Abstract
A primary function of the brain is to store and retrieve information. Except for working memory, where extracellular recordings demonstrate persistent discharges during delay-response tasks, it has been difficult to link memories with changes in individual neurons or specific synaptic connections. Here, we demonstrate that transient stimuli are reliably encoded in the ongoing activity of brain tissue in vitro. We found that the patterns of synaptic input onto dentate hilar neurons predict which of four pathways were stimulated with an accuracy of 76% and performed significantly better than chance for >15 s. Dentate gyrus neurons also could accurately encode temporal sequences using population representations that were robust to variation in sequence interval. These results demonstrate direct neural encoding of temporal sequences in the spontaneous activity of brain tissue and suggest a novel local circuit mechanism that may contribute to diverse forms of short-term memory.
A fundamental property of the central nervous system is the ability to encode and retrieve information. In mammals, declarative memory function is typically divided into behavioral tasks that promote short- or long-term storage of items such as names, places and specific temporal sequences1,2. While the specific cellular origin of individual long-term declarative memories has remained elusive, a large literature highlights the importance of several critical brain regions, including the prefrontal cortex 3 and the hippocampal formation4 for encoding short-term, or working, memories. Extracellular unit recordings in these brain areas often display periods of persistent spiking activity at elevated frequencies when animals are required to retain transiently presented sensory information. This “delay-period activity” typically lasts for seconds and is extinguished when the animal initiates a behavioral response to indicate whether it correctly remembered the correct transient stimulus3,5. The reduction in persistent spiking during error trials—trials in which the animal made an incorrect behavioral response following the delay period—argues that delay-period activity in those specific neurons reflects activity in neural circuits encoding that short-term memory, rather than a response to the cue stimulus or a motor plan associated with the behavioral output. These classic studies in primates, along with more recent reports in rodents6–8, remain one of the clearest examples in which the spiking activity of individual neurons can be directly tied to specific memories.
While abundant evidence relates delay-period persistent spiking activity to short-term memories, the underlying cellular mechanisms that mediate this firing mode have not been determined. Donald Hebb postulated in 19499 that short-term memories could be maintained by reverberant activity that circulates through networks of interconnected excitatory neurons. Indeed, transient stimuli can trigger persistent spiking activity in artificial neural networks that contain recurrent excitatory connections10–13 although maintaining stable firing frequencies in individual circuit elements during the storage operation requires highly precise tuning of synaptic connection strengths10,14,15. Some types of CNS neurons also contain constellations of intrinsic conductance that enable them to maintain simple memories by firing persistently for several seconds following transient depolarizing stimuli even when pharmacologically isolated from all other neurons16,17. Computational models that combine individual simulated neurons with intrinsic persistence and recurrent excitatory connections are capable of generating persistent firing modes without the high connection weight tuning precision required to enable simple associative network models to fire persistently11,14,18,19. While biological experiments have demonstrated examples of both intrinsic persistence16,17,20 and recurrent excitatory connections21–23 in different brain regions, the absence of a tractable in vitro system capable of short-term information storage has limited the opportunities to determine which specific mechanisms are required to generate short-term mnemonic representations of transient stimuli.
Here, we demonstrate that four distinct patterns can be reliably encoded within the spontaneous synaptic activity in conventional rodent hippocampal slice preparations. Each pattern is evoked by briefly activating a different subset of entorhinal input axons (the perforant path, PP) using an array stimulation electrode; information is read out by recording intracellularly from two or three dentate hilar neurons, which sample the activity of both dentate granule cells24 and semilunar granule cells (SGCs; Fig. 1a), a recently-discovered excitatory cell type that responds to PP stimulation with graded depolarizing plateau potentials20,25,26. We find that synaptic barrages evoked in downstream hilar neurons by persistent firing in SGCs reliably encode both the identity of individual stimuli and temporal sequences of PP stimuli. Decoding both individual stimuli and temporal sequences relied on population representations of synaptic inputs to dentate hilar cells. Short-term encoding of sequences was robust to perturbation of sequence interval, suggesting that contextual coding in these experiments arises from stimulus order rather than the delay between stimuli. While several in vivo recording studies suggested that neurons in the hippocampal formation are involved in short-term memory function4,7, these results provide the first demonstration that neural networks within the dentate gyrus are capable of encoding both multiple transient inputs and context-dependent information such as the order of temporal sequences.
RESULTS
The dentate gyrus supports multiple distinct neural representations
Transient stimulation of the perforant path (2 × 200 μs shocks) leads to sustained increases in the frequency of spontaneous EPSPs recorded in dentate hilar neurons over multiple seconds (Fig. 1b), as described in a recent report20. That study found that the circuitry contained in a conventional hippocampal slice preparation could encode two distinct activity patterns. Here, we first asked whether more than two patterns could be encoded in the dentate gyrus and what factors govern coding accuracy.
Hilar cell responses to stimuli at different PP locations were recorded after placing an array electrode (4 contacts, 115 μm spacing) in the middle molecular layer of the dentate gyrus. Activation of each electrode contact (A, B, C, or D) individually typically elicited synaptic barrages (average EPSP frequency of spontaneous EPSPs in barrage = 18.7±0.5 Hz vs. 3.0±0.1 Hz immediately before PP stimulation; P<0.0001; n=33 cells). The 3.5 min interval between episodes was significantly longer than synaptic barrages triggered by PP stimuli (decay tau ~8 s,20) and we did not observe steady changes in baseline EPSP frequency during experiments. While responses evoked by different electrodes in this experiment had different mean EPSP frequencies, the range of EPSP frequencies in each response overlapped (Fig. 1c-d) and were only infrequently statistically separable (see below).
We next asked if the separation of responses evoked by stimuli at different locations could be increased by sampling the hilar network more densely using multiple simultaneous intracellular recordings (Fig. 2a, Supplemental Fig. 1). Using this approach, responses to each stimulus in an experiment could be plotted in the 3-dimensional space formed by the frequency of EPSPs recorded in each of three hilar neurons during the initial 4 s after the stimulus (Fig. 2b). In most experiments, including the example shown in Fig. 2b, responses to each stimulus type were clustered in different regions within the 3-dimensional plot of EPSP frequency in each hilar cell. By analyzing synaptic responses of hilar networks in EPSP frequency space, we can directly apply standard statistical and clustering methods along biologically-meaningful dimensions without requiring dimensionality reduction techniques such as principal component analysis. Since the presentation order was randomized, response clustering was unlikely to represent short-term plasticity associated with repeated stimulation at the same location.
While many experiments yielded visually distinct response clusters from each stimulus location, as in Fig. 2b, linear discriminant analysis (LDA) offers a rigorous method to determine whether specific pairs of responses to different stimulus locations (A/B, B/C, etc) were statistically separable. Statistical significance on 6 pairwise LDA tests are required to completely separate the responses to all four stimuli (P<0.0083 for each LDA test, reflecting a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons). This criterion was met in 50% of the experiments analyzed (6/12; EPSP frequencies assayed in initial 4 s of each response; Fig. 2c), indicating that the ability of the dentate gyrus to generate short-term representations of 4 transient inputs is robust. Responses to three stimuli were statistically separable in half of the remaining 6 experiments (3/12 experiments). On average, we obtained 4.8±0.5 out of the 6 separation planes required to separate four response patterns, compared with 0.83 planes expected by chance (P<0.001; n=12 experiments, 9 triple recordings and 3 paired recordings; see Online Methods for details). Our ability to statistically separate four patterns was greatest (5.6±0.7 significant planes) when we only considered the 9 experiments with simultaneous triple recordings. Eliminating data from one or two neurons in the 9 triple recording experiments substantially degraded the statistically separate responses to different stimuli (Fig. 2d); synaptic barrages recorded from one hilar neuron typically were only able to discriminate between two out of the four response patterns (1.7±1.4 statistically significant planes; Fig. 2d).
The ability of hilar networks to create mnemonic representations of transient stimuli was relatively independent of the window duration over which EPSP frequency was analyzed (Supplemental Fig. 2), with only a minor (13.8 %) reduction in the number of significant LDA planes obtained with 1 s analysis windows, compared with 4 s windows used in the initial analysis. We next conducted a separate set of triple recordings at relatively depolarized membrane potentials (~-60 mV) using two stimulus locations to ask if stimulus identity also was represented in the spike output of hilar cells. In each experiment in which responses to A and B stimuli were separable using LDA of EPSP frequency across the three cells (n=3; LDA p values ranged between 2.0 × 10−9 and 2.2 x 10−4), we found stimulus-specific suprathreshold responses based on spike frequency assayed in the same 4 s window (LDA p values ranged between 0.002 and 0.016; mean spike frequency 3.0 Hz following PP stimuli versus 0.06 Hz in control conditions). These results demonstrate that both the synaptic input to hilar cells and the resulting spike output can represent stimulus identity. We also found significantly more variation between responses from different stimulus locations than responses evoked by the same location using an omnibus measure of response separation (OVL, 12/12 experiments with p(OVL)<0.05; Fig. 2e and see Supplemental Fig. 3) that is used to assay overlapping distributions. Responses to the four stimuli were not separable based on LDA of baseline (pre-stimulus) EPSP frequencies (mean 1.0 plane; significantly different from the number of planes from stimulus responses; P<0.0001). The stimulus intensities used to evoke synaptic barrages were not significantly different on each contact (F=1.23; P>0.05; one-way ANOVA; n=180). These results demonstrate that transient stimuli activating 4 different sets of entorhinal inputs to the dentate gyrus resulted in at least 3 distinct patterns of hilar synaptic activity in a majority of experiments; all four responses were statistically separated in 50% of the experiments (Fig. 2c).
We next assayed the accuracy of recall based on comparing individual responses to the average response recorded at each stimulus location. We first tested the accuracy of response classification by predicting stimulus identity based on the nearest average response (centroid) computed from the first 4 s of each response (Fig. 3a). In this decoding method, if a response triggered by an A stimulus was closest to the average response to all A stimuli, the analysis would generate a correct A prediction. By contrast, if the response triggered by that stimulus was closest to the average response of all C trials in that experiment, then an incorrect prediction (that the response was triggered by C instead of the correct answer, A) would be recorded. Over all 12 experiments, this method predicted which one of the 4 possible stimulus locations was activated with an accuracy of 76.1% (n=180 trials with 137 correct and 43 incorrect predictions). This prediction accuracy is significantly greater than the 25% accuracy expected by chance (S.D. = 3.2%; P<0.0001; also significantly greater than the accuracy obtained following shuffling stimulus identities; P<0.0001; Fig. 3b). We did not observe a correlation between the physical distance between pairs of stimulating electrodes tested and the Euclidean distance between the resulting synaptic barrages (Fig. 3c). Neither was there a correlation between the inter-electrode distance and the accuracy differentiating hilar responses (e.g., A/B was differentiated as accurately as A/D; Fig. 3d). These results demonstrate that the dentate gyrus can generate distinct and repeatable population responses patterns even when nearby stimulation electrodes are activated, arguing that separation of network states in EPSP frequency space is not related to the physical separation between the stimulating electrodes used.
Origin of recall errors
We next focused on the 43 trials with classification errors to determine the factors that govern accuracy of short-term information storage in the dentate gyrus. In Fig. 3e, we plotted the distance between response pairs that were always correctly classified (black symbols) and those that were misidentified at least once in an experiment (red symbols). While the range of mean Euclidean distances between responses that were accurately predicted spanned 50 Hz, all classification errors arose from experiments where average responses between pairs of stimuli were separated by less than 30 Hz. The distance between average responses that were misidentified (10.2±1.0 Hz; n=43) was significantly less than the distance between responses that were always correctly classified (21.6±1.3 Hz; n=44; P<0.002). Half of all errors occurred when discriminating between two average responses separated by less than 7 Hz (Fig. 3f).
These results suggest that one factor governing mnemonic coding accuracy is the separation of responses to different stimuli in EPSP frequency space. While restricting recall tests to trials with well-separated average responses improved accuracy (not shown), there was a relatively poor correlation between the distance between pairs of average responses and the accuracy differentiating between those responses (R2 = 0.13; P>0.05; Fig. 3g). This model of variation in accuracy also did not predict the average separation between centroid pairs that were always classified correctly (black symbol). However, scaling the distance between responses by the pooled variance of each response improved the correlation with accuracy (P<0.0001; F=33.1; Fig. 3h). Moreover, this model, based solely on imperfectly classified trials, predicted the scaled distance between perfectly classified trials (black symbol). These results suggest that much of the variability in recall accuracy (62% of the variance) can be explained by a simple two-component model based on the Euclidean distance between average responses and the scatter around those average responses.
Time course of response separability
Synaptic barrages evoked by PP stimuli decayed with an average tau of ~ 8 s20 and decreased to 39.8% of the initial frequency after 18 s in the present study (n=12 cells; Fig. 4a). When displayed in 3-dimensional EPSP frequency space (Fig. 4b), average responses evoked by different stimulating electrodes followed distinct trajectories toward the origin and did not coalesce even though overall EPSP frequency decreased continuously in all responses. We next asked how long hilar responses evoked by different electrodes remain statistically separable and found that hilar networks could still represent information > 4 s after transient molecular layer stimulation, despite decreasing synaptic drive. The mean number of separable responses decreased from 3.25 to 2.25 over 18 s (black symbols in Fig. 4c). Three experiments maintained perfect response separation (4 separable responses, 6/6 significant planes; red plot in Fig. 4c) for 8 s. Overall classification accuracy decreased to 55.3% at 18 s after stimulation (n=12 experiments). The duration representations remained distinct was inversely correlated with initial pattern overlap (Fig. 4e; R2 = 0.62; F=15.4; P<0.005), suggesting that hilar networks reliably encode over time stimuli that create highly disparate responses. We also tested whether responses to different stimuli could be differentiated across different time windows (e.g., A at 5 s versus B at 8 s). The analysis presented in Supplemental Fig. 4 suggests that responses to different stimuli remain distinct across different times (no statistically significant difference between same-time and different time pairwise comparisons; P>0.7; K-S test).
The ability of the rodent dentate gyrus to encode four representations following transient molecular layer stimulation is shown graphically in Supplemental Fig. 5. Each possible stimulus location is represented by a character (A-D) whose contrast is a function of the distance to the average response (see Online Methods). A trial with a population response very close to the mean response to stimulus B would be represented by a dark “B” superimposed on dim “A”, “C”, and “D” characters. This method illustrates both the high accuracy of dentate representations of stimulus identity (only 1 of 14 consecutive trials was incorrect) and the persistence of the population coding in the dentate hilus. This visual analysis was cross-validated in in Fig. 5, where half the data set was used to compute mean response centroids and the other half used to generate the character display.
Dentate gyrus circuits encode temporal sequence order
Besides representing individual stimulus positions, hilar networks reliably encoded different temporal sequences. Distinct hilar up-states were evoked by activation of the same four stimuli in different order (ABCD vs. DCBA; Fig. 6a-b). Hilar synaptic responses to forward- and reverse-order sequences were clustered and reproducible (Fig. 6c; 5 s intervals) and could be statistically separated using the same LDA methods used to analyze responses to individual stimuli. Responses to forward- and reverse-order sequences were statistically separable in 10 of 11 hilar triple recording experiments (at least P<0.05; Fig. 6d). Sequence separability did not reflect simply the differences between the final responses in each sequence. In 8 of the 11 experiments, the sequence response was statistically different from the response to the final stimulus presented in isolation. Sequence identity was accurately predicted on the basis of hilar synaptic responses (classification accuracy = 95.6±1.9 %; significantly greater than expected by chance; P<0.0001; n=11 experiments). Synaptic barrages associated with sequential stimulation were blocked by the NMDA receptor antagonist MK801 (10 μM; Fig. 6e), as described previously for responses to single location stimuli 20. Blockade of NMDA receptors did not affect the basal frequency of AMPA receptor-mediated EPSPs recorded in hilar neurons (P > 0.05; Fig. 6e pre-sequence bars), suggesting that this treatment suppressed stimulus-evoked network up-states.
Hilar representations of temporal sequences did not arise from simple summation of invariant responses to individual stimuli. Rather, responses to each stimulus varied greatly depending on the temporal order within the sequence (Fig. 6f), suggesting that responses to specific stimuli reflected sequence history. While the magnitude of the change in the hilar response tended to decline with each successive stimulus in the train (20.2% mean reduction during each interval), the average magnitude change following the fourth stimulus was still significantly above the basal frequency of spontaneous EPSPs (3.3±0.5 Hz; P<0.0001; n=11 experiments). The history dependence of responses to stimulus sequences was evident when vector representations of responses were added head-to-tail (Fig. 6g) and also when responses to the same stimuli were plotted from the same origin (Supplemental Fig. 6). Over 11 triple recording experiments, hilar sequence responses significantly diverged following the second stimuli (Fig. 6h) and failed to converge at the end of the sequence. (Convergence would be expected if responses to sequential stimuli were commutative.) Increasing the inter-stimulus interval from 5 to 120 s eliminated most of the history dependence (Fig. 6g-h), suggesting a temporal limit to the ability of the dentate gyrus to encode sequential stimuli in vitro. We also found statistically-significant sequence-specific hilar responses when forward and reverse sequential stimuli were repeated at 8 s intervals (3 of out 3 experiments with LDA p values < 0.05; Supplemental Fig. 7) but only infrequently using 2 s intervals (1 of 3 experiments with P<0.05; Supplemental Fig. 8).
Sequence classification accuracy (forward or reverse) also was robust to small perturbations of inter-stimulus interval. Intracellular responses of hilar cells to shortened DCBA stimulus trains (4 s instead of the standard 5 s interval) more closely resembled responses to standard DCBA than to ABCD stimulus trains (Fig. 7a). Population responses to shortened (4 s) DCBA trains also overlapped with responses to 5 s DCBA stimuli in EPSP frequency space (Fig. 7b) and were closer to the centroid of the 5 s DCBA response than to the 5s ABCD response (Fig. 7c). In 5 experiments tested, the plane generated by LDA of sequences with 5 s inter-stimulus intervals accurately classified 4 s reverse sequences (85.4±9.0 % correct; purple bar in Fig. 7d; not significantly different than the overall accuracies classifying 5-s interval stimulus trains; green and orange bars in Fig. 7d; P>0.05). These results demonstrate that synaptic responses recorded in small groups of hilar neurons in vitro can encode both stimulus and sequence identities and are robust to variations in sequence interval.
Population representations of biological information in the dentate gyrus
Hilar representations of stimulus information could arise from a simple coding strategy based on the magnitude of the distinct EPSP frequencies across hilar cells. Alternatively, stimulus location and sequence information could be encoded by diverse population activity patterns reflecting differential responses in individual hilar neurons (Fig. 8a). We first asked if eliminating the information associated with differences in vector magnitude impaired information encoding. Responses to each of 4 stimulus locations remained well clustered following vector normalization (two examples shown in Fig. 8b). The wide distribution of vector directions over 9 triple recording experiments (Fig. 8c) suggests there was minimal systematic bias to generate clusters in one particular region. Eliminating response magnitude information only slightly reduced the number of significant LDA separation planes (from 4.8±0.5 to 3.9±0.7; P>0.05; n=12; Mann-Whitney; Fig. 8d) while eliminating vector direction information (retaining only vector magnitudes) significantly impaired response discrimination (1.4±0.4 planes; significantly less than both actual and normalized; P<0.01). Normalized population responses also tracked actual discrimination performance well over time (Fig. 8e) and remained significantly above chance performance over the time period analyzed. Hilar responses to sequential stimuli were similarly robust to normalization (10/11 experiments with separable forward and reverse responses following magnitude normalization; P<0.05 from LDA), implying that the dentate gyrus uses primarily distributed population codes to represent stimulus information in these experiments. Sequence identity was classified accurately in both control and normalized conditions over 6 s and prediction accuracy remained significantly better than expected by chance over the time window tested (Fig. 8f).
DISCUSSION
We find that dentate hilar neurons reliably encode information as distinct patterns of spontaneous synaptic activity that persist for seconds and that resemble the persistent activity patterns recorded in nonhuman primates3,4,27 and rodents7 during cross-modality working memory tasks. The same cellular mechanism that encodes isolated stimuli appears also to generate reliable contextual representations of stimuli presented within temporal sequences. Responses of dentate hilar neurons accurately predicted the identity of sequences of stimuli, were distinct from responses to the final stimulus presented in isolation in most experiments, and were robust to perturbation of sequence interval. Our results suggest that both sequential and non-sequential information were represented by population codes in the dentate gyrus.
Information representation in the dentate gyrus in vitro
While hilar synaptic barrages have been reported previously in response to PP stimulation20, this study is the first to demonstrate that changes in the ongoing synaptic input to dentate gyrus neurons can encode both the identity of more than two stimuli presented individually and in temporal sequences. Stimulus-evoked synaptic barrages reliably represented information that could be decoded using a standard classification method28 assaying the Euclidean distances between a response in a single trial and the four centroids that reflect average responses to the four stimuli. The classification accuracy we found using this unbiased decoding approach (~76% correct trials) was significantly greater than expected by chance; high classification accuracy was maintained when we constructed a naïve decoder based on centroids computed from half of the data set (Fig. 5). The success of this simple, distance-based decoder was unexpected given the small number of nearby hilar cells sampled and argues that the ability of the dentate gyrus to generate multiple distinct activity patterns is robust and is preserved in acute hippocampal slice preparations. While experimental limitations restricted these experiments to assaying closely-spaced groups of neurons, asking whether widely-separated populations of hilar cells also can represent information should be possible in future experiments using optical activity probes. The success of the decoding method employed in this study suggests that the separation between different population response patterns is typically large enough to overcome noise and any potential synaptic plasticity associated with repeated trials in the same experiment. Most decoding errors occurred in experiments where pairs of response centroids occupied similar locations within EPSP frequency space (Fig. 3e-f). A simple model that included both the separation between centroids and the dispersion of individual responses around each centroid28 predicted the minimal inter-centroid distance required for reliable (error-free) decoding (Fig. 3h).
In addition to assessing accuracy using a Euclidean distance-based decoder, we used two independent methods (overlap coefficient, OVL and linear discriminant analysis, LDA) to test whether hilar population responses evoked by different stimuli were distinct. These methods demonstrated that responses evoked by individual stimuli or temporal sequences of stimuli were statistically separable in most experiments. Our experiments took advantage of intracellular recordings to reveal the underlying synaptic events that excite hilar neurons, rather than inferring synaptic drive from extracellularly-recorded spiking activity. One tradeoff with this approach, however, is the limited time over which three simultaneous intracellular recordings could be maintained (typically 60-90 min). This limitation restricted our ability to gather the hundreds of repetitions typically required for rigorous information-theoretic analysis.
Previous work demonstrated that PP-evoked synaptic barrages are unlikely to result from recurrent excitatory connections. The majority of mossy cell axonal projections are to contralateral and distal ipsilateral granule cells29—projections that are largely absent in the transverse hippocampal slices employed in this study. The local projections of glutamateric hilar mossy cells are primarily to GABAergic interneurons and only infrequently (0.5% connection probability) form recurrent monosynaptic connections onto other nearby mossy cells30. Instead, hilar synaptic barrages appear to result from persistent spiking activity in semilunar granule cells (SGCs), a newly-discovered excitatory cell type in dentate molecular layer25,26. SGCs in the molecular layer respond to transient PP stimuli with depolarizing plateau potentials that require L- and T-type voltage-gated calcium channels20. Granule cells, by contrast, are typically inhibited by PP stimuli that trigger hilar synaptic barrages. Both granule cells and SGCs are polarized neurons whose axonal arbors are excluded from regions containing their own somatodendritic compartments25,26, indicating that neither cell type forms recurrent excitatory connections. Our ability to trigger statistically separable hilar population responses may result from recruitment of persistent spiking activity in different subsets of SGCs that project diffusely to hilar neurons. Alternatively, the distinct patterns we observe may result from stimulus-specific graded plateau depolarizations20 within a uniform subset of SGCs or from differential activation of inhibitory local circuits. Further experiments monitoring larger subsets of dentate gyrus neurons will be required to determine the extent of the SGC network excited by different stimuli in order to distinguish among these hypotheses.
Previous in vitro work in neocortical slices31 demonstrated that proportionally balanced excitation and inhibition enables self-sustaining activity mediated by recurrent excitatory connections, termed “up-states”. Cortical up-states occur spontaneously in acute brain slices bathed in solutions that enhance excitability and also can be triggered by synaptic stimulation31,32. While these “up-states” can form representations of a single stimulus (whether a stimulus was presented or not), these network states are not synapse specific31. By contrast, PP-evoked hilar synaptic barrages are highly synapse specific and are capable of representing at least four distinct patterns. Cortical networks also exhibit spontaneous neuronal avalanches that persist for 10s of ms33, a much faster time scale than that of the persistent activity reported here.
In addition to representing specific stimulus locations, we find that hilar synaptic barrages accurately represented temporal sequences of stimuli. Rather than simply trigger stereotyped, stimulus-specific activity patterns, stimulus sequences typically triggered new activity patterns that were distinct from those evoked by the final stimulus in the train (ABCD was different from D presented alone; Supplemental Fig. 8). This context dependence of stimulus representation, where the response to a specific stimulus was dependent on prior stimuli, also was apparent when synaptic inputs during sequence trains were analyzed (Fig. 6e). Surprisingly, classification accuracy for decoding temporal sequences was robust to a perturbation in sequence interval (Fig. 7d), suggesting that context dependence arises primarily from the relative order of the stimuli. Semilunar granule cells often receive inhibitory postsynaptic responses following hilar and molecular layer stimulation25 that could provide a cellular substrate for context dependence.
While sequence memory often involves the hippocampal formation1 and has been demonstrated in computational models of cortical networks34–37, reliable short-term storage of temporal information has only been infrequently demonstrated at the cellular level. While Branco et al.38 recently used focal glutamate uncaging at multiple sites along one dendrite to generate sequence-dependent intracellular responses, this mechanism presumably operates primarily through rapid cell-autonomous mechanisms. By contrast, the dentate gyrus circuits activated in the present study reliably encoded temporal sequences with substantially longer time intervals and could be decoded accurately only by comparing activity across multiple simultaneously-recorded hilar cells. Multiple groups have identified “state-dependent” network responses in vitro39 and in vivo40 that reflect the history of previously presented stimuli. While the contextual responses we report in the dentate gyrus in vitro likely reflect state-dependent processes, our results differ from previous reports in several respects. We find that hilar networks represent information over longer time periods (many seconds) compared with previous state-dependent studies39,40. Hilar networks can encode the order of temporal sequences, not only immediately following the sequence, but also for several seconds afterward. Finally, our results demonstrate rapid encoding of information while previous in vitro work required many repeated presentations of stimuli39, implying a gradual entrainment or learning process.
Relationship to delay-period activity recorded during working memory tasks
Much of our understanding of the cellular basis of short-term information storage is based on classic extracellular recording studies that demonstrated persistent spiking of neocortical3,6,8,27 and hippocampal4,7 units during the delay period of short-term memory tasks. These studies showed that the particular units recruited during the delay period depended on the stimulus type (e.g., spatial location) and that delay-period activity was typically absent or reduced in trials when the preferred stimulus was presented and the subject made an incorrect behavioral response3,27. Extracellular recording studies have demonstrated elevated firing frequencies for as long as 20 s during working memory tasks5,41, arguing that cortical networks must contain mechanisms that support persistent activity over this time scale. However, neurons with a diversity of persistent firing durations typically contribute to delay-period activity during working memory tasks and only a fraction are typically active during the entire delay phase. The subset of delay-period neurons with prolonged persistent activity may play a critical role as “summators” that integrate activity from neurons that are active over different time scales6,8. While it is difficult to determine the kinetics of the mechanisms underlying persistent activity during working memory tasks since the learned motor output typically extinguishes persistent firing3, our experiments demonstrate that the circuitry contained in transverse hippocampal slices, including SGCs with intrinsic persistence, are sufficient to represent information over these long behavioral time scales. Multiple studies8,42 have demonstrated that compact representations based on population decoding approaches can accurately predict behavioral responses in working memory tasks. Similarly, preserving the heterogeneity in hilar cell responses while ignoring differences in the overall amplitude of responses (i.e., normalization) maintained accurate decoding in our experiments. By contrast, average rate-based coding schemes that ignored the differential contribution of each neuron to the population response failed to classify accurately either location or sequence stimuli.
One likely function of dentate gyrus neurons in vivo hypothesized by Marr43 and subsequent researchers 44 is to transform highly overlapping entorhinal input patterns into more distinct output patterns. While this potential functional role has only recently begun to be tested experimentally45,46, our demonstration that multiple, reproducible population patterns can be generated in dentate gyrus neurons is consistent with the hypothesis that the neural networks in this brain region can stably represent multiple population patterns. Semilunar granule cells may play an important role during pattern decorrelation by generating a “latch” function that enables hilar neurons to maintain their activity following the transient presentation of a particular entorhinal input pattern. Future in vivo studies will be required to follow the downstream consequences of hilar persistent activity since most of the axonal projections of mossy cells extend to distant ipsilateral and contralateral granule cells29,47, which are not present in transverse hippocampal slices.
The largely feedforward local circuits that appear to support short-term storage of information in the dentate gyrus suggest a fundamentally different mechanism than the recurrent excitatory networks commonly employed in artificial neural network models of working memory10–13. Despite extensive efforts to identify biological persistent activity modes mediated by recurrent excitatory synaptic connections48, very little experimental evidence supports the classic hypothesis that reverberant network activity functions to encode and maintain information proposed by Hebb9. Our finding that sequential stimulation did not reset hilar persistent activity to a new state corresponding to the most recent stimulus is also inconsistent with the predictions from theoretical studies employing attractor-based networks with recurrent connectivity49. At the other extreme, multiple groups have demonstrated neuron cell types that can fire persistently following transient stimuli through cell-autonomous intrinsic mechanisms16,17 and several theoretical studies have demonstrated that combining intrinsic persistence with recurrent excitatory connections relaxes the requirements for precise tuning of synaptic weights11,14,18,19. The alternative intrinsic/feedforward mechanism we hypothesize enables hilar neurons to represent stimulus and sequence identity is related to a recent theoretical study37 arguing that purely feedforward circuits can generate prolonged network activity when the effective network time constant is extended by creating multiple serial processing stages. Presumably, the number of stages required to generate delay-period activity would be reduced if some stages of the feedforward network employed neurons with intrinsic persistence, such as SGCs.
The dentate gyrus is unusual in the high frequency of spontaneous synaptic activity recorded in hilar neurons and because of semilunar granule cells, a subtype of excitatory projection with multistability under standard physiological conditions. Populations of hilar neurons presumably can encode stimulus and sequence identity in our experiments because at least some divergent SGC-to-hilar neuron synaptic connections are preserved in conventional horizontal brain slices. Revealing persistent activity modes in the dentate gyrus was facilitated by the anatomical separation of multistable excitatory neurons (SGCs) and downstream hilar neurons. However, other examples of bistability and multistability of excitatory projection neurons in more hetereogenous brain regions have been reported following bath application of modulatory neurotransmitters, raising the possibility that the hybrid intrinsic/feedforward mechanism we propose in the dentate gyrus may be generalizable. For example, a subclass of neurons in entorhinal cortex are capable of persistent firing following transient stimulation in the presence of muscarinic receptor agonists16. To date, no group has reported whether activation of different synaptic inputs to entorhinal cortex leads to mnemonic encoding. Transient inputs also can trigger cell-autonomous persistent firing in neocortical neurons when intrinsic excitability is enhanced following cholinergic receptor activation50, providing a potential connection between intrinsic/feedforward persistent mechanisms we propose in the dentate gyrus and classic delay-period activity recorded in prefrontal cortical neurons in vivo.
METHODS
Methods and associated references are available in the online version of the paper.
ONLINE METHODS
Animals
Horizontal slices (300 μm thick) of the ventral hippocampus were prepared from P14-25 Sprague-Dawley rats anesthetized with ketamine, as described previously 20,30. Slices were incubated at 30 °C for 30 min and then maintained at room temperature until needed. All experiments were carried out under guidelines approved by the Case Western Reserve University Animal Care and Use Committee.
Electrophysiology
All intracellular recordings were performed in a submerged recording chamber maintained at 30°C and perfused with an extracellular solution containing (in mM): 124 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1.23 NaH2PO4, 1.2 MgSO4, 26 NaHCO3, 10 dextrose, 2.5 CaCl2, equilibrated with 95% O2/5% CO2 (pH 7.3). Whole-cell patch clamp recordings were made using AxoPatch 1D amplifiers (Molecular Devices) and borosilicate glass pipettes (3-10 MΩ). Recording electrodes contained (in mM): 140 K methylsulfate, 4 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 0.2 EGTA, 4 MgATP, 0.3 Na3GTP, 10 phosphocreatine, adjusted to pH 7.3 and ~ 290 mOsm. Individual neurons were visualized under IR-DIC video microscopy (Zeiss Axioskop FS1) prior to patch-clamp recording. We typically restricted our experiments to a set of two or three nearby hilar cells with inter-soma distances less than 100 μm. Intracellular recordings were low-pass filtered at 2 kHz (FLA-01, Cygnus Technology) and acquired at 5 kHz using a simultaneously-sampling 16-bit data acquisition system (ITC-18, Instrutech) operated by custom software written in VB.NET (Microsoft) and Matlab (Mathworks). Intracellular voltages were not corrected for the liquid junction potential. A matrix microelectrode consisting of 4 sharpened tungsten monopolar electrodes (115 μm spacing; FHC) was used for extracellular stimulation. Stimulus intensity was controlled by a custom-built constant-voltage stimulus isolation unit. Hilar barrages were evoked most reliably by paired stimuli (500 ms inter-stimulus intervals), as described previously 20. All population responses reported in this study reflect paired stimuli at this interval; responses to sequential stimuli (e.g., ABCD) reflect paired shocks at each matrix electrode contact at the indicated interval (paired shocks every 4-120 s). Hilar recordings and population responses were typically stable over 10-15 paired stimuli or temporal sequences of different stimuli (3.5 min delay between different stimuli in both experiment types, except for 120 s sequential stimuli experiments). We collected the data set of experiments using different stimulus locations in one continuous series after an initial set of pilot studies to optimize stimulus and recording conditions. Only experiments in which hilar cell recordings degraded or in which we obtained fewer than 3 responses from each stimulus type were excluded from the analysis.
Data analysis and statistics
Hilar cells were identified by their intrinsic properties (mean spike time, spike clustering, and spike AHP) following 4 s-duration depolarizing and hyperpolarizing current steps, as described previously 30. Most recordings in this study were from presumptive hilar mossy cells. Spontaneous EPSPs were identified automatically using a custom detection algorithm 30. Except where noted, all data are presented as mean ± SEM. Statistical significance was determined using Student’s t-test, unless otherwise specified.
Analysis of hilar population responses
EPSPs were typically detected in a 4-s window that began 0.5 s after the last shock of each paired stimulus. The mean EPSP frequency within this window in each recorded hilar cell was used to analyze population responses. We approached the question of whether population responses evoked by different stimulus electrodes were distinct using three complementary methods. First, we assessed the separability of population responses by calculating the overlap coefficient (OVL 51,52; Supplemental Fig. 3). In this method, we computed the distribution of Euclidean distances from individual population responses (the mean EPSP frequencies in the same 4 s analysis window applied to each simultaneously-recorded hilar cell) with the centroid calculated from all responses evoked by the stimulus electrode in the same experiment (“Cis” distances). The distribution of these Cis distances was then compared with the distribution of “Trans” distances calculated from dissimilar data point/centroid pairs (e.g., A points to the centroid of all B points). The overlap coefficient OVL was computed from Gaussian fits of Cis and Trans distributions. The statistical significance of the OVL coefficient was determined by bootstrap methods (20,000 randomizations of Cis/Trans labels on each actual distance). All location experiments analyzed in this study yielded statistically distinct Cis/Trans distributions (all OVL calculated reflected P<0.05; Fig. 2e).
Second, we used linear discriminant analysis (LDA 53–55) to determine the number of statistically separable population responses in each experiment. Complete classification of population responses to all 4 stimulus locations (A-D) required 6 statistically significant pairwise LDA tests (A/B, B/C, etc), each with P<0.0083 reflecting a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Plots of the number of discrimination planes in different conditions (Fig. 2d, Fig. 8d-e, Supplemental Fig. 2) reflect the number of pairwise LDA tests with P < 0.05. We estimated the number of planes expected by chance by randomly re-assigning EPSP frequencies in each hilar cell to different stimulus identities. We recalculated the number of statistically significant LDA planes following 30 iterations to generate an estimate of the number of planes expected by chance. A statistically significant OVL coefficient combined with 6/6 possible LDA tests (with P < 0.0083) represents a conservative test for response separability, a criterion that was met in 50% of the experiments we analyzed. If one experiment failed to generate 4 separable responses, it was re-analyzed to determine if three (3 appropriate planes with P<0.016) or two (one plane with P<0.05) responses could be isolated. We calculated the time course of response separability using a sliding 4-s window and repeated the LDA at 1 s intervals. Times indicated reflect the beginning of each 4-s time window. We also used LDA based on the mean EPSP frequency in 4-s windows that began 0.5 s after the last stimulus in each sequence (e.g., 0.5 s after the D in ABCD and after the A in DCBA) to determine if population responses to different temporal sequences were significantly different. We employed the same time windows when analyzing sequences with both 4- and 5-s intervals.
Finally, we assessed the accuracy of hilar population responses in predicting stimulus identity, assayed by nearest stimulus centroid. Classification accuracy was calculated for the entire data set from the ratio of trials classified correctly to the total number of trials tested. Sequence identity was predicted from the sign of the response distance to the LDA plane.
Character intensity plots (Fig. 5 and Supplemental Fig. 5) reflect the relative Euclidean distances between each population response and the four stimulus centroids, recomputed at 1 s intervals. Each location reflects the superposition of four characters (A,B,C,D) whose intensities are scaled to reflect their relative distance to stimulus centroids. The scaling function was normalized by the sum of the scaled distances to all four centroids:
where d represents the Euclidean distance between a single response and a response centroid and x is the stimulus identity being tested. This transformation resulted in one very dark (high contrast) character superimposed on three light (low contrast) characters if a response was very close to a stimulus centroid. We then iterated through all four possible centroids at each time step:
where p is character intensity, i represents stimulus identity, j represents the time bin, k represents the trial, and FC represents the centroid vector for one stimulus identity at one time bin. The relative distance between forward and reverse responses in Fig. 7c was calculated as the signed length of the projection of the population responses onto the difference vector:
where δ represents the difference vector of the mean population responses for forward and reverse sequences and F is the population response vector. Data analysis was performed in Matlab (Mathworks) and Origin (OriginLab).
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank S. Solla for helpful discussions and R. Galan and P. Larimer for comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by NIH grants NS33590 and DC04285. R.A.H. was supported by NIH training grant T32-GM007250.
Footnotes
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS R.A.H. and B.W.S. designed the experiments, analyzed the data, prepared the figures and wrote the paper. R.A.H. performed all the experiments.
COMPETING FINANCIAL INTERESTS The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- 1.Fortin NJ, Agster KL, Eichenbaum HB. Critical role of the hippocampus in memory for sequences of events. Nat. Neurosci. 2002;5:458–462. doi: 10.1038/nn834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Averbeck BB, Lee D. Prefrontal neural correlates of memory for sequences. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:2204–2211. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4483-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Funahashi S, Bruce CJ, Goldman-Rakic PS. Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey’s dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 1989;61:331–349. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.61.2.331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Colombo M, Gross CG. Responses of inferior temporal cortex and hippocampal neurons during delayed matching to sample in monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) Behav. Neurosci. 1994;108:443–455. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.108.3.443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Fuster JM, Jervey JP. Inferotemporal neurons distinguish and retain behaviorally relevant features of visual stimuli. Science. 1981;212:952–955. doi: 10.1126/science.7233192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Batuev AS, Kursina NP, Shutov AP. Unit activity of the medial wall of the frontal cortex during delayed performance in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 1990;41:95–102. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(90)90145-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hampson RE, Heyser CJ, Deadwyler SA. Hippocampal cell firing correlates of delayed-match-to-sample performance in the rat. Behav. Neurosci. 1993;107:715–739. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.107.5.715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Baeg EH, et al. Dynamics of population code for working memory in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2003;40:177–188. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00597-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hebb DO. The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. John Wiley & Sons; New York: 1949. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Seung HS. How the brain keeps the eyes still. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 1996;93:13339–13344. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.23.13339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Camperi M, Wang XJ. A model of visuospatial working memory in prefrontal cortex: recurrent network and cellular bistability. J Comput Neurosci. 1998;5:383–405. doi: 10.1023/a:1008837311948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Compte A, Brunel N, Goldman-Rakic PS, Wang XJ. Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a cortical network model. Cereb. Cortex. 2000;10:910–923. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.9.910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Miller P, Brody CD, Romo R, Wang X-J. A recurrent network model of somatosensory parametric working memory in the prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2003;13:1208–1218. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Seung HS, Lee DD, Reis BY, Tank DW. Stability of the memory of eye position in a recurrent network of conductance-based model neurons. Neuron. 2000;26:259–271. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81155-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mensh BD, Aksay E, Lee DD, Seung HS, Tank DW. Spontaneous eye movements in goldfish: oculomotor integrator performance, plasticity, and dependence on visual feedback. Vision Res. 2004;44:711–726. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Egorov AV, Hamam BN, Fransén E, Hasselmo ME, Alonso AA. Graded persistent activity in entorhinal cortex neurons. Nature. 2002;420:173–178. doi: 10.1038/nature01171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Pressler RT, Strowbridge BW. Blanes cells mediate persistent feedforward inhibition onto granule cells in the olfactory bulb. Neuron. 2006;49:889–904. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.02.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Koulakov AA, Raghavachari S, Kepecs A, Lisman JE. Model for a robust neural integrator. Nat. Neurosci. 2002;5:775–782. doi: 10.1038/nn893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Goldman MS, Levine JH, Major G, Tank DW, Seung HS. Robust persistent neural activity in a model integrator with multiple hysteretic dendrites per neuron. Cereb. Cortex. 2003;13:1185–1195. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Larimer P, Strowbridge BW. Representing information in cell assemblies: persistent activity mediated by semilunar granule cells. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:213–222. doi: 10.1038/nn.2458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.MacVicar BA, Dudek FE. Local synaptic circuits in rat hippocampus: interactions between pyramidal cells. Brain Res. 1980;184:220–223. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(80)90602-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Miles R, Wong RK. Excitatory synaptic interactions between CA3 neurones in the guinea-pig hippocampus. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 1986;373:397–418. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp016055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Song S, Sjöström PJ, Reigl M, Nelson S, Chklovskii DB. Highly nonrandom features of synaptic connectivity in local cortical circuits. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:e68. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Acsády L, Kamondi A, Sík A, Freund T, Buzsáki G. GABAergic cells are the major postsynaptic targets of mossy fibers in the rat hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:3386–3403. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-09-03386.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Williams PA, Larimer P, Gao Y, Strowbridge BW. Semilunar granule cells: glutamatergic neurons in the rat dentate gyrus with axon collaterals in the inner molecular layer. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:13756–13761. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4053-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gupta A, Elgammal FS, Proddutur A, Shah S, Santhakumar V. Decrease in tonic inhibition contributes to increase in dentate semilunar granule cell excitability after brain injury. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:2523–2537. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4141-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Funahashi S, Chafee MV, Goldman-Rakic PS. Prefrontal neuronal activity in rhesus monkeys performing a delayed anti-saccade task. Nature. 1993;365:753–756. doi: 10.1038/365753a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Crowe DA, Averbeck BB, Chafee MV. Rapid sequences of population activity patterns dynamically encode task-critical spatial information in parietal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:11640–11653. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0954-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Buckmaster PS, Wenzel HJ, Kunkel DD, Schwartzkroin PA. Axon arbors and synaptic connections of hippocampal mossy cells in the rat in vivo. J. Comp. Neurol. 1996;366:271–292. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19960304)366:2<270::aid-cne7>3.0.co;2-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Larimer P, Strowbridge BW. Nonrandom local circuits in the dentate gyrus. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:12212–12223. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3612-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Shu Y, Hasenstaub A, McCormick DA. Turning on and off recurrent balanced cortical activity. Nature. 2003;423:288–293. doi: 10.1038/nature01616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.MacLean JN, Watson BO, Aaron GB, Yuste R. Internal dynamics determine the cortical response to thalamic stimulation. Neuron. 2005;48:811–823. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Beggs JM, Plenz D. Neuronal avalanches in neocortical circuits. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:11167–11177. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-35-11167.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Abbott LF, Blum KI. Functional significance of long-term potentiation for sequence learning and prediction. Cereb. Cortex. 1996;6:406–416. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.3.406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Diesmann M, Gewaltig MO, Aertsen A. Stable propagation of synchronous spiking in cortical neural networks. Nature. 1999;402:529–533. doi: 10.1038/990101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Mongillo G, Barak O, Tsodyks M. Synaptic theory of working memory. Science. 2008;319:1543–1546. doi: 10.1126/science.1150769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Goldman MS. Memory without feedback in a neural network. Neuron. 2009;61:621–634. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Branco T, Clark BA, Häusser M. Dendritic discrimination of temporal input sequences in cortical neurons. Science. 2010;329:1671–1675. doi: 10.1126/science.1189664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Johnson HA, Goel A, Buonomano DV. Neural dynamics of in vitro cortical networks reflects experienced temporal patterns. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:917–919. doi: 10.1038/nn.2579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Nikolić D, Häusler S, Singer W, Maass W. Distributed fading memory for stimulus properties in the primary visual cortex. PLoS Biol. 2009;7:e1000260. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Miyashita Y, Chang HS. Neuronal correlate of pictorial short-term memory in the primate temporal cortex. Nature. 1988;331:68–70. doi: 10.1038/331068a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Barak O, Tsodyks M, Romo R. Neuronal population coding of parametric working memory. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:9424–9430. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1875-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Marr D. Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 1971;262:23–81. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Rolls ET, Treves A. Neural networks in the brain involved in memory and recall. Prog. Brain Res. 1994;102:335–341. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60550-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S, Moser M-B, Moser EI. Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science. 2007;315:961–966. doi: 10.1126/science.1135801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.McHugh TJ, et al. Dentate gyrus NMDA receptors mediate rapid pattern separation in the hippocampal network. Science. 2007;317:94–99. doi: 10.1126/science.1140263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Deadwyler SA, West JR, Cotman CW, Lynch GS. A neurophysiological analysis of commissural projections to dentate gyrus of the rat. J. Neurophysiol. 1975;38:167–184. doi: 10.1152/jn.1975.38.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Aksay E, et al. Functional dissection of circuitry in a neural integrator. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:494–504. doi: 10.1038/nn1877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Brunel N, Wang XJ. Effects of neuromodulation in a cortical network model of object working memory dominated by recurrent inhibition. J Comput Neurosci. 2001;11:63–85. doi: 10.1023/a:1011204814320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Krnjević K, Phillis JW. Pharmacological properties of acetylcholine-sensitive cells in the cerebral cortex. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 1963;166:328–350. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1963.sp007107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Inman HF, Bradley EL. The overlapping coefficient as a measure of agreement between probability distributions and point estimation of the overlap of two normal densities. Communications in Statistics - Theory and Methods. 1989;18:3851–3874. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Clemons TE, Bradley EL. A nonparametric measure of the overlapping coefficient. Computational Statistics & Data Analysis. 2000;34:51–61. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Fisher RA. The use of multiple measurements in taxonomic problems. Annals Human Genetics. 1936;7:179–188. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Klecka W. Discriminant Analysis. Sage Publications; Newbury Park, CA: 1980. [Google Scholar]
- 55.MacLeod K, Bäcker A, Laurent G. Who reads temporal information contained across synchronized and oscillatory spike trains? Nature. 1998;395:693–698. doi: 10.1038/27201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.