Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Feb 26.
Published in final edited form as: Ann Neurol. 2013 Feb 26;73(2):170–179. doi: 10.1002/ana.23836

Cardiovascular Health through Young Adulthood and Cognitive Functioning in Midlife

Jared P Reis 1, Catherine M Loria 1, Lenore J Launer 2, Stephen Sidney 3, Kiang Liu 4, David R Jacobs Jr 5,6, Na Zhu 5, Donald M Lloyd-Jones 4, Ka He 7, Kristine Yaffe 8
PMCID: PMC3608821  NIHMSID: NIHMS428048  PMID: 23443990

Abstract

Objective

To examine the association between overall cardiovascular health as recently defined by the American Heart Association in young adulthood to middle-age and cognitive function in midlife. Overall ideal cardiovascular health incorporates 7 metrics, including the avoidance of overweight or obesity, a healthful diet, nonsmoking, and physical activity, total cholesterol, blood pressure, and fasting glucose at goal levels.

Methods

This analysis of the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study, a multicenter community-based study with 25 years of follow-up, included 2,932 participants aged 18 to 30 years at baseline (Year 0) who attended follow-up exams at Years 7 and 25. Cardiovascular health metrics were measured at each examination. The Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST), modified Stroop Test, and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) were completed at Year 25.

Results

A greater number of ideal cardiovascular metrics in young adulthood and middle-age was independently associated with better cognitive function in midlife (p-trend<0.01, for all). Specifically, each additional ideal metric was associated with 1.32 more symbols on the DSST (95% CI: 0.93 to 1.71), a 0.77-point lower interference score on the Stroop Test (−1.03 to −0.45), and 0.12 more words on the RAVLT (0.04 to 0.20). Participants who had ≥5 ideal metrics at a greater number of the 3 examinations over the 25-year period exhibited better performance on each cognitive test in middle-age (p-trend<0.01, for all).

Interpretation

Ideal cardiovascular health in young adulthood and its maintenance to middle-age is associated with better psychomotor speed, executive function, and verbal memory in midlife.


Currently, modifiable lifestyle behaviors and the control or avoidance of cardiovascular disease risk factors represent some of the most promising strategies for the prevention of dementia. Obesity, poor diet, smoking, lack of physical activity, and elevated levels of total cholesterol, blood pressure, and fasting glucose in middle-age have each been found to be associated with cognitive decline and dementia in later adulthood.16 In their recently released Strategic Impact Goals for 2020, the American Heart Association (AHA) detailed ideal levels for all 7 of these modifiable health behaviors and factors in order to define a new concept termed “cardiovascular health,” which has subsequently been shown to be strongly associated with a lower incidence of cardiovascular disease and related mortality.7, 8 Since many of these behaviors and factors often coexist and participate in mutual causal pathways, an overall ideal cardiovascular health profile may also be more effective in reducing the risk of cognitive dysfunction with aging than any single factor; however, little is known about the collective association between these 7 cardiovascular health components and cognition.

Cardiovascular risk factors measured during middle-age are associated with later adulthood cognitive dysfunction and decline.911 However, few, if any, studies have determined whether health factor levels in young adulthood may be associated with cognitive function in middle-age. If early adult cardiovascular health is associated with cognitive function in middle-age, increased efforts to encourage young adults to achieve and maintain these levels might lead to more adults reaching midlife at low risk for cognitive impairment and dementia in later life. The present study was conducted to investigate the simultaneous association between the 7 AHA ideal cardiovascular health components in young adulthood to middle-age and cognitive function in midlife.

The Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study provided an ideal setting in which to address this question because the community-based sample of participants were prospectively followed for 25 years with repeat detailed assessments of all 7 AHA cardiovascular health components beginning early in adulthood. The hypothesis was that a greater number of ideal components present during young adulthood to middle-age would be associated with better cognitive health in midlife.

Subjects and Methods

Study population

The CARDIA Study is a multicenter longitudinal study of the development and determinants of cardiovascular disease over time in 5,115 young adults initially aged 18–30 years in 1985–1986. Black and white adults were recruited from 4 US cities (Birmingham, Alabama; Chicago, Illinois; Minneapolis, Minnesota; and Oakland, California) with population-based samples approximately balanced within center by sex, age (18–24 years and 25–30 years), race (white, black), and education (≤high school, >high school). To date, participants have been re-examined 2, 5, 7, 10, 15, 20, and 25 years after baseline and retention rates across examinations were 91%, 86%, 81%, 79%, 74%, 72%, and 72%, respectively. Further details of study recruitment and design are available.12 All participants provided written informed consent at each examination, and institutional review boards from each field center and the coordinating center approved the study annually.

Participants for the current study included 2,932 adults who attended the Year 0 (baseline), 7, and 25 examinations and completed cognitive testing at Year 25. The choice of these 3 examinations was based on the assessment of dietary intake (Years 0, 7, and 20) and cognitive function (Year 25) in CARDIA. Dietary intake collected at Year 20 was used to approximate diet at Year 25. Women who were pregnant at the time of the Year 0, 7, 20, or 25 examinations were excluded.

Ascertainment of health behaviors and factors

Standardized protocols for data collection and quality control were used across study centers and examinations. Participants were asked to fast for at least 12 h before each examination and to avoid smoking or engaging in heavy physical activity for at least 2 h. Weight and height were measured at each examination with participants wearing light examination clothes and no shoes. Body weight was measured to the nearest 0.2 kg with a calibrated balance-beam scale. Height was measured with a vertical ruler to the nearest 0.5 cm. Body mass index (BMI) was calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared. Dietary intake for the past 28 days was collected through an interviewer-administered diet history questionnaire specifically developed for the CARDIA study at Year 0, 7, and 20.13 The validity and reliability of this diet history questionnaire was evaluated and was described in detail.14 Diet data for participants with extreme energy intake (<800 or >8,000 kcal/d for men and <600 or > 6,000 kcal/d for women) were set to missing. Cigarette smoking status was based on self-report. Physical activity was measured with the CARDIA Physical Activity History questionnaire which queries the amount of time per week spent in 13 categories of leisure, occupational, and household physical activities over the past 12 mo.15 Physical activity level was summarized as units of total activity incorporating moderate and high intensity activities.

Blood pressure was measured on the right arm with a Hawksley random zero sphygmomanometer (WA Baum Company, Copaigue, NY) by trained and certified technicians using standardized methods after the participant had rested for 5 minutes at Year 0 and 7. At Year 25, a digital blood pressure monitor was used (Omron HEM-907XL; Online Fitness, Santa Monica, CA). Three measurements were obtained at 1-min intervals. The average of the second and third measurements was used in analyses.

Blood was drawn by venipuncture according to a standard protocol.16 Total cholesterol was measured enzymatically as previously described.17 Glucose was measured at Year 0 using the hexokinase ultraviolet method by American Bio-Science Laboratories (Van Nuys, CA), and at Year 7 and 25 using hexokinase coupled to glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase by Linco Research (St. Louis, MO).

Definition of cardiovascular health metrics

We classified each health behavior and factor using the AHA definition of ideal, intermediate, and poor health with slight modifications made only for diet and physical activity (Supplemental Table 1).18

Cognitive function assessment

A battery of standardized tests to measure cognitive function was included at the Year 25 examination. Cognitive training and certification of CARDIA technicians was performed centrally by CARDIA investigators and coordinating center data quality assurance staff. The Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST), a subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (3rd edition), assesses an array of cognitive domains, most prominently visual motor speed, sustained attention, and working memory. The range of scores is 0 to 133, with increasing scores indicating better performance. The Stroop Test evaluates the ability to view complex visual stimuli and to respond to one stimulus dimension while suppressing the response to another dimension, an “executive” skill largely attributed to frontal lobe function.19 The interference score provides a measure of how much additional executive processing is needed to respond to an incongruent trial; thus, a higher interference score indicates worse performance on the task. The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) assesses the ability to memorize and to retrieve words (verbal memory). Results from the long delay (10 min) free recall were used in analyses. The range of scores is 0 to 15, with increasing scores indicating better performance.

Statistical analysis

Multiple imputation was used to impute missing values using the sequential regression imputation approach that is implemented in the software package IV Eware.20 Five datasets were generated using data from all 8 examinations resulting in complete Year 0, 7, and 25 data for the sample of 2,932 participants. Each complete data set was analyzed separately and results from the 5 analyses were combined using Rubin’s rules.21 We also performed a sensitivity analysis on 1,753 participants who had complete information on all health behaviors and factors across all 3 exams and covariates.

The total number of cardiovascular health components at ideal levels was calculated based upon the average level of each factor across the Year 0, 7, and 25 exams as well as the number present at Year 0 and 25, separately. The score ranged from 0 (no health behaviors or factors at ideal levels) to 7 (all health behaviors and factors at ideal levels). We also examined mean cognitive function scores according to the presence of 5 or more ideal health metrics at none, 1, 2, or all 3 of the Year 0, 7, and 25 examinations. A threshold of 5 or more ideal metrics was used to provide more statistically stable estimates since too few participants had 6 or 7 ideal metrics.

Multivariable linear regression was used to estimate the association between the cardiovascular health components, the number of exams with 5 or more at ideal levels, and each cognitive function score after adjusting for age (years), sex, race (black, white), educational attainment (years), alcohol use (ml/day, average, Year 0, or Year 25), and study center (Birmingham, Chicago, Minneapolis, Oakland). Tests for a linear trend were determined by entering each individual health component, the total number of ideal components, or the number of examinations with 5 or more at ideal levels into the model as an ordinal term. Tests of statistical significance were 2-tailed, with an alpha level of 0.05. SAS version 9.1 (Cary, NC) was used to perform all analyses.

Results

Of the 2,932 participants who attended the Year 0, 7, and 25 examinations and completed cognitive testing at Year 25, 44.8% were black and 55.4% were women. Supplemental Table 2 compares the baseline characteristics of those included in the analysis to those who were not included. A similar proportion of women and nondrinkers were included in the analysis as compared to those who were not included. The prevalence of ideal health for BMI, physical activity, total cholesterol, blood pressure, and glucose at baseline were also similar. However, included participants were somewhat more likely to be younger, white, have a higher education, and have an ideal diet score and smoking status.

Prevalence of ideal health for the individual and total number of components at baseline and Year 25 are displayed in Figures 1 and 2. After 25 years, the prevalence of meeting each ideal metric decreased for all behaviors and factors except nonsmoking, which increased during this period (Figure 1). At baseline, few participants met only 0, 1, or 2 ideal metrics; most met 4 or 5, while 17.5% and 1.5% met 6 or 7, respectively (Figure 2). At Year 25, fewer participants met 4 or more metrics; participants most commonly had 3. Those who met a greater number throughout young adulthood were younger at baseline, more likely to be white, and attain a college education (Table 1). Mean levels of the individual behaviors and factors at baseline were associated with the total number of ideal health components, except for sodium intake.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Prevalence of individual ideal cardiovascular health components at baseline and Year 25, CARDIA (n = 2,932). Healthful diet score is based on diet measured at baseline and Year 20.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Prevalence of ideal cardiovascular health components at baseline and Year 25, CARDIA (n = 2,932).

Table 1.

Baseline characteristics according to the number of ideal cardiovascular health components based upon the average of baseline, Year 7, and Year 25 values, CARDIA (n = 2,932).

No. of ideal cardiovascular health components
p-trend
0–1 (n = 149) 2 (n = 347) 3 (n = 547) 4 (n = 807) 5 (n = 707) 6 (n = 361) 7 (n = 14)
Age, years 26.0 26.1 25.1 25.0 25.0 24.8 26.3 < 0.001
Men, % 41.6 45.0 44.4 46.2 45.3 41.0 40.0 > 0.2
Black, % 69.1 61.1 57.0 48.6 31.8 18.8 7.1 < 0.001
Education (college), Year 25, % 26.2 35.2 41.7 48.0 60.7 76.2 78.6 < 0.001
Nondrinker, % 41.6 39.2 37.8 40.2 36.5 33.5 35.7 0.08
Health behaviors and factors
 Body mass index, kg/m2 29.1 27.9 26.0 24.4 22.5 21.3 21.6 < 0.001
 Never smoker or quit > 12 mo, % 46.3 56.5 62.5 68.8 73.9 79.8 92.9 < 0.001
 Physical activity, exercise units 250.6 345.6 357.0 419.9 464.5 590.4 611.4 < 0.001
 Total cholesterol, mg/dL 199.5 192.9 182.6 175.2 171.0 163.2 165.8 < 0.001
 Systolic blood pressure, mmHg 115.6 115.1 111.4 110.3 107.7 105.6 103.4 < 0.001
 Diastolic blood pressure, mmHg 73.1 70.9 69.1 68.5 67.1 66.4 64.4 < 0.001
 Fasting glucose, mg/dL 89.2 85.7 82.1 81.1 80.6 80.7 81.9 < 0.001
 Dietary intake
  Total energy, kcal/d 2,831.7 2,844.8 2,917.5 2,861.8 2,777.6 2,729.0 2,208.4 0.09
  Total fruits/vegetables, servings/d 4.3 4.5 4.6 4.9 5.4 6.2 8.2 < 0.001
  Sodium, mg/d 4,079.5 4,190.6 4,324.5 4,284.5 4,142.3 4,076.0 3,198.4 > 0.2
  Fish, oz/wk 4.3 4.6 4.7 5.3 5.4 6.0 13.9 < 0.001
  Whole grains, servings/d 0.9 1.0 1.1 1.2 1.5 1.9 3.7 < 0.001
  Sugar-sweetened beverages, oz/wk 97.8 84.0 88.1 76.4 60.3 44.5 4.3 < 0.001

Values are mean or percent as appropriate.

Mean scores at Year 25 on the DSST, Stroop Test, and RAVLT were 70.1±16.2 symbols (range: 8 to 125), 22.7±11.1 points (range: −24 to 127), and 8.4±3.3 words (range: 0 to 15), respectively. A greater number of ideal cardiovascular health components present at Year 0, Year 25, and based on the average of Year 0, 7, and 25 levels was strongly associated with better performance on all 3 cognitive tests (Table 2). Specifically, each additional ideal component (based on average levels) was associated with 1.32 more symbols on the DSST (95% CI: 0.93 to 1.71), a 0.77-point lower interference score on the Stroop Test (95% CI: −1.03 to −0.45), and 0.12 more words recalled on the RAVLT (95% CI: 0.04 to 0.20). These associations did not vary significantly by race (p-interaction >0.2, for all).

Table 2.

Multivariable-adjusted1 mean (95% CI) Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST), Stroop Test, and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) scores at Year 25 according to number of ideal cardiovascular health components at baseline (Year 0), Year 25, and average of Year 0, 7, and 25 values, CARDIA (n = 2,932).

Year 0 Year 25 Average
DSST (symbols)
 No. of ideal health components
  0–1 62.9 (56.6, 69.2) 66.7 (65.2, 68.2) 66.8 (64.6, 69.1)
  2 67.3 (64.9, 69.8) 68.1 (67.0, 69.1) 65.8 (64.4, 67.3)
  3 68.0 (66.7, 69.4) 68.8 (67.8, 69.8) 66.8 (65.6, 67.9)
  4 68.5 (67.6, 69.4) 70.5 (69.4, 71.6) 70.4 (69.5, 71.4)
  5 69.8 (68.9, 70.6) 69.9 (68.5, 71.3) 70.3 (69.3, 71.3)
  6 70.2 (69.0, 71.5) 72.5 (70.0, 75.0) 71.4 (70.0, 72.9)
  7 69.7 (65.5, 73.8) 71.7 (64.2, 79.2) 72.9 (65.8, 80.0)
   p-trend < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001
  Per each additional ideal component 0.84 (0.37, 1.30) 0.94 (0.55, 1.34) 1.32 (0.93, 1.71)
Stroop Test (points)
 No. of ideal health components
  0–1 30.7 (26.0, 35.4) 24.2 (23.1, 25.4) 25.7 (24.0, 27.3)
  2 25.1 (23.3, 27.0) 23.4 (22.6, 24.2) 25.0 (23.9, 26.1)
  3 23.8 (22.8, 24.8) 22.9 (22.2, 23.7) 23.4 (22.5, 24.2)
  4 23.2 (22.5, 23.9) 23.0 (22.1, 23.8) 22.7 (22.0, 23.4)
  5 22.6 (22.0, 23.3) 22.4 (21.4, 23.5) 22.3 (21.5, 23.1)
  6 22.2 (21.3, 23.1) 21.5 (19.6, 23.3) 22.0 (20.9, 23.1)
  7 24.0 (20.9, 27.1) 21.8 (16.2, 27.4) 21.4 (16.1, 26.6)
   p-trend < 0.001 0.004 < 0.001
  Per each additional ideal component −0.65 (−0.99, −0.31) −0.40 (−0.69, −0.11) −0.77 (−1.03, −0.45)
RAVLT (words)
 No. of ideal health components
  0–1 6.7 (5.4, 8.0) 7.8 (7.5, 8.2) 7.5 (7.1, 8.0)
  2 8.0 (7.4, 8.5) 8.0 (7.7, 8.2) 7.8 (7.5, 8.1)
  3 8.0 (7.7, 8.3) 8.3 (8.1, 8.5) 8.2 (7.9, 8.4)
  4 8.2 (8.0, 8.4) 8.2 (8.0, 8.4) 8.4 (8.2, 8.6)
  5 8.2 (8.0, 8.3) 8.4 (8.1, 8.7) 8.2 (8.0, 8.4)
  6 8.5 (8.3, 8.8) 8.6 (8.1, 9.1) 8.3 (8.0, 8.6)
  7 7.5 (6.6, 8.3) 8.6 (7.1, 10.2) 7.5 (6.0, 9.0)
   p-trend 0.003 0.003 0.005
  Per each additional ideal component 0.12 (0.02, 0.22) 0.12 (0.04, 0.20) 0.12 (0.04, 0.20)
1

Adjusted for age (years), sex, race (black, white), educational attainment (years), alcohol use (ml/d at baseline, Year 25, or average), and study center (Birmingham, Chicago, Minneapolis, Oakland).

Table 3 shows the multivariable-adjusted mean scores for the DSST, Stroop Test, and RAVLT for participants who had 5 or more ideal health components at none, 1, 2, or all 3 of the Year 0, 7, and 25 examinations. Mean scores were higher for the DSST and RAVLT, and lower for the Stroop Test among participants who had 5 or more ideal components at a greater number of exams (p-trend<0.01, for all).

Table 3.

Multivariable-adjusted1 mean (95% CI) Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST), Stroop Test, and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) scores at Year 25 by presence of ≥5 ideal cardiovascular health components at none, 1, 2, or all 3 of the baseline (Year 0), Year 7, and Year 25 examinations, CARDIA (n = 2,932).

≥5 ideal cardiovascular health components present at: n (%) DSST (symbols) Stroop Test (points) RAVLT (words)
No exams 1091 (37.2) 67.8 (67.0, 68.7) 23.8 (23.2, 24.4) 8.0 (7.8, 8.1)
1 exam 806 (27.5) 69.0 (68.0, 69.9) 23.0 (22.3, 23.7) 8.3 (8.1, 8.5)
2 exams 679 (23.2) 70.1 (69.0, 71.3) 22.5 (21.7, 23.3) 8.2 (8.0, 8.5)
All 3 exams 356 (12.1) 71.4 (69.8, 73.1) 21.8 (20.7, 23.0) 8.5 (8.2, 8.8)
 p-trend < 0.001 0.001 0.006
1

Adjusted for age (years), sex, race (black, white), educational attainment (years), alcohol use (average ml/d), and study center (Birmingham, Chicago, Minneapolis, Oakland).

We also examined the association between each of the 7 individual health behaviors and factors and cognitive function (Table 4). Smoking status and blood pressure displayed significant graded associations with all 3 tests of cognition (p-trend<0.01, for all). BMI, diet, physical activity, and glucose were each associated with performance on the DSST (p-trend<0.01, for all). BMI, physical activity, and glucose were also associated with scores on the Stroop Test (p-trend<0.05, for all). Total cholesterol was associated with results on the RAVLT only (p-trend<0.001).

Table 4.

Multivariable-adjusted1 mean (95% CI) Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST), Stroop Test, and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) scores at Year 25 by individual cardiovascular health components based upon the average of baseline, Year 7, and Year 25 levels, CARDIA (n = 2,932).

DSST (symbols) Stroop Test (points) RAVLT (words)
Body mass index
 Ideal health 69.7 (68.9, 70.5) 22.7 (22.0, 23.2) 8.1 (8.0, 8.3)
 Intermediate health 68.9 (68.0, 69.7) 22.7 (22.1, 23.3) 8.2 (8.0, 8.4)
 Poor hevalth 68.3 (67.3, 69.4) 24.3 (23.6, 25.1) 8.2 (7.9, 8.4)
  p-trend 0.04 0.002 >0.2
Diet score
 Ideal health 70.0 (66.1, 74.7) 23.1 (19.9, 26.3) 7.8 (6.9, 8.7)
 Intermediate health 69.9 (69.0, 70.8) 22.6 (21.9, 23.3) 8.3 (8.1, 8.5)
 Poor health 68.6 (67.9, 69.3) 23.3 (22.8, 23.8) 8.1 (8.0, 8.2)
  p-trend 0.03 0.1 >0.2
Smoking
 Ideal health 70.1 (69.5, 70.6) 22.6 (22.2, 23.1) 8.3 (8.2, 8.4)
 Intermediate health 67.3 (64.1, 70.4) 22.8 (20.4, 25.1) 7.9 (7.3, 8.6)
 Poor health 64.3 (62.9, 65.6) 25.3 (24.3, 26.3) 7.6 (7.3, 7.9)
  p-trend <0.001 <0.001 <0.001
Physical activity
 Ideal health 70.1 (69.4, 70.8) 22.6 (22.1, 23.1) 8.1 (8.0, 8.3)
 Intermediate health 68.2 (67.3, 69.0) 23.5 (22.9, 24.1) 8.4 (8.2, 8.5)
 Poor health 66.0 (64.1, 67.8) 24.3 (23.0, 25.8) 7.7 (7.3, 8.1)
  p-trend <0.001 0.006 >0.2
Total cholesterol
 Ideal health 69.2 (68.6, 69.8) 22.9 (22.4, 23.3) 8.3 (8.2, 8.4)
 Intermediate health 68.6 (67.8, 69.5) 23.5 (22.9, 24.2) 8.0 (7.8, 8.2)
 Poor health 70.5 (67.4, 73.6) 23.1 (20.8, 25.4) 7.5 (6.8, 8.1)
  p-trend >0.2 0.2 <0.001
Blood pressure
 Ideal health 69.9 (69.3, 70.6) 22.6 (22.1, 23.1) 8.3 (8.2, 8.4)
 Intermediate health 68.1 (67.3, 68.9) 23.5 (22.9, 24.1) 8.0 (7.8, 8.2)
 Poor health 63.4 (59.8, 67.1) 29.0 (26.3, 31.6) 8.0 (7.3, 8.7)
  p-trend <0.001 <0.001 0.008
Glucose
 Ideal health 69.3 (68.8, 69.9) 22.9 (22.4, 23.3) 8.2 (8.1, 8.3)
 Intermediate health 68.8 (67.4, 70.3) 23.9 (22.8, 25.0) 8.1 (7.8, 8.4)
 Poor health 62.2 (59.1, 65.4) 25.6 (23.3, 28.0) 7.5 (6.8, 8.1)
  p-trend 0.001 0.006 0.1
1

Adjusted for age (years), sex, race (black, white), educational attainment (years), alcohol use (average ml/d), and study center (Birmingham, Chicago, Minneapolis, Oakland).

We also created a score out of the 7 health behaviors and factors ranging from 0 to 14 by assigning a value of 0 for poor health, 1 for intermediate health, and 2 for ideal health based upon average levels for each component. Each unit increase in the score was associated with 1.08 more symbols on the DSST (95% CI: 0.81, 1.35), a 0.65-point lower interference score on the Stroop Test (95% CI: −0.86, −0.45), and 0.11 more words recalled on the RAVLT (95% CI: 0.06, 0.17).

A sensitivity analysis on the 1,753 participants with complete information on all health behaviors and factors and covariates revealed similar results. Specifically, each additional component in the ideal range (based upon average levels of the components) was associated with 1.39 more symbols on the DSST (95% CI: 0.91, 1.89), a 0.78-point lower interference score on the Stroop Test (95% CI: −1.13, −0.43), and 0.11 more words recalled on the RAVLT (95% CI: 0.01, 0.22).

Discussion

In this longitudinal community-based study of adults, we found a strong graded association between a greater number of ideal cardiovascular health components from young adulthood to middle-age and better visual motor speed, executive function, and verbal memory in middle-age. This association was independent of a number of potential confounding factors including educational attainment and alcohol consumption. In addition, participants who achieved and maintained a greater number of ideal components throughout young adulthood exhibited better performance on each test of cognition during middle-age.

It is becoming increasingly clear that dementia has a long preclinical period. This observation has led to calls for the study of risk factors and cognitive outcomes many years prior to the clinical diagnosis of disease.22, 23 Several longitudinal studies have shown that midlife risk factor levels, including blood pressure, cholesterol, glucose, and lifestyle behaviors are associated with cognitive function and impairment in later adulthood.24, 25 In addition, early pathologic evidence of Alzheimer’s disease and white matter hyperintensities on magnetic resonance images has been observed in middle-aged adults.26, 27 Our results add to the previous evidence by showing that cardiovascular behaviors and factors beginning at a younger age in the adult lifecourse may influence cognitive function at older ages. Future studies are needed to determine whether ideal cardiovascular health in young adulthood contributes to delays in aging related cognitive decline and a lower risk of dementia through older adulthood.

Previous studies have investigated the individual components or groups of components of the ideal cardiovascular health score included in the current study. Mid- or late-life hypertension, dyslipidemia, diabetes, obesity, smoking, physical inactivity, and poor diet have each been shown to be independently associated with a greater risk of developing cognitive impairment or dementia due to vascular disease or Alzheimer’s disease.1, 2, 46, 11, 28 The metabolic syndrome, a cluster of metabolic risk factors, including abdominal obesity, hypertension, dyslipidemia, and hyperglycemia has been shown to contribute to cognitive decline and impairment.29, 30 In addition, a greater number of behaviors, including smoking, alcohol abstinence, low physical activity, and low fruit and vegetable intake has been reported to be associated with poor executive function and memory in late middle-age.25 However, to the best of our knowledge, no available study has investigated the combined association between both cardiovascular health behaviors and metabolic factors and cognitive function. Accumulating evidence suggests ideal cardiovascular health defined using various definitions including the AHA 2020 goals is strongly associated with a lower risk of cardiovascular events and related mortality, a greater quality of life, and lower Medicare expenditures in later life.7, 3134 The results of the present study further suggest that ideal cardiovascular health consistent with AHA 2020 goals from young adulthood to middle-age is also strongly associated with better performance on tests designed to assess a wide variety of cognitive domains in mid-life.

The mean differences we observed in cognitive test scores with each additional ideal health component was clearly modest; nevertheless, effect sizes of that magnitude may have clinical relevance. For example, in our study population every 1-year increase in age was associated with 0.56 less symbols substituted correctly on the DSST, a 0.28 higher interference score on the Stroop Test, and 0.03 less words recalled correctly on the RAVLT (data not shown). Therefore, the effect sizes we observed with each additional ideal health component correspond to an age difference of 2–4 years. In addition, in a 3-year follow-up study of individuals with minimal cognitive impairment, a 1-point difference in baseline DSST score was significantly associated with a higher risk of Alzheimer’s disease.35 In another study of community dwelling adults aged >70 years, a 1-point difference in baseline DSST score was associated with a 3% higher risk of dementia.36

Results of the current study are also consistent with a number of recent reports suggesting that few US adults meet the AHA 2020 ideal goals for all 7 ideal health components.7, 8, 31, 37 Only 1.5% and 0.4% of participants met all 7 ideal metrics in young adulthood and middle-age, respectively. In an analysis of data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, Yang et al.8 estimated that only about 1.2% to 2.0% of the US adult population aged ≥20 years met all 7 ideal goals between 1988–1994 and 2005–2010. Despite these low prevalence estimates, the majority of participants in the current study (51.9%) achieved at least 5 ideal metrics in young adulthood (i.e., baseline; Figure 2), however, only 12.1% of participants maintained this number through early adulthood to middle-age (Table 3). It is clear that reaching the AHA 2020 ideal goals poses a significant challenge for clinical and public health professionals. Our data suggest that coordinated efforts should begin earlier in adulthood in order to stem the adoption of adverse health behaviors that become more frequent in middle-age.

Of the 7 behaviors and factors investigated in the current study, 6 showed significant graded associations with visual motor speed, 5 with executive function, and 3 with memory. These results suggest that delays in aging related declines in these cognitive domains may be feasible when individuals move from poor to intermediate health and intermediate to ideal health. The reasons for the differences in the associations between the specific health behaviors and factors and the cognitive domains are not clear, but may be due at least in part to the fact that memory is likely to be less influenced by vascular risk factors, findings that have been observed by a number of studies.10, 25, 30

Blood pressure and smoking during young adulthood to middle-age displayed strong, graded associations with each cognitive domain in midlife. Midlife blood pressure ranks as an important modifiable risk factor for late-life cognitive decline,10 mild cognitive impairment,9 and vascular dementia.38 Several prospective studies also show an increased risk for cognitive decline in smokers compared to nonsmokers.1 As mentioned earlier, our findings provide further evidence that the levels of these behaviors and factors as well as others during early adulthood to middle-age may also be important determinants of cognitive outcomes in later life.

Important strengths of our study include a population-based sampling method; a biracial cohort; extensive data on potential confounders; a large sample size well balanced with respect to age, sex, race, and education that increased precision and permitted simultaneous adjustment for multiple variables; repeat detailed assessments of cardiovascular health behaviors and factors across a 25-year period beginning in early adulthood; and the standardized data collection protocols and rigorous quality control of the CARDIA study.

Nevertheless, several limitations deserve mention. First, we assessed cognitive function at only a single time in midlife and therefore we were unable to determine whether ideal cardiovascular health in young adulthood is associated with delays in aging related cognitive decline. It may be possible that greater cognitive ability led to a lifestyle of healthier behaviors in young adulthood to middle-age.39 Second, participants included in the analysis were somewhat more likely at baseline to have a healthier diet and an ideal smoking status than those not included. This may have led to an underestimation of the association between ideal cardiovascular health and cognitive function. Third, since our analysis included several health behaviors and factors measured across 3 examinations, many participants were missing at least 1 measurement. However, we noted similar results using our multiple imputed datasets and another dataset including participants with complete information on all study variables. Fourth, this is an observational analysis, and residual confounding, particularly by socioeconomic status, may be present. Conversely, our results were robust to adjustment for midlife educational attainment, making it improbable that the observed associations are owing completely to residual confounding.

In summary, the present long-term community-based study showed that few adults in young adulthood or middle-age met all 7 AHA 2020 goals for ideal cardiovascular health. Nevertheless, there was a strong graded association between the presence of a greater number of ideal cardiovascular health components in young adulthood to middle-age and better cognitive health in midlife. In addition, participants who achieved and maintained a greater number of ideal components throughout young adulthood exhibited better performance on each test of cognition in middle-age. These results suggest that attainment of the AHA 2020 goals for ideal cardiovascular health in young adulthood and maintenance to middle-age could have important implications for cognitive outcomes in later life.

Supplementary Material

Supp Table S1
Supp Table S2

Acknowledgments

The Coronary Risk Development in Young Adults Study (CARDIA) is conducted and supported by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI) in collaboration with the University of Alabama at Birmingham (N01-HC95095 & N01-HC48047), Kaiser Foundation Research Institute (N01-HC48050), Northwestern University (N01-HC48049), and the University of Minnesota (N01-HC48048).

References

  • 1.Anstey KJ, von Sanden C, Salim A, O’Kearney R. Smoking as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. American journal of epidemiology. 2007;166:367–378. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Beydoun MA, Beydoun HA, Wang Y. Obesity and central obesity as risk factors for incident dementia and its subtypes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev. 2008;9:204–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2008.00473.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Reitz C, Tang MX, Manly J, et al. Hypertension and the risk of mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:1734–1740. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.12.1734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Saczynski JS, Jonsdottir MK, Garcia ME, et al. Cognitive impairment: an increasingly important complication of type 2 diabetes: the age, gene/environment susceptibility--Reykjavik study. American journal of epidemiology. 2008;168:1132–1139. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Solomon A, Kareholt I, Ngandu T, et al. Serum total cholesterol, statins and cognition in non-demented elderly. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30:1006–1009. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Weuve J, Kang JH, Manson JE, et al. Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. Jama. 2004;292:1454–1461. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.12.1454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Folsom AR, Yatsuya H, Nettleton JA, et al. Community prevalence of ideal cardiovascular health, by the American Heart Association definition, and relationship with cardiovascular disease incidence. Journal of the American College of Cardiology. 2011;57:1690–1696. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2010.11.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Yang Q, Cogswell ME, Flanders WD, et al. Trends in cardiovascular health metrics and associations with all-cause and CVD mortality among US adults. Jama. 2012;307:1273–1283. doi: 10.1001/jama.2012.339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kivipelto M, Helkala EL, Hanninen T, et al. Midlife vascular risk factors and late-life mild cognitive impairment: A population-based study. Neurology. 2001;56:1683–1689. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.12.1683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Knopman D, Boland LL, Mosley T, et al. Cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive decline in middle-aged adults. Neurology. 2001;56:42–48. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.1.42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Launer LJ, Masaki K, Petrovitch H, et al. The association between midlife blood pressure levels and late-life cognitive function. The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Jama. 1995;274:1846–1851. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hughes GH, Cutter G, Donahue R, et al. Recruitment in the Coronary Artery Disease Risk Development in Young Adults (Cardia) Study. Controlled clinical trials. 1987;8:68S–73S. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(87)90008-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.McDonald A, Van Horn L, Slattery M, et al. The CARDIA dietary history: development, implementation, and evaluation. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 1991;91:1104–1112. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Liu K, Slattery M, Jacobs D, Jr, et al. A study of the reliability and comparative validity of the cardia dietary history. Ethnicity & disease. 1994;4:15–27. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Jacobs DR, Jr, Hahn LP, Haskell WL, et al. Validity and reliability of short physical activity history: CARDIA and Minnesota Heart Health Program. J Cardiopulm Rehabil. 1989;9:448–459. doi: 10.1097/00008483-198911000-00003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Friedman GD, Cutter GR, Donahue RP, et al. CARDIA: study design, recruitment, and some characteristics of the examined subjects. Journal of clinical epidemiology. 1988;41:1105–1116. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(88)90080-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bild DE, Jacobs DR, Liu K, et al. Seven-year trends in plasma low-density-lipoprotein-cholesterol in young adults: the CARDIA Study. Annals of epidemiology. 1996;6:235–245. doi: 10.1016/1047-2797(96)00005-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lloyd-Jones DM, Hong Y, Labarthe D, et al. Defining and setting national goals for cardiovascular health promotion and disease reduction: the American Heart Association’s strategic Impact Goal through 2020 and beyond. Circulation. 2010;121:586–613. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.109.192703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.MacLeod CM. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol Bull. 1991;109:163–203. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Raghunathan TE, Lepkowski JM, VanHoewyk J, Solenberger P. A multivariate technique for multiply imputing missing values using a sequence of regression models. Survey Methodology. 2001;27:85–95. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Little RJA, Rubin DB. Statistical Analysis with Missing Data. 2. New York: John Wiley; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Curb JD. The tangled story of plaques and arteries. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:1257–1258. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53365.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Launer LJ. The epidemiologic study of dementia: a life-long quest? Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26:335–340. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Gorelick PB, Scuteri A, Black SE, et al. Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia: a statement for healthcare professionals from the american heart association/american stroke association. Stroke; a journal of cerebral circulation. 2011;42:2672–2713. doi: 10.1161/STR.0b013e3182299496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Sabia S, Nabi H, Kivimaki M, et al. Health behaviors from early to late midlife as predictors of cognitive function: The Whitehall II study. American journal of epidemiology. 2009;170:428–437. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Braak H, Thal DR, Ghebremedhin E, Del Tredici K. Stages of the pathologic process in Alzheimer disease: age categories from 1 to 100 years. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2011;70:960–969. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e318232a379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Trauninger A, Leel-Ossy E, Kamson DO, et al. Risk factors of migraine-related brain white matter hyperintensities: an investigation of 186 patients. J Headache Pain. 2011;12:97–103. doi: 10.1007/s10194-011-0299-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Scarmeas N, Stern Y, Mayeux R, et al. Mediterranean diet and mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2009;66:216–225. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2008.536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Yaffe K, Haan M, Blackwell T, et al. Metabolic syndrome and cognitive decline in elderly Latinos: findings from the Sacramento Area Latino Study of Aging study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55:758–762. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01139.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Yaffe K, Kanaya A, Lindquist K, et al. The metabolic syndrome, inflammation, and risk of cognitive decline. Jama. 2004;292:2237–2242. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.18.2237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ford ES, Greenlund KJ, Hong Y. Ideal cardiovascular health and mortality from all causes and diseases of the circulatory system among adults in the United States. Circulation. 2012;125:987–995. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.111.049122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Daviglus ML, Liu K, Pirzada A, et al. Cardiovascular risk profile earlier in life and Medicare costs in the last year of life. Archives of internal medicine. 2005;165:1028–1034. doi: 10.1001/archinte.165.9.1028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Daviglus ML, Liu K, Pirzada A, et al. Favorable cardiovascular risk profile in middle age and health-related quality of life in older age. Archives of internal medicine. 2003;163:2460–2468. doi: 10.1001/archinte.163.20.2460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Yang Q, Cogswell ME, Flanders WD, et al. Trends in Cardiovascular Health Metrics and Associations With All-Cause and CVD Mortality Among US Adults. Jama. 2012 doi: 10.1001/jama.2012.339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Sarazin M, Berr C, De Rotrou J, et al. Amnestic syndrome of the medial temporal type identifies prodromal AD: a longitudinal study. Neurology. 2007;69:1859–1867. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000279336.36610.f7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Holtzer R, Verghese J, Wang C, et al. Within-person across-neuropsychological test variability and incident dementia. Jama. 2008;300:823–830. doi: 10.1001/jama.300.7.823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Shay CM, Ning H, Allen NB, et al. Status of cardiovascular health in US adults: prevalence estimates from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys (NHANES) 2003–2008. Circulation. 2012;125:45–56. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.111.035733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Launer LJ, Ross GW, Petrovitch H, et al. Midlife blood pressure and dementia: the Honolulu-Asia aging study. Neurobiol Aging. 2000;21:49–55. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(00)00096-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Deary IJ, Whiteman MC, Starr JM, et al. The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: following up the Scottish mental surveys of 1932 and 1947. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;86:130–147. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp Table S1
Supp Table S2

RESOURCES