentional inertia and negative priming.

The findings reported here thus aid in interpreting of previous findings with children since they flesh out an explicit model of what the task is assessing, and the model engenders specific predictions. Such an explicit model is difficult to derive from children per se since their data are categorical (error rates). The fleshing out of this explicit model offers new insights into the competence underlying the task, and suggests new designs to evaluate this competence in children. However, we are careful to note that the pattern of results reported here may not hold for children because they may differentially weight one attentional process over the other. Further, the domain under study may influence the development of these attentional processes. Research has addressed the latter issue, and suggests that the domain under study matters for whether attentional processes such as negative priming show developmental change. For example, there is little evidence for negative priming in 7-year-old children in a Stroop task (Tipper, Bourque, Anderson, & Brehaut, 1989), but there is evidence for negative priming during spatial attention in infants (Amso & Johnson, 2005, 2008) and children (Simone & McCormick, 1999), during location based and conceptual-level tasks in children as young as five (Pritchard & Neumann, 2004), and during set-shifting tasks in preschoolers (Chevalier & Blaye, 2008; Müller et al., 2006; Zelazo et al., 2003). But the former question of whether processes of attentional inertia or negative priming might be more influential to cognitive flexibility at different points in development remains unanswered, and the developmental trajectory on the way to adult competence is a matter for future work.

Fortunately, the paradigm developed here is appropriate for investigating this question. For example, Müller and colleagues (2006) showed that children improve on the negative priming version of the DCCS with age, which was taken to indicate that negative priming effects actually decline with age. We might therefore expect that if children were tested under the paradigm developed here, they would show a decrease with age in the RT difference between the NP, IR, and AR conditions and their respective neutral control conditions (i.e., TC and NC conditions). However, it might also be the case that attentional inertia influences performance on the task differently across age. For example, with adults we found that repetition of prime target and distractor stimulus values leads to greater interference than repetition of the prime distractor values alone (IR > NP; IR > SI), which suggests attentional inertia contributes to performance on the task in addition to negative priming. If the weighting of one or both of these processes changes with development, this would be reflected in age-related changes in the difference between target-distractor repetition conditions (i.e., attentional inertia + negative priming) and distractor-only repetition conditions (i.e., negative priming only). That is, IR > NP and IR > SI differences should change with age. Further, if theories suggesting that attentional inertia is overcome with developing inhibitory control are correct (Diamond et al., 2005 ; Diamond & Kirkham, 2005; Kirkham et al., 2003), we should see a reduction in these differences with age. The paradigm developed here can potentially tease apart these processes, but obviously these predictions need to be empirically verified with children. With this in mind, we turn to a detailed analysis of the primary experimental results and their implications for explanations of cognitive inflexibility.

Interference During the Probe Stems In Part from the Ignored Stimulus Values of the Prime (Negative Priming)

  • Shifting to the previously irrelevant dimension of the prime was more difficultwhen the stimulus values from the prime distractor were repeated during the probe compared to when neither the prime target values nor the prime distractor values were repeated during the probe (NP > TC).

  • Shifting to the previously irrelevant dimension of the prime was more difficultthan shifting to a neutral dimension, even when the previously relevantdimension did not vary during the probe and thus could not be a basis for selection (SI > SNC).

These two findings suggest that negative priming of the prime distractor is occurring, extending prior findings that negative priming is a source of difficulty during set-shifting tasks (Chevalier & Blaye, 2008; Müller et al., 2006; Zelazo et al., 2003). The NP condition was more difficult than the TC condition, which is compatible with work showing that younger preschoolers fail the Negative Priming version of the DCCS, but they pass the Total Change version (Müller et al., 2006; Zelazo et al., 2003). Shifting to the prime distractor dimension is even difficult when it is impossible to identify the odd object based on the previously relevant dimension because this dimension does not vary (e.g., when all objects are the same color), suggesting negative priming of the distractor is a source of interference over and above interference from the prime target. The fact that SNC was faster than SI also suggests the constant dimension of the prime trial (e.g., size) does not contribute to interference during the probe trial, indicating that it is not processed in the same way as the attended to and ignored stimulus dimensions (i.e., those dimensions in direct conflict). Thus, conflict between competing perceptual representations, requiring selective attention/ignoring of one stimulus over the other, appears to be necessary for interference to occur on the probe trial. This would explain why shifting tasks containing no direct perceptual conflict, such as the Reversal Version of the DCCS (Perner & Lang, 2002), are easy even though the complexity of the rules is similar to that of more difficult conflict-containing shifting tasks (also see Müller et al., 2006; Experiment 2).

Negative Priming Does Not Occur at the Level of Abstract Task Set

  • When the specific values of both the prime target and distractor were removed during the probe, shifting to the previously ignored dimension was not more difficult than shifting to a neutral dimension (TC was not significantly different from NC).

This finding suggests negative priming is not associated with a particular dimension, a finding we return to later in the discussion.

Interference During the Probe Stems from Both the Attended-To and Ignored Stimulus Values of the Prime (Attentional Inertia and Negative Priming)

  • When the stimulus values of both the target and distractor dimensions of the prime were repeated during the probe, shifting to the previously irrelevant dimension was more difficult than switching to a neutral dimension (IR > NC).

  • When the stimulus values of both the target and distractor dimensions of the prime were repeated during the probe, shifting to the previously relevant dimension was not easier than switching to a neutral dimension (i.e., there was no facilitation effect when the prime target was also the probe target; AR was not significantly different from NC).

  • When the stimulus values of both the target and distractor dimensions of the prime were repeated during the probe, shifting to the previously irrelevant dimension of the prime was more difficult than (a) shifting to the previously irrelevant dimension of the prime when the stimulus values from only the prime distractor were repeated during the probe, and the prime target values were removed (IR > NP); (b) shifting to the previously irrelevant dimension of the prime when the stimulus values from only the prime distractor were repeated during the probe, and the prime target dimension did not vary (IR > SI).

Both attentional inertia and negative priming predict the finding that shifting to the previously irrelevant dimension is more difficult than shifting to a neutral dimension (IR > NC), but for different reasons. Indeed, the IR condition is isomorphic to the standard DCCS and FIST tasks, which pose a problem for both preschoolers (Jacques & Zelazo, 2001; Zelazo et al., 2003 ) and adults (Diamond & Kirkham, 2005). The finding is not surprising, then, as this condition presents interference from both the previously relevant and irrelevant stimulus values, and shifting to the distractor dimension is particularly difficult. The surprising finding here is that there was no facilitation effect for AR compared to NC. It is well known that attending to a particular stimulus can facilitate responding to that stimulus on subsequent trials ( known as “positive” or “repetition” priming; Schneider & Logan, 2005; Tulving & Schacter, 1990; Wiggs & Martin, 1998), and since attentional inertia emphasizes the maintenance of a pre-shift “mind set”, a facilitation effect would be consistent with the theory. Further, the lack of facilitation is not likely due to a floor effect, as the responses during NC were faster than AR in both experiments. Due to the fact that in AR the target of the prime and of the probe are the same, it is difficult to see this result would be predicted by attentional inertia without taking into account negative priming of the distractor stimulus. However, it may also be the case that repetition priming of the prime target offset negative priming of the prime distractor, which explains why AR was not significantly slower than NC. Another finding supports the notion that both attentional inertia and negative priming contribute to task difficulty; repetition of prime target and distractor stimulus values leads to greater interference than repetition of the prime distractor values alone (IR > NP; IR > SI). These findings suggest that difficulty in set-shifting tasks stems from interference from the stimulus values of both the previously relevant and previously irrelevant dimensions.

Analysis of Errors

Analysis of the pattern of errors showed that participants made more errors on neutral-control conditions (i.e., NC in both experiments, and SNC in Experiment 2) than they did during the experimental conditions. However, errors themselves were infrequent, and although reliable the effect sizes for the differences were small (neutral conditions elicited one additional error, on average, than experimental conditions). Despite this, it is important to note that the error analysis appears inconsistent with the analysis of RT differences—conditions eliciting fast RTs also elicited more errors. Further, although the outcomes are consistent with a speed-accuracy-trade-off (i.e., the phenomenon that faster responses tend to produce more errors; Liu & Smith, 2009; Miller et al., 2008; Thorpe et al., 1996), we cannot rule out additional factors that may have led to errors on these conditions (e.g., fast guesses, guesses based on the habitual response over trials, multiple runs of the process under study, inattention). In fact, it is possible that the increase in errors for neutral conditions in the present design is a function of the fact that the NC and SNC conditions are the only two conditions in which the probe target dimension does not appear as a possible matching dimension in the prime (see Figure 2).

The inconsistency of errors and RT findings is significant given the fact that investigations of cognitive inflexibility in children often rely on error rates and not RT (thus, from an error analysis point of view alone, one might have drawn the conclusions that the NC condition is most difficult, even though it is associated with accuracy at near ceiling, and much faster correct responses). However, it is important to remember that the increased errors were found for neutral control conditions, and these conditions are not typically found in studies with children. In fact, for conditions inspired by studies with children, the RT differences are consistent with what has been found in children—for example, the IR and NP conditions are more difficult than their respective control conditions, as assessed by RT, and children make errors on these manipulations for the FIST and DCCS tasks (Jacques & Zelazo, 2001; Zelazo et al., 2003). It is possible that the apparent discrepancy can be explained by the addition of neutral conditions that include a dimension in the probe that is not expected from the prime, by speed-accuracy-trade-off, or by a combination of other factors that are not under experimental control.

Theoretical explanations for cognitive inflexibility on set-shifting tasks

Attentional inertia and negative priming find commonality with theories of task switching in the adult literature, notably with a model known as task-set inertia. This model suggests that shift costs arise from proactive interference of both persisting facilitation or positive priming of previously relevant (but now irrelevant) stimulus attributes, and persisting suppression or negative priming of the previously irrelevant (but now relevant) stimulus attributes (Allport et al., 1994; Allport & Wylie, 1999, 2000). Thus, successful selective attention in these tasks stems from a combination of successful target activation and successful distractor suppression. It can readily be seen that this model simultaneously encompasses aspects of both the attentional inertia and negative priming theories from the developmental literature.

The task-set inertia model is one exemplar of accounts that emphasize stimulus-based priming (Allport & Wylie, 2000; Koch & Allport, 2006; Schneider & Logan, 2005), or bottom-up, stimulus driven contributions to task-switch costs (Meiran, Chorev, & Sapir, 2000; Waszak, Hommel, & Allport, 2003). Alternatively, top-down accounts emphasize competition between endogenous task-sets (Meiran et al., 2000; Rogers & Monsell, 1995). Task-set, in the cases considered here, refers to abstract representations that apply to all realizations of the goals of a task. For example, a task-set in the present study would apply the broad rule “attend to color”, covering all realizations of that rule, rather than the more “item specific” representations relevant for stimulus values (e.g., “red” or “blue”).

The issue of whether interference occurs at the level of task set or at the level of individual stimulus values has not been sufficiently characterized in the developmental literature. Both attentional inertia and negative priming focus on interference from stimulus values (Kirkham et al., 2003; Müller et al., 2006). There is, however, some evidence that task set interference occurs as well; approximately 25% of children still fail the Total Change version of the DCCS, even though there are no interfering stimulus values during the postswitch phase of this task (Zelazo et al., 2003). Empirical findings from the adult literature support a contribution for both sources of interference (Allport et al., 1994; Allport & Wylie, 2000; Experiment 5; Besner, 2001; Klein, 1964; Koch, 2001; Meiran et al., 2000 ; Monsell, Taylor, & Murphy, 2001). Mayr and Keele (2000) and others (Schuch & Koch, 2003) provide evidence that shifting to a task set that has recently been abandoned is more difficult than shifting to a task set that has been abandoned less recently, even when interference effects from specific stimulus values are controlled. These latter findings suggest that, in order to implement a new task set, the previous task set must be actively inhibited (a process termed backward inhibition ; Mayr & Keele, 2000). Indeed, many authors acknowledge task switch costs arise from both task priming and higher-level task set reconfiguration (Allport & Wylie, 2000; Goschke, 2000; Monsell, Yeung, & Azuma, 2000), although the contribution of these sources to switch costs potentially differs by the task under study (Yeung & Monsell, 2003).

Our data speak to the source of interference from negative priming, at the level of stimulus values or at the level of task set. If there is negative priming of the distractor task set (e.g., suppression of “categorize by shape” when “categorize by color” is the relevant task set), when all stimulus values are removed during the probe, it should be easier to shift to a neutral stimulus (as in NC) than to a stimulus in which the prime distractor task set is the probe target task set (as in TC; i.e., NC < TC). In the present study, we did not find this to be the case; TC was not significantly different from NC . Thus, while the data do provide evidence for negative priming of specific stimulus values, on this particular task negative priming of the previous task set does not occur, a finding that is consistent with Mayr and Keele (2000; Experiment 2). For task switch paradigms, this might explain the phenomenon that children can often state correctly the rules for the task, but fail to employ them (i.e., they can state correctly where blue bowls go in the shape game, even after the rule switch, but fail to sort the cards correctly; Zelazo, Frye, & Rapus, 1996). In the case of verbally stating the rules, the conflicting perceptual stimulus (test card paired with target card) is not a focus of attention. If this conflict contributes to interference in part via a process of negative priming, as we argue, proactive interference may not occur when this conflict is not visually present.

Top-down control processes, inhibition, and developing cognitive flexibility

The results of the present study suggest that bottom-up processes are a primary contribution to difficulty on set-shifting tasks. However, although bottom-up processes may lead to proactive interference or to negative priming, they do not necessarily resolve this interference (Arrington & Logan, 2005). Thus a central developmental question remains unanswered: how do children come to succeed on set-shifting tasks? Competing explanations often center on whether an active inhibitory process is necessary to resolve interference. Some authors have rejected this idea. For example, the negative priming theory favored by Müller, Zelazo, and colleagues calls on a top-down cognitive process, application of a higher-order rule, to overcome both proactive interference and negative priming (Müller et al., 2006; Zelazo et al., 2003; see Andrews, Halford, Bunch, Bowden, & Jones, 2003; Halford, Wilson, & Phillips, 1998 for similar arguments). Several connectionist models explain the suppression of prepotent responses without reference to a specific active inhibitory mechanism (Cohen, Dunbar, & McClelland, 1990; Kimberg & Farah, 1993; Morton & Munakata, 2002; Munakata, Morton, & Yeyrs, 2003). Alternatively, in some models the development of inhibition is a central mechanism explaining the development of cognitive flexibility (Carlson & Moses, 2001; Carlson, Moses, & Claxton, 2004; Dempster, 1993). For example, attentional inertia is overcome by successful inhibition of the interfering stimulus, a process that is immature early in development (Diamond et al., 2005; Diamond & Kirkham, 2005; Kirkham et al., 2003).

Inhibitory processes might also be necessary to explain negative priming and its development, although this continues to be a matter of debate. On one hand, when people attend to the target they might need to actively inhibit representations of the distractor (Houghton & Tipper, 1994, 1996; Neill, 1977; Tipper, 1985; Tipper & Cranston, 1985). On the other hand, negative priming might be explained by memory retrieval mechanisms, without reference to a specific inhibition mechanism (Neill & Valdes, 1992; Neill, Valdes, Terry, & Gorfein, 1992; Park & Kanwisher, 1994). Although data are often interpreted as favoring either inhibition or memory accounts to the exclusion of each other (e.g., MacLeod, Chiappe, & Fox, 2002), an explanation presented by Tipper (2001) suggests there is no necessary conflict between these accounts—rather, a complete account would need to consider both inhibition at the encoding stage, and memory processes at retrieval. Developmental investigations might inform on these competing theories, but surprisingly few studies of negative priming have been conducted with children (Amso & Johnson, 2005, 2008; Chevalier & Blaye, 2008; Müller et al., 2006; Pritchard & Neumann, 2004; Simone & McCormick, 1999; Tipper et al., 1989 for exceptions). In fact, the developmental progression of negative priming remains largely uncharacterized during preschool and early childhood, which is the age range in which tasks like the DCCS and the FIST are typically applied. Isolating how specific attentional mechanisms contribute to successful set-shifting during this age range may require either new methodologies (e.g., such as those developed in the present paper and others; Chevalier & Blaye, 2008; Maes, Damen, & Eling, 2004; Maes, Vich, & Eling, 2006) and/or more intricate cognitive models to explain how processes contributing to successful cognitive flexibility are employed in some situations, but not others.

In summary, the findings reported here have implications for theories of the development of cognitive flexibility. First, they represent empirical evidence that both suppressing attention to the previously relevant stimulus, and engaging attention to the previously irrelevant stimulus, contributes to cognitive inflexibility on set-shifting tasks. Second, the findings speak to the need to pay greater attention to the perceptual and conceptual sources of interference in set-shifting tasks—i.e., whether interference occurs at the level of task stimulus or task set, or both. Finally, the study demonstrates the value of alternative approaches to investigating executive function development with the specific aim of determining the underlying cognitive and perceptual processes that contribute to impaired and successful task performance. This information is important not only for informing theories of cognitive flexibility in typical development, but it is essential if tasks like the FIST are applied to clinical populations (Jacques & Zelazo, 2001).

Acknowledgments

Author Note: I wish to thank Michael Andric, Charles Gaylord, Uri Hasson, Willis Overton, Meredith Rowe, and Pascale Tremblay. Thank you to Michael Schachter for help with programming. This research was supported by a post-doctoral fellowship from the National Institutes of Health (NIDCD: F32-DC008909).

Footnotes

1

All Cohen's d effect sizes were calculated using the pooled standard deviation of the conditions (Dunlap, Cortina, Vaslow, & Burke, 1996).

References

  1. Allport A, Styles EA, Hsieh S. Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In: Umilta C, Moscovitch M, editors. Attention and performance XV. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1994. pp. 421–452. [Google Scholar]
  2. Allport A, Wylie G. Task-switching positive and negative priming of task-set. In: Humphreys GW, Duncan J, Treisman AM, editors. Attention, space, and action: studies in cognitive neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1999. pp. 273–296. [Google Scholar]
  3. Allport A, Wylie G. Task-switching, stimulus response bindings, and negative priming. In: Monsell S, Driver J, editors. Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVII. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2000. pp. 35–70. [Google Scholar]
  4. Amso D, Johnson SP. Selection and inhibition in infancy: evidence from the spatial negative priming paradigm. Cognition. 2005;95(2):27–36. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Amso D, Johnson SP. Development of visual selection in 3- to 9-month olds: Evidence from saccades to previously ignored locations. Infancy. 2008;13(6):675–686. doi: 10.1080/15250000802459060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Andrews G, Halford GS, Bunch KM, Bowden D, Jones T. Theory of mind and relational complexity. Child Development. 2003;74:1476–1499. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Arrington CM, Logan GD. Voluntary task switching: Chasing the elusive homunculus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2005;31:683–702. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.4.683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological) 1995;57(1):289–300. [Google Scholar]
  9. Besner D. The myth of ballistic processing: Evidence from Stroop's paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 2001;8(2):324–330. doi: 10.3758/bf03196168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Blaye A, Jacques S. Categorical flexibility in preschoolers: Contributions of conceptual knowledge and executive control. Developmental Science. 2009;12(6):863–873. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00832.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carlson SM, Moses LJ. Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Development. 2001;72(4):1032–1053. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carlson SM, Moses LJ, Claxton L. Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: An investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2004;87:299–319. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chalmers KA, Halford GS. Young children's understanding of oddity: Reducing complexity by simple oddity and “most different” strategies. Cognitive Development. 2003;18(1):1–23. [Google Scholar]
  14. Chevalier N, Blaye A. Cognitive flexibility in preschoolers: The role of representation activation and maintenance. Developmental Science. 2008;11(3):339–353. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00679.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cohen JD, Dunbar K, McClelland JL. On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review. 1990;97:332–361. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.3.332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cragg L, Chevalier N. The processes underlying flexibility in childhood. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2009;iFirst:1–24. doi: 10.1080/17470210903204618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dempster FN. Resistance to interference: Developmental changes in a basic processing mechanism. In: Howe ML, Pasnak R, editors. Emerging themes in cognitive development. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1993. pp. 3–27. [Google Scholar]
  18. Diamond A, Carlson SM, Beck DM. Preschool children's performance in task switching on the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task: Separating the dimensions aids the ability to switch. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;28:689–729. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Diamond A, Kirkham N. Not quite as grown-up as we like to think: Parallels between cognition in childhood and adulthood. Psychological Science. 2005;16(4):291–297. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01530.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dick AS. Unpublished dissertation. Temple University; Philadelphia, PA: 2006. The development of cognitive flexibility. [Google Scholar]
  21. Dunlap WP, Cortina JM, Vaslow JB, Burke MJ. Meta-analysis of experiments with matched groups or repeated measures designs. Psychological Methods. 1996;1(2):170–177. [Google Scholar]
  22. Fox E. Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 1995;2:145–173. doi: 10.3758/BF03210958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Frye D, Zelazo PD, Palfai T. Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development. 1995;10(4):483–527. [Google Scholar]
  24. Goschke T. Intentional configuration and involuntary persistance in task-set switching. In: Monsell S, Driver JS, editors. Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2000. pp. 331–355. [Google Scholar]
  25. Halford GS, Wilson WH, Phillips S. Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1998;21:803–865. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x98001769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Houghton G, Tipper SP. A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In: Dagenbach D, Carr T, editors. Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1994. pp. 53–112. [Google Scholar]
  27. Houghton G, Tipper SP. Inhibitory mechanisms of neural and cognitive control: Applications to selective attention and sequential action. Brain and Cognition. 1996;30:20–43. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1996.0003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Jacques S, Zelazo PD. The Flexible Item Selection Task (FIST): A measure of executive function in preschoolers. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2001;20(3):573–591. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2003_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Jacques S, Zelazo PD. On the possible roots of cognitive flexibility. In: Homer BD, Tamis-LeMonda CS, editors. The development of social cognition and communication. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2005. pp. 53–81. [Google Scholar]
  30. Keysar B, Lin S, Barr DJ. Limits on theory of mind use in adults. Cognition. 2003;89:25–41. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00064-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kimberg DY, Farah MJ. A unified account of cognitive impairments following frontal lobe damage: The role of working memory in complex, organized behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1993;122:411–428. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.122.4.411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kirkham NZ, Cruess L, Diamond A. Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science. 2003;6(5):449–476. [Google Scholar]
  33. Klein GS. Semantic power measured through the interference of words with color-naming. American Journal of Psychology. 1964;77:576–588. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kloo D, Perner J. Disentangling dimensions in the dimensional change card-sorting task. Developmental Science. 2005;8:44–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00392.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Koch I. Automatic and intentional activation of task sets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2001;27(6):1474–1486. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.27.6.1474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Koch I, Allport A. Cue-based preparation and stimulus-based priming of tasks in task switching. Memory and Cognition. 2006;34(2):433–444. doi: 10.3758/bf03193420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kray J, Kipp KH, Karbach J. The development of selective inhibitory control: The influence of verbal labeling. Acta Psychologica. 2009;130:48–57. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Liu CC, Smith PL. Comparing time-accuracy curves: Beyond goodness-of-fit measures. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2009;16(1):190–203. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.1.190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. MacLeod CM, Chiappe DL, Fox E. The crucial roles of stimulus matching and stimulus identity in negative priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2002;9(3):521–528. doi: 10.3758/bf03196308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Maes JHR, Damen MDC, Eling PATM. More learned irrelevance than perseveration errors in rule shifting in healthy subjects. Brain and Cognition. 2004;54:201–211. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Maes JHR, Vich J, Eling PATM. Learned irrelevance and response perseveration in a total change dimensional shift task. Brain and Cognition. 2006;62:74–79. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Malley GB, Strayer DL. Effect of stimulus repetition on positive and negative identity priming. Perception and Psychophysics. 1995;57(5):657–667. doi: 10.3758/bf03213271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. May CP, Kane MJ, Hasher L. Determinants of negative priming. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;118:35–54. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mayr U, Keele SW. Changing internal constraints on action: The role of backward inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2000;129:4–26. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.129.1.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Meiran N, Chorev Z, Sapir A. Component processes in task switching. Cognitive Psychology. 2000;41:211–253. doi: 10.1006/cogp.2000.0736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Miller J, Sproesser G, Ulrich R. Constant versus variable response signal delays in speed-accuracy trade-offs: Effects of advance preparation for processing time. Perception and Psychophysics. 2008;70(5):878–886. doi: 10.3758/pp.70.5.878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Monsell S, Taylor TJ, Murphy K. Naming the color of a word: Is it responses or task sets that compete. Memory and Cognition. 2001;29(1):137–151. doi: 10.3758/bf03195748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Monsell S, Yeung N, Azuma R. Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task? Psychological Research. 2000;63:250–264. doi: 10.1007/s004269900005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Morton JB, Munakata Y. Active versus latent representations: A neural network model of perseveration, dissociation, and decalage. Developmental Psychobiology. 2002;40:255–265. doi: 10.1002/dev.10033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Müller U, Dick AS, Gela K, Overton WF, Zelazo PD. The role of negative priming in preschoolers' flexible rule use on the Dimensional Change Card Sort task. Child Development. 2006;77(2):395–412. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00878.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Müller U, Zelazo PD, Lurye LE, Liebermann DP. The effect of labeling on preschool children's performance in the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task. Cognitive Development. 2008;23:395–408. [Google Scholar]
  52. Munakata Y, Morton JB, Yeyrs BE. Children's perseveration: Attentional inertia and alternative accounts. Developmental Science. 2003;6:471–473. [Google Scholar]
  53. Neill WT. Inhibitory and facilitatory processes in selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1977;3:444–450. [Google Scholar]
  54. Neill WT, Valdes LA. Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1992;18(3):565–576. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.18.5.993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Neill WT, Valdes LA, Terry KM, Gorfein DS. Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1992;18(5):993–1000. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.18.5.993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Park J, Kanwisher N. Negative priming for spatial locations: Identity mismatching, not distractor inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1994;20:613–623. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.3.613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Perner J, Lang B. What causes 3-year-olds' difficulty on the dimensional change card sorting task. Infant and Child Development. 2002;11:93–105. [Google Scholar]
  58. Pritchard VE, Neumann E. Negative Priming Effects in Children Engaged in Nonspatial Tasks: Evidence for Early Development of an Intact Inhibitory Mechanism. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40(2):12. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ratcliff R. Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin. 1993;114(3):510–532. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Rogers RD, Monsell S. Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1995;124:207–231. [Google Scholar]
  61. Rosenthal R, Rosnow RL, Rubin DB. Contrasts and effect sizes in behavioral research: A correlational approach. Cambridge University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  62. Schneider DW, Logan GD. Modeling task switching without switching tasks: A short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2005;134(3):343–367. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.3.343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Schuch S, Koch I. The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2003;29(1):92–105. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.29.1.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sheskin DJ. Handbook of parametric and non-parametric statistical procedures. 4th. Boca Raton, London, New York: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  65. Simone PM, McCormick EB. Effect of a Defining Feature on Negative Priming Across the Life Span. Visual Cognition. 1999;6(5):587–606. [Google Scholar]
  66. Thorpe S, Fize D, Marlot C. Speed of processing in the human visual system. Nature. 1996;381:520–522. doi: 10.1038/381520a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Tipper SP. The negative priming effect: Inhibitory effects of ignored primes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1985;37A:571–590. doi: 10.1080/14640748508400920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Tipper SP, Bourque TA, Anderson SH, Brehaut JC. Mechanisms of attention: a developmental study. Journal of experimental child psychology(Print) 1989;48(3):353–378. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(89)90047-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Tipper SP, Cranston M. Selective attention and priming: Inhibitory and facilitatory effects of ignored primes. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1985;37(4):591–611. doi: 10.1080/14640748508400921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Towse JN, Redbond J, Houston-Price CMT, Cook S. Understanding the dimensional change card sort: Perspectives from task success and failure. Cognitive Development. 2000;15(3):347–365. [Google Scholar]
  71. Tulving E, Schacter D. Priming and human memory systems. Science. 1990;247(4940):301–306. doi: 10.1126/science.2296719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Waszak F, Hommel B, Allport A. Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology. 2003;46:361–413. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0285(02)00520-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Wiggs CL, Martin A. Properties and mechanisms of perceptual priming. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1998;8:227–233. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(98)80144-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Yeung N, Monsell S. Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: The role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2003;29(2):455–469. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Yeyrs BE, Munakata Y. When labels hurt but novelty helps: Children's perseveration and flexibility in a card-sorting task. Child Development. 2006;77(6):1589–1607. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00961.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Zelazo PD. The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols. 2006;1:297–301. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Zelazo PD, Frye D, Rapus T. An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development. 1996;11:37–63. [Google Scholar]
  78. Zelazo PD, Müller U, Frye D, Marcovitch S. The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2003;68(3, Serial No. 274) doi: 10.1111/j.0037-976x.2003.00260.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES