Abstract
Although significant advances have been made in our understanding of the neural basis of action observation and intention understanding in the last few decades by studies demonstrating the involvement of a specific brain network (action observation network; AON), these have been largely based on experimental studies in which people have been considered as strictly isolated entities. However, we, as social species, spend much more of our time performing actions interacting with others. Research shows that a person's position along the continuum of perceived social isolation/bonding to others is associated with a variety of physical and mental health effects. Thus, there is a crucial need to better understand the neural basis of intention understanding performed in interpersonal and emotional contexts. To address this issue, we performed a meta-analysis using of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies over the past decade that examined brain and cortical network processing associated with understanding the intention of others actions vs. those associated with passionate love for others. Both overlapping and distinct cortical and subcortical regions were identified for intention and love, respectively. These findings provide scientists and clinicians with a set of brain regions that can be targeted for future neuroscientific studies on intention understanding, and help develop neurocognitive models of pair-bonding.
Keywords: social neuroscience, embodied cognition, intention understanding, love, neuroimaging, fMRI meta-analysis
Introduction
Throughout the past three decades, a growing number of studies have shown that one understands actions and intentions of other people by shaping one's understanding and anticipation of the environment based on one's own motor system (Jeannerod, 2001; Van Overwalle and Baetens, 2009; Becchio et al., 2012). Theories on embodied cognition and simulation extend these findings by suggesting that reading the intention of others occurs through a direct and automatic matching process between observed and performed actions, and via the re-activation of the bodily states that were originally active during past self-related sensori-motor experiences—as if the observers were “reliving” the observed motor experiences (Rizzolatti et al., 2001; Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Niedenthal et al., 2005; Lewis et al., 2006; Niedenthal, 2007; Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia, 2008; Grafton, 2009). The recent development in neuroimaging sheds lights on the neural activations mediating this mechanism (Grafton, 2009; Becchio et al., 2012). For instance, neuroimaging studies show that reading intentions of others recruit brain areas that are also activated when someone performs the same action (Grafton, 2009, for review; Ortigue et al., 2009; Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia, 2010). Interestingly, these brain areas can be divided into two functionally separable brain networks (Grafton, 2009; Ortigue et al., 2009). The first, referred to as the “action observation network” (AON), involves an inferior fronto-parietal network (FPN) and includes a subset of areas that are associated within the putative human Mirror Neuron System (hMNS; Grafton et al., 1996; Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Desmurget et al., 2009; Grafton, 2009). This AON system is thought to be particularly important for integrating sensori-motor information during perceptual judgments about actions (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004), and also for understanding hand-object interactions and intentions on the basis of embodied cognitive mechanisms (Iacoboni et al., 2005; Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia, 2007; Grafton, 2009). The second brain network, referred to as the “social-network” (SN), recruits brain areas involved in social interaction (Grafton, 2009; Sugiura et al., 2009; Wakusawa et al., 2009; Canessa et al., 2012). This SN includes the medial prefrontal cortex, precuneate cortex, insula, and amygdala (Wheatley et al., 2007; Grafton, 2009). Both networks include the posterior part of the superior temporal sulcus (pSTS); the superior temporal gyrus (STG), the middle temporal gyrus (MTG), and the part of the angular gyrus that is near the ascending limb of STS (Allison et al., 2000; Pelphrey et al., 2004; Thompson et al., 2005, 2007; Pelphrey and Morris, 2006; Materna et al., 2008). Interestingly, the STS region, notably its posterior part (pSTS), is also recruited by relatively low level processes such as observation of visual biological motion (Jellema et al., 2000), auditory biological actions (Bidet-Caulet et al., 2005; Gazzola et al., 2006; Lewis et al., 2011), and other operations such as social inferential processing in tasks requiring mentalizing, and theory of mind (Grezes et al., 2004; Saxe et al., 2004; Schultz et al., 2004; Grossman et al., 2005; Frith and Frith, 2006; Brass et al., 2007; Van Overwalle and Baetens, 2009).
Although these findings provide valuable information about the brain mechanisms involved in the understanding of actions performed by strangers, they do not tell much about the brain mechanisms involved in the understanding of a significant other [a person with whom the participant intends to be with (i.e., a participant's partner in an intimate relationship or a best friend in an companionate relationship)]. To date, studies on intention understanding have been largely based on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies in which participants have been considered as strictly isolated entities i.e., focusing mostly on the action type rather than on the relationship with the agent and the observer. However, people typically spend most of their time in social settings interacting with significant others. Research shows that a person's position along the continuum of perceived social isolation/bonding to others is associated with a variety of physical and mental health effects (Cacioppo and Cacioppo, 2012). For instance, people who subjectively feel isolated live shorter lives than those who feel they have strong, dependable, meaningful social bonds (Cacioppo and Patrick, 2008; Cacioppo and Cacioppo, 2012 for review).
As a consequence, there is a health-related need to better understand the functional dynamic of our brain during actions performed in an interpersonal context. This is critical as we spend much of our lifetime interacting with significant others, acquaintances, as well as strangers.
A growing body of research in psychology highlights the importance of studying the processing of significant others by demonstrating the influence of implicit processing of significant others (compared to strangers) on the individual's perception and cognitive processes. For instance, evidence suggests that the emotional bond between an actor and a perceiver may facilitate mutual intention perception, with a stronger bond associated with faster intention understanding (Cutting and Kozlowski, 1977; Ortigue and Bianchi-Demicheli, 2008; Ortigue et al., 2010a).
Inspired by the theories on embodied cognition and simulation theories, one explanation for this facilitation effect is that intention understanding may be based, in part, upon mechanisms of self-expansion among significant others. Through self-expansion mechanisms, a collective unconscious mental representation may be formed among individuals who share self-characteristics, values, and actions in a common environment (Agnew and Etcheverry, 2006). In line with evolutionary theory's claim that intense emotional experiences during a lifetime (e.g., passionate love) may be a central human motivation to expand one's self (Aron and Aron, 1996; Barkow et al., 1992), the self-expansion theory of pair-bonding is a hallmark in dyadic relationships (Aron and Aron, 1996). As an illustration, couples in love often refer to one another as the “better half” or the “completion of oneself,” and they refer to “We” rather than “I” (Hatfield and Sprecher, 1986; Hatfield and Rapson, 1993; Aron and Aron, 1996), therefore suggesting that there is a cognitive expansion of their self in a beloved, and vice versa an integration of the beloved's values and characteristics in their self (Hatfield and Walster, 1978; Aron and Aron, 1996; Ortigue and Bianchi-Demicheli, 2008). From a cognitive and social viewpoint, self-expansion means that each partner makes a decision (conscious or not) to include the significant other in their own mental self-representation (Aron and Aron, 1996), allowing the formation of a shared mental representation of the self and partner. Recent neuroimaging studies of love (Cacioppo et al., 2012, for review) provide further support in favor of this self-expansion model of love by demonstrating a recruitment of self-related brain network in people who are in love (see Cacioppo et al., 2012, for review). Based on these recent findings in social neuroscience and relationship science (Ortigue et al., 2009, 2010a,b; Cacioppo et al., 2012; Canessa et al., 2012) and based on the self-expansion theory of pair-bonding (Aron and Aron, 1996; Bianchi-Demicheli et al., 2006; Ortigue and Bianchi-Demicheli, 2008; Ortigue et al., 2010a,b), we hypothesized a common pattern (notably within SN) of activation between love and intention tasks (Ortigue and Bianchi-Demicheli, 2008). The rationale for identifying areas of overlap between tasks that involve love and intention is that love varies as a function of the extent to which an individual prefers or desires interaction with another person. This preference or desire, in turn, may activate networks associated more with behavioral intentions in everyday life.
To test this hypothesis, we statistically explored the neural similarities and differences of the neural bases between intention and passionate love for a partner by performing a meta-analysis of fMRI studies involving intention understanding and love, respectively.
Materials and methods
Literature search
We performed a systematic review of functional neuroimaging studies of intention understanding and passionate love, respectively. All papers and books in the literature published up to May 2011 (inclusive) were considered for this review, subject to two general limitations: the publication had to be a manuscript, chapter or book, and the title and abstract had to be available in English. Materials were identified through computer-based search, as described below.
Selection criteria for intention understanding literature
Our systematic computer-based search was based on the published literature of functional neuroimaging studies on intention understanding using MEDLINE library through PubMed database. Key words used for this search were “intention understanding,” “action understanding,” “fMRI,” and “neuroimaging.” Publications were selected on the basis of the following criteria: (1) fMRI neuroimaging studies; (2) with healthy adult participants, and (3) paradigms included stories (text or cartoons) or video-clips on intention understanding only. In all selected studies, participants' instruction was either to observe intentions, to infer the intentions of the actions performed by others, or to answer questions about the intention of the actions (i.e., “why”). In all the studies we classified as “intention understanding” agents were strangers (unfamiliar people) only. A list of the studies and contrast conditions for intention understanding are shown in Table A1.
Selection criteria for passionate love literature
We similarly performed a computer-based search of functional neuroimaging studies on passionate love using MEDLINE library through PubMed database. Expanding on our earlier study (Ortigue et al., 2010b), we used the key words “love,” “couple,” “fMRI,” and “neuroimaging.” Publications were selected on the basis of the following criteria: (1) fMRI neuroimaging studies; (2) with healthy adult participants in love with a partner, (3) paradigms included viewing partner's face or partner's name, i.e., tasks related to their beloved partner. A list of the studies and contrast conditions for passionate love are shown in Table A2.
Meta-analysis methods
To provide readers with a synthesized and statistical view of the common and different brain networks mediating intention understanding and passionate love, we analyzed the distribution of peak coordinates related to intention understanding (Figure 1, blue), passionate love (red), and regions common to both (purple). Using techniques reported previously by our group (Lewis, 2006; Ortigue et al., 2010b; Cacioppo et al., 2012), we adopted a Multi-level Kernel Density Analysis (MKDA) approach (Wager et al., 2009). This approach quantitatively tested for consistency and specificity of regional activation across the two sets of studies: it minimizes biases such as having one study that reports many activation foci from dominating the meta-analysis, and it accounts for the smoothness of reported data, false-positive rates, and statistical power. Thus, the reported peak coordinates within a study's contrast maps are weighted in the meta-analysis by study quality and sample size. We calculated the number of statistical contrast maps that activated each voxel in the brain using 10 mm kernel (roughly matching the three dimensional spatial resolution of the reported data). Monte Carlo simulations (10,000 iterations) were used to obtain a threshold and establish statistical significance against a null hypothesis that activated regions in the resulting pair-wise contrast maps are not spatially consistent (i.e., that they are randomly distributed throughout the brain). The use of the distribution of maximum values provides a strong control of family wise error rate and is an established method for multiple comparisons correction (Nichols and Holmes, 2002). All voxels (which constitute the various brain region volumes) whose density exceeded the 99.9th percentile value under the null hypothesis were considered significant (i.e., Family Wise Error Rate corrected for spatial extent at p < 0.001). Brain coordinates obtained using the MKDA method above were entered into the SPM8 software program (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) using Anatomy Toolbox version 18 (Eickhoff et al., 2005) in order to facilitate identification and labeling of each activation peak. The MNI coordinates and volumes of significantly overlapping clusters (brain foci) were extracted using the AFNI software plug-in 3dcluster (Cox, 1996). Displayed localizations were further validated by visual inspection relative to the Duvernoy and Bourgoin brain atlas (Duvernoy and Bourgoin, 1999). To visualize the meta-analysis results (Figure 1), the significantly overlapping contrast indicator maps for the intention studies and the love studies, and the intention-overlap-love studies were projected (using MNI-Talairach coordinate space) onto the left and right hemisphere Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based atlas (PALS atlas), which is an atlas of cortical surfaces that represent the averaged cortical surfaces of 12 individuals (http://brainmap.wustl.edu; Van Essen, 2005). The left and right cortical surfaces were inflated to reveal major sulci of the brain to facilitate viewing of the data.
Results
Based on our search criteria, we found a total of 25 fMRI studies. This included 17 studies (21 experimental paradigms) for intention understanding (Table A1; Pelphrey et al., 2004; Walter et al., 2004; den Ouden et al., 2005; Iacoboni et al., 2005; Hamilton and Grafton, 2006, 2008; Wang et al., 2006; Brass et al., 2007; Buccino et al., 2007; Ciaramidaro et al., 2007; de Lange et al., 2008; Ortigue et al., 2009; Liew et al., 2010; Newman-Norlund et al., 2010; Ramsey and Hamilton, 2010; Carter et al., 2011; Jastorff et al., 2011) and eight studies (10 experimental paradigms) for passionate love (Table A2; Bartels and Zeki, 2000; Aron et al., 2005; Ortigue et al., 2007; Kim et al., 2009; Zeki and Romaya, 2010; Stoessel et al., 2011; Xu et al., 2011; Acevedo et al., 2012), involving a total of 457 participants. The number of participants included in each study ranged from 10 to 36 (for further details see Tables A1, A2).
Results confirm previous studies by demonstrating that understanding intentions of strangers involved the brain areas involved in both SN and AON, including areas sustaining embodied cognition, simulation, and self-other perception (such as vMPFC, BA6, MTG/STG, Angular gyrus, see Table A3; Figure 1, blue) as well as in the precuneus and the left anterior cingulate cortex, and the right middle and posterior cingulate cortex. In addition, activations in subcortical areas, such as putamen activity were also observed (see Table A3 for further details). Similarly, results on passionate love activations reinforced previous studies (e.g., Acevedo et al., 2012 and Cacioppo et al., 2012) by demonstrating a distributed cortical and subcortical network of SN as well as brain areas known to be involved in partner preference, rewarding experiences, goal-directed actions, habit formation, and decision-making (for further details see Table A4).
Common brain network between intention understanding and passionate love
Our fMRI meta-analysis revealed a shared brain network between intention understanding and passionate love (Figure 1; purple; Table A5) that includes brain regions sustaining social cognition, embodied cognition, mentalizing about self-other, such as bilateral pSTS/IPL, bilateral inferior frontal cortex (IFC), ventro-medial prefrontal cortex (vMPFC), anterior insula as well as brain regions involved in the mesolimbic and nigrostriatal dopaminergic pathways (caudate nucleus, thalamus, putamen, and parahippocampal area; Table A5).
Discussion
The present research highlights a shared network between love and intention, which includes (1) areas that overlap with areas related to dopamine circuits; and (2) several regions implicated in social cognition, embodied cognition, attachment, mental state representation, and self-representation. These results are consistent with previous studies indicating that both love and intention involve goal-directed and rewarding behaviors towards a specific partner. The recruitment of dorsal parts of the striatum, such as the caudate and putamen, which are innervated by dopamine coming from both the VTA and substantia nigra, is in line with recent work in animals showing that these brain areas are critical in the development of a pair bond and conditioned partner preference (Pfaus, 1999, 2009; Young et al., 2005, for review), as well as in the activation of stereotyped motor patterns (habits) related to conditioned incentive cues (Everitt and Robbins, 2005). Although dopamine transmission in the ventral striatum has been shown to be stimulated in the presence of both unconditioned and conditioned rewarding incentive cues stimuli (Pfaus et al., 2001; Aragona et al., 2003; Postuma and Dagher, 2006; Pfaus, 2009, for review), responses made to conditioned rewards appear to involve more dorsal striatal networks, among which are outputs of the dorsal striatum to cortical regions such as the insula (Postuma and Dagher, 2006)—a brain region which binds integrated visceral feedback with emotional and cognitive responses (Craig, 2002; Ibanez et al., 2010; Berntson et al., 2011; Cacioppo et al., 2012).
The overlap between passionate love and intention understanding in brain areas, such as the vMPFC, is consistent with a growing body of studies unraveling the recruitment of this brain area during tasks that require introspections about self and by tasks that require inferences about the minds of others perceived to be similar to self (Jenkins et al., 2008). The activation of the anterior insula is also consistent with earlier studies from our laboratory and others implicating a role in meta-representations of self (for further details see, Cacioppo et al., 2012). Other overlapping areas were also activated, such as left and right IFC and MTG/pSTS with a slight left hemisphere lateralization. These areas are in close proximity to classical Broca's and Wernicke's language regions, which entail frontal and temporo-parietal regions that are more highly interconnected via the arcuate fasciculus white matter fiber tracts in humans relative to chimpanzees and to monkeys (Rilling, 2008). One intriguing prospect is that the evolution of these interconnected inferior fronto-parietal regions may relate to pair-bonding and degree of social attachments, as well as other more uniquely human qualities such as language (Kim et al., 2010).
By identifying specific functional brain regions and networks in a large sample of healthy subjects, the present analysis reinforces the consistency and specificity of the brain regions that are being reported in the burgeoning body of studies on love and intention understanding. Interestingly, by revealing an additive brain network for both intention understanding and passionate love, the present findings offer a new way to look at the neurobiology of the loving mind during embodied cognition through the lens of a specific subset of AON and SN regions of interest. This provides scientists and clinicians with a unique and strong rationale to further investigate neurocognitive models that may explain the modulations of these common regions and brain networks in future studies on intention understanding in dyads.
One limitation of the present meta-analysis study was some of the fMRI studies of intention contrasted the condition of interest either to resting state(s) (e.g., Iacoboni et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2006; Newman-Norlund et al., 2010), while others used a variety of different control conditions. Consequently, the resulting activation patterns (Figure 1) must be interpreted with this caveat in mind. However, the goal of the present study was to provide a first pass glimpse at determining (and identifying) candidate brain regions or networks that may show preferential or selective activation to scenarios related to embodied cognition, pair-bonding, and self-expansion mechanisms. The present results do provide novel support for an overlapping brain network between these mechanisms. Future fMRI studies investigating the processing of significant others (compared to strangers) during intention understanding in the same paradigm is thus warranted/needed. The systematic neuroscientific study of the modulations of the neural network for understanding the intentions of significant others in healthy subjects, neurological patients after brain damage, and patients suffering from chronic social and affective disorders (such as autism), will provide critical insights on the spatio-temporal dynamics of self-expansion and embodied mechanisms that may mediate dyadic interactions, notably among couples.
Financial disclosure/funding
The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article: Swiss National Science Foundation (Grant #PP00_1_128599/1 to Stephanie Cacioppo), the Mind Science Foundation (Grant #TSA2010-2 to Stephanie Cacioppo, Francesco Bianchi-Demicheli), NCRR NIH COBRE Grant E15524 (Grant #E15524 to the Sensory Neuroscience Research Center of West Virginia University to James W. Lewis), the MOE Project of Key Research Institute of Humanities and Social Sciences (Grant #12JJD190004 to Yi-Wen Wang), and the Program for New Century Excellent Talents in Universities (Grant #NCET-11-1065 to Yi-Wen Wang).
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Appendix
Table A1.
Study (first author listed) | Year | No of subjects | No of women | No of right-handed | Stimuli | Experimental comparisons |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Brass | 2007 | 15 | 7 | 15 | Video-clips | Implausible > ordinary motion |
Buccino | 2007 | 20 | 10 | 20 | Video-clips | Attend to intention > viewing |
Video-clips | Incorrect unintended motion > ordinary motion | |||||
Carter | 2011 | 17 | 8 | 15 | Video-clips | Human reaching movements: goal shift > goal miss |
Ciaramidaro | 2007 | 12 | 6 | 12 | Comic strips | Intentional (communicative) > physical event |
Comic strips | Intentional (social) > physical event | |||||
Comic strips | Intentional (private) > physical event | |||||
de Lange | 2008 | 19 | 10 | 19 | Pictures | Ordinary > extraordinary intentions |
den Ouden | 2005 | 11 | 11 | Not specified | Scenarios | Intentional action > physical event |
Hamilton | 2006 | 20 | 11 | 19 | Video-clips | Repetition suppression for action goal |
Hamilton | 2008 | 20 | 13 | 20 | Video-clips | Repetition suppression for action outcome |
Iacoboni | 2005 | 23 | 15 | 23 | Video-clips | Hand grasping > rest |
Jastorff | 2011 | 15 | 9 | 15 | Video-clips | Non-rational > rational movements |
Liew | 2010 | 18 | 8 | 18 | Video-clips | Hand gestures > still control |
Newman-Norlund | 2010 | 18 | 10 | 18 | Video-clips | Object-directed actions > rest |
Ortigue | 2009 | 24 | 0 | 24 | Video-clips | Repetition suppression for intention |
Pelphrey | 2004 | 12 | 7 | 12 | Video-clips | Incorrect > correct motion |
Ramsey | 2010 | 25 | 17 | 24 | Video-clips | Repetition suppression for object-goal |
Walter (1) | 2004 | 13 | 7 | 13 | Comic strips | Intentional action > physical event |
Walter (2) | 2004 | 12 | 6 | 12 | Comic strips | Intentional action > physical event |
Wang | 2006 | 12 | 6 | 12 | Scenarios | Attend to face > rest |
Total | 306 | 161 | 291 |
Table A2.
Study (first author listed) | Year | No of subjects | No of women | No of right-handed | Stimuli | Experimental comparisons |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Acevedo | 2012 | 17 | 10 | 17 | Faces | Partner > high familiar acquaintance |
Faces | Partner > close friend | |||||
Aron | 2005 | 17 | 10 | 17 | Faces | Beloved > familiar neutral acquaintance |
Bartels | 2000 | 17 | 11 | 16 | Faces | Beloved > friend |
Kim | 2009 | 10 | 5 | 10 | Faces | Beloved > friend (early) |
Faces | Beloved > friend (late) | |||||
Ortigue | 2007 | 36 | 36 | 36 | Names | Beloved > friend or stranger |
Stoessel | 2011 | 12 | 6 | 12 | Pictures | Beloved > erotic (happy condition) |
Xu | 2011 | 18 | 10 | 18 | Faces | Beloved > familiar neutral acquaintance |
Zeki | 2010 | 24 | Not specified | Not specified | Faces | Beloved > neutral |
Total | 151 | 88 | 126 |
Table A3.
Label | Left hemisphere | Right hemisphere | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MNI coordinates | Number of voxels | MNI coordinates | Number of voxels | |||||
x | y | z | x | y | z | |||
vmPFC | 6 | 52 | −12 | 4000 | ||||
Inferior frontal gyrus (p. Opercularis) (BA 44: 20%) | 46 | 10 | 27 | 17904 | ||||
Superior frontal gyrus (BA 6: 50%) | −18 | 2 | 67 | 7048 | ||||
SMA (BA 6: 20%) | −6 | 20 | 51 | 6648 | ||||
BA 6 (20%) | 20 | −5 | 48 | 40 | ||||
Precentral gyrus (BA 6: 60%) | 36 | −12 | 58 | 4120 | ||||
Precentral gyrus (BA 44: 30%) | −46 | 7 | 33 | 29,424 | ||||
BA 45 (10%) | 37 | 33 | 0 | 3184 | ||||
Middle temporal gyrus/STG | −47 | −50 | 18 | 73,992 | ||||
Heschls gyrus (Insula/Ig1: 60%; TE1.1: 50%; OP2: 30%) | −33 | −28 | 13 | 464 | ||||
Gyrus ambiens | 46 | −6 | −23 | 7688 | ||||
Angular gyrus (IPC/PFm: 20%; PGa: 20%; hIP1: 10%) | 47 | −46 | 24 | 73,608 | ||||
Precuneus (SPL/7P: 20%; SPL/7A: 20%) | −2 | −55 | 47 | 17,744 | ||||
Anterior cingulate cortex | −3 | 49 | 11 | 10,424 | ||||
7 | 41 | 29 | 7928 | |||||
Middle cingulate cortex (BA 6: 10%) | 10 | −6 | 44 | 2864 | ||||
Posterior cingulate cortex | 12 | −41 | 10 | 7736 | ||||
Putamen | −26 | 10 | −6 | 4120 | ||||
Fornix | −4 | −2 | 10 | 4120 | ||||
Calcarine gyrus (BA 17: 50%; BA 18: 10%) | 16 | −82 | 14 | 4088 | ||||
Calcarine gyrus (BA 17: 70%; BA 18: 20%) | 23 | −99 | −2 | 3424 | ||||
Middle occipital gyrus (IPC/PGp: 30%) | −35 | −84 | 29 | 3024 | ||||
Optic radiations | −26 | −34 | 10 | 2336 |
Table A4.
Label | Left hemisphere | Right hemisphere | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MNI coordinates | Number of voxels | MNI coordinates | Number of voxels | |||||
x | y | z | x | y | z | |||
vmPFC | 10 | 39 | −11 | 2744 | ||||
−5 | 49 | −8 | 9384 | |||||
dlPFC | −7 | 51 | 22 | 5056 | ||||
SMA (BA 6: 60%; BA 4: 10%) | 10 | −18 | 61 | 24,760 | ||||
Postcentral gyrus (BA 3b: 60%; BA 1: 10%; BA 6: 10%) | 61 | −3 | 24 | 6784 | ||||
Superior temporal gyrus (TE3: 10%) | 63 | −14 | −6 | 3656 | ||||
Superior temporal gyrus (IPC/PFcm: 10%; OP1: 10%; TE1.1: 10%) | −56 | −28 | 8 | 4120 | ||||
Posterior middle temporal gyrus (V5: 10%) | −55 | −67 | 2 | 3312 | ||||
Inferior temporal gyrus | −40 | −8 | −28 | 4120 | ||||
Middle cingulate cortex | 10 | 5 | 32 | 16 | ||||
Supramarginal gyrus (IPC/PFm: 70%; IPC/PF: 30%; IPC/PGa: 20%) | 62 | −45 | 25 | 7040 | ||||
Supramarginal gyrus (IPC/PF: 60%; IPC/PFm: 40%; IPC/PFcm: 10%) | −63 | −46 | 25 | 4096 | ||||
Superior parietal lobule (SPL/7A: 40%; BA2: 30%; SPL/5L: 30%) | −23 | −50 | 53 | 3128 | ||||
PGa (10%) | 40 | −48 | 15 | 16 | ||||
Calcarine gyrus (BA 17: 100%; BA 18: 30%) | 9 | −90 | 1 | 22,440 | ||||
Cuneus | 18 | −70 | 24 | 6192 | ||||
Thalamus | −3 | −12 | 1 | 284,552 | ||||
Cerebellum | 42 | −57 | −28 | 7512 | ||||
34 | −45 | −48 | 6656 | |||||
−36 | −78 | −40 | 3896 | |||||
PASSIONATE LOVE ACTIVATIONS (USING SPHERICAL KERNEL OF 3 mm) | ||||||||
Superior frontal gyrus | −24 | 12 | 66 | 648 | ||||
Posterior superior temporal gyrus (IPC: 50%) | −50 | −38 | 18 | 600 | ||||
Superior medial frontal gyrus | −9 | 27 | 59 | 1288 | ||||
−10 | 52 | 16 | 632 | |||||
Middle orbital gyrus/orbitofrontal | 2 | 62 | −8 | 648 | ||||
Gyrus rectus (orbitofrontal) | −4 | 60 | −24 | 648 | ||||
Middle frontal gyrus | −30 | 54 | 2 | 648 | ||||
Superior anterior medial frontal gyrus | 8 | 54 | 22 | 1272 | ||||
11 | 65 | 5 | 1264 | |||||
Inferior frontal gyrus (p. orbitalis) | 35 | 33 | −8 | 1200 | ||||
Inferior frontal gyrus (p. triangularis) | −37 | 28 | 0 | 16 | ||||
−37 | 26 | −2 | 16 | |||||
−33 | 35 | −2 | 824 | |||||
56 | 28 | 8 | 648 | |||||
Middle frontal gyrus | 30 | 47 | 4 | 1200 | ||||
−30 | 44 | 11 | 344 | |||||
Anterior cingulate cortex | 8 | 40 | 9 | 1152 | ||||
8 | 28 | 16 | 648 | |||||
−6 | 42 | −6 | 600 | |||||
Middle cingulate cortex | 4 | −22 | 42 | 648 | ||||
12 | −22 | 34 | 616 | |||||
14 | −20 | 43 | 40 | |||||
Posterior cingulate cortex | −1 | −32 | 26 | 2440 | ||||
Inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform area | 42 | −52 | −12 | 648 | ||||
Superior temporal gyrus | −50 | −28 | 2 | 648 | ||||
Temporal pole | 40 | 8 | −28 | 648 | ||||
−34 | 18 | −22 | 648 | |||||
Caudate nucleus | 54 | −32 | 8 | 648 | ||||
−8 | 20 | 0 | 648 | |||||
17 | 1 | 23 | 1024 | |||||
10 | 16 | 5 | 1136 | |||||
Insula | 39 | −6 | −7 | 2632 | ||||
−42 | −9 | −6 | 2112 | |||||
−36 | 18 | −4 | 648 | |||||
−34 | −22 | 10 | 608 | |||||
SMA | −1 | 10 | 65 | 1296 | ||||
4 | 4 | 52 | 648 | |||||
Thalamus | −8 | −9 | −2 | 1288 | ||||
10 | −24 | 10 | 648 | |||||
Heschel gyrus | 45 | −17 | 8 | 1280 | ||||
Cerebellum | 10 | −44 | −7 | 1168 | ||||
Cerebellar vermis | −2 | −58 | −10 | 648 | ||||
Putamen | −22 | 7 | −8 | 1024 | ||||
−18 | 19 | −9 | 896 | |||||
Precuneus | 8 | −51 | 22 | 976 | ||||
Parahippocampal region (Amygdala: 10%) | 19 | 2 | −13 | 896 | ||||
Pallidum | 22 | −1 | 6 | 888 | ||||
Rolandic operculum | 60 | −14 | 12 | 648 | ||||
Postcentral gyrus | −62 | −4 | 22 | 600 | ||||
Lingual gyrus (Hippocampus: 40%) | −11 | −38 | −10 | 424 |
Table A5.
Label | Left hemisphere | Right hemisphere | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MNI coordinates | Number of voxels | MNI coordinates | Number of voxels | |||||
x | y | z | x | y | z | |||
dlPFC | 2 | 41 | 36 | 32 | ||||
−5 | 54 | 19 | 2000 | |||||
vmPFC | 9 | 45 | −11 | 280 | ||||
4 | 55 | −11 | 1904 | |||||
Superior frontal gyrus (BA 6: 40%) | 30 | −12 | 62 | 424 | ||||
Inferior frontal gyrus (p. Orbitalis) | 39 | 31 | −4 | 824 | ||||
Inferior frontal gyrus (p. Triangularis) (BA 45: 60%) | 50 | 30 | 16 | 632 | ||||
Inferior frontal gyrus (p. Triangularis) (BA 45: 60%; BA 44: 20%) | −46 | 24 | 21 | 5232 | ||||
Inferior frontal gyrus (p. Opercularis) | 39 | 18 | 13 | 232 | ||||
Inferior frontal gyrus (p. Opercularis) (BA 44: 40%) | 56 | 9 | 21 | 272 | ||||
Postcentral gyrus (IPC/PFop: 30%; OP4: 30%; OP3: 10%) | 62 | −16 | 23 | 1248 | ||||
Postcentral gyrus (BA 2: 80%; SPL/7PC: 20%; BA 3b: 10%) | 28 | −42 | 57 | 1104 | ||||
SMA (BA 6: 70%) | 4 | −5 | 48 | 48 | ||||
SMA | −6 | 21 | 47 | 2024 | ||||
SMA (BA 6: 60%) | −10 | −5 | 69 | 80 | ||||
Inferior temporal gyrus | −49 | −51 | −20 | 3032 | ||||
Middle temporal gyrus (V5: 10%) | −53 | −68 | 2 | 2312 | ||||
Superior temporal gyrus (OP1: 10%) | −56 | −30 | 6 | 2424 | ||||
Superior temporal gyrus (IPC/PF: 60%; IPC/PFm: 30%; PGa: 20%) | −60 | −46 | 23 | 1648 | ||||
Supra marginal gyrus (IPC/PFm: 70%; IPC/PF: 30%; IPC/PGa: 30%) | 61 | −45 | 24 | 5800 | ||||
Anterior cingulate cortex | −7 | 40 | −2 | 744 | ||||
−7 | 30 | 6 | 224 | |||||
6 | 34 | 22 | 3696 | |||||
Posterior cingulate cortex | 7 | −40 | 14 | 3024 | ||||
−2 | −53 | 29 | 1088 | |||||
Superior parietal lobule (SPL/7PC: 30%; SPL/7A: 20%; hIP3: 10%) | −26 | −53 | 53 | 1096 | ||||
Lingual gyrus (BA 17: 50%; BA 18: 10%) | 29 | −59 | 3 | 904 | ||||
Inferior temporal gyrus | 46 | −56 | −23 | 776 | ||||
Cuneus (BA 18: 10%; BA 17: 10%) | 16 | −77 | 18 | 672 | ||||
Precuneus | 25 | −45 | 8 | 568 | ||||
Precuneus (SPL/5L: 40%; SPL/7a: 20%; SPL/5M: 10%) | −14 | −50 | 57 | 16 | ||||
Anterior insula (Id1: 40%) | 43 | −4 | −14 | 272 | ||||
Anterior insula | 35 | 28 | 5 | 256 | ||||
Hippocampus | 37 | −14 | −19 | 1224 | ||||
Putamen | −27 | 10 | −6 | 3488 | ||||
Fornix | −3 | −3 | 11 | 3368 | ||||
Calcarine gyrus (BA 17: 50%; BA 18: 10%) | 14 | −87 | 14 | 1424 | ||||
Calcarine gyrus (BA 17: 80%; BA 18: 10%) | 21 | −95 | −2 | 896 | ||||
Optic radiations | −26 | −36 | 11 | 1608 | ||||
−33 | −54 | −2 | 56 |
Note: Activations of 10 voxels or above are reported here. All brain areas reported are significant at p < 0.001, corrected.
Abbreviations
- dlPFC
dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex
- vmPFC
ventro-medial Prefrontal Cortex
- SMA
Supplementary Motor Area
- STG
Superior Temporal Gyrus
- IPC/PF
part of Inferior Parietal Cortex
- IPC/PFm
part of Inferior Parietal Cortex
- IPC/PGp
part of Inferior Parietal Cortex
- PGa
part of Parietal area
- hIP1
human Intraparietal area 1 (Choi et al., 2006)
- hIP3
human Intraparietal area 1 (Choi et al., 2006)
- Insula/Ig1 (Insular Lobe granular area; Kurth et al., 2010)
60%
- TE1.1
part of the Primary Auditory Cortex (Morosan et al., 2001)
- OP1
part of Parietal Operculum (Eickhoff et al., 2006)
- OP2
part of Parietal Operculum (Eickhoff et al., 2006)
- SPL/7PC
part of Superior Parietal Lobule
- SPL/7A
part of Superior Parietal Lobule
- SPL/5M
part of Superior Parietal Lobule (Duvernoy and Bourgoin, 1999).
References
- Acevedo B. P., Aron A., Fisher H. E., Brown L. L. (2012). Neural correlates of long-term intense romantic love. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 145–159 10.1093/scan/nsq092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agnew C. R., Etcheverry P. E. (2006). Cognitive interdependence: considering self-in-relationship, in Self and Relationships: Connecting Intrapersonal and Interpersonal Processes, eds Vohs K. D., Finkel E. J. (New York, NY: Guilford; ), 274–293 [Google Scholar]
- Allison T., Puce A., McCarthy G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 267–278 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01501-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aragona B. J., Liu Y., Curtis J. T., Stephan F. K., Wang Z. (2003). A critical role for nucleus accumbens dopamine in partner-preference formation in male prairie voles. J. Neurosci. 23, 3483–3490 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron A., Fisher H., Mashek D. J., Strong G., Li H., Brown L. L. (2005). Reward, motivation, and emotion systems associated with early-stage intense romantic love. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 327–337 10.1152/jn.00838.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron E. N., Aron A. (1996). Love and expansion of the self: the state of the model. Pers. Relat. 3, 45–58 [Google Scholar]
- Barkow J. H., Cosmides L., Tooby J. (1992). The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York, NY: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
- Bartels A., Zeki S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love. Neuroreport 11, 3829–3834 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becchio C., Manera V., Sartori L., Cavallo A., Castiello U. (2012). Grasping intentions: from thought experiments to empirical evidence. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:117 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berntson G. G., Norman G. J., Bechara A., Bruss J., Tranel D., Cacioppo J. T. (2011). The insula and evaluative processes. Psychol. Sci. 22, 80–86 10.1177/0956797610391097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bianchi-Demicheli F., Grafton S. T., Ortigue S. (2006). The power of love on the human brain. Soc. Neurosci. 1, 90–103 10.1080/17470910600976547 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bidet-Caulet A., Voisin J., Bertrand O., Fonlupt P. (2005). Listening to a walking human activates the temporal biological motion area. Neuroimage 28, 132–139 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brass M., Schmitt R. M., Spengler S., Gergely G. (2007). Investigating action understanding: inferential processes versus action simulation. Curr. Biol. 17, 2117–2121 10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buccino G., Baumgaertner A., Colle L., Buechel C., Rizzolatti G., Binkofski F. (2007). The neural basis for understanding non-intended actions. Neuroimage 36, 119–127 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo J. T., Patrick B. (2008). Loneliness: Human Nature and the Need for Social Connection. New York, NY: W. W. Norton and Company [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo S., Bianchi-Demicheli F., Frum C., Pfaus J. G., Lewis J. W. (2012). The common neural bases between sexual desire and love: a multilevel kernel density fMRI analysis. J. Sex. Med. 9, 1048–1054 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2012.02651.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo S., Cacioppo J. T. (2012). Decoding the invisible forces of social connections. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:51 10.3389/fnint.2012.00051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canessa N., Alemanno F., Riva F., Zani A., Proverbio A. M., Mannara N., et al. (2012). The neural bases of social intention understanding: the role of interaction goals. PLoS ONE 7:e42347 10.1371/journal.pone.0042347 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter E. J., Hodgins J. K., Rakison D. H. (2011). Exploring the neural correlates of goal-directed action and intention understanding. Neuroimage 54, 1634–1642 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.077 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi J. C., Park S. K., Kim Y.-H., Shin Y. W., Kwon J. S., Kim J. S., et al. (2006). Different brain activation patterns to pain and pain-related unpleasantness during the menstrual cycle. Anesthesiology 105, 120–127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ciaramidaro A., Adenzato M., Enrici I., Erk S., Pia L., Bara B. G., et al. (2007). The intentional network: how the brain reads varieties of intentions. Neuropsychologia 45, 3105–3113 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox R. W. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 655–666 10.1038/nrn894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cutting J. E., Kozlowski L. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: gait perception without familiarity cues. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 9, 353–356 [Google Scholar]
- de Lange F. P., Spronk M., Willems R. M., Toni I., Bekkering H. (2008). Complementary systems for understanding action intentions. Curr. Biol. 18, 454–457 10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- den Ouden H. E., Frith U., Frith C., Blakemore S. J. (2005). Thinking about intentions. Neuroimage 28, 787–796 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desmurget M., Reilly K. T., Richard N., Szathmari A., Mottolese C., Sirigu A. (2009). Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans. Science 324, 811–813 10.1126/science.1169896 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duvernoy H. M., Bourgoin P. (1999). The Human Brain: Surface, Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy with MRI, and Blood Supply. Wien: Springer [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff S. B., Stephan K. E., Mohlberg H., Grefkes C., Fink G. R., Amunts K., et al. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage 25, 1325–1335 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff S. B., Schleicher A., Zilles K., Amunts K. (2006). The human parietal operculum. I. Cytoarchitectonic mapping of subdivisions. Cereb. Cortex 16, 254–267 10.1093/cercor/bhi105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt B. J., Robbins T. W. (2005). Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1481–1489 10.1038/nn1579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frith C. D., Frith U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50, 531–534 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gazzola V., Aziz-Zadeh L., Keysers C. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Curr. Biol. 16, 1824–1829 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grafton S. T. (2009). Embodied cognition and the simulation of action to understand others. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1156, 97–117 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04425.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grafton S. T., Arbib M. A., Fadiga L., Rizzolatti G. (1996). Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography. Exp. Brain Res. 112, 103–111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grezes J., Frith C., Passingham R. E. (2004). Brain mechanisms for inferring deceit in the actions of others. J. Neurosci. 24, 5500–5505 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0219-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossman E. D., Battelli L., Pascual-Leone A. (2005). Repetitive TMS over posterior STS disrupts perception of biological motion. Vision Res. 45, 2847–2853 10.1016/j.visres.2005.05.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton A. C., Grafton S. (2006). Goal representation in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. J. Neurosci. 26, 1133–1137 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4551-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton A. F., Grafton S. T. (2008). Action outcomes are represented in human inferior frontoparietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1160–1168 10.1093/cercor/bhm150 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatfield E., Rapson R. L. (1993). Historical and cross-cultural perspectives on passionate love and sexual desire. Annu. Rev. Sex Res. 4, 67–97 [Google Scholar]
- Hatfield E., Sprecher S. (1986). Measuring passionate love in intimate relationships. J. Adolesc. 9, 383–410 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatfield E., Walster G. W. (1978). A New Look at Love. Lanham, MD: University Press of America [Google Scholar]
- Iacoboni M., Molnar-Szakacs I., Gallese V., Buccino G., Mazziotta J. C., Rizzolatti G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one's own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biol. 3:e79 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ibanez A., Gleichgerrcht E., Manes F. (2010). Clinical effects of insular damage in humans. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 397–410 10.1007/s00429-010-0256-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jastorff J., Clavagnier S., Gergely G., Orban G. A. (2011). Neural mechanisms of understanding rational actions: middle temporal gyrus activation by contextual violation. Cereb. Cortex 21, 318–329 10.1093/cercor/bhq098 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeannerod M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage 14, S103–S109 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jellema T., Baker C. I., Wicker B., Perrett D. I. (2000). Neural representation for the perception of the intentionality of actions. Brain Cogn. 44, 280–302 10.1006/brcg.2000.1231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins A. C., Macrae C. N., Mitchel J. P. (2008). Repetition suppression of ventromedial prefrontal activity during judgements of self and others. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 4507–4512 10.1073/pnas.0708785105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim P., Leckman J. F., Mayes L. C., Newman M. A., Feldman R., Swain J. E. (2010). Perceived quality of maternal care in childhood and structure and function of mothers' brain. Dev. Sci. 13, 662–673 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00923.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim W., Kim S., Jeong J., Lee K.-U., Ahn K.-J., Chung Y.-A., et al. (2009). Temporal changes in functional magnetic resonance imaging activation of heterosexual couples for visual stimuli of loved partners. Psychiatry Investig. 6, 19–25 10.4306/pi.2009.6.1.19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurth F., Zilles K., Fox P. T., Laird A. R., Eickhoff S. B. (2010). A link between the systems: functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 519–534 10.1007/s00429-010-0255-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis J. W. (2006). Cortical networks related to human use of tools. Neuroscientist 12, 211–231 10.1177/1073858406288327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis J. W., Phinney R. E., Brefczynski J. A., DeYoe E. A. (2006). Lefties get it “right” when hearing tool sounds. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1314–1330 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.8.1314 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis J. W., Talkington W. J., Puce A., Engel L. R., Frum C. (2011). Cortical networks representing object categories and high-level attributes of familiar real-world action sounds. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2079–2101 10.1162/jocn.2010.21570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liew S. L., Han S., Aziz-Zadeh L. (2010). Familiarity modulates mirror neuron and mentalizing regions during intention understanding. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1986–1997 10.1002/hbm.21164 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Materna S., Dicke P. W., Thier P. (2008). The posterior superior temporal sulcus is involved in social communication not specific for the eyes. Neuropsychologia 46, 2759–2765 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.05.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morosan P., Rademacher J., Schleicher A., Amunts K., Schormann T., Zilles K. (2001). Human primary auditory cortex: cytoarchitectonic subdivisions and mapping into a spatial reference system. Neuroimage 13, 684–701 10.1006/nimg.2000.0715 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman-Norlund R., van Schie H. T., van Hoek M. E. C., Cuijpers R. H., Bekkering H. (2010). The role of inferior frontal and parietal areas in differentiating meaningful and meaningless object-directed actions. Brain Res. 1315, 63–74 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.11.065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nichols T. E., Holmes A. P. (2002). Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 1–25 10.1002/hbm.1058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niedenthal P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science 316, 1002–1005 10.1126/science.1136930 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niedenthal P. M., Barsalou L. W., Winkielman P., Krauth-Gruber S., Ric F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 9, 184–211 10.1207/s15327957pspr0903_1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ortigue S., Bianchi-Demicheli F. (2008). Why is your spouse so predictable? Connecting mirror neuron system and self-expansion model of love. Med. Hypotheses 71, 941–944 10.1016/j.mehy.2008.07.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ortigue S., Bianchi-Demicheli F., Hamilton A. F., Grafton S. T. (2007). The neural basis of love as a subliminal prime: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1218–1230 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1218 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ortigue S., Patel N., Bianchi-Demicheli F., Grafton S. T. (2010a). Implicit priming of embodied cognition on human motor intention understanding in dyads in love. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 27, 1001–1015 [Google Scholar]
- Ortigue S., Bianchi-Demicheli F., Patel N., Frum C., Lewis J. W. (2010b). Neuroimaging of love: fMRI meta-analysis evidence toward new perspectives in sexual medicine. J. Sex. Med. 7, 3541–3552 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2010.01999.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ortigue S., Thompson J. C., Parasuraman R., Grafton S. T. (2009). Spatio-temporal dynamics of human intention understanding in temporo-parietal cortex: a combined EEG/fMRI repetition suppression paradigm. PLoS ONE 4:e6962 10.1371/journal.pone.0006962 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelphrey K., Morris J. P., McCarthy G. (2004). Grasping the intentions of others: the perceived intentionality of an action influences activity in the superior temporal sulcus during social perception. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1706–1716 10.1162/0898929042947900 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelphrey K. A., Morris J. P. (2006). Brain mechanisms for interpreting the actions of others from biological-motion cues. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 136–140 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00423.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfaus J. G. (1999). Neurobiology of sexual behavior. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9, 751–758 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00034-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfaus J. G. (2009). Pathways of sexual desire. J. Sex. Med. 6, 1506–1533 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2009.01309.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfaus J. G., Kippin T. E., Centeno S. (2001). Conditioning and sexual behavior: a review. Horm. Behav. 40, 291–321 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1686 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Postuma R. B., Dagher A. (2006). Basal ganglia functional connectivity based on a meta-analysis of 126 positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging publications. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1508–1521 10.1093/cercor/bhj088 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramsey R., Hamilton A. F. (2010). Understanding actors and object-goals in the human brain. Neuroimage 50, 1142–1147 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rilling J. K. (2008). The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 426–428 10.1038/nn2072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G., Craighero L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G., Fogassi L., Gallese V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 661–670 10.1038/35090060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G., Sinigaglia C. (2007). Mirror neurons and motor intentionality. Funct. Neurol. 22, 205–210 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G., Sinigaglia C. (2008). Further reflections on how we interpret the actions of others. Nature 455, 589 10.1038/455589b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G., Sinigaglia C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 264–274 10.1038/nrn2805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saxe R., Xiao D. K., Kovacs G., Perrett D. I., Kanwisher N. (2004). A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions. Neuropsychologia 42, 1435–1446 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz J., Imamizu H., Kawato M., Frith C. D. (2004). Activation of the human superior temporal gyrus during observation of goal attribution by intentional objects. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1695–1705 10.1162/0898929042947874 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoessel C., Stiller J., Bleich S., Boensch D., Doerfler A., Garcia M., et al. (2011). Differences and similarities on neuronal activities of people being happily and unhappily in love: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuropsychobiology 64, 52–60 10.1159/000325076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sugiura M., Wakusawa K., Sekiguchi A., Sassa Y., Jeong H., Horie K., et al. (2009). Extraction of situational meaning by integrating. multiple meanings in a complex environment: a functional MRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2676–2688 10.1002/hbm.20699 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson J. C., Clarke M., Stewart T., Puce A. (2005). Configural processing of biological motion in human superior temporal sulcus. J. Neurosci. 25, 9059–9066 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2129-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson J. C., Hardee J. E., Panayiotou A., Crewther D., Puce A. (2007). Common and distinct brain activation to viewing dynamic sequences of face and hand movements. Neuroimage 37, 966–973 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen D. C. (2005). A Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage 28, 635–662 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F., Baetens K. (2009). Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis. Neuroimage 48, 564–584 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wager T. D., Lindquist M. A., Nichols T. E., Kober H., Van Snellenberg J. X. (2009). Evaluating the consistency and specificity of neuroimaging data using meta-analysis. Neuroimage 45(1 Suppl.), S210–S221 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.061 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wakusawa K., Sugiura M., Sassa Y., Jeong H., Horie K., Sato S., et al. (2009). Neural correlates of processing situational relationships between a part and the whole: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 48, 486–496 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walter H., Adenzato M., Ciaramidaro A., Enrici I., Pia L., Bara B. G. (2004). Understanding intentions in social interaction: the role of the anterior paracingulate cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1854–1863 10.1162/0898929042947838 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang A. T., Lee S. S., Sigman M., Dapretto M. (2006). Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 1, 107–121 10.1093/scan/nsl018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wheatley T., Milleville S. C., Martin A. (2007). Understanding animate agents: distinct roles for the social network and mirror system. Psychol. Sci. 18, 469–474 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01923.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu X., Aron A., Brown L., Cao G., Feng T., Weng X. (2011). Reward and motivation systems: a brain mapping study of early-stage intense romantic love in Chinese participants. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 249–257 10.1002/hbm.21017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young L. J., Murphy Young A. Z., Hammock E. A. D. (2005). Anatomy and neurochemistry of the pair bond. J. Comp. Neurol. 493, 51–57 10.1002/cne.20771 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeki S., Romaya J. P. (2010). The brain reaction to viewing faces of opposite- and same-sex romantic partners. PLoS ONE 5:e15802 10.1371/journal.pone.0015802 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]