Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2006 Sep 18;86(2):209–219. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.07.033

Progestins influence motivation, reward, conditioning, stress, and/or response to drugs of abuse

Cheryl A Frye 1,*
PMCID: PMC3613144  NIHMSID: NIHMS452845  PMID: 16979750

Abstract

Progesterone (pregn-4-ene-3,20-dione; P) and its metabolite 5α-pregnan-3α-ol-20-one (3α,5α-THP) are secreted by ovaries, adrenals, and glial cells. 3α,5α-THP in the midbrain ventral tegmental area mediates sexual receptivity of rodents through its actions at GABAA, NMDA, and/or D1 receptors. The extent to which 3α,5α-THP may influence anti-anxiety/anti-stress effects, conditioning and/or reward through these substrates and/or by altering hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis function is discussed. Biosynthesis of 3α,5α-THP occurs in responses to mating and may underlie some of the rewarding aspects of sexual behavior. Recent findings from our laboratory which demonstrate that progestins can enhance approach to novel stimuli, conditioning, and reinforcement are reviewed. How progestins' effects on these processes may underlie response to drugs of abuse is considered and new findings which demonstrate interactions between progestins and cocaine are presented.

Keywords: Neurosteroid; Non-genomic; GABAA; D1 receptors; NMDA rececptor; Anxiety; Affect; Learning; Cocaine; Progesterone; 3α,5α-THP

1. Introduction

Hormones are trophic factors that profoundly influence brain and behavior. In my laboratory, we are particularly interested in the steroid hormone progesterone (pregn-4-ene-3,20-dione; P), which is the primary progestin secreted by the ovaries, and to a lesser extent, the adrenals. In the brain, P is converted by the 5α-reductase enzyme to dihydroprogesterone (5β-Pregnan-3,20-dione-DHP), which like P, binds with a high affinity for intracellular progestin receptors (PRs) that are located in the hypothalamus and other brain regions (reviewed in Blaustein et al., 1994; Iswari et al., 1986; Smith et al., 1974). DHP is subsequently converted in brain by the 3α-hydroxysteroid oxidoreductase enzyme to form 5α-pregnan-3α-ol-20-one (3α,5α-THP), which in physiological concentrations, does not bind readily to PRs (Rupprecht and Holsboer, 1999). 3α,5α-THP is secreted by the ovaries and adrenals and is formed centrally from metabolism of peripheral P and/or DHP. As well, 3α,5α-THP is synthesized centrally in the brain by glial cells in response to stress, independent of peripheral gland secretion. Centrally, 3α,5α-THP has actions via GABAA, NMDA, and/or dopamine receptors, and downstream signal transduction pathways, rather than intracellular progestin receptors, which is a substrate for P's actions (Frye, 2001a,b; 2006a,b; Frye and DeBold, 1992; Frye and Vongher, 1999a,b; Frye et al., 1993, 1999, 2000, 2006a,b,c,d, e,f; Petralia and Frye, 2004). 3α,5α-THP can also have effects to dampen hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis function (Patchev et al., 1994, 1996). Indeed, it has been hypothesized that 3α,5α-THP is a very important neuroendocrine regulator that may be involved in homeostatic responses, as demonstrated by its increase in response to stress and subsequent effects to enhance parasympathetic activity (Engel and Grant, 2001). Thus, this review paper discusses how variations in progestins may mediate behavioral processes, such as reward, conditioning, and/or stress, which may influence susceptibility and/or response to drugs of abuse.

2. Progestins and reproductive cycles

Progestins co-vary with estradiol (17β-estra-1,3,5(10)-triene-3,17 β-diol; E2) across reproductive cycles. Throughout development, females have greater variations in, and higher levels of, P and 3α,5α-THP than do males. During the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle, progestin levels of women (P: 1–2 nmol/l; 3α,5α-THP 0.3 nmol/l) are low similar to that of men (P: 1–2 nmol/l; 3α,5α-THP 0.3 nmol/l). However, during the luteal phase (P: 25 nmol/l; 3α,5α-THP 2 nmol/l) and pregnancy (P: 650 nmol/l; 3α,5α-THP 14 nmol/l), progestin levels of women are much higher than are men's (Genazzani et al., 1998; Pearson Murphy and Allison, 2000). This same pattern occurs for rats and mice (Frye and Bayon, 1999; Frye and Vongher, 1999a,b,c,d,e, 2001; Holzbauer, 1975, 1976, 1985; Holzbauer et al., 1985). During the diestrous phase of the estrous cycle, progestin levels of female rats (P: 20–60 nmol/l; 3α,5α-THP 5–25 nmol/l) are low similar to that of males (P: 1–45 nmol/l; 3α,5α-THP 1–20 nmol/l). However, during the proestrous phase (P: 60–100 nmol/l; 3α,5α-THP 25–40 nmol/l) and pregnancy (P: 75–150 nmol/l; 3α,5α-THP 50–75 nmol/l), progestin levels of females are much higher than that of males.

Sex differences in progestin levels are mainly due to gonadal and adrenal sources; however, central biosynthesis of 3α,5α- THP may also be a source of differences in progestin concentrations. E2 also varies with progestins over reproductive cycles and can enhance biosynthesis of progestins in brain and may influence processes related to effects of drugs of abuse (Carroll et al., 2004; Cheng and Karavolas, 1973; Frye and Rhodes, 2005a,b,c; Malendowicz, 1976; Resko et al., 1986; Vongher and Frye, 1999). However, effects of E2 are not discussed further here, as this topic is addressed comprehensively in another contribution to this special issue.

3. Progestins and motivated behaviors

In animal models, progestins can influence the expression of motivated behaviors, such as feeding, fighting, fleeing, and mating. During reproductive cycles, when progestin levels are higher, the incidence of many motivated behaviors, including feeding, anti-conflict behavior, running wheel activity and lever presses, and sexual behavior, are greater than during the lowprogestin phases of the cycle (Canonaco et al., 1990; Gerall and Dunlap, 1973; Gerall et al., 1973; Kanarek and Beck, 1980; Roberts et al., 1989a,b; Roth et al., 2005). Ovariectomy (ovx), removal of the ovaries which are the primary endogenous peripheral source of progestins, obviate cyclic increases in these motivated behaviors. Administration of P and/or its metabolites, but not vehicle, reinstates increases in food consumption, antiaggressive behavior, running wheel activity and lever presses, and sexual behavior of ovx rats to levels which are comparable to that observed over the estrous cycle (Bless et al., 1997; Canonaco et al., 1990; Chen et al., 1996; Frye, 2001a,b; Marrone et al., 1975; Mascarenhas et al., 1992; Miczek et al., 2003; Pinna et al., 2005).

4. Progestins and lordosis

The lordosis reflex that sexually-receptive rodents display in response to mating-relevant stimulation is a motivated behavior that has been extensively utilized to ascertain effects and mechanisms of progestins. Using a standard laboratory mating paradigm, we have placed proestrous females with male rodents in a small arena, and the incidence and intensity of the female's lordosis response is assessed for a maximum of 10 min. Mice (c57s) that have higher incidence and intensity of lordosis on initial mating have greater central levels 3α,5α-THP than P in the hypothalamus and midbrain, brain areas that are required for P-facilitated mating (Frye and Vongher, 2001). As well, there are differences in lordosis and midbrain 3α,5α-THP levels of adult rats that were selectively bred for divergent anxiety responses to maternal separation as infants (Frye et al., 2006b,c,d). Adult rats that demonstrated higher infantile anxiety responses showed significantly greater incidence and intensity of lordosis, solicitation behavior, anti-aggressive behavior, and midbrain 3α,5α-THP than do their counterparts that were bred for low anxiety responses perinatally (Table 1). These findings suggest that individual variability in reproductive behavior may be associated with endogenous differences in 3α,5α-THP.

Table 1.

Reproductive and endocrine measures of adult, proestrous rats (n = 8–16/grp) that were selectively bred for infantile vocalization responses to maternal separation

Measure Rats selectively-
bred for response to
maternal separation
as infants
High-
anxiety
Low-
anxiety
Incidence of lordosis (%) 80 ± 6* 54 ± 10
Intensity of lordosis 2.3 ± 0.2* 1.4 ± 0.3
Incidence of solicitation (%) 54 ± 9# 33 ± 10
Incidence of aggression (%) 17 ± 5* 36 ± 11
Plasma 3α,5α-THP levels (not-tested rats — nmol/l) 110 ± 9# 84 ± 10
Plasma 3α,5α-THP levels (tested rats — nmol/l) 168 ± 21* 90 ± 13
Midbrain 3α,5α-THP levels (not-tested rats — nmol/g) 27 ± 5# 16 ± 4
Midbrain 3α,5α-THP levels (tested rats — nmol/g) 18 ± 2* 10 ± 1
*

Indicates analyses of variance or t-tests reveal a significant difference (p<0.05) between groups.

#

Indicates analyses of variance or t-tests reveal a tendency for differences (p<.0.10) between groups.

Manipulating levels of progestins also alters reproductive behavior. Administering P or 3α,5α-THP systemically, to the hypothalamus, and/or midbrain, facilitates lordosis behavior of ovx, E2-primed rodents (Frye, 2001a,b; Frye and Gardiner, 1996; Frye and Vongher, 1999c,d, 2001; Frye et al., 2004). Experimental manipulations that increase 3α,5α-THP, independent of P levels in midbrain, are sufficient to enhance lordosis (Frye, 2001a,b; Frye et al., 2003, 2004; Frye and Petralia, 2003a,b; Frye and Seliga, 2003). Inhibiting P's metabolism to 3α,5α-THP by systemic or intra-VTA infusions of metabolism inhibitors decreases lordosis commensurate with lowering 3α,5α-THP levels (Frye and Vongher, 2001; Frye et al., 1998; Petralia et al., 2001; Petralia and Frye, 2005). These findings demonstrate that there are causal effects of 3α,5α-THP in the midbrain VTA to facilitate lordosis.

Findings, such as these, have led us to consider the extent to which effects of 3α,5α-THP on lordosis are related to its effects on anxiety and/or changes in sensory processing. As such, we have begun to use another approach to examine this.

5. Progestins and paced mating

Although using lordosis as a bioassay has been advantageous to begin to elucidate progestins' actions in the VTA, there are serious limitations to this approach. First, lordosis may not be the most sensitive behavioral measure. It is a species-typical, stereotypic posture that female rodents assume to enable mating. Due to its reflexive nature, lordosis may be more impervious to manipulations, or subtle variations in response to manipulations, may not be evident. Second, the experimental paradigm of standard mating, which is typically employed when lordosis is used as a bioassay, is limited. In a standard mating paradigm, rats are typically placed in small arenas (aquaria) that may not enable the full complement of behaviors to be expressed and/or readily observed. Indeed, in a standard mating paradigm, male rats readily maneuver females into corners. Because females cannot escape in this situation, mating is very “efficient” and involves minimal social interaction, limiting the face validity and broader interpretations from this measure.

More naturalistic mating is characterized by exploration and affiliations, social behaviors that bring individuals together, as well as reproductive and aggressive behaviors (Carter et al., 1999). Reproduction requires that exploration occurs to find mates, that fearful responses to potential mates are suppressed, and that approaches are made to stimuli that previously elicited aggressive responses (Carter et al., 1999). Hence, exploration must be enhanced and aggressive behaviors inhibited for mating to occur. The prior section discussed evidence that 3α,5α-THP mediates lordosis. Notably, 3α,5α-THP may have a role in other mating-relevant behaviors (solicitation, aggression, anxiety), which suggest its functional role extends beyond the reflexive lordosis posture. The approaches and data in support of this are presented below.

One approach that we have used to address progestins' role and mechanisms in motivated behaviors beyond lordosis is to utilize a more ethologically-relevant mating paradigm. In seminatural “paced” mating paradigms, female rats control sexual contacts from a male, the mating sequence takes longer to occur, and the resulting fertility and fecundity are greater than what occurs with standard mating (Coopersmith and Erskine, 1994; Frye and Erskine, 1990). Interestingly, female rats that can pace their reproductive contacts, but not those which are standard mated, demonstrate mating-induced conditioned place preference (CPP; Paredes and Alonso, 1997; Frye et al., 1998; Gonzalez-Flores et al., 2004). Notably, formation of 3α,5α-THP is necessary for paced mating to occur and paced mating (like standard mating—Table 1) also increases biosynthesis of 3α,5α-THP (Frye et al., 1996, 1998, 2000; Frye, 2002; Paredes and Alonso, 1997). Although these findings suggest that formation of 3α,5α-THP may underlie some of the uniquely rewarding aspects of paced mating, whether paced mating requires 3α,5α-THP stimulation of GABAA receptors, and/or can be mimicked by other GABAA agonists is not known but is the subject of future investigations in our laboratory.

6. Progestins and approach of novel stimuli

Because a defining aspect of paced mating is that female rats approach males, and progestins are integral for this, we have begun to investigate whether progestins enhance interaction with other novel stimuli. To address whether P would also influence approach of ovx rats towards a novel female, ovx rats were administered P (4 mg/kg, SC) or sesame oil vehicle and then placed in an open field with a novel conspecific. During a 5 min observation period, P administered rats (109±7 s) spent significantly longer in social interaction than did vehicle-administered rats (35±5 s). To ascertain whether these effects extended to novel stimuli, ovx rats were placed in an open field with two novel objects that were the same and the duration of time they spent investigating these objects was recorded for 3 min. Immediately after this training, rats were administered P (4 mg/kg, SC) or vehicle. Four hours later, rats returned to the open field and the duration of time spent exploring a novel and a familiar object was recorded. As data shown in Table 2 indicate, P administration increased the time spent investigating the novel object. Although there were no controls for habituation to the novel situation as opposed to the object in this paradigm, similar results were observed when effects of progestins on exploration of the novel arm in a Y-maze were investigated using a protocol previously described (Conrad et al., 2004). As Table 2 shows, P also increased the time spent in the novel arm over that observed with vehicle administration. As well, the P regimen used produced physiological levels of P and 3α,5α-THP in plasma and midbrain. Together, these findings suggest that progestins may enhance approach to novel stimuli; however, whether approach is facilitated due to increases in locomotion, effects on memory and/or anxiety were not revealed.

Table 2.

Effects of P (4 mg/kg SC; n=6–8/grp) or vehicle (sesame oil, 0.2 cc, SC; n= 6–8/grp) administration to ovariectomized rats on behavioral measures of approach in the social interaction, object recognition, and Y-maze tasks and endocrine measure (plasma and midbrain P and 3α,5α-THP levels)

Task/measure (units) Compound administered
Progesterone Vehicle
Social interaction/time with conspecific
(% of total time)
37 ± 7* 11 ± 5
Object recognition/time with new object
(% of total time)
76 ± 7* 42 ± 11
Y-maze/time in novel arm (% of total time) 62 ± 6* 48 ± 2
Plasma P levels (nmol/l) 96 ± 15* 44 ± 10
Plasma 3α,5α-THP levels (nmol/l) 48 ± 21# 21 ± 12
Midbrain P levels (nmol/g) 46 ± 13* 16 ± 4
Midbrain 3α,5α-THP levels (nmol/g) 23 ± 7# 15 ± 5
*

Indicates analyses of variance or t-tests reveal a significant difference (p<0.05) between groups.

#

Indicates analyses of variance or t-tests reveal a tendency for differences (p<0.10) between groups.

7. Progestins, estrogen and conditioning

Progestins, in conjunction with E2, may influence discrimination and/or recall of familiar and/or novel factors in the environmental stimuli. Implicit memory involves associative learning, when two events occur together and one learns about the association between them. This kind of cognitive process relies upon the striatum and amygdala, for emotional associations. Effects of hormones on conditioning were originally proposed by one of the pioneering researchers in Behavioral Endocrinology, Frank Beach, when he was mentored by Karl Lashley, an eminent investigator in learning and memory (Beach, 1937). During behavioral estrus (proestrus), when rats are most receptive to mating, acquisition of conditioned avoidance responses is reduced compared to on diestrous (Diaz-Veliz et al., 2000), which may enable aversive aspects of mating to be tolerated. Environmental stimuli associated with mating, when E2 and progestin levels are elevated, readily become conditioned stimuli (Domjan, 2005). For example, rats that have high E2 and progestin levels readily learn to show preference for a setting associated with mating (Frye et al., 1998; Gonzalez-Flores et al., 2004; Oldenburger et al., 1992; Paredes and Alonso, 1997). Further, when there are endogenous or exogenous increases in E2 and P, the rewarding value of brain stimulation is enhanced (Bless et al., 1997) and levels of dopamine in the VTA and nucleus accumbens are greater (Russo et al., 2003a,b). Together these findings suggest that progestins and/or E2 may enhance conditioning for rewarding stimuli associated with mating.

As discussed above, female rats that are allowed to pace their contacts with males readily show a preference for the place associated with mating (Paredes and Alonso, 1997; Frye et al., 1998, 2000; Gonzalez-Flores et al., 2004). We have investigated this further to ascertain estrous cycle differences in pacing-enhanced conditioning. Experimental female rats were placed on one side of a conditioning chamber that allows them to “pace” and/or control their interactions with a male for 20 min. 6 h later, females' preference for the side associated with the male was assessed in a 30min test. Our data (Fig. 1) shows that rats in the Pdominant cycle phase, behavioral estrus (proestrus), are more readily conditioned than diestrous or estrous rats, which have low P levels but discrepant estrogen levels. Proestrous spent more time on the side associated with the male (125±11 s) than did estrous (20±8 s) or diestrous (23±3 s) rats. These findings suggest that progestins, rather than E2, may underlie the hormonal effects on conditioning observed. Furthermore, they are commensurate with the findings from other laboratories which demonstrate that conditioning to sexually-relevant stimuli can occur readily (Domjan, 2005; Paredes and Alonso, 1997) with even a one-trial conditioned place preference approach. As discussed below, typically 4 or 6 pairings is necessary when hormones are administered alone without being paired with sexually-relevant stimuli. Perhaps conditioning occurs so readily in response to sexually relevant because of enhanced neurosteroidogensis that mating can evoke (Frye, 2001a,b), which may serve to consolidate reproductively-relevant information and/or reduce anxiety to a novel context.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The percentage difference in time spent on the male-associated side of the chamber for rats (n=2–4/grp) in proestrus (behavioral estrous; black bar) or estrous (diagonal striped bar) versus that of control diestrous rats for mating-induced conditioned place preference. Female rats in proestrus show a greater mating-induced conditioned place preference than do rats in estrus. * Indicates analyses of variance or t-tests reveal a significant difference (p<0.05) between groups.

8. Progestins alone and conditioning

Evidence from the literature suggests that progestins can enhance conditioning in the absence of E2. Administration of 3α,5α-THP to mice produces CPP (Finn et al., 1997) and state dependent reward (Romieu et al., 2005).Among rats, 3α,5α-THP administration can dose-dependently increase the release of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens (Rouge-Pont et al., 2002). However, a conditioned place aversion has been demonstrated among rats administered 3α,5α-THP (Beauchamp et al., 2000).

Progestins can act as a discriminative stimulus in rats perhaps related to their profound anesthetic effects (Selye, 1942). P (100 mg/kg, IP, 15–30 min before the test) and a synthetic hypnotic (viadril 25 mg/kg) exert similar discriminative stimulus effects in the T-maze (De Beun, 1999; Stewart et al., 1967). In other drug discrimination paradigms, P is discernable from vehicle, and generalizes to pentobarbital (Gorzalka et al., 1995; Heinsbroek et al., 1987a,b). In conditioned aversion tasks, P decreases avoidance behavior (Farr et al., 1995; Manshio and Gershbein, 1975). Findings from people are limited. Subjective memory complaints during pregnancy are associated with impairments in implicit memory (Buckwalter et al., 1998), which may be related to progestins having effects on optimal levels of arousal for performance. Because these findings suggest that progestins may have salient effects to enhance conditioning, we have begun to investigate this further using the CPP task.

9. Progestins and conditioned place preference

Conditioned place preference has been used to determine the rewarding effects of compounds by establishing the contingent associations between an agent administered and environmental stimuli paired with the agent (White and Carr, 1985). We have demonstrated that progestins can enhance CPP of rats (Frye, 2006a), which is mediated by the nucleus accumbens. In our CPP paradigm for rats, after 2 days of habituation to the CPP chamber, there is a baseline test day. Rats were then assigned to either receive vehicle or P (4 mg/kg) paired with the originally nonpreferred side of the chamber on conditioning days (1–2 and 5–7). All rats receive vehicle paired with the originally preferred side of the chamber on control days (3–4 and 8–9). 24 h after the last pairing, rats are given a preference test. InOVXrats, pairing P with the originally non-preferred side of the chamber nearly doubled the time spent on the originally non-preferred side of the chamber on the test day. Notably this was not seen in vehicle-administered rats (Fig. 2). These data suggest that P can enhance CPP of rats.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

The percentage difference in time spent on the originally non-preferred side of the chamber (control) when ovx rats (n=8–12/grp) that had been administered progesterone (4 mg/kg, SC; black bar) versus vehicle (open bar) on training days were subsequently tested for side preference. * Indicates analyses of variance revealed a significant increase (p<0.05) in conditioned place preference for P compared to vehicle administered ovx rats. *p<0.05.

To begin to address the putative mechanisms by which P may influence CPP, we have also investigated P's effects on CPP when administered to wildtype or progestin receptor knockout (PRKO) mice. A slightly different CPP paradigm is utilized for mice (Finn et al., 1997). Mice were habituated for 1 day to the conditioning apparatus. For the next 4 days, mice were administered P (10 mg/kg) or vehicle and placed in the conditioning chamber that had either a grid floor or a floor with holes in it. The following 4 days, mice were administered vehicle and placed in the chamber that had a floor type opposite to that they were exposed to previously. On test day, the floor of the conditioning chamber was equally divided so that half is a “grid” floor and half is a “hole” floor. The amount of time that mice spend on each floor type was recorded. P had similar effects to produce a place preference in wildtype and PR knockout (PRKO) mice compared to vehicle (Fig. 3). These data suggest that P can produce a CPP in mice and that these effects can occur independent of actions at nuclear PRs. Given that PRKO mice readily convert P to 3α,5α-THP (Frye et al., 2006d; Frye and Vongher, 1999a,b,c,d,e), 3α,5α-THP does not bind to PRs (Rupprecht and Holsboer, 1999), and 3α,5α-THP has been demonstrated to enhance CPP ofmice in this paradigm (Finn et al., 1997), these findings are consistent with the notion that 3α,5α-THP may underlie effects of progestins to enhance conditioning. These findings that 3α,5α-THP may underlie such effects also provoke the question as to whether these data may not be explained by effects on conditioning and instead by simply a reduction in anxiety. Indeed, we have demonstrated that this P regimen can also reduce anxiety behavior, enhance 3α,5α-THP levels, and have agonist-like actions atGABAA receptors similarly among wildtype and PRKO mice (Frye et al., 2006b,c,d).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

The percentage difference in time spent on the conditioned floor of wildtype (WT; black bars) or PRKO (horizontal striped bars) mice (n=4–8/grp) that were administered P (10 mg/kg SC) paired with conditioning versus vehicle (sesame oil). P administration increases conditioned place preference in both WT and PRKO mice compared to vehicle control. * Indicates analyses of variance revealed a significant increase (<0.05) in conditioned place preference at test time for P administered mice compared to vehicle controls. *p<0.05.

10. Progestins, stress, HPA function

It has been proposed that 3α,5α-THP may be an important neuroendocrine regulator that may mediate responses to stress and/or environmental stimuli (Engel and Grant, 2001). In support, previous studies showthat both 3α,5α-THP antibody (Purdy et al., 1991) and THDOC administration (Owens et al., 1992) to rats reduce plasma corticosterone levels in response to stress, showing that neurosteroids attenuate HPA axis. These effects are likely due to inhibition of hypothalamic GABAA receptors that regulate CRH transcription, peptide levels, secretion and subsequent activation of pituitary and adrenal responses. For example, administration of 3α,5α-THP to ovx rats decreases hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis function. Acute administration of 3α,5α-THP counteracts the anxiogenic activity of CRH and interferes with corticosteroid mediated regulation of CRH release and gene transcription (Patchev et al., 1994). Acute administration of P can buffer glucocorticoid feedback on the gene expression of CRH in the hypothalamus and corticosteroid receptors in hippocampus (Patchev and Almeida, 1996). Based upon these findings, we investigated whether P administration to ovx rats alters plasma corticosterone levels. Ovx rats were administered 4 mg/kg of P or 3α,5α-THP 3 h before tested in the open field, plus maze, and social interaction tasks. Immediately following this test battery, serum was collected and corticosterone levels were measured by radioimmunoassay. As Fig. 4 illustrates, behavioral testing produced modest increases in plasma corticosterone secretion of ovx rats, but P or 3α,5α-THP administration attenuated this. These data, and the findings from the literature discussed above, suggest that P and 3α,5α-THP may enhance parasympathetic activity and thereby have effects to quiet stress-induced HPA (over)-activation.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Plasma levels of corticosterone following behavior testing for ovx rats (n=4–8/grp) administered P (4 mg/kg, SC; black bars), 3α,5α-THP (4 mg/kg, SC; grey bars) or vehicle (sesame oil 0.2 cc; open bars). * Indicates analyses of variance revealed a significant decrease (p<0.05) in corticosterone levels for P or 3α,5α-THP-administered mice.

The other main body of evidence that 3α,5α-THP may be an important neuroendocrine regulator that mediates stress responses is that 3α,5α-THP is increased rapidly in response to environmental stimuli. In support, a number of different types of acute stressors can alter 3α,5α-THP production. Elevations in plasma and central 3α,5α-THP levels occur rapidly (within 5 min) in response to ambient temperature swim stress (Purdy et al., 1991). Ether exposure can increase plasma levels of P (Erskine and Kornberg, 1992). Exposure to acute foot shock can increase levels of 3α,5α-THP in the cerebral cortex (Drugan et al., 1993). In addition to these life-threatening stimuli, other less aversive events can also alter 3α,5α-THP production. As previously indicated, mating produces rapid and robust increases inmidbrain 3α,5α-THP levels in the absence of peripheral sources of steroid hormones from the ovaries and/or adrenals (Frye et al., 1996, 1998; Frye and Bayon, 1999; Frye, 2001a,b).

In addition to behavioral experiences having salient effects to alter 3α,5α-THP production, evidence is emerging that some drugs of abuse may also alter neuroosteroidogenesis. Administration of various drugs of abuse has also been demonstrated to enhance production of 3α,5α-THP. For example, systemic administration of delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol can increase cortical levels of 3α,5α-THP (Grobin et al., 2005). High, but not lower, dosages of morphine also enhance cortical levels of 3α,5α-THP (Grobin et al., 2005) and 3α,5α-THP administration amplifies the release of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens in response to morphine (Rouge-Pont et al., 2002). Ethanol administration increases plasma and cortical concentrations of 3α,5α-THP (Barbaccia et al., 1999; Hirani et al., 2002; Janis et al., 1998; Morrow et al., 1999; VanDoren et al., 2000). The levels of 3α,5α-THP following these drug regimen are commensurate with those naturally produced by other rewarding behaviors, such as mating (Frye et al., 2001). P and 3α,5α-THP administration regimen that facilitate mating do so in part through their actions at GABAA, NMDA and/or D1 receptors in the midbrain. It is possible that drug-induced increases in 3α,5α-THP biosynthesis may have effects on anxiety/stress, conditioning, and/or rewarding effects through actions at these substrates. Given that: anxiety/stress responses, motivation, conditioning, and reward are processes that underlie drug effects, 3α,5α-THP can have a bearing upon these factors, and some drugs have been demonstrated to alter 3α,5α-THPlevels,we have begun to investigate interactions between progestins and cocaine. Evidence is discussed below that suggest that gender and/or hormonal milieu, associated with differences in progestin concentrations, may mediate response to cocaine.

11. Gender/sex differences in response to drugs of abuse

Gender differences in drug addiction, relapse, craving, rate of drug use, and subjective effects of people suggest that women may be more sensitive to effects of drugs, such as cocaine (Chen and Kandel, 2002; Kosten et al., 1996; Robbins et al., 1999). Although men havemore opportunities to try cocaine,women are as likely as men to use cocaine once exposure has occurred (Van Etten et al., 1999). Women are more likely to experience more nervousness after cocaine use, take longer to feel subjective effects of cocaine, report less euphoria and dysphoria, have more severe drug use, and have stronger cravings in response to cues, than do men (Kosten et al., 1996). Thus, women may experience greater physiological arousal in response to cocaine and the subjective effects may persist for longer, leading perhaps to more severe drug use and/or stronger cravings in response to cues.

There are also sex differences in response to cocaine in animal models. Female rats have greater locomotor and stereotypic behaviors than do male rats after acute and chronic cocaine administration (Festa et al., 2003, 2004; Van Etten and Anthony, 2001). Female rats require lower dosages of cocaine to achieve responses similar to those of male rats, and their behavioral responses persist longer than do male responses (Festa et al., 2004). Female rats self-administered cocaine faster and more often and develop faster contiguous associations between environmental context and cocaine's rewarding properties than do male rats (Lynch et al., 2000; Lynch and Carroll, 1999, 2000). Thus, female, as compared to male, rats may show greater sensitivity to the psychomotor effects of cocaine, and may more readily consume cocaine and condition to its effects.

12. Progestins and response to drugs of abuse

Women's hormonal milieu may influence their subjective response to cocaine. Women who use cocaine have attenuated subjective responses and less desire to smoke cocaine during the progestin-dominant luteal phase than during the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle (Evans et al., 2002; Sofuoglu et al., 2002). Oral administration of Pmay attenuate some of the subjective and/ or the cardiovascular effects of cocaine self-administration in both men and women (Evans and Foltin, 2006; Sofuoglu et al., 2002; Sofuoglu et al., 2004). Thus, these findings suggest that progestins may dampen women's response to the effects of cocaine.

In animal models, progestins may also dampen effects of cocaine. In support, psychomotor effects of cocaine and selfadministration are lower during the progestin-dominant phase of the estrous phase (Quinones-Jenab et al., 1999; Roberts et al., 1989a). Progesterone administration to rats attenuates CPP induced by cocaine and E2-induced cocaine sensitivity/sensitization (Becker, 1999; Jackson et al., 2006; Niyomchai et al., 2005; Russo et al., 2003a,b; Sircar and Kim, 1999). Thus, these findings suggest that P may dampen rats' psychological and/or physiological response to the effects of cocaine.

To address this further, in collaboration with Dr. Quiñones-Jenab, we have investigated what the effects of cocaine are on HPA responses and de novo production of 3α,5α-THP and the extent to which P administration may alter these effects. Adult ovx female and gonadectomized male rodents were administered P (500 µg, SC) or sesame oil vehicle 3 to 4 h prior to cocaine (5 mg/kg IP for males or 20 mg/kg IP for females) or saline vehicle administration. As Table 3 indicates, P had modest effects in male and female rats to dampen plasma corticosterone secretion compared to vehicle administration. Cocaine had very dramatic effects to increase corticosterone secretion but co-administration of P dampened these responses. In the serum, striatum and hippocampus, cocaine also enhanced P and 3α,5α-THP levels, to concentrations which were comparable to that produced by P administration in the absence of cocaine, but co-administration of P and cocaine dampened down these stress-induced increases in neuroendocrine levels. Notably, the levels of progestins produced by cocaine or P alone were commensurate with circulating concentrations of P that we have previously demonstrated can produce mnemonic effects in learning tasks mediated by the striatum or hippocampus, whereas co-administration of P and cocaine produced lower levels of progestins and corticosterone, which may underlie the effects of P administration to obviate cocaine-induced CPP (Frye et al., 2006b,c,d; Frye and Rhodes, 2006; Niyomchai et al., 2005; Russo et al., 2003b; Walf et al., in press). Further, these findings suggest that physiological and/or interoceptive effects of cocaine may involve stress-induced increases in corticosteroids and progestins and that administration of P may dampen some of the psychotropic effects of cocaine by attenuating stress-induced activation of neuroendocrine responses. It is important to note that it has also been reported that cocaine has no effect on 3α,5α-THP levels (Grobin et al., 2005).

Table 3.

Effect of progesterone and/or cocaine administration on plasma corticosterone (B, nmol/l), circulating progesterone (P) and 3α,5α-THP levels (nmol/l) and P and 3α,5α-THP (nmol/g) levels in the striatum and hippocampus (n=6–8 observations/grp)

Plasma
Striatum
Hippocampus
B P 3α,5α-
THP
P 3α,5α-
THP
P 3α,5α-
THP
♀, Vehicle,
saline
4 ± 1 55 ± 9 30 ± 3 18 ± 6 12 ± 4 46 ± 10 37 ± 15
♀, Vehicle,
cocaine
42 ± 12 95 ± 15 52 ± 8 29 ± 10 20 ± 6 75 ± 10 64 ± 16
♀, P, saline 2 ± 1 92 ± 28 54 ± 4 21 ± 8 20 ± 6 58 ± 15 52 ± 7
♀, P, cocaine 32 ± 6 36 ± 5 36 ± 6 16 ± 9 13 ± 5 48 ± 14 32 ± 9
♂, Vehicle,
saline
2 ± 1 41 ± 3 20 ± 3 15 ± 4 10 ± 3 35 ± 9 26 ± 9
♂, Vehicle,
cocaine
27 ± 8 73 ± 12 45 ± 9 22 ± 5 18 ± 6 49 ± 15 36 ± 10
♂, P, saline 1 ± 1 82 ± 20 39 ± 10 19 ± 6 14 ± 5 42 ± 14 30 ± 10
♂, P, cocaine 21 ± 5 60 ± 19 29 ± 9 18 ± 6 12 ± 5 37 ± 11 33 ± 9

13. Progestins, sensory and/or attentional processing

It is also important to note that progestins can also influence sensory and/or attentional processes. There are menstrual cycle variations in sensitivity to tactile stimuli, olfaction and visual detection (Bereiter and Barker, 1980; DeMarchi and Tong, 1972; Diamond et al., 1972; Henkin, 1974; Kenshalo, 1996; Sommer, 1973; Zimmerman and Parlee, 1973). Among rodents, size of receptive fields for the whisker barrel, flank, or perineum is greater when hormone levels are high (Frye and Rhodes, 2005a,b,c; Kow and Pfaff, 1983). Progestins can also enhance attentional processes. Rats in behavioral estrus show less distractability than do diestrous rats (Birke et al., 1979). Improved performance on perceptual restructuring tasks is seen during the progestin dominant luteal phase (Broverman et al., 1968; Klaiber et al., 1974). Thus, it will be important to evaluate how progestins' effects on these processes may also influence the outcomes discussed above.

14. Summary

Findings have been presented that demonstrated that: P and/or 3α,5α-THP can enhance, or be increased by rewarding behavior and that some of these effects occur independent of actions at intracellular PRs, implying actions of 3α,5α-THP via its substrates, which include GABAA, NMDA, and/or D1 receptors. Administration of P or 3α,5α-THP can enhance approach and dampen corticosterone secretion. 3α,5α-THP is increased in response to novel stressors, including cocaine administration. P administration can dampen stress hormone secretion in response to cocaine administration. These findings suggest that progestins may have effects on reward, conditioning and/or stress hormone secretion that may influence vulnerability to drug abuse.

Acknowledgments

Grant support was provided by the National Science Foundation (IBN03-16083) and the National Institute of Mental Health (MH06769). Technical assistance provided by Caryn Duffy, Kassandra Edinger, Dr. Madeline Rhodes, and/or Alicia Walf is greatly appreciated. Collaboration with Dr. Quiñones-Jenab was integral for the latter study.

References

  1. Barbaccia ML, Affricano D, Trabucchi M, Purdy RH, Colombo G, Agabio R, et al. Ethanol markedly increases “GABAergic” neurosteroids in alcoholpreferring rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 1999;384:R1–R2. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(99)00678-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Beach F. Neural basis of innate behavior I: effects of cortical lesions on the maternal behavior pattern in the rat. J Comp Psychol. 1937;24:393–436. [Google Scholar]
  3. Beauchamp MH, Ormerod BK, Jhamandas K, Boegman RJ, Beninger RJ. Neurosteroids and reward: allopregnanolone produces a conditioned place aversion in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2000;67:29–35. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(00)00299-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Becker JB. Gender differences in dopaminergic function in striatum and nucleus accumbens. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1999;64:803–812. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(99)00168-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bereiter DA, Barker DJ. Hormone-induced enlargement of receptive fields in trigeminal mechanoreceptive neurons. I. Time course, hormone, sex and modality specificity. Brain Res. 1980;184:395–410. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(80)90808-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Birke LI, Andrew RJ, Best SM. Distractibility changes during the oestrous cycle of the rat. Anim Behav. 1979;27:597–601. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(79)90196-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blaustein JD, Tetel MJ, Ricciardi KH, Delville Y, Turcotte JC. Hypothalamic ovarian steroid hormone-sensitive neurons involved in female sexual behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1994;19:505–516. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(94)90036-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bless EP, McGinnis KA, Mitchell AL, Hartwell A, Mitchell JB. The effects of gonadal steroids on brain stimulation reward in female rats. Behav Brain Res. 1997;82:235–244. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(96)00129-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Broverman DM, Klaiber EL, Kobyashi Y, Vogel W. Roles of activation and inhibition in sex differences in cognitive abilities. Psychol Rev. 1968;75:23–50. doi: 10.1037/h0027284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buckwalter JG, Stanxzyk FZ, McCleary CA, Bluestein BW, Buckwalter DK, Rankin KP, et al. Pregnancy, the postpartum, and steroid hormones: effects on cognition and mood. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1998;24:69–84. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(98)00044-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Canonaco M, Valenti A, Maggi A. Effects of progesterone on [35S] t-butylbicyclophosphorothionate binding in some forebrain areas of the female rat and its correlation to aggressive behavior. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1990;37:433–438. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(90)90008-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carroll ME, Lynch WJ, Roth ME, Morgan AD, Cosgrove KP. Sex and estrogen influence drug abuse. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2004;25:273–279. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2004.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Carter C, Lederhendler I, Kirkpatrick B, editors. The integrative neurobiology of affiliation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1999. pp. p. ix–p. xiv. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chen K, Kandel D. Relationship between extent of cocaine use and dependence among adolescents and adults in the United States. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002;68:65–85. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(02)00086-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chen SW, Rodriguez L, Davies MF, Loew GH. The hyperphagic effect of 3α-hydroxylated pregnane steroids in male rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1996;53:777–82. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02142-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cheng YJ, Karavolas HJ. Conversion of progesterone to 5α-pregnane-3,20-dione and 3α-hydroxy-5α-pregnan-20-one by rat medical basal hypothalami and the effects of estradiol and stage of estrous cycle on the conversion. Endocrinology. 1973;93:1157–1162. doi: 10.1210/endo-93-5-1157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Conrad CD, Jackson JL, Wieczorek L, Baran SE, Harman JS, Wright RL, et al. Acute stress impairs spatial memory in male but not female rats: influence of estrous cycle. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2004;78:569–579. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.04.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Coopersmith C, Erskine MS. Influence of paced mating and number of intromissions on fertility in the laboratory rat. J Reprod Fertil. 1994;102:451–458. doi: 10.1530/jrf.0.1020451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. De Beun R. Hormones of the hypothalamo–pituitary–gonadal axis in drug discrimination learning. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1999;64:311–317. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(99)00056-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. DeMarchi GW, Tong JE. Menstrual, diurnal, and activation effects on the resolution of temporally paired flashes. Psychophysiology. 1972;9:362–367. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1972.tb03220.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Diamond M, Diamond AL, Mast M. Visual sensitivity and sexual arousal levels during the menstrual cycle. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1972;155:170–176. doi: 10.1097/00005053-197209000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Diaz-Veliz G, Butron S, Benavides MS, Dussaubat N, Mora S. Gender, estrous cycle, ovariectomy, and ovarian hormones influence the effects of diazepam on avoidance conditioning in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2000;66:887–892. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(00)00283-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Domjan M. Pavlovian conditioning: a functional perspective. Annu Rev Psychol. 2005;56:179–206. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Drugan RC, Park R, Kaufman L, Holmes PV. Etiology of the sexual dimorphism in renal peripheral benzodiazepine receptor response to stress in rats. Horm Behav. 1993;27:348–365. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1993.1026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Engel SR, Grant KA. Neurosteroids and behavior. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2001;46:321–348. doi: 10.1016/s0074-7742(01)46067-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Erskine MS, Kornberg E. Stress and ACTH increase circulating concentrations of 3 alpha-androstanediol in female rats. Life Sci. 1992;51:2065–2071. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(92)90157-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Evans SM, Foltin RW. Exogenous progesterone attenuates the subjective effects of smoked cocaine in women, but not in men. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:659–674. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Evans SM, Haney M, Foltin RW. The effects of smoked cocaine during the follicular and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle in women. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;159:397–406. doi: 10.1007/s00213-001-0944-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Farr SA, Flood JF, Scherrer JF, Kaiser FE, Taylor GT, Morley JE. Effect of ovarian steroids on footshock avoidance learning and retention in female mice. Physiol Behav. 1995;58:715–723. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)00124-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Festa ED, Jenab S, Chin J, Gazi FM, Wu HB, Russo SJ, et al. Frequency of cocaine administration affects behavioral and endocrine responses in male and female Fischer rats. Cell Mol Biol (Noisy-le-grand) 2003;49:1275–1280. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Festa ED, Russo SJ, Gazi FM, Niyomchai T, Kemen LM, Lin SN, et al. Sex differences in cocaine-induced behavioral responses, pharmacokinetics, and monoamine levels. Neuropharmacology. 2004;46:672–687. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2003.11.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Finn DA, Phillips TJ, Okorn DM, Chester JA, Cunningham CL. Rewarding effect of the neuroactive steroid 3 alpha-hydroxy-5 alpha-pregnan-20-one in mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1997;56:261–264. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(96)00218-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Frye CA. The role of neurosteroids and non-genomic effects of progestins and androgens in mediating sexual receptivity of rodents. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2001a;37:201–222. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(01)00119-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Frye CA. The role of neurosteroids and nongenomic effects of progestins in the ventral tegmental area in mediating sexual receptivity of rodents. Horm Behav. 2001b;40:226–233. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Frye CA, Bayon LE. Mating stimuli influence endogenous variations in the neurosteroids 3α,5αTHP and 3α-Diol. J Neuroendocrinol. 1999;11:839–847. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.1999.00379.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Frye CA, DeBold JF. Muscimol facilitates sexual receptivity in hamsters when infused into the ventral tegmentum. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1992;42:879–887. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(92)90044-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Frye CA, Erskine MS. Influence of time of mating and paced copulation on induction of pseudopregnancy in cyclic female rats. J Reprod Fertil. 1990;90(2):375–385. doi: 10.1530/jrf.0.0900375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Frye CA, Gardiner SG. Progestins can have a membrane-mediated action in rat midbrain for facilitation of sexual receptivity. Horm Behav. 1996;30:682–691. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1996.0069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Frye CA, Vongher JM. Progestins’ rapid facilitation of lordosis when applied to the ventral tegmentum corresponds to efficacy at enhancing GABAA receptor activity. J Neuroendocrinol. 1999a;11:829–837. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.1999.00367.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Frye CA, Vongher JM. GABAA, D1, and D5, but not progestin receptor, antagonist and anti-sense oligonucleotide infusions to the ventral tegmental area of cycling rats and hamsters attenuate lordosis. Behav Brain Res. 1999b;103:23–34. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(99)00020-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Frye CA, Vongher JM. Progesterone has rapid and membrane effects in the facilitation of female mouse sexual behavior. Brain Res. 1999c;815:259–269. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(98)01132-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Frye CA, Vongher JM. GABA(A),D1, and D5, but not progestin receptor, antagonist and anti-sense oligonucleotide infusions to the ventral tegmental area of cycling rats and hamsters attenuate lordosis. Behav Brain Res. 1999d;103:23–34. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(99)00020-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Frye CA, Vongher JM. 3α,5α-THP in the midbrain ventral tegmental area of rats and hamsters is increased in exogenous hormonal states associated with estrous cyclicity and sexual receptivity. J Endocrinol Investig. 1999e;22:455–464. doi: 10.1007/BF03343590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Frye CA, Vongher JM. Progesterone and 3α, 5α-THP enhance sexual receptivity in mice. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115(5):1118–1128. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Frye CA, Petralia SM. Lordosis of rats is modified by neurosteroidogenic effects of membrane benzodiazepine receptors in the ventral tegmental area. Neuroendocrinology. 2003a;77:71–82. doi: 10.1159/000068338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Frye CA, Petralia SM. Mitochondrial benzodiazepine receptors in the ventral tegmental area modulate sexual behaviour of cycling or hormone-primed hamsters. J Neuroendocrinol. 2003b;15:677–686. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2003.01047.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Frye CA, Seliga AM. Effects of olanzapine infusions to the ventral tegmental area on lordosis and midbrain 3α,5α-THP concentrations in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2003;170:132–139. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1523-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Frye CA, Rhodes ME. Administration of estrogen to ovariectomized rats promotes conditioned place preference and produces moderate levels of estrogen in the nucleus accumbens. Brain Res. 2005a;1067:209–215. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Frye CA, Rhodes ME. Estrogen-priming can enhance progesterone’s antiseizure effects in part by increasing hippocampal levels of allopregnanolone. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2005b;81:907–916. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Frye CA, Rhodes ME. Progesterone's 5α-reduced metabolite-3α,5α-THP, mediates lateral displacement of hamsters. Brain Res. 2005c;1038:59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Frye CA, Mermelstein PG, DeBold JF. Bicuculline infused into the hamster ventral tegmentum inhibits, while sodium valproate facilitates, sexual receptivity. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1993;46:1–8. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(93)90308-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Frye CA, Bayon LE, Pursnani N, Purdy RH. The neurosteroids, progesterone and 3α,5α-THP, enhance sexual motivation, receptivity, and proceptivity in female rats. Brain Res. 1998;808:72–83. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(98)00764-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Frye CA, Bayon LE, Vongher JM. Intravenous progesterone elicits a more rapid induction of lordosis in rats than does SKF38393. Psychobiology. 1999;28:99–109. [Google Scholar]
  54. Frye CA, Murphy RE, Platek SM. Anti-sense oligonucleotides, for progestin receptors in the VMH and glutamic acid decarboxylase in the VTA, attenuate progesterone-induced lordosis in hamsters and rats. Behav Brain Res. 2000;115:55–64. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00242-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Frye CA, Petralia SM, Rhodes ME, Stein B. Fluoxetine may influence lordosis of rats through effects on midbrain 3 alpha, 5 alpha-THP concentrations. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;1007:37–41. doi: 10.1196/annals.1286.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Frye CA, Rhodes ME, Petralia SM, Walf AA, Sumida K, Edinger KL. 3alpha–hydroxy–5alpha–pregnan–20–one in the midbrain ventral tegmental area mediates social, sexual, and affective behaviors. Neuroscience. 2006a;138(3):1007–1014. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Frye CA, Sumida K, Dudek BC, Harney JP, Lydon JP, O'malley BW, et al. Progesterone’s effects to reduce anxiety behavior of aged mice do not require actions via intracellular progestin receptors. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006b;186:312–322. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0309-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Frye CA, Sumida K, Lydon JP, O'Malley BW, Pfaff D. Progesterone and its metabolite-3α,5α-THP, facilitate sexual behavior of aged mice. Psychopharmacology. 2006c;85:423–432. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0300-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Frye CA, Sumida K, Zimmerberg B, Brunelli SA. Rats bred for high versus low anxiety responses neonatally demonstrate increases in lordosis, pacing behavior, and midbrain 3α,5α-THP levels as adults. Behav Neurosci. 2006d;120:281–289. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.2.281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Frye CA, Walf AA, Petralia SM. Progestin in facilitation of lordosis in rodents involves adenylyl cyclase activity in the ventral tegmental area. Horm Behav. 2006e Aug;50(2):237–244. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Frye CA, Walf AA, Petralia SM. In the ventral tegmental area, progestins have actions at D1 receptors for lordosis of hamsters and rats that involve GABAA receptors. Horm Behav. 2006f Aug;50(2):332–337. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Genazzani AR, Petraglia F, Bernardi F, Casarosa E, Salvestroni C, Tonetti A, et al. Circulating levels of allopregnanolone in humans: gender, age endocrine influences. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1998;83:2099–2103. doi: 10.1210/jcem.83.6.4905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Gerall AA, Dunlap JL. The effect of experience and hormones on the initial receptivity in female and male rats. Physiol Behav. 1973;10:851–854. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(73)90053-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Gerall AA, Dunlap JL, Hendricks SE. Effect of ovarian secretions on female behavioral potentiality in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1973;82:449–465. doi: 10.1037/h0034113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Gonzalez-Flores O, Camacho FJ, Dominguez-Salazar E, Ramirez-Orduna JM, Beyer C, Paredes RG. Progestins and place preference conditioning after paced mating. Horm Behav. 2004;46:151–157. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Gorzalka BB, Wilkie DM, Hanson LA. Discrimination of ovarian steroids by rats. Physiol Behav. 1995;58:1003–1011. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)00146-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Grobin AC, VanDoren MJ, Porrino LJ, Morrow AL. Cortical 3 alpha-hydroxy-5 alpha-pregnan-20-one levels after acute administration of Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol, cocaine and morphine. Psychopharmacology. 2005;179:544–550. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2084-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Heinsbroek RP, van Haaren F, Zantvoord F, van de Poll NE. Discriminative stimulus properties of pentobarbital and progesterone in male and female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1987a;28:371–374. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(87)90455-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Heinsbroek RP, van Haaren F, Zantvoord F, van de Poll NE. Effects of pentobarbital and progesterone on random ratio responding in male and female rats. Psychopharmacology. 1987b;93:178–181. doi: 10.1007/BF00179930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Henkin RI. Sensory changes during the menstrual cycle. In: Van De Wiele RL, Richart RM, Halberg F, Ferin M, editors. Biorhythms and human reproduction. New York: Wiley; 1974. pp. 277–285. [Google Scholar]
  71. Hirani K, Khisti RT, Chopde CT. Behavioral action of ethanol in Porsolt’s forced swim test: modulation by 3 alpha-hydroxy-5 alpha-pregnan-20-one. Neuropharmacology. 2002;43:1339–1350. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(02)00330-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Holzbauer M. Physiological variations in the ovarian production of 5α-pregnane derivatives with sedative properties in the rat. J Steroid Biochem. 1975;6:1307–1310. doi: 10.1016/0022-4731(75)90357-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Holzbauer M. Physiological aspects of steroids with anesthetic properties. Med Biol. 1976;54:227–242. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Holzbauer M. In vivo secretion of 3α -hydroxy-5α -2pregnan-20-one, a potent anesthetic steroid, by the adrenal gland of the rat. J Steroid Biochem. 1985;22:97–102. doi: 10.1016/0022-4731(85)90147-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Holzbauer M, Birmingham MK, De Nicola AF, Oliver JT. In vivo secretion of 3 alpha-hydroxy-5 alpha-pregnan-20-one, a potent anaesthetic steroid, by the adrenal gland of the rat. J Steroid Biochem. 1985;22:97–102. doi: 10.1016/0022-4731(85)90147-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Iswari S, Colas AE, Karavolas HJ. Binding of 5 alpha-dihydroprogesterone and other progestins to female rat anterior pituitary nuclear extracts. Steroids. 1986;47(2–3):189–203. doi: 10.1016/0039-128x(86)90088-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Jackson LR, Robinson TE, Becker JB. Sex differences and hormonal influences on acquisition of cocaine self-administration in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(1):129–138. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Janis GC, Devaud LL, Mitsuyama H, Morrow AL. Effects of chronic ethanol consumption and withdrawal on the neuroactive steroid 3alpha-hydroxy-5alpha-pregnan-20-one in male and female rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1998;22:2055–2061. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Kanarek RB, Beck JM. Role of gonadal hormones in diet selection and food utilization in female rats. Physiol Behav. 1980 Feb;24(2):381–386. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(80)90102-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Kenshalo DR. Changes in the cool threshold associated with phases of the menstrual cycle. J Appl Physiol. 1996;21:1031–1039. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1966.21.3.1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Klaiber E, Broverman D, Vogel W, Kobayashi M. Rhythms in plasma MAO activity, EEG and behavior during the menstrual cycle. In: Ferin M, Halberg F, Richart RM, Van De Wiele RL, editors. Biorhythms and human reproduction. New York: Wiley; 1974. pp. 124–55. [Google Scholar]
  82. Kosten TR, Kosten TA, McDougle CJ, Hameedi FA, McCance EF, Rosen MI, et al. Gender differences in response to intranasal cocaine administration to humans. Biol Psychiatry. 1996;39:147–148. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00386-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Kow L-M, Pfaff DW. Effects of estrogen treatment on the size of receptive field and response threshold of pudendal nerve in the female rat. Neuroendocrinology. 1983;13:299–313. doi: 10.1159/000122214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Lynch WJ, Carroll ME. Sex differences in the acquisition of intravenously self-administered cocaine and heroin in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1999;144:77–82. doi: 10.1007/s002130050979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Lynch WJ, Carroll ME. Reinstatement of cocaine self-administration in rats: sex differences. Psychopharmacology. 2000;148:196–200. doi: 10.1007/s002130050042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Lynch WJ, Arizzi MN, Carroll ME. Effects of sex and the estrous cycle on regulation of intravenously self-administered cocaine in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2000;152:132–139. doi: 10.1007/s002130000488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Malendowicz LK. Sex differences in adrenocortical structure and function. III. The effects of postpubertal gonadectomy and gonadal hormone replacement on adrenal cholesterol sidechain cleavage activity and on steroids biosynthesis by rat adrenal homogenates. Endokrinologie. 1976;67:26–35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Manshio DT, Gershbein LL. Avoidance and poke behavior in rats after gonadectomy and hormanal treatment. Res Commun Chem Pathol Pharmacol. 1975;12:473–480. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Marrone BL, Roy EJ, Wade GN. Progesterone stimulates running wheel activity in adrenalectomized–ovariectomized rats. Horm Behav. 1975;6:231–236. doi: 10.1016/0018-506x(75)90010-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Mascarenhas JF, Borker AS, Venkatesh P. Differential role of ovarian hormones for taste preferences in rats. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 1992;36:244–246. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Miczek KA, Fish EW, De Bold JF. Neurosteroids, GABAA receptors, and escalated aggressive behavior. Horm Behav. 2003;44:242–257. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Morrow AL, Janis GC, VanDoren MJ, Matthews DB, Samson HH, Jonak PH, et al. Neurosteroids mediate pharmacological effects ethanol: a new mechanism of ethanol action? Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1999 Dec;23(12):1933–1940. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1999.tb04094.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Niyomchai T, Russo SJ, Festa ED, Akhavan A, Jenab S, Quinones-Jenab V. Progesterone inhibits behavioral responses and estrogen increases corticosterone levels after acute cocaine administration. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2005;80:603–610. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Oldenburger WP, Everitt BJ, de Jonge FH. Conditioned place preference induced by sexual interaction in female rats. Horm Behav. 1992;26:214–228. doi: 10.1016/0018-506x(92)90043-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Owens MJ, Ritchie JC, Nemeroff CB. 5 alpha-pregnane-3 alpha-21-diol-20-one (THDOC) attenuates mild stress-induced increases in plasma corticosterone via a non-glucocorticoid mechanism: comparison with alprazolam. Brain Res. 1992;573:353–355. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90788-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Paredes RG, Alonso A. Sexual behavior regulated (paced) by the female induces conditioned place preference. Behav Neurosci. 1997;111:123–128. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.111.1.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Patchev VK, Almeida OF. Gonadal steroids exert facilitating and “buffering” effects on glucocorticoid-mediated transcriptional regulation of corticotropin-releasing hormone and corticosteroid receptor genes in rat brain. J Neurosci. 1996;16:7077–7084. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-21-07077.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Patchev VK, Shoaib M, Holsboer F, Almeida OF. The neurosteroid tetrahydroprogesterone counteracts corticotropin-releasing hormone-induced anxiety and alters the release and gene expression of corticotropin-releasing hormone in the rat hypothalamus. Neuroscience. 1994;62:265–271. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)90330-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Patchev VK, Hassan AH, Holsboer DF, Almeida OF. The neurosteroid tetrahydroprogesterone attenuates the endocrine response to stress and exerts glucocorticoid-like effects on vasopressin gene transcription in the rat hypothalamus. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1996;15:533–540. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(96)00096-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Pearson Murphy BE, Allison CM. Determination of progesterone and some of its neuroactive ring A-reduced metabolites in human serum. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol. 2000;74:137–142. doi: 10.1016/s0960-0760(00)00098-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Petralia SM, Frye CA. In the Ventral Tegmental Area, G-proteins and cAMP mediate 3α,5α-THP’s actions at dopamine type 1 receptors for lordosis of rats. Neuroendocrinology. 2004;80:233–243. doi: 10.1159/000082752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Petralia SM, Frye CA. In the ventral tegmental area picrotoxin blocks FGIN 1–27–induced increases in sexual behavior of rats and hamster. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005 Mar;178(2–3):174–182. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2001-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Petralia SM, Jahagirdar V, Frye CA. Inhibiting biosynthesis and/or metabolism of progestins in the ventral tegmental area attenuates lordosis of rats in behavioural oestrus. J Neuroendocrinol. 2005;17:545–552. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2005.01342.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Pinna G, Costa E, Guidotti A. Changes in brain testosterone and allopregnanolone biosynthesis elicit aggressive behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:2135–4040. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409643102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Purdy RH, Morrow AL, Moore PH, Jr, Paul SM. Stress-induced elevations of gamma-aminobutyric acid type A receptor-active steroids in the rat brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1991;88:4553–4557. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.10.4553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Quinones-Jenab V, Ho A, Schlussman SD, Franck J, Kreek MJ. Estrous cycle differences in cocaine-induced stereotypic and locomotor behaviors in Fischer rats. Behav Brain Res. 1999;101:15–20. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(98)00073-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Resko JA, Stadelman HL, Handa RJ. Control of 5α-reduction of testosterone in neuroendocrine tissues of female rats. Biol Reprod. 1986;34:870–877. doi: 10.1095/biolreprod34.5.870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Robbins SJ, Ehrman RN, Chidress AR, O'Brian CP. Comparing levels of cocaine cue reactivity in male and female outpatients. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1999;53:223–230. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(98)00135-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Roberts DC, Bennett SA, Vickers GJ. The estrous cycle affects cocaine self-administration on a progressive ratio schedule in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1989a;98:408–411. doi: 10.1007/BF00451696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Roberts DC, Loh EA, Vickers G. Self-administration of cocaine on a progressive ratio schedule in rats: dose-response relationship and effect of haloperidol pretreatment. Psychopharmacology. 1989b;97:535–538. doi: 10.1007/BF00439560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Romieu P, Lucas M, Maurice T. Sigma(1) receptor ligands and related neuroactive steroids interfere with the cocaine-induced state of memory. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005 Aug;31 doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300885. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Roth ME, Negus SS, Knudson IM, Burgess MP, Mello NK. Effects of gender and menstrual cycle phase on food-maintained responding under a progressive-ratio schedule in cynomolgus monkeys. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2005;82:735–743. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.11.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Rouge-Pont F, Mayo W, Marinelli M, Gingras M, Le Moal M, Piazza PV. The neurosteroid allopregnanolone increases dopamine release and dopaminergic response to morphine in the rat nucleus accumbens. Eur J Neurosci. 2002;16:169–173. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02084.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Rupprecht R, Holsboer F. Neuroactive steroids: mechanisms of action and neuropsychopharmacological perspectives. Trends Neurosci. 1999;22:410–416. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(99)01399-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Russo SJ, Festa ED, Fabian SJ, Gazi FM, Kraish M, Jenab S, et al. Gonadal hormones differentially modulate cocaine-induced conditioned place preference in male and female rats. Neuroscience. 2003a;120:523–533. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00317-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Russo SJ, Jenab S, Fabian SJ, Festa ED, Kemen LM, Quinones-Jenab V. Sex differences in the conditioned rewarding effects of cocaine. Brain Res. 2003b;970:214–220. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(03)02346-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Selye H. Correlations between the chemical structure and pharmacological actions of the steroids. Endocrinology. 1942;30:437–453. [Google Scholar]
  118. Sircar R, Kim D. Female gonadal hormones differentially modulate cocaine-induced behavioral sensitization in Fischer, Lewis, and Sprague–Dawley rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1999;289:54–65. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Smith HE, Smith RG, Toft DO, Neergaard JR, Burrows EP, O'Malley BW. Binding of steroids to progesterone receptor proteins in chick oviduct and human uterus. J Biol Chem. 1974;249:5924–5932. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Sofuoglu M, Babb DA, Hatsukami DK. Effects of progesterone treatment on smoked cocaine response in women. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;72:431–435. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00716-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Sofuoglu M, Mitchell E, Kosten TR. Effects of progesterone treatment on cocaine responses in male and female cocaine users. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2004;78:699–705. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Stewart J, Krebs WH, Kaczender E. State-dependent learning produced with steroids. Nature. 1967;216:1223–1224. doi: 10.1038/2161223a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Sommer B. The effect of menstruation on cognitive and perceptual–motor behavior: a review. Psychosom Med. 1973;35:515–534. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197311000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. VanDoren MJ, Matthews DB, Janis GC, Grobin AC, Devaud LL, Morrow AL. Neuroactive steroid 3alpha-hydroxy-5alpha-pregnan-20-one modulates electrophysiological and behavioral actions of ethanol. J Neurosci. 2000;20:1982–1989. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-05-01982.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Van Etten ML, Anthony JC. Male–female differences in transitions from first drug opportunity to first use: searching for subgroup variation by age, race, region, and urban status. J Women’s Health Gend-Based Med. 2001;10:797–804. doi: 10.1089/15246090152636550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Van Etten ML, Neumark YD, Anthony JC. Male–female differences in the earliest stages of drug involvement. Addiction. 1999;94:1413–1419. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.949141312.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Vongher JM, Frye CA. Progesterone in conjunction with estradiol has neuroprotective effects in an animal model of neurodegeneration. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1999;64:777–785. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(99)00140-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Walf AA, Rhodes ME, Frye CA. Ovarian steroids enhance object recognition in naturally-cycling and ovariectomized, hormone-primed rats. Neurobiol Learn Memory in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. White NM, Carr GD. The conditioned place preference is affected by two independent reinforcement processes. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1985;23:37–42. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(85)90127-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Zimmerman E, Parlee MB. Behavioral changes associated with the menstrual cycle: an experimental investiation. J Appl Soc Psychol. 1973;3:335–344. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES