Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2011 Dec 19;20(2):92–106. doi: 10.1037/a0026463

Impaired Control over Alcohol Use: An Under-Addressed Risk Factor for Problem Drinking in Young Adults?

Robert F Leeman 1, Julie A Patock-Peckham 2, Marc N Potenza 3
PMCID: PMC3613490  NIHMSID: NIHMS371377  PMID: 22182417

Abstract

Impaired control over alcohol use may be defined as “a breakdown of an intention to limit consumption in a particular situation” (Heather, Tebbutt, Mattick, & Zamir, 1993, p. 701) and has long been considered an important feature of alcohol dependence. Evidence suggests impaired control is highly relevant to young adult problem drinking. In the natural history of problem drinking, impaired control tends to develop early and may predict alcohol-related problems prospectively in undergraduates. Impaired control over alcohol use may be a facet of generalized behavioral under-control specifically related to drinking. In particular, impaired control is theoretically and empirically related to impulsivity. The question of whether impaired control represents a facet of impulsivity or a related but separate construct requires further study. However, theoretical arguments and empirical evidence suggest that there are unique qualities to the constructs. Specifically, existing data suggest that self-report measures of impaired control and impulsivity over alcohol use relate distinctly to problem drinking indices in young adults. Several lines of future research concerning impaired control are suggested, using the impulsivity literature as a guide. We conclude that impaired control is a valuable construct to the study of young adult problem drinking and that measures of impaired control should be included in more young adult alcohol studies. The extent to which impaired control over the use of other substances and impaired control over engagement in other addictive behaviors are clinically relevant constructs requires additional study.

Keywords: alcohol, alcohol-related problems, college drinking, dopamine, impulsivity


Impaired control over alcohol use constitutes “a breakdown of an intention to limit consumption in a particular situation” (Heather, Tebbutt, Mattick, & Zamir, 1993, p. 701). Difficulty limiting alcohol use may manifest itself as a diminished ability to avoid alcohol use altogether or to control alcohol use once initial consumption has begun (Heather et al., 1993; Kahler, Epstein, & McCrady, 1995). As far back as the 18th century, impaired control has been viewed as an important feature of addictions (Levine, 1978), and continues to occupy a key place in recent definitions. O’Brien, Volkow and Li (2006) defined addiction as “loss of control over intense urges to take the drug even at the expense of adverse consequences” (p. 764, emphasis added). Though impaired control pertains to addiction in general (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000), research on impaired control over alcohol use is arguably more developed than in other addictive behaviors. Research suggests impaired control’s relevance may extend to younger drinkers, who may not yet be addicted (e.g., Chung & Martin, 2002; Leeman, Toll, Taylor & Volpicelli, 2009; Patock-Peckham, Cheong, Balhorn, & Nagoshi, 20011).

Alcohol-related impaired control may be a facet of a more generalized failure of behavioral control specifically related to drinking (Patock-Peckham et al., 2001). In particular, impaired control is theoretically (Bickel & Marsh, 2001) and empirically (e.g., Nagoshi, 1999; Patock-Peckham, King, Morgan-Lopez, Ulloa, & Filson Moses, 2011; Patock-Peckham & Morgan-Lopez, 2006) related to impulsivity, a construct that may be defined as “a predisposition toward rapid, unplanned reactions to internal or external stimuli with diminished regard to the negative consequences of these reactions to the impulsive individual or others” (Brewer & Potenza, 2008; Moeller, Barratt, Dougherty, Schmitz, & Swann, 2001). Impulsivity is a complex, multi-faceted construct with relevance to a wide range of psychiatric conditions including substance use disorders (Moeller et al., 2001). The core components of impulsivity (e.g., choice, response and reflection components, as reviewed in Verdejo-Garcia et al, 2008) have been found to relate differently to various aspects of alcohol involvement and other addictive behaviors (Dick et al., 2010; Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2008; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001). Though the constructs of impaired control and impulsivity are related, data suggest that self-report measures of impaired control over alcohol use and impulsivity relate distinctly to problem drinking indices in young adults. These findings, along with other empirical data and theoretical reasoning, suggest that impaired control and impulsivity represent independent constructs.

The theoretical value of the impaired control construct may be two-fold with these scenarios being non-mutually-exclusive. Impaired control may be one way in which elevated problem drinking risk is manifested in impulsive individuals. Findings in which impaired control partially mediated relationships between impulsivity and alcohol involvement variables among undergraduates support this notion (Leeman, Fenton, Kulesza, Stewart, Taylor, & Copeland, 2009; Patock-Peckham & Morgan-Lopez, 2006; Patock-Peckham, King, Morgan-Lopez et al., 2011). Impaired control may also uniquely capture risk related to behavioral under-control among those who are not considered impulsive in their everyday lives. This possibility is supported by findings that impaired control over alcohol use retained significant relationships with alcohol involvement indices with impulsivity included in the statistical models tested in each of the above studies. These issues will be addressed in detail later in this manuscript.

Progressions of addictions have been characterized by shifts from impulsive, novelty-driven or sensation-seeking behaviors toward compulsive or habit-driven behaviors (Brewer and Potenza, 2008; Dalley, Everitt, & Robbins, 2011; Everitt & Robbins, 2005; Fineberg et al. 2010). As such, impaired control and impulsivity appear applicable to a broad range of addictive processes, both substance- and non-substance-related. At the same time, impaired control and impulsivity may differentially relate to various addictions. For example, impulsivity-related constructs have been closely linked to addictive processes involving stimulants, alcohol, cigarettes and gambling and arguably less so to other drug use behaviors (e.g., MDMA/ecstasy) (Verdejo-Garcia, Lawrence, & Clark, 2008). The extent to which relationships between impaired control and other addictive behaviors follow patterns similar to impulsivity and its core components is a topic for future research. Evidence suggests impaired control is relevant to conceptualizations of alcohol use behaviors. Further, alcohol use is an issue of importance to young adult health (Johnston, O’Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2011; Littlefield & Sher, 2009) and evidence points to a role for impaired control in young adult drinking. Arguably, research on impaired control over other addictive behaviors is not as well developed as it is for alcohol. For these reasons, we will focus this review on impaired control over alcohol use.

The goal of this manuscript is to review relevant theory and data regarding impaired control as a possible risk factor for problem drinking in young adults. The review will begin with a more detailed description of the construct and a brief discussion of its history in the study of addiction, particularly alcoholism. We then present evidence supporting impaired control’s relevance to young adult alcohol use behavior. The important issue of impaired control’s relationship to impulsivity and other aspects of behavioral under-control is then addressed. In these earlier parts of the review, we focus our attention on issues of relevance to impaired control for which there is empirical support. In a latter section, we outline a non-exhaustive list of topics for future research on impaired control over alcohol use, using the impulsivity literature as a guide. We conclude by considering implications of the material presented.

Background Regarding Impaired Control

Historically, the concept of impaired control has been considered particularly relevant to alcohol use behaviors and disorders (Edwards & Gross, 1976; Jellinek, 1960; Levine, 1978; O’Brien et al., 2006). Impaired control over alcohol use may relate to progression of alcoholism as drinkers may experience difficulty limiting alcohol use early on, which may later be experienced as habitual or compulsive (with an accompanying perceived inability to stop using) with greater alcohol dependence severity (Modell, Mountz, Glaser, & Lee, 1993). Jellinek (1960) made “loss of control” once drinking sessions have begun the defining characteristic of one subtype of alcoholism, the other being defined by an “inability to abstain.” Later, Edwards and Gross (1976) included Jellinek’s subtypes in their definition of the alcohol dependence syndrome, which more broadly influenced the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) substance dependence criteria (see Kahler et al., 1995 for a history of the impaired control construct). Accordingly, two of the seven dependence criteria in the 4th edition text revision of the DSM (DSM-IV-TR, APA, 2000) relate to these aspects of impaired control. Relevant terms pertaining to impaired control are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1.

Relevant Terms Concerning Impaired Control over Alcohol Use

Aspect of impaired
control
Exact wording of DSM-
IV criteria (APA, 2000,
p. 197)
Common shorthand for
DSM-IV criteria
Exact wording of ICD-10
criterion (WHO, 1992, p.
75)
Terminology from
Jellinek’s
conceptualization
Difficulty controlling alcohol use once it has started The substance is often taken in larger amounts or over a longer period than was intended Larger/longer Difficulties in controlling substance-taking behaviour in terms of its onset, termination, or levels of use Loss of control
Difficulty avoiding alcohol use altogether There is a persistent desire or unsuccessful efforts to cut down or control substance use Quit/control Included in above criterion Inability to abstain

Taken together, impaired control may be defined as difficulty limiting alcohol consumption despite intent to do so with a diminished ability to avoid alcohol altogether and trouble controlling alcohol use once it has begun representing important components (Heather et al. 1993). Thus the term “impaired control” subsumes Jellinek’s two alcoholism subtypes and the two relevant DSM-IV substance dependence criteria (Kahler et al., 1995). Presently, the term “impaired control” is favored over “loss of control,” based on the view that control over addictive behaviors is relative, rather than absolute (Heather et al., 1993; Lyvers, 2000).

Competing notions of impaired control as a two-pronged (i.e., difficulty abstaining from and controlling alcohol use once it has begun) and as a unitary construct are a source of complexity in understanding the construct and its assessment. The notion of impaired control having two components has some intuitive appeal. In addition, research in young drinkers has shown noticeably differing rates of endorsement of the two DSM-IV criteria related to impaired control (e.g., Beseler, Taylor, & Leeman, 2010; Chung & Martin, 2002). However, analyses of various self-report items pertaining to impaired control (Kahler et al., 1995) and research on multiple-item, self-report measures (e.g., the Impaired Control Scale [ICS; Heather et al., 1993] and the impaired control subscale of the Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire [YAACQ; Read, Kahler, Strong, & Colder, 2006]) have shown that items assessing difficulty abstaining and controlling alcohol use once it has started tend to be highly correlated. Moreover, self-report items of impaired control have similar relationships to alcohol consumption variables and load onto single factors in factor analyses. These results suggest impaired control is a unitary construct and many individuals who report difficulty abstaining from alcohol also report trouble limiting use once it has started (Kahler et al., 1995). Notably, impaired control is captured in one criterion in the tenth revision of the International Classification of Diseases and Health Problems (ICD-10; WHO, 1992) (Table 1). It is possible that disparities in results may be due, in part, to the subjective element of impaired control contributing more to self-reports (e.g., “I have had an irresistible urge to continue drinking once I started;” “I have found it difficult to resist drinking even for a single day” [ICS; Heather et al., 1993]) than to diagnostic criteria, which may be assessed by clinicians and are often rated in an “all or none” fashion, unlike rating scales, which allow degrees of endorsement. Unless otherwise noted, “impaired control” denotes a unitary construct encapsulating difficulty abstaining and/or controlling alcohol use once it has begun.

Another issue that contributes to complexity in the assessment of impaired control is its subjective component. Intentions to limit alcohol use--entirely (i.e., abstention) or to a degree (i.e., moderate use)--are inherent in impaired control and introduce a subjective aspect (Kahler et al., 1995; Heather, 1995). Impaired control is not merely a pattern of frequent heavy drinking. Based on a series of analyses, Kahler et al. (1995) concluded “none of the (impaired control-related) items were associated with a particular drinking pattern” (p. 1025). Rather, impaired control is a tendency for alcohol use to run counter to prior plans. This subjective aspect presents a challenge in the measurement of impaired control via self-report (Heather, 1995) and additional challenges when assessing impaired control behaviorally as the same behaviors may reflect different levels of impaired control given differences in intentions. This issue may introduce particular challenges for translational research (e.g., how does one accurately assess limits set in rodents?). Consistently, many of those whose drinking is problematic may not set limits on their drinking behaviors and thus may not endorse impaired control (Chick, 1980).

The traditional retrospective report approach is typical in the assessment of impaired control; thus, an important question is how commonly attempts to limit alcohol use are endorsed by young adults. Sugarman and Carey (2007) found, on average, undergraduates reported using more than half of the drinking control strategies in a self-report measure within the past two weeks. This is consistent with reports from other investigators who have found mean scores on measures of protective strategies to be at about the middle of the possible range of scores (Martens, Martin, Littlefield, Murphy, & Cimini, in press; Palmer, Corbin, & Cronce, 2010). These findings suggest some interest in limiting drinking among undergraduates.

Relevance to Young Adult Problem Drinking

Impaired control may be an important construct in identifying which young drinkers are at risk for problem drinking (Heather et al., 1993). Evidence suggests impaired control may represent a risk factor for problem drinking in general (e.g., Heather et al., 1993), as well as a prospective predictor of alcohol-related problems among young adults (Leeman, Toll et al., 2009). These findings are important to consider from the perspective that neural circuitry supporting higher order self-regulation tends not to be fully developed in adolescents (Bava & Tapert, 2010; Casey, Duhoux, & Cohen, 2010; Chambers, Taylor, & Potenza, 2003; Rutherford, Mayes, & Potenza, 2010) and undergoes changes through young adulthood (Wiers et al., 2007).

Young adult heavy drinking is a public health concern. According to the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC), 39% of 18-to-25-year-olds reported heavy episodic drinking at least once in the past month (Harrison, Desai, & McKee, 2008). This level of alcohol consumption is related to serious consequences, such as traffic accidents (Hingson, Zha, & Weitzman, 2009; Yi, Williams, & Smothers, 2004). While many young adults will “mature out” of heavy use by their mid-to-late twenties, a minority will continue heavy use and may encounter clinically significant problems (Jackson, Sher, Gotham, & Wood, 2001). Thus, additional research is needed to determine which young adults may be at heightened risk of negative outcomes (Littlefield & Sher, 2009).

Impaired control over alcohol use emerges relatively early in the natural history of problem drinking (Heather, 1995). Alcoholic adults asked to recount which dependence symptoms developed the earliest frequently cite impaired control (Chick & Duffy, 1979; Langenbucher & Chung, 1995). In adolescents, 93.5% of those meeting DSM-IV alcohol dependence criteria endorsed the “larger/longer” criterion and 58.1% endorsed the “quit/control” criterion (Table 1) (Martin, Kaczynski, Maisto, Bukstein, & Moss, 1995). In a high-risk community sample of adolescent and young adult drinkers, “larger/longer” was the alcohol use disorder (i.e., alcohol abuse or dependence) criterion most likely to be endorsed first (32.9%) and to have lifetime endorsement (61.1%). “Quit/control” was also relatively common (first symptom: 19.2%, lifetime: 34.9%) (Buu, Wang, Schroeder, Kalaida, Puttler, & Zucker, in press). In a general community sample of adolescents and young adults in Germany, “larger/longer” and “quit/control” were the third and fourth most commonly endorsed dependence criteria (3.4% and 3%, respectively). For comparison, tolerance was the most common at 13.1% (Behrendt et al., 2008). In a sample of undergraduates with lifetime alcohol exposure, the “larger/longer” criterion was the second most commonly endorsed in the past year (by 34.8%) after tolerance (45.6%). “Quit/control” was endorsed less frequently (9.6%) (Beseler et al., 2010). Roughly similar endorsement was found in another recent sample of undergraduate current drinkers: “larger/longer”: 43.7%, “quit/control”: 8.1% (Hagman & Cohn, 2011).

Impaired control is relevant to young adult problem drinking via (1) alcohol-related problems (i.e., negative consequences of alcohol use and dependence symptoms [White & Labouvie, 1989]) and (2) heavy use. Heavy patterns could include “heavy episodic drinking,” defined as five or more drinks on one occasion for males, four or more for females (Wechsler, Moeyknes, Davenport, Castillo, & Hansen, 1995), or “binge drinking,” defined as heavy episodic drinking occurring within two hours or less (NIAAA, 2004). Difficulty limiting alcohol consumption may lead to heavy drinking in many cases (Leeman, Fenton, & Volpicelli, 2007). In addition, individuals with difficulty controlling their alcohol use will likely fail to constrain their drinking sufficiently to avoid negative consequences (Patock-Peckham et al., 2001).

Significant relationships have been reported between impaired control and alcohol involvement in adolescents and young adults (Table 2). Impaired control has been implicated in the development of alcohol dependence in longitudinal studies involving adolescents and young adults. Buu et al. (in press) concluded that early “quit/control” onset was indicative of an initial stage of alcohol dependence in a high-risk sample of adolescents and young adults. Behrendt et al. (2008) found that early “larger/longer” onset was associated with elevated risk of alcohol dependence in a German community sample. Impaired control self-reports have also been incorporated into statistical models with other key variables and have predicted unique variance in alcohol involvement among undergraduates (Table 2). ICS scores during freshman year predicted frequency of heavy episodic drinking and alcohol-related problems cross-sectionally (Leeman, Fenton, et al., 2007) and alcohol-related problems prospectively during senior year in a model including several other predictor variables including drinks per week, gender, sensation seeking and expectancies of alcohol’s disinhibiting effects. ICS scores showed moderate stability from freshman to senior year (Leeman, Toll, et al., 2009). In another study, ICS scores significantly predicted alcohol-related problems but not alcohol consumption in cross-sectional regression models (Nagoshi, 1999). In two other studies, ICS scores predicted alcohol-related problems and alcohol consumption in cross-sectional path models (Patock-Peckham et al., 2001; Patock-Peckham & Morgan-Lopez, 2006). In the latter study, impaired control’s relationship to alcohol-related problems was somewhat stronger than its relationship to alcohol use (Patock-Peckham & Morgan-Lopez, 2006).

Table 2.

Relationships between Self-Reported Impaired Control and Alcohol Involvement in Adolescents and Young Adults

Authors Yr Sample IC measure Alcohol variables/comparison Summary of IC findings
Behrendt et al.* 08 Community sample in Germany DSM-IV criteria (Table 1), via interview Alcohol dependence diagnosis “Larger/longer” onset in general and early onset in particular were associated with increased risk of development of alcohol dependence
Buu, Wang, Schroeder, Kalaida, Puttler, & Zucker* IP High-risk community sample first assessed during childhood DSM-IV criteria, via interview Alcohol dependence diagnosis Having “quit control” or “larger/longer” as one’s first AUD symptom conveyed no unique risk of alcohol dependence in a regression model. However, based on the pattern of “quit/control” onset over time, it was thought to be indicative of an initial stage of alcohol dependence.
Chung & Martin 02 Adolescents clinical setting & community DSM-IV criteria, via interview Drinks per occasion
Freq alcohol use
Freq heavy episodic drinking
ADS score
Moderate, significant correlations between both criteria and all alcohol variables, except for a small, significant correlation between ‘quit/control’ and frequency of alcohol use.
Hustad, Barnett, Borsari, & Jackson * 10 First-year undergrads YAACQ IC Comparison between brief alcohol intervention & control Significantly larger decrease in IC in intervention compared to control
Leeman, Corbin, Fucito, Urwin, & O’Malley 11 Undergrads, alcohol users Single item self-report Interest in clinical trial involving naltrexone Significant relationship to clinical trial interest
Leeman, Fenton, & Volpicelli 07 First-year undergrads, alcohol users ICS, Part 2 Freq heavy episodic drinking
Alcohol-related problems
Significant predictor of both variables in regression models
Leeman, Toll, Taylor, & Volpicelli * 09 T1: First-yr undergrads, alcohol users
T2: Senior-yr
ICS, Part 2 Freq heavy episodic drinking

Alcohol-related problems
Significant predictor of alcohol-related problems but not of heavy drinking in prospective regression models.
Nagoshi 99 Undergrads, alcohol users ICS, Part 3 Quantity/freq alcohol use
Alcohol-related problems
Significant predictor of alcohol-related problems, but not of alcohol use in regression models
Oddy & Barry 09 Undergrads, alcohol users ICS—all parts Comparison between heavier and lighter social drinkers Heavier social drinkers scored significantly higher
Palfai & Ostafin 10 Hazardous drinkers (M = 22.5 yrs) ICS, Parts 2 & 3 Past month drinking quantity Moderate, significant partial correlations holding gender constant
Patock-Peckham, Cheong, Balhorn, & Nagoshi 01 Undergrads, alcohol users ICS, Part 3 Quantity/freq alcohol use
Alcohol-related problems
Significant predictor of both variables in path models
Patock-Peckham, Hutchinson, Cheong & Nagoshi 98 Undergrads, alcohol users ICS, Part 3 Alcohol-related problems
Freq alcohol use, freq inebriation, quantity of use
Large, significant correlation with alcohol-related problems; small, significant correlation with frequency of use; moderate, significant correlations with inebriation and quantity of use
Patock-Peckham, King, Morgan-Lopez, Ulloa & Filson-Moses 11 Undergrads ICS, Part 3 Alcohol-related problems Significant predictor in path models
Patock-Peckham & Morgan-Lopez 06 Undergrads, alcohol users ICS, Part 3 Quantity/freq alcohol use
Alcohol-related problems
Significant predictor of both variables in path models, relationship to alcohol-related problems stronger
Read, Beattie, Chamberlain, & Merrill 08 Undergrads, alcohol users YAACQ IC Comparison between heavier and lighter drinkers Heavier drinkers scored significantly higher than lighter drinkers
Read, Kahler, Strong, & Colder 06 Undergrads, alcohol users YAACQ IC Freq any & heavy episodic use
Alcohol-related problems
Significant correlations with all variables, magnitude not reported
Read, Merrill, Kahler, & Strong * 07 Undergrads YAACQ IC Freq any & heavy episodic use
Quantity of alcohol use
Moderate, significant correlations with frequency of any and heavy drinking; nonsignificant correlation with quantity of use

Note. Results from the most sophisticated statistical test reported are included in table.

*

indicates prospective study, all other studies cross-sectional.

Small correlation (r = up to 0.29), moderate correlation (r = 0.30–0.49), large correlation (0.50 or higher). Yr = year, IC = impaired control, IP = in press, AUD = alcohol use disorder (i.e., alcohol abuse or dependence), ADS = Alcohol Dependence Scale (Skinner & Allen, 1982), ICS = Impaired Control Scale, YAACQ IC = impaired control subscale of Young Adult Consequences Questionnaire, freq = frequency of, T1 = time 1, T2 = time 2. Part 2 of the ICS assesses past difficulties limiting alcohol use. Part 3 of the ICS measures beliefs about future difficulties.

In summary, impaired control has been associated with alcohol consumption and related problems among young adults. Relationships to alcohol-related problems have been particularly strong in several studies. Notably, impaired control has predicted unique variance in problem drinking indices in statistical models including several other predictors of drinking outcomes.

Relationships between Impaired Control and Impulsivity

Theory, self-report and limited pharmacologic evidence suggest that impaired control and impulsivity are related. Impaired control could theoretically represent another facet of impulsivity. While the term “impulsivity” has at times been used and defined loosely, more recently it has been fractionated into specific components, such as response and choice forms (Dalley et al., 2011; Dick et al. 2010; Potenza & de Wit 2010; Winstanley, Theobald, Dalley, & Robbins, 2004). Response impulsivity has been defined as deficient inhibitory control over reinforcing thoughts and behaviors (Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2008). Choice impulsivity has been defined as a tendency to select immediate rather than delayed or probabilistic benefits (Green & Myerson, 2009). A component of impulsivity involving diminished reflection has also been described, defined as a failure to gather and evaluate information optimally before making a decision (Kagan, 1966; Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2008). Within these domains, self-report and behavioral assessments may not correlate significantly (Krishnan-Sarin et al., 2007; Reynolds, Ortengren, Richards, & de Wit, 2006), suggesting that how individuals react hypothetically and in real-life situations may vary. Given that the focus of the present review is on impaired control and since the construct of impulsivity and its relationship to alcohol involvement has been addressed thoroughly in several reviews cited above, a full review of impulsivity is outside the scope of this manuscript.

There are theoretical commonalities between impaired control over alcohol use and impulsivity that likely apply to multiple facets of the impulsivity construct. Impaired control and impulsivity both involve difficulties maintaining control. Both constructs involve action despite possible negative consequences. This is reflected in the aforementioned definition of impulsivity (Brewer & Potenza, 2008; Moeller et al., 2001) and empirical relationships among young adults between self-reported impaired control and alcohol-related problems (Table 2). Theoretically, impaired control may be considered a facet of impulsivity with impaired control representing a manifestation of impulsivity in the alcohol use/abuse realm (Patock-Peckham & Morgan-Lopez, 2006; Patock-Peckham et al., 2001) and potentially, in the use and abuse of other substances.

Cross-sectional evidence in adolescents and undergraduates indicates significant, small-to-moderate relationships between impaired control and measures tapping into response and reflection impulsivity (correlations typically in the 0.20–0.30 range) (Table 3). Most of these studies utilized samples of current alcohol users. To date, three self-report measures have been utilized in this research: the impulsiveness subscale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire, version 7 (I.7; Eysenck, Pearson, Easting, & Allsopp, 1985); the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, version 11 (BIS-11; Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995) and the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale, original version (Whiteside & Lynam, 2001). Dick et al. (2010) equated the negative urgency subscale of the UPPS with the attentional impulsiveness subscale of the BIS-11. Arguably, both pertain to response impulsivity while some negative urgency items arguably pertain to reflection impulsivity as well. Similarities between the Eysenck measure and the motor and nonplanning subscales of the BIS-11 have been noted previously (Patton & Stanford, 2012). These measures arguably relate primarily to reflection impulsivity.

Table 3.

Relationships between Self-Reported Impaired Control and Impulsivity in Adolescents and Young Adults

Authors Yr Sample IC scale Impulsivity
scale
Summary of findings
Chung & Martin 02 Adolescents: clinical setting & community DSM-IV criteria, via interview Eysenck I.7 Moderate, significant correlation between “larger/longer” criterion & impulsivity. Non-significant correlation between impulsivity and “quit/control”
Leeman, Fenton, Kulesza, Stewart, Taylor, & Copeland 09 Undergrads, alcohol users ICS, Part 2 BIS-11 Moderate, significant correlation between IC and all three subscales as well as total BIS-11 score.
Nagoshi 99 Undergrads, alcohol users ICS, Part 3 Eysenck I.7 Moderate, significant correlation
Patock-Peckham, King, Backer-Fulghum, Morgan-Lopez, & Leeman 11 Undergrads ICS, Part 3 UPPS Significant path from negative urgency score to impaired control. No other significant paths in SEM to predict drinking quantity and alcohol-related problems.
Patock-Peckham, King, Morgan-Lopez, Ulloa & Filson-Moses 11 Undergrads ICS, Part 3 Eysenck I.7 Significant path from impulsivity to impaired control among both men and women in a multiple group SEM model to predict alcohol-related problems.
Patock-Peckham & Morgan-Lopez 06 Undergrads, alcohol users ICS, Part 3 Eysenck I.7 Significant path from impulsiveness to impaired control in SEM model to predict alcohol frequency/quantity of use and alcohol-related problems

Note. All studies were cross-sectional. Moderate correlation (0.30–0.49). Yr = year, IC = impaired control, Eysenck I.7 = impulsiveness subscale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire, version 7 (Eysenck, Pearson, Eastin, & Allsopp, 1985), ICS = Impaired Control Scale, BIS-11= Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, version 11 (Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995), UPPS = UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale, original version (Whiteside & Lynam, 2001), SEM = structural equation model. Part 2 of the ICS assesses past difficulties limiting alcohol use. Part 3 of the ICS measures beliefs about future difficulties

Limited pharmacologic evidence suggests relationships between impaired control and choice, response and reflection impulsivity. Modell, Mountz and Beresford (1990) proposed impaired control may be triggered by impairment of inhibitory mechanisms in the striatum resulting from acute dopamine release following alcohol use. Modell et al. (1993) obtained some evidence supporting this position from a study of alcohol administration/self-administration following injection of the D2-like receptor antagonist haloperidol or saline in alcohol dependent individuals of varying ages who self-reported frequent impaired control. Following haloperidol, participants reported significantly less impaired control (difficulty resisting an alcoholic beverage if offered) following a priming drink of alcohol than when the priming drink followed saline. After the priming drink, participants were allowed to self-administer additional alcohol. Fewer subsequent drinks were consumed following haloperidol than following saline. Dopaminergic activity has also been tied to response and choice impulsivity (Jentsch & Taylor, 1999). Reduced dopamine D2/D3 (“D2-like”) receptor availability has been related to higher overall BIS-11 (Patton et al., 1995) scores (Buckholtz et al., 2010). Similarly, in rats, impulsive response on an attentional task was associated with lower striatal D2-like receptor binding (Dalley et al., 2007). These findings parallel observations that continued seeking and taking of substances may involve reduced D2-like receptor availability (Volkow, Fowler, & Wang, 2003). Dopaminergic activity has also been hypothesized to contribute to greater delay discounting (i.e., devaluation of distal compared with more immediate rewards) in those with substance use disorders (Schultz, 2011). High concentrations of dopamine in subcortical areas are associated with rapid, dominant, approach responses, while high concentrations in the cortex may serve to weaken inhibitory control and increase response impulsivity. As Jentsch & Taylor (1999) and others (e.g., Arnsten, 2007) have argued, homeostatic dopamine levels in specific brain regions may influence cognitions and behaviors, including those involving self-control, such that levels that are too high or too low are suboptimal. Given negative results for dopamine antagonists in alcohol dependence treatment (Walter, Ramskogler, Semler, Lesch, & Platz, 2001), one should be careful in considering dopaminergic contributions to alcoholism, particularly as potential targets for treatment development. Nonetheless, these findings suggest a role for dopaminergic activity in impaired control over alcohol use and in multiple forms of impulsivity.

While impulsivity and impaired control over alcohol use are related constructs, theoretical arguments and empirical evidence suggest there are also unique qualities to the constructs. These theoretical arguments likely apply to multiple forms of impulsivity as well. While impulsivity is a general tendency that can affect myriad aspects of an individual’s life, impaired control as defined here represents a tendency toward dysregulated response to alcohol use specifically (Patock-Peckham, King, Morgan-Lopez et al., 2011) and possibly addictive behaviors by extension. Accordingly, those with impaired control over alcohol use may not be impulsive in general. There are other examples of individual difference variables related to alcohol response. For instance, evidence suggests non-dependent heavy drinkers tend to self-report greater stimulation than non-dependent lighter drinkers, particularly on the ascending limb of the blood-alcohol curve2 (Quinn & Fromme, 2011).

Another theoretical distinction between impulsivity and impaired control over alcohol use concerns the limit setting aspect of impaired control. While impaired control entails lack of adherence to an intention to limit drinking behavior, with impulsivity there need not ever be any intent to limit behavior (Bickel & Marsch, 2001). Even if limit setting among those with impaired control over alcohol use tends not to be successful ultimately, this inclination toward self-regulation may distinguish it from impulsivity.

Existing data suggest that self-reported impaired control over alcohol use and forms of impulsivity each have clinically relevant, distinct relationships to problem drinking indices in young adults. In studies of undergraduates, statistical models suggest partial mediation of relationships between self-reported Eysenck I.7 scores (Eysenck et al., 1985) and alcohol use (Patock-Peckham & Morgan-Lopez, 2006) and between Eysenck I.7 scores and alcohol-related problems (Patock-Peckham & Morgan-Lopez, 2006; Patock-Peckham, King, Morgan-Lopez et al., 2011) by impaired control. Similarly, impaired control partially mediated relationships between total score on the BIS-11 and binge drinking frequency and, in separate analyses, between alcohol-related problems and the BIS-11 total score, and specifically with the attentional and nonplanning BIS-11 subscales (Leeman, Fenton et al., 2009). Evidence of partial mediation suggests that for some young adult drinkers, impaired control may be a mechanism underlying the relationship between impulsivity and alcohol involvement. Thus, impaired control may be one way in which problem drinking risk is manifested in impulsive individuals. At the same time, significant, direct relationships between impulsivity and alcohol involvement remained in these models. Impaired control was a significant predictor of alcohol involvement variables with impulsivity included in the model as well. This pattern of findings suggests that while impulsivity and impaired control are related (Table 3), they make unique contributions to alcohol use and related problems. The question of whether impaired control represents a facet of impulsivity or a separate but theoretically related construct requires further study.

While impulsivity is arguably the aspect of behavioral under-control (see Sher & Trull, 1994) that is the most relevant to impaired control over alcohol use, other constructs are also potentially relevant. Two constructs—self-regulation and sensation seeking—have been studied in relationship to impaired control. Self-regulation has been defined as a tendency to be able to plan and conduct efforts to achieve adaptive goals, often requiring delayed gratification (Hustad, Carey, Carey, & Maisto, 2009). Low impaired control was found to mediate negative relationships between generalized self-regulation and both alcohol use and alcohol-related problems in undergraduates (Patock-Peckham et al. 2001). There is also limited evidence that impaired control is related to sensation seeking, defined as “the seeking of varied, novel, complex, and intense sensations and experiences, and the willingness to take physical, social, legal, and financial risks for the sake of such experiences” (Zuckerman, 1994, p. 27, emphasis in original). Leeman, Toll et al. (2009) found moderate, significant cross-sectional correlations between impaired control and sensation seeking at Time 1 (freshman year) and Time 2 (senior year) of a prospective study. Patock-Peckham, King, Backer-Fulghum et al. (2011) reported no significant path from sensation seeking to impaired control in a structural equation model involving undergraduate survey data. In summary, impaired control may relate to behavioral under-control constructs besides impulsivity, but relatively few results are available.

Pertinent Research Questions Concerning Impaired Control over Alcohol Use

While we have some knowledge of relationships between self-reported impaired control and problem drinking in young adults and about relationships between impaired control and self-reported impulsivity, a number of key questions have yet to be addressed. After examining the relevant literature on impaired control over alcohol use and impulsivity, we arrived at the list of research questions that will be addressed in this section. Data addressing these outstanding questions could potentially enhance our understanding of impaired control. Given the importance of impaired control to problem drinking risk in young adults, data pertaining to these questions could also improve our understanding of problem drinking risk and treatment in this population. Research on impulsivity has advanced a great deal, in part through the use of a wide variety of research approaches including self-reports, laboratory measures, neuroimaging and animal models (see Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2008, for a review). The impulsivity literature will be used as a guide in outlining some key research questions concerning impaired control over alcohol use.

Is Laboratory/Translational Research Concerning Impaired Control Possible?

Impulsivity research has benefitted from human and animal laboratory studies. In human laboratory studies, many of which have involved young adults, participants have been administered different doses of alcohol and then tested with various tasks indicative of response (e.g., Fillmore, 2004; de Wit, Crean, & Richards, 2006) and choice impulsivity (Reynolds et al., 2006). Impulsive task performance following alcohol administration has also been tested as a predictor of ad libitum consumption (i.e., the participant chooses how much alcohol to have within certain limits) in a subsequent session (Weafer & Fillmore, 2008). A recent series of studies has shown that young adults can be primed or trained to respond in a disinhibited or inhibited fashion, and that these states relate to increased and decreased subsequent alcohol consumption, respectively (Houben, Nederkoorn, Wiers, Jansen, 2011; Jones, Guerrieri, Fernie, Cole, Goudie, & Field, 2011). Animal models have offered valuable results, including high-ethanol-preferring mice making more impulsive choices on a delay-discounting (i.e., choice impulsivity) task than low-ethanol-preferring mice (Oberlin & Grahame, 2009). Animal models have also demonstrated that alcohol can increase impulsive choices. This choice pattern was predictive of greater subsequent ad libitum ethanol consumption (Poulos, Parker, & Le, 1998).

A human laboratory model of impaired control could be developed in which participants are allowed to self-administer alcohol in a manner that adheres to or violates a limitation, which would be associated with alternate reiniforcement or punishment (e.g., gain or loss of money). Translational alcohol research is of great value (Crabbe, 2010; Leeman, Heilig, Cunningham, Stevens, Duka, & O’Malley, 2010); however, development of an accompanying animal model could be complex given that a limit on alcohol use cannot be conveyed directly to or by animals. Perhaps animals could be trained to self-administer alcohol first and then subsequently, a punishment for greater than moderate consumption or an alternate reward (e.g., food) for adhering to moderate levels could be introduced. An animal’s lack of ability to drink moderately could be indicative of impaired control. However, the extent to which such models could distinguish impaired control from choice impulsivity warrants careful consideration.

Testing animals and humans on both the ascending and descending limb of the blood alcohol curve would be important given findings that some effects of alcohol (e.g., stimulant and sedative effects; Quinn & Fromme 2011) may be relatively stronger or weaker dependent upon the limb of the curve. Alcohol-related cognitive and behavioral impairment has also been found to differ based on limb of the blood alcohol curve in young adults (Ostling & Fillmore, 2010). These factors have all been found to relate to amount of alcohol consumed and consequently may be relevant to impaired control over alcohol use.

These types of laboratory models would be valuable for multiple reasons. Such models would be helpful in further elucidating relationships between impaired control and impulsivity (see below) and for testing, on a preliminary basis, treatments (i.e., pharmacotherapies in animals and humans and counseling modalities in humans) that may ameliorate impaired control over alcohol use. Laboratory models would also offer a means of testing relationships between impaired control and variables pertaining to alcohol-related reinforcement. These include craving (Addolorato et al., 2005; Rodd, Bell, Sable, Murphy, & McBride, 2004), cue-induced alcohol consumption (Cooney, Litt, Morse, Bauer, & Gaupp, 1997; Pickering & Liljequist, 2003), alcohol priming effects (de Wit 1996; 2000), stimulant effects of alcohol (Crabbe, Bell, & Ehlers, 2010; Morean & Corbin, 2010) and consumption in response to stress (Sinha, 2001; Volpicelli, 1987) for both humans and animals and alcohol-related expectancies (Jones, Corbin, & Fromme, 2001) and motives (Cooper, Russell, Skinner, & Windle, 1992) for humans. Given their reinforcing qualities and relationships to increased alcohol use, relevance of these variables to impaired control seems likely. These variables could be assessed in the course of laboratory studies should such models of impaired control be developed. Lastly, unlike cross-sectional self-report studies, research involving laboratory models can be used to draw conclusions regarding impaired control as a cause of alcohol use in general and rapid or heavy use, in particular.

How Does Impaired Control Relate to Laboratory Measures of Impulsivity?

Alcohol use has been found to increase both response and choice impulsivity (Dick et al., 2010; Perry & Carroll, 2008; Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2008). One or more of these cognitive effects may increase the likelihood of impaired control over alcohol use. The effect of even small-to-moderate doses of alcohol (equivalent to one or two drinks; Weafer & Fillmore, 2008) on response impulsivity (Fillmore, 2004; Marczinski, Abroms, Van Selst, & Fillmore, 2005; de Wit, Crean, & Richards, 2006; Marczinski & Fillmore, 2005b; Mulvihill, Skilling, & Vogel-Sprott, 1997; Ostling & Fillmore, 2010; Perry & Carroll, 2008) may make it difficult for some drinkers to ignore salient, alcohol-related cues (Bechara, 2005; Bond, 1998; Field et al., 2010) or resist urges to drink further (de Wit, 2000). It is potentially problematic that behavioral activation (i.e., appetitive, approach behaviors, see Carver & White, 1994; Gray, 1987; Houben et al., 2011; Wiers et al., 2007) remains relatively stable following low-to-moderate alcohol doses (de Wit et al., 2000; Gauggel et al., 2010; Marczinski & Fillmore, 2005a; Miller & Fillmore, in press; Mulvihill et al. 1997), while inhibitory control declines (Weafer & Fillmore, 2008). Accordingly, one’s ability to seek alcohol may remain intact while the ability to inhibit this impulse and maintain limits on alcohol use may be compromised. Further, alcohol’s negative effects on controlled, effortful cognitive processes (Abroms, Gottlob, & Fillmore, 2006; Giancola, Josephs, Parrott, & Duke, 2010) may make it difficult for some drinkers to utilize cognitions that facilitate controlled drinking (e.g., moderate drinking strategies, reasons for limiting drinking) (Finn & Hall, 2004). Animal and human evidence suggests that accentuation of choice impulsivity may require higher alcohol doses than response impulsivity (Poulos et al., 1998; Reynolds et al., 2006). Thus, after consumption of greater quantities of alcohol, longer-term consequences could be ignored in favor or more immediate rewards related to alcohol use.

Future research could examine relationships between these cognitive impairments and impaired control over alcohol use directly, with the use of laboratory measures of impulsivity. Several different types of studies could be conducted. Correlational studies would provide some insight into the magnitude of relationships between self-reported impaired control and performance on tasks assessing response impulsivity (e.g., go/no-go and stop-signal tasks) and impulsive choice (e.g., delay-discounting tasks). These laboratory measures could also be incorporated into alcohol administration studies. Following baseline completion of an impaired control self-report and a battery of laboratory measures, an approach similar to that of Modell et al. (1993) could be utilized. A priming dose of alcohol could be administered followed by a rating of impaired control (i.e., perceived difficulty in limiting consumption should more alcohol be offered) and completion of the same laboratory measure(s). Changes in self-reported impaired control could be related to decrements in task performance following alcohol administration. Again, like Modell et al., subsequent ad libitum consumption could be offered, followed by additional impaired control ratings and laboratory measure completion. This added step would allow for examination of relationships between further changes in task performance and patterns of consumption potentially indicative of impaired control. Tasks assessing impulsive choice or response could be incorporated into studies utilizing human and animal laboratory models of impaired control, should those be developed. This would allow for possible translational research to explore further relationships between impaired control and core components of impulsivity.

Is Impaired Control a Longitudinal Predictor of Problem Drinking?

Longitudinal research has determined that impulsivity and related constructs assessed during childhood are predictive of subsequent problem drinking in late adolescence/early young adulthood (Mezzich, Tarter, Feske, Kirisci, McNamee, & Day, 2007) and late young adulthood (Hicks, Iacono, & McGue, 2010). In an adult sample of non-dependent, heavy drinkers (baseline mean age of 37.7 years), high self-reported impulsivity on the BIS-11 (Patton et al., 1995) and longer stop-signal reaction times on a stop-signal task (Logan, 1994) were predictive of elevated risk for development of alcohol dependence at a 4-year follow-up (Rubio et al., 2008).

There is limited evidence supporting impaired control as a prospective predictor of problem drinking in a community sample of adolescents/young adults (Behrendt et al., 2008) and in undergraduates (Leeman, Toll et al., 2009). The impaired control subscale of the YAACQ predicted frequency of any alcohol consumption and heavy alcohol use in a short-term prospective study (Read et al., 2007). There is also one prospective, intervention study finding in young adults (see below) of which we are aware. These findings suggest that self-reported impaired control assessed relatively soon after onset of alcohol consumption (e.g., during adolescence) may predict problem drinking subsequently. However, more long-term longitudinal studies are needed in order to make definitive conclusions regarding the possibility that impaired control over alcohol use may have a causal role in subsequent problem drinking.

Can Impaired Control Be Reduced in the Course of Alcohol Treatment?

While impaired control measures are arguably under-utilized in young adult alcohol studies in general, when they are included it is typically as a predictor (e.g., Nagoshi, 1999; Leeman, Toll et al., 2009) or as a mediating variable along the alcohol-related problems pathway (Patock-Peckham et al., 2001; Patock-Peckham & Morgan-Lopez, 2006; Patock-Peckham et al., 2011). Yet, there is scant evidence regarding impaired control as an outcome in young adult alcohol research. This pattern parallels the historical use of impulsivity constructs; however, recent studies have suggested that self-reported impulsivity may change over time (Littlefield, Sher, & Wood, 2010) and that both self-report and behavioral measures of impulsivity may change during treatment (Blanco et al., 2009; Grant, Chamberlain, Odlaug, Potenza, Kim, 2010; Potenza, Sofuoglu, Carroll, & Rounsaville, 2011) and thus might represent important treatment targets for addiction research. In correlations and cross-sectional models, several variables relate to impaired control in young adults including alcohol-related expectancies (Nagoshi, 1999; Leeman, Toll et al., 2009) and motives (Leeman, Fenton et al., 2007). The testing of models utilizing treatment and other prospective data to predict impaired control would allow for more definitive determinations of which variables are related to impaired control and which may predict its onset. Research regarding which variables concerning alcohol-related reinforcement (e.g., craving, stimulant effects) predict impaired control may be particularly important.

Interventions designed for young adult heavy drinkers, such as Brief Alcohol Screening and Intervention for College Students (BASICS; Dimeff, Baer, Kivlahan, & Marlatt, 1999), attempt to enhance drinking control. Thus, impaired control may represent an important alternate outcome measure for studies testing such interventions. Accordingly, YAACQ impaired control subscale scores decreased significantly in incoming undergraduates one month after a brief, web-based intervention, in comparison with an assessment-only control (Hustad, Barnett, Borsari, & Jackson, 2010). Another clinical implication is that endorsement of “problems with controlling drinking” predicted interest in an alcohol reduction clinical trial involving opiate antagonist naltrexone in a university survey (Leeman, Corbin, Fucito, Urwin, & O’Malley, 2011).

How Is Impaired Control Initiated During a Drinking Session?

Self-reported impaired control relates to various measures of alcohol involvement in young adults (see Table 2). Currently, little is known regarding how impaired control becomes activated during a drinking session. Modell et al. (1993) showed increases in self-reported impaired control following initial drink consumption; however, little is known about the manner in which various factors act separately or in combination to increase or decrease impaired control. Relevant variables include cognitive impairments triggered by alcohol use (see above), social/interpersonal factors (e.g., peer pressure), academic or work demands (e.g., having/not having an early work day or exam the next day); aspects of the environment (e.g., availability of a designated driver); behavioral economic factors (e.g., cost of alcohol) and religiosity. Human and animal laboratory models could be utilized to address at least some of these factors in “real time.” Ecological momentary assessment techniques (see Shiffman, Stone, & Hufford, 2008) in humans could also enable relevant data collection “in the field” during drinking sessions.

What Regions of the Brain and Neurotransmitter Systems Tend To Be Compromised?

Little work has been done to relate impaired control directly to brain function or neurotransmitter systems, with the exception of Modell and colleagues’ work (1990; 1993) concerning relationships between dopaminergic function and impaired control. Research on impulsivity and research linking alcohol use to particular neurotransmitters or brain regions may help guide future research. Reviewing the multiple neurotransmitter systems and brain regions relevant to impulsivity and substance use and addiction is outside the scope of this manuscript (see Brewer & Potenza, 2008; Verdejo-Garcia et al., 2008 for recent reviews). Briefly, multiple neurotransmitter systems (e.g., dopaminergic, serotonergic, noradrenergic, glutamatergic, and opioidergic) have been implicated in impulsivity and addictions, and multiple neural systems involving cortical, subcortical and/or limbic structures appear relevant to impulsivity and addictions. Specifically, structural (e.g., volumetric), functional (involving functional magnetic resonance imaging), ligand-based (e.g., using positron emission tomography), and white matter (e.g., diffusion tensor imaging) studies have linked impulsivity to brain structure/function to impulsivity and addictions, and these studies provide a framework to test how impaired control might relate to different stages and extents of alcohol use.

Criticisms and Implications Regarding Impaired Control

Evidence suggests impaired control is particularly relevant to young adult drinkers. The prevalence of reports of impaired control by young drinkers with relatively brief drinking histories and by those who do not drink excessively has raised concerns that impaired control may not consistently reflect compulsive use indicative of addiction (Caetano, 1999; Chung & Martin, 2002; 2005). Caetano (1999) analyzed reasons for endorsement of DSM-IV alcohol dependence criteria by adult current drinkers in two national samples and expressed concern due to frequent attribution of social motives underlying endorsement of both impaired control criteria. Among adolescents in addiction treatment programs, Chung and Martin (2005) also found frequent attribution of social factors underlying “larger/longer” endorsement.

Impaired control is often endorsed by young people who do not drink compulsively, and this may reflect impaired control’s value as an early predictor of problem drinking. More specifically, impaired control may relate to progression of alcoholism as drinkers may experience difficulty limiting alcohol use early on, which may later be experienced as habitual or compulsive (Modell et al., 1993). It may be that the majority of problem drinking young adults are at this earlier stage in which they are beginning to experience some difficulty limiting their use, but have not yet reached compulsive or addictive use. This also fits with the modern notion of impaired control as being on a continuum, not absolute (Heather et al., 1993; Lyvers, 2000).

While attribution of social motives to the endorsement of impaired control may be viewed as concerning, the aforementioned findings of impaired control accounting for unique variance in alcohol involvement and problem drinking indices (see Table 2) argues against substantial confounding by social motives and other related variables. According to Caetano (1999), part of the reason for concern regarding social motives is that these motives tend not to typify compulsive drinking. In young adults, a great deal of high-risk drinking occurs in social contexts. Drinking games (Borsari, 2004) and extreme binge drinking as part of 21st birthday celebrations (Rutledge, Park, & Sher, 2008) are two examples. Therefore, drinking attributed to social factors should not be dismissed as non-problematic, especially among young adults.

In conclusion, impaired control is a valuable construct to the study of young adult problem drinking. In the natural history of problem drinking, impaired control appears to be one of the earliest symptoms to develop. The question of whether impaired control over alcohol use most appropriately represents a facet of impulsivity or a separate but related construct requires further research. However, data suggest that self-report measures of impaired control have clinically relevant, distinct relationships to problem drinking indices in young adults as compared to multiple self-report measures of impulsivity. Despite these data, structured measures of impaired control are infrequently used in alcohol studies of young adults. At a minimum, existing data suggest that impaired control should be assessed routinely in studies as a possible predictor, mediator and/or outcome variable, particularly in studies involving young adults. Given the brief length of reliable, valid self-report measures (e.g., the ICS and the impaired control subscale of the YAACQ), assessment of impaired control is also practical.

Acknowledgments

Dr. Potenza has received financial support or compensation for the following: Dr. Potenza has consulted for and advised Boehringer Ingelheim; has consulted for and has financial interests in Somaxon; has received research support from the National Institutes of Health, Veteran’s Administration, Mohegan Sun Casino, the National Center for Responsible Gaming and its affiliated Institute for Research on Gambling Disorders, and Psyadon, Forest Laboratories, Ortho-McNeil, Oy-Control/Biotie and Glaxo-SmithKline pharmaceuticals; has participated in surveys, mailings or telephone consultations related to drug addiction, impulse control disorders or other health topics; has consulted for law offices and the federal public defender’s office in issues related to impulse control disorders; provides clinical care in the Connecticut Department of Mental Health and Addiction Services Problem Gambling Services Program; has performed grant reviews for the National Institutes of Health and other agencies; has guest-edited journal sections; has given academic lectures in grand rounds, CME events and other clinical or scientific venues; and has generated books or book chapters for publishers of mental health texts. The content of this manuscript is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agencies. This manuscript was supported in part by the NIH (K01 AA 019694, K05 AA014715, R01 DA019039, P20 DA027844, RC1 DA028279), the VA VISN1 MIRECC, the Connecticut Department of Mental Health and Addiction Services and a Center of Research Excellence Award from the National Center for Responsible Gaming and its affiliated Institute for Research on Gambling Disorders.

Footnotes

The authors report that they have no financial conflicts of interest with respect to the content of this manuscript.

1

Nagoshi, Patock-Peckham and colleagues have utilized the term “drinking control” to refer to the ability to stop drinking (Nagoshi, 1999; Patock-Peckham, Hutchinson, Cheong, & Nagoshi, 1998; Patock-Peckham, Cheong, Balhorn, & Nagoshi, 2001; Patock-Peckham, King, Morgan-Lopez, Ulloa, & Filson Moses, 2011; Patock-Peckham & Morgan-Lopez, 2006). Drinking control is the polar opposite of impaired control and researchers utilizing the term “drinking control” have made use of an impaired control measure valanced in the opposite direction. For consistency, we will utilize only the term “impaired control” throughout the rest of this review.

2

The ascending limb of the blood alcohol curve reflects when more alcohol is being absorbed into the blood stream than is being excreted from the body. Typically, this denotes the first 20–30 minutes of consumption when most individuals experience the stimulating effects of alcohol. In contrast, the descending limb of the blood alcohol curve reflects when alcohol is being excreted at a faster rate than it is being absorbed into the blood. Typically, the descending limb is characterized by the depressant effects of alcohol consumption and may last for several hours depending upon peak blood alcohol concentration (McKim, 2006).

Contributor Information

Robert F. Leeman, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine

Julie A. Patock-Peckham, Department of Psychology, Arizona State University

Marc N. Potenza, Departments of Psychiatry, Child Study and Neurobiology, Yale University School of Medicine

References

  1. Abroms BD, Gottlob L, Fillmore MT. Alcohol effects on inhibitory control of attention: Distinguishing between intentional and automatic mechanisms. Psychopharmacology. 2006;188:324–334. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0524-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Addolorato G, Leggio L, Abenavoli L, Gasbarrini G the Alcoholism Treatment Study Group. Neurobiochemical and clinical aspects of craving in alcohol addiction: a review. Addictive Behaviors. 2005;30:1209–1224. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association (APA) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edition, text revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  4. Arnsten AF. Catecholamine and second messenger influences on prefrontal cortical networks of “representational knowledge”: A rational bridge between genetics and the symptoms of mental illness. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17(Suppl 1):i6–i15. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1458–1463. doi: 10.1038/nn1584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Behrendt S, Wittchen H-U, Hofler M, Lieb R, Ping Low NC, Rehm J, Beesdo K. Risk and speed of transitions to first alcohol dependence symptoms in adolescents: A 10-year longitudinal community study in Germany. Addiction. 2008;103:1638–1647. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02324.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beseler C, Taylor LA, Leeman RF. An Item Response Theory analysis of the DSM-IV alcohol use disorder criteria and “binge” drinking in undergraduates. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2010;71:418–423. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2010.71.418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bickel WK, Marsch LA. Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: Delay discounting processes. Addiction. 2001;96:73–86. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961736.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Blanco C, Potenza MN, Kim SW, Ibanez A, Zaninelli R, Saiz-Ruiz J, Grant JE. A pilot study of impulsivity and compulsivity in pathological gambling. Psychiatry Research. 2009;167:161–168. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.04.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bond AJ. Drug-induced behavioral disinhibition: Incidence, mechanisms and therapeutic implications. CNS Drugs. 1998;9:41–57. [Google Scholar]
  11. Borsari B. Drinking games in the college environment: A review. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 2004;48:29–51. [Google Scholar]
  12. Brewer JA, Potenza MN. The neurobiology and genetics of impulse control disorders: Relationships to drug addictions. Biochemical Pharmacology. 2008;75:63–75. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2007.06.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Buckholtz JW, Treadway MT, Cowan RL, Woodward ND, Li R, Ansari, Zald DH. Dopaminergic network differences in human impulsivity. Science. 2010;329:532. doi: 10.1126/science.1185778. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Buu A, Wang W, Schroder SA, Kalaida NL, Puttler LI, Zucker R. Developmental emergence of alcohol use disorder symptoms and their potential as early indicators for progression to alcohol dependence in a high-risk sample: A longitudinal study from childhood to early adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. doi: 10.1037/a0024926. (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Caetano R. The identification of alcohol dependence criteria in the general population. Addiction. 1999;94:255–267. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.9422559.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;67:319–333. [Google Scholar]
  17. Casey BJ, Duhoux S, Cohen MM. Adolescence: What Do Transmission, Transition, and Translation Have to Do with It? Neuron. 2010:749–760. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chambers RA, Taylor JR, Potenza MN. Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period of addiction vulnerability. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1041–1052. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chick J. Is there a unidimensional alcohol dependence syndrome? British Journal of Addictions. 1980:265–280. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1980.tb01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chick J, Duffy JC. Application to the alcohol dependence syndrome of a method of determining the sequential development of symptoms. Psychological Medicine. 1979;9:313–319. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700030828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Chung T, Martin CS. Concurrent and discriminant validity of DSM-IV symptoms of impaired control over alcohol consumption in adolescents. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2002;26:485–492. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Chung T, Martin CS. What were they thinking? Adolescents’ interpretations of DSM-IV alcohol symptom queries and implications for diagnostic validity. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2005;80:191–200. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cooney NL, Litt MD, Morse PA, Bauer LO, Gaupp L. Alcohol cue reactivity, negative-mood reactivity, and relapse in treated alcoholic men. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997;106:243–250. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.2.243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cooper ML, Russell M, Skinner JB, Windle M. Development and validation of a three-dimensional measure of drinking motives. Psychological Assessment. 1992;4:123–132. [Google Scholar]
  25. Crabbe J. Consilience of rodent and human phenotypes relevant for alcohol dependence. Addiction Biology. 2010;15:103–108. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2009.00188.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Crabbe J, Bell RL, Ehlers CL. Human and laboratory rodent low response to alcohol: is better consilience possible? Addiction Biology. 2010:125–144. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2009.00191.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dalley JW, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron. 2011;69:680–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dalley JW, Fryer TD, Brichard L, Robinson ES, Theobald DE, Lääne K, Robbins TW. Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Science. 2007;315:1267–1270. doi: 10.1126/science.1137073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. de Wit H. Priming effects with drugs and other reinforcers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1996;4:5–10. [Google Scholar]
  30. de Wit H. Laboratory-based assessment of alcohol craving in social drinkers. Addiction. 2000;95:S165–S169. doi: 10.1080/09652140050111735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. de Wit H. Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes. Addiction Biology. 2009;14:22–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00129.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. de Wit H, Crean J, Richards JB. Effects of d-amphetamine and ethanol on a measure of behavioral inhibition in humans. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2000;114:830–837. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.4.830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dick DM, Smith GT, Olausson P, Mitchell S, Leeman RF, O’Malley SS, Sher KJ. Understanding the construct of impulsivity and its relationship to alcohol use disorders. Addiction Biology. 2010;15:217–226. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2009.00190.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Dimeff LA, Baer JS, Kivlahan DR, Marlatt GA. Brief Alcohol Screening and Intervention for College Students (BASICS): A harm reduction approach. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  35. Edwards G, Gross MM. Alcohol dependence: Provisional description of a clinical syndrome. British Medical Journal. 1976;1:1058–1061. doi: 10.1136/bmj.1.6017.1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: From actions to habits to compulsion. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1481–1489. doi: 10.1038/nn1579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Eysenck SBG, Pearson PR, Easting G, Allsopp JF. Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults. Personality and Individual Differences. 1985;6:613–619. [Google Scholar]
  38. Field M, Wiers RW, Christiansen P, Fillmore MT, Verster JC. Acute Alcohol Effects on Inhibitory Control and Implicit Cognition: Implications for Loss of Control Over Drinking. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research. 2010;34:1346–1352. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01218.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Fillmore MT. Drug abuse as a problem of impaired control: Current approaches and findings. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews. 2003;2:179–197. doi: 10.1177/1534582303257007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Fineberg NA, Potenza MN, Chamberlain SR, Berlin HA, Menzies L, Bechara A, Hollander E. Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviors, from animal models to endophenotypes: a narrative review. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:591–604. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Finn PR, Hall J. Cognitive ability and risk for alcoholism: short-term memory capacity moderates personality-risk for alcohol abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:569–581. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.4.569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gauggel S, Heusinger A, Forkmann T, Boecker M, Lindenmeyer J, Cox WM, Staedtgen M. Effects of alcohol cue exposure on response inhibition in detoxified alcohol-dependent patients. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2010;34:1584–1589. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01243.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Giancola PR, Josephs RA, Parrott DJ, Duke AA. Alcohol myopia revisited: Clarifying aggression and other acts of disinhibition through a distorted lens. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2010;5:265–278. doi: 10.1177/1745691610369467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Grant JE, Chamberlain SR, Odlaug BL, Potenza MN, Kim SW. Memantine shows promise in reducing gambling severity and cognitive inflexibility in pathological gambling: A pilot study. Psychopharmacology. 2010;212:603–612. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1994-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Gray JA. The psychology of fear and stress. 2nd ed. New York: Cambridge; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  46. Green L, Myerson J. Experimental and correlational analyses of delay and probability discounting. In: Madden GJ, Bickel WK, editors. Impulsivity: The behavioral and neurological science of discounting. Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association; 2009. pp. 67–92. [Google Scholar]
  47. Hagman BT, Cohn AM. Toward DSM-V: Mapping the alcohol use disorder continuum in college students. Drug & Alcohol Dependence. 2011;118:202–208. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Harrison EL, Desai RA, McKee SA. Non-daily smoking and alcohol use, hazardous drinking, and alcohol diagnoses among young adults: Findings from the NESARC. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research. 2008;32:2081–2087. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00796.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Heather N. Impaired control: A concept of fundamental significance. Addiction. 1995;90:1025–1036. [Google Scholar]
  50. Heather N, Tebbutt JS, Mattick RP, Zamir R. Development of a scale for measuring impaired control over alcohol consumption: a preliminary report. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1993;54:700–709. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1993.54.700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hicks BM, Iacono WG, McGue M. Consequences of an adolescent onset and persistent course of alcohol dependence in men: Adolescent risk factors and adult outcomes. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research. 2010;34:819–833. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01154.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hingson RW, Zha W, Weitzman ER. Magnitude of and trends in alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18–24, 1998–2005. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2009;(suppl 16):12–20. doi: 10.15288/jsads.2009.s16.12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Houben K, Nederkoorn C, Wiers RW, Jansen A. Resisting temptation: Decreasing alcohol-related affect and drinking behavior by training response inhibition. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2011;116:132–136. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hustad JTP, Barnett NP, Borsari B, Jackson K. Web-based alcohol prevention for incoming college students: A randomized controlled trial. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35:183–189. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hustad JTP, Carey KB, Carey MP, Maisto S. Self-regulation, alcohol consumption and consequences in college student heavy drinkers: A simultaneous latent growth analysis. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2009;70:373–382. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2009.70.373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Jackson KM, Sher KJ, Gotham HJ, Wood PK. Transitioning into and out of large-effect drinking in young adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001;110:378–391. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.110.3.378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Jellinek EM. The disease concept of alcoholism. New Haven, CT: College and University Press; 1960. [Google Scholar]
  58. Jentsch JD, Taylor JR. Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: Implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146:373–390. doi: 10.1007/pl00005483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2010. Volume II: College students and adults ages 19–50. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  60. Jones A, Guerrieri R, Fernie G, Cole J, Goudie A, Field M. The effects of priming restrained versus disinhibited behaviour on alcohol-seeking in social drinkers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2011;113:55–61. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Jones BT, Corbin W, Fromme K. A review of expectancy theory and alcohol consumption. Addition. 2001;96:57–72. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961575.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kagan J. Reflection-impulsivity: The generality and dynamics of conceptual tempo. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1966;71:17–24. doi: 10.1037/h0022886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kahler CW, Epstein EE, McCrady BA. Loss of control and inability to abstain: The measurement of and the relationship between two constructs in male alcoholics. Addiction. 1995;90:1025–1036. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1995.90810252.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Krishnan-Sarin S, Reynolds B, Duhig AM, Smith A, Liss T, McFetridge A, Potenza MN. Behavioral impulsivity predicts treatment outcome in a smoking cessation program for adolescent smokers. Drug & Alcohol Dependence. 2007;88:79–82. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Langenbucher JW, Chung T. Onset and staging of DSM- IV alcohol dependence using mean age and survival hazard methods. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1995;104:346–354. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.104.2.346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Leeman RF, Corbin WR, Fucito LM, Urwin JW, O’Malley SS. Predictors of interest in an alcohol reduction clinical trial of naltrexone among undergraduates. 2011 doi: 10.4172/2155-6105.1000151. Manuscript submitted for publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Leeman RF, Fenton M, Kulesza M, Stewart DW, Taylor LA, Copeland AL. Impaired control mediates associations between impulsivity and alcohol-related problems. Poster presented at the satellite meeting of the International Society for Research on Impulsivity held at the annual meeting of the Research Society on Alcoholism; San Diego, CA. 2009. Jun, [Google Scholar]
  68. Leeman RF, Fenton M, Volpicelli JR. Impaired control and undergraduate problem drinking. Alcohol & Alcoholism. 2007;42:42–48. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agl095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Leeman RF, Heilig M, Cunningham CL, Stevens DN, Duka T, O’Malley SS. Ethanol consumption: How should we measure it? Achieving consilience between human and animal phenotypes. Addiction Biology. 2010;15:109–124. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2009.00192.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Leeman RF, Toll BA, Taylor LA, Volpicelli JR. Alcohol-induced disinhibition expectancies and impaired control as prospective predictors of problem drinking in undergraduates. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2009;23:553–563. doi: 10.1037/a0017129. Correction published in vol. 24, p. 435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Leeman RF, Toll BA, Volpicelli JR. The Drinking-Induced Disinhibition Scale (DIDS): A measure of three types of disinhibiting effects. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:1200–1219. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Levine HG. The discovery of addiction: Changing conceptions of habitual drunkenness in America. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1978;39:143–174. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1978.39.143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Littlefield AK, Sher KJ. Alcohol use disorders in young adulthood. In: Grant JE, Potenza MN, editors. Young adult mental health. New York: Oxford University Press; 2009. pp. 292–310. [Google Scholar]
  74. Littlefield AK, Sher KJ, Wood PK. Do changes in drinking motives mediate the relation between personality change and the “maturing out” of problem drinking? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010:93–105. doi: 10.1037/a0017512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Logan GD. On the ability to inhibit thought and action. A users' guide to the stop signal paradigm. In: Dagenbach D, Carr TH, editors. Inhibitory processes in attention, memory and language. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1994. pp. 189–236. [Google Scholar]
  76. Lyvers M. “Loss of control” in alcoholism and drug addiction: A neuroscientific interpretation. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2000;8:225–249. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.8.2.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Marczinski CA, Abroms BD, Van Selst M, Fillmore MT. Alcohol-induced impairment of behavioral control: Differential effects on engaging vs. disengaging responses. Psychopharmacology. 2005;182:452–459. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0116-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Marczinski CA, Fillmore MT. Alcohol increases reliance on cues that signal acts of control. Experimental & Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2005a;13:15–24. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.13.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Marczinski CA, Fillmore MT. Compensating for alcohol-induced impairment of control: effects on inhibition and activation of behavior. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005b;181:337–46. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2269-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Martens MP, Martin JL, Littlefield AK, Murphy JG, Cimini MD. Changes in protective behavioral strategies and alcohol use among college students. Drug and Alcohol Abuse. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.04.020. (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Martin CS, Kaczynski NA, Maisto SA, Bukstein OM, Moss HB. Patterns of DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence symptoms in adolescent drinkers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1995;56:672–680. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1995.56.672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. McKim WA. Drugs and behavior: An introduction to behavioral pharmacology (6th ed.) Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  83. Mezzich AC, Tarter RE, Feske U, Kirisci L, McNamee RL, Day B-S. Assessment of risk for substance use disorder consequent to consumption of illegal drugs: Psychometric validation of the neurobehavior disinhibition trait. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2007;21:508–513. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.4.508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Miller MA, Fillmore MT. Persistence of attentional bias toward alcohol-related stimuli in intoxicated social drinkers. Drug & Alcohol Dependence. 2011;117:184–189. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Modell JG, Mountz JM, Beresford TP. Basal ganglia/limbic striatal and thalamocortical involvement in craving and loss of control in alcoholism. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 1990;2:123–144. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2.2.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Modell JG, Mountz JM, Glaser FB, Lee JY. Effect of haloperidol on measures of craving and impaired control in alcoholic subjects. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1993;17:234–240. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1993.tb00755.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;158:1783–1793. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Morean ME, Corbin WR. Subjective response to alcohol: a critical review of the literature. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2010;34:385–395. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01103.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Mulvihill LE, Skilling TA, Vogel-Sprott M. Alcohol and the ability to inhibit behavior in men and women. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1997;58:600–605. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1997.58.600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Nagoshi CT. Perceived control of drinking and other predictors of alcohol use and problems in a college student sample. Addiction Research. 1999;7:291–306. [Google Scholar]
  91. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. NIAAA Newsletter: Winter; 2004. NIAAA council approves definition of binge drinking. http://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/Newsletter/winter2004/Newsletter_Number3.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  92. O'Brien CP, Volkow N, Li T-K. What’s in a word? Addiction versus dependence for DSM-V. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163:764–765. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.5.764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Oddy BW, Barry RJ. The relationship of N2 and P3 to inhibitory processing of social drinkers in a go/no-go task. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2009;72:323–330. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Oberlin BG, Grahame NJ. High-alcohol preferring mice are more impulsive than low-alcohol preferring mice as measured in the delay discounting task. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2009;33:1294–1303. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.00955.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Ostling EW, Fillmore MT. Tolerance to the impairing effects of alcohol on the inhibition and activation of behavior. Psychopharmacology. 2010;212:465–473. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1972-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Palfai TP, Ostafin BD. Action identification of drinking and self-control. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2010;24:145–150. doi: 10.1037/a0018249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Palmer RS, Corbin WR, Cronce JM. Protective strategies: A mediator of risk associated with age drinking onset. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35:486–491. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.12.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Patock-Peckham JA, Cheong J, Balhorn ME, Nagoshi CT. A social learning perspective: A model of parenting styles, self-regulation, perceived drinking control, and alcohol use and problems. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2001;25:1284–1292. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Patock-Peckham JA, Hutchinson GT, Cheong J, Nagoshi CT. Effect of religion and religiosity on alcohol use in a college student sample. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1998;49:81–88. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(97)00142-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Patock-Peckham JA, King KM, Backer-Fulghum LM, Morgan-Lopez AA, Leeman RF. Multidimensional impulsiveness and the gender specific mediational links between parenting styles, drinking control, and alcohol-related problems. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2011 Jun;35(Supplement 1):207A. [Google Scholar]
  101. Patock-Peckham JA, King KM, Morgan-Lopez AA, Ulloa EC, Filson Moses JM. The gender specific mediational links between parenting styles, parental monitoring, impulsiveness, drinking control, and alcohol-related problems. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2011;72:247–258. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2011.72.247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Patock-Peckham JA, Morgan-Lopez AA. College drinking behaviors: Mediational links between parenting styles, impulse control, and alcohol-related outcomes. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2006;20:117–125. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.20.2.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Patton JH, Stanford MS. Psychology of impulsivity. In: Grant JE, Potenza MN, editors. Oxford handbook of impulse control disorders. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012. pp. 262–275. [Google Scholar]
  104. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1995;51:768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Perry JL, Carroll ME. The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse. Psychopharmacology. 2008;200:1–26. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1173-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Perry JL, Larson EB, German JP, Madden GJ, Carroll ME. Impulsivity (delay discounting) as a predictor of acquisition of IV cocaine self-administration in female rats. Psychopharmacology. 2005;178:193–201. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1994-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Pickering C, Liljequist S. Cue-induced behavioral activation: A novel model of alcohol craving? Psychopharmacology. 2003:307–313. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1454-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Potenza MN, de Wit H. Control yourself: Alcohol and impulsivity. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research. 2010;34:1303–1305. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01214.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Potenza MN, Sofuoglu M, Carroll KM, Rounsaville BJ. Neuroscience of behavioral and pharmacological treatments for addictions. Neuron. 2011;69:695–712. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Poulos CX, Parker JL, Le DA. Increased impulsivity after injected alcohol predicts later alcohol consumption in rats: Evidence for “loss-of-control drinking” and marked individual differences. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1998;112:1247–1257. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.112.5.1247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Quinn PD, Fromme K. Subjective response to alcohol challenge: A quantitative review. Alcoholism Clinical & Experimental Research. 2011;35:1759–1770. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01521.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Read JP, Kahler CW, Strong DR, Colder CR. Development and preliminary validation of the young adult alcohol consequences questionnaire. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2006;67:169–177. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Read JP, Merrill JE, Kahler CW, Strong DR. Predicting functional outcomes among college drinkers: Reliability and predictive validity of the Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:2597–2610. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.06.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Read JP, Beattie M, Chamberlain R, Merrill JE. Beyond the “binge” threshold: Heavy drinking patterns and their association with alcohol involvement indices in college students. Addictive Behaviors. 2008;33:225–234. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Reynolds B, Ortengren A, Richards JB, de Wit H. Dimensions of impulsive behavior: Personality and behavioral measures. Personality and Individual Differences. 2006;40:305–315. [Google Scholar]
  116. Rodd ZA, Bell RL, Sable HJK, Murphy JM, McBride WJ. Recent advances in animal models of alcohol craving and relapse. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, & Behavior. 2004;79:439–450. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Rubio G, Jimenez M, Rodrigues-Jimenez R, Martinez I, Avila C, Ferre F, Palomo T. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2008:1681–1687. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00746.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Rutherford HJV, Mayes LC, Potenza MN. Neurobiology of Adolescent Substance Use Disorders: Implications for Prevention and Treatment. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2010;19:479–492. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2010.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Rutledge PC, Park A, Sher KJ. 21st birthday drinking: Extremely extreme. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2008;76:511–516. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.3.511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Schultz W. Potential vulnerabilities of neuronal reward, risk, and decision mechanisms to addictive drugs. Neuron. 2011;69:603–617. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Sher KJ, Trull TJ. Personality and disinhibitory psychopathology: Alcoholism and antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1994;103:92–102. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.103.1.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford M. Ecological momentary assessment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2008;4:1–32. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Sinha R. How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology. 2001:343–359. doi: 10.1007/s002130100917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Sugarman DE, Carey KB. The relationship between drinking control strategies and college student alcohol use. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2007;21:338–345. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.3.338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Verdejo-García A, Lawrence AJ, Clark L. Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008;32:777–810. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ. The addicted human brain: Insights from imaging studies. Journal of Clinical Investigation. 2003;111:1444–1451. doi: 10.1172/JCI18533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Walter H, Ramskogler K, Semler B, Lesch OM, Platz W. Dopamine and alcohol relapse: D1 and D2 antagonists increase relapse rates in animal studies and in clinical trials. Journal of Biomedical Science. 2001;8:83–88. doi: 10.1007/BF02255975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Weafer J, Fillmore MT. Individual differences in acute alcohol impairment of inhibitory control predict ad libitum alcohol consumption. Psychopharmacology. 2008;201:315–324. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1284-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Wechsler H, Moeyknes B, Davenport A, Castillo S, Hansen J. The adverse impact of heavy episodic drinkers on other college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1995;56:628–634. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1995.56.628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. White HR, Labouvie EW. Towards the assessment of adolescent problem drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1989;50:30–37. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1989.50.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Whiteside SP, Lynam DR. The five factor model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30:669–689. [Google Scholar]
  132. Wiers RW, Bartholow BD, van den Wildenberg E, Thush C, Engels RCME, Sher KJ, Stacy AW. Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: A review and a model. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2007;86:263–283. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.09.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. Fractionating impulsivity: Contrasting effects of central 5-HT depletion on different measures of impulsive behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1331–1343. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. World Health Organization (WHO) Staff. ICD-10 classifications of mental and behavioural disorder: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Albany, NY: World Health Organization; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  135. Yi H, Williams GD, Smothers BA. Trends in Alcohol-Related Fatal Traffic Crashes: United States, 1977–2002. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; 2004. Surveillance Report No. 69. [Google Scholar]
  136. Zuckerman M. Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimum level of arousal. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1994. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES