Abstract
CONTEXT
High unintended pregnancy rates, and inconsistencies between reported pregnancy intentions and contraceptive behaviors, have been well documented among young U.S. women. Women’s beliefs about the benefits of childbearing and motherhood may be related to the apparent disconnect between pregnancy intentions and reproductive outcomes.
METHODS
Perceived benefits of childbearing and feelings about a potential pregnancy were assessed among 1,377 women aged 15–24 (most of them black or Latina) participating in a longitudinal study in 2005–2008. The women, who were initiating hormonal contraception at public family planning clinics and did not want to become pregnant for one year, were followed for 12 months. Differences in perceived benefits of childbearing by participant characteristics were examined with linear regression, using a new multi-item measure. Cox proportional hazard regression was used to investigate the association of perceived benefits of childbearing with subsequent contraceptive discontinuation and pregnancy.
RESULTS
Perceptions of the benefits of childbearing decreased with increasing age (coefficient, −0.04), and white women perceived fewer benefits to childbearing than blacks (−0.2) and Latinas (p ≤ .01). As women’s perception of the benefits of childbearing increased, their one-year pregnancy rates increased, after demographic characteristics and feelings about a potential pregnancy were controlled for (hazard ratio, 1.2). Benefits of childbearing were not associated with contraceptive discontinuation.
CONCLUSIONS
To better assess pregnancy risk among young women wanting to avoid pregnancy, it may be useful to acknowledge that they hold not only explicit pregnancy desires, but also beliefs about the benefits of childbearing, which may influence sexual behavior and pregnancy.
Despite the availability of a range of contraceptive options in the United States, unintended pregnancy rates remain high, particularly among young women. According to the 2006–2008 National Survey of Family Growth (NSFG), 82% of pregnancies among adolescents and 64% among women aged 20–24 are unintended.1 However, young women’s pregnancy intentions are often inconsistent with their contraceptive behaviors.2–4 In a 2008–2009 representative survey of unmarried 18–29-year-olds in the United States, half of sexually active young women who were not planning to become pregnant either were not using contraceptives or were using a method inconsistently.5 In the 2006–2008 NSFG, 19% of adolescents and 14% of women aged 20–24 who were sexually active and did not want to become pregnant were not using a method.6 Unintended pregnancy can have significant consequences for both mother and child, and is indicative of unprotected sex and risk for STDs.7
While an extensive literature has identified barriers to contraceptive use that may explain these apparent inconsistencies,8,9 other research has focused on the idea that for some young women, ambivalence about pregnancy or positive views of motherhood reduce motivation to use contraceptives.2,3,10,11 Qualitative studies have revealed that although women are aware of the drawbacks of childbearing, they also perceive advantages. In a study conducted among black women, participants spoke about the benefits of having a baby, including that it would provide them with someone to love and an opportunity to assert responsibility; foster connections with boyfriends, friends and family; and restore their self-confidence.12 Disadvantaged adolescents in other studies have said that teenage pregnancy would give them a purpose in life and allow them to become mothers while they were young and energetic.13,14 These attitudes may influence contraceptive use and pregnancy, even among young women who do not explicitly want to become pregnant. To fully understand why some women, particularly nulliparous young women, who do not desire pregnancy still do not use contraceptives effectively, it may be necessary to capture more subtle feelings, such as their perceptions of benefits of motherhood.
Scales measuring certain aspects of attitudes toward childbearing or motherhood have been proposed; however, none has been subjected to rigorous psychometric assessment, and most have focused on specific attitudinal dimensions, such as romanticized beliefs about pregnancy and parenthood,15 traditional versus liberal views of women’s roles,16,17 postpartum women’s identity as mothers18 and how parenting affects an individual’s life.14,19–21 Most scales were developed among predominantly white, educated or nonadolescent populations of women, and are not appropriate for use among diverse populations of young women at highest risk for unintended pregnancy. Two scales, to our knowledge, have focused on high-risk youths’ perceptions of the consequences of parenthood: the Positive Orientation Toward Early Motherhood scale, developed among black adolescents;19 and the Perceived Consequences of Teenage Childbearing scale, developed among high school students, most of whom were Latina.14 While these scales provide an important starting point, neither has undergone rigorous psychometric analysis, and neither has been assessed in relationship to subsequent reproductive health outcomes, such as contraceptive or sexual behavior or pregnancy.
For this study, we developed and validated a measure, the Benefits of Childbearing (BOC) scale, to assess perceived benefits to childbearing among a cohort of low-income urban females aged 15–24 who were seeking hormonal contraception. The study examines associations between young women’s perceptions of benefits of childbearing and their contraceptive use and pregnancy experience over the next year. We hypothesized that women’s beliefs about the benefits of childbearing would be positively associated with their rates of contraceptive discontinuation and pregnancy, even after their stated feelings about a potential pregnancy were controlled for.
METHODS
Conceptual Model
Most approaches to understanding pregnancy are grounded in a planned behavior framework:22 Women are viewed as formulating pregnancy intentions and as acting, to the extent possible, according to their intentions. Some researchers, however, have questioned whether this model fully captures pregnancy-related decision making.23 A young woman’s attitudes toward childbearing and pregnancy can be complex, encompassing a range of contradictory emotions; some young women who do not explicitly want to become pregnant still believe that a pregnancy would make them happy.2 In this study, we take into account that attitudes, which are shaped by social, family and gender norms,12,24 may influence sexual and contraceptive behavior, even when individuals may not be consciously aware of them.25,26
Study Design
Data were drawn from 1,377 adolescents and young women who were initiating hormonal contraception and participating in a longitudinal study of contraceptive use and pregnancy.27 The study was conducted in 2005–2008 at four Planned Parenthood clinics in the San Francisco Bay Area. The clinics serve a racially diverse population of women who generally are low-income and have public or no medical insurance. Potential participants were referred to research assistants after their visit with a clinic provider, following standard clinic protocol, in which they elected to initiate a hormonal contraceptive method (the vaginal ring, patch, pill or injectable) for the first time. Women were eligible if they were 15–24 years old and unmarried, were English or Spanish speakers, were not pregnant and did not want to become pregnant in the next year. Because screening occurred after women had chosen to adopt a hormonal method, the cohort likely excluded women who wanted to participate in the study but who did not truly want to avoid pregnancy. Participants provided written informed consent, and study protocols were approved by the Committee on Human Research, University of California, San Francisco.
Participants completed self-administered questionnaires at baseline and at three, six and 12 months after baseline. The questionnaire was administered, in English or Spanish, via laptop computer (to reduce social desirability bias associated with interviewer-administered questionnaires). Ninety-four percent of participants completed the baseline interview at a clinic site, 3% completed the interview at a location more convenient for the participant and 2% were read the computer questionnaire over the phone by a research assistant. The baseline questionnaire included items on social and demographic characteristics, contraceptive use, pregnancy history, feelings about a potential pregnancy and childbearing attitudes. The follow-up questionnaires measured contraceptive discontinuation and pregnancies. Instruments were pilot-tested to ensure that they were at an appropriate reading level and understood by respondents. Urine pregnancy tests were performed at baseline and at the six- and 12-month visits. Participants received $20–30 for completing each study visit.
Measures
Perceived benefits of childbearing were measured using the nine-item BOC scale. (See appendix for details on scale development.) Items were based on prior scales14,19 and formative qualitative research.28 Respondents were asked the extent to which they agreed that having a baby would give them someone to love; would make them feel important; would help them keep the baby’s father around; would help them get money from the baby’s father; could get them out of a bad situation; would make them a woman; would make other people think they are important; would strengthen their relationship with the baby’s father; and would mean that someone will love them. Responses were rated on a five-point scale (0=“strongly disagree,” 1=”disagree,” 2=”neither agree nor disagree,” 3=”agree” and 4=”strongly agree”). Higher scores corresponded to more favorable beliefs about the benefits of childbearing. The scale had high internal consistency (separation reliability coefficient, 0.82) and validity. Scores were standardized for analyses.
Because some young women may have positive attitudes toward the prospect of pregnancy, even when they do not desire pregnancy,2,29 we assessed anticipated feelings about a hypothetical pregnancy. We used the following question on the baseline survey: “How would you feel if you got pregnant in the next three months (very upset, somewhat upset, I wouldn’t care, somewhat pleased, very pleased, don’t know)?” This question was placed near the end of the survey to minimize the degree to which participants would feel pressure to provide a negative response, having already stated that they did not desire pregnancy. For analyses, “don’t know” responses were categorized with “I wouldn’t care”; although these responses reflect different attitudes, they represent similar degrees of ambivalence.
The social and demographic variables assessed were age, race or ethnicity (black, Latina, white, Asian/Pacific Islander, mixed/other), mother’s education (high school or less, more than high school), whether the respondent had a prior pregnancy, whether the respondent had any children, and school/employment status (in school or employed, neither in school nor employed). Participants who indicated they were of mixed heritage were asked with which racial or ethnic group they identified most and were categorized accordingly, because attitudes toward motherhood and childbearing are likely influenced by social and cultural norms that may be specific to different racial and ethnic groups.12,30
Baseline measures also included the contraceptive method selected, interview mode and recruitment clinic.
The outcome variables for the prospective analyses were contraceptive discontinuation and pregnancy. Discontinuation of use of the hormonal method started at enrollment was measured by self-report at follow-up interviews. Incident pregnancies were captured by self-report, urine pregnancy test and clinic chart review. Each outcome variable was examined both as a simple dichotomous measure and as a “time to event” measure. We estimated the time to discontinuation or pregnancy by using the midpoint between the date of the interview at which it was reported and the date of the preceding interview.31 For example, if a participant was still using her contraceptive method at the three-month interview and reported discontinuation at her six-month interview, we estimated her time to discontinuation at 4.5 months.
To capture inconsistent method use and switching to other effective methods, we created an ordinal variable that categorized women as having continued their baseline method with no breaks in use; continued with breaks; switched to another effective method, with no breaks; switched to another effective method, with breaks; or discontinued and did not initiate use of another effective method. (If a woman did not report on her consistency of use, we considered her to have breaks in use.)
Analyses
Descriptive analyses explored participant characteristics, feelings about potentially becoming pregnant and BOC scale scores. The degree to which BOC scores correlated with women’s stated feelings about a potential pregnancy was assessed using Spearman’s rho32; differences in mean score by feelings about a potential pregnancy were assessed in a linear regression model.*
We examined mean BOC scores by respondents’ social and demographic characteristics and baseline contraceptive method. Bivariate and multivariate linear regression models, which controlled for interview mode and clinic site, were used to assess differences in mean scores between groups. Postestimation F tests were used to conduct pairwise comparisons by race or ethnicity and by contraceptive method.
We assessed the proportion of participants discontinuing their new method, and the proportion becoming pregnant, at each follow-up interview and over a year. We also calculated the overall discontinuation and pregnancy rates among all participants over a year.27 Then, we used bivariate and multivariate Cox proportional hazard models to examine differences in these rates by perceived benefits of childbearing and feelings about a potential pregnancy. Multivariate models included the BOC and feelings about potential pregnancy variables, as well as social and demographic characteristics, clinic site, baseline contraceptive method and interview mode. Postestimation F tests were used to assess significance of differences between each pair of coefficients for categorical variables. Hazard analyses included the 1,309 participants who completed at least one follow-up visit. A participant contributed time to the analysis until she discontinued her hormonal contraceptive method or became pregnant, was lost to follow-up or exited the study at one year.
Because the analysis of contraceptive discontinuation did not capture women who used their methods inconsistently or switched to other effective methods, we also conducted ordinal logistic regression with the contraceptive use pattern variable as the outcome. To assess how accurately this variable might have captured women’s actual use, we examined occurrence of pregnancy across contraceptive use patterns. We also used a Cox proportional hazard model to examine differences in pregnancy rates between the ordered categories of contraceptive use. All analyses were conducted in Stata, version 12.
RESULTS
On average, participants were 19.2 years old (standard deviation, 2.5). Two-thirds were adolescents, and one-third were aged 20–24 (Table 1). Overall, 41% identified as black, 29% as Latina, 13% as white, 12% as Asian or Pacific Islander, and 6% as mixed or other. Half of participants had ever been pregnant, and one in five had children. Forty-four percent reported that they would be very upset if they became pregnant. However, more than half of participants expressed some degree of ambivalence, saying they would be somewhat upset (25%), would not care or did not know (18%) or would be somewhat pleased (10%) about a potential pregnancy. Four percent said they would be very pleased.
TABLE 1.
Characteristic | % (N=1,377) |
---|---|
Age | |
15–17 | 35.4 |
18–19 | 31.8 |
20–24 | 32.8 |
Race/ethnicity | |
Black | 40.8 |
Latina | 29.0 |
White | 12.7 |
Asian/Pacific Islander | 11.9 |
Mixed/other | 5.6 |
Mother’s education | |
≤high school | 60.6 |
>high school | 39.4 |
Prior pregnancy | |
No | 51.2 |
Yes | 48.8 |
Has any children | |
No | 80.2 |
Yes | 19.8 |
School/employment status | |
Neither in school nor employed | 17.4 |
In school or employed | 82.6 |
Baseline contraceptive method | |
Pill | 31.1 |
Patch | 28.8 |
Ring | 18.7 |
Injectable | 21.4 |
Feeling if became pregnant | |
Very upset | 43.6 |
Somewhat upset | 24.8 |
Wouldn’t care/don’t know | 18.3 |
Somewhat pleased | 9.8 |
Very pleased | 3.5 |
Total | 100.0 |
Participants’ raw BOC scores covered the full range of the scale (0–36), with a unimodal, right-skewed distribution and a cluster of individuals at zero. Standardized scores ranged from −3.1 to 4.3 and had a mean of zero. Participants most frequently agreed (either strongly or somewhat) that having a baby would give them someone to love (44%) and would mean somebody will love them (34%). They least often perceived that having a baby would help them get money from the father (5%) and would make other people think they are important (5%).
When mean scores across the levels of feelings about a potential pregnancy were compared, scores were higher for those who would be somewhat upset, would not care or did not know, or would be somewhat pleased or very pleased by a pregnancy than for those who would be very upset if they were to become pregnant (coefficients, 0.5, 0.4, 0.6 and 0.7, respectively; p≤.001 for each). Still, BOC scores varied widely among women within each level of the pregnancy feelings variable, and these two measures were moderately correlated (rho, 0.23).
In an adjusted model, BOC scores varied by all participant characteristics except one: Scores were similar between women with and without children (Table 2). Our adjusted model indicated that the older a woman was, the fewer benefits of childbearing she perceived (coefficient, −0.04). Compared with black women, Asians and Pacific Islanders reported more benefits (0.3), and white women reported fewer (−0.2); Latinas reported levels of benefits similar to black women. Asians and Pacific Islanders, Latinas and black women all reported more benefits than white women. Participants whose mothers had more than a high school education reported fewer benefits than those whose mothers had less education (−0.1). Women who were in school or employed perceived more benefits than those who were neither in school nor employed (0.2). Scores were generally similar by contraceptive method adopted, except that women selecting the injectable perceived more benefits to childbearing than women starting the pill (0.2); women selecting the injectable also perceived more benefits than those starting the ring (p≤.01). Perceptions of benefits did not differ by recruitment site or interview mode (not shown).
TABLE 2.
Characteristic | Mean score | Coefficient |
---|---|---|
Age | na | −0.04 (−0.07 to −0.02)*** |
Race/ethnicity | ||
Black | −0.01 | ref |
Latina | 0.05 | 0.04 (−0.10–0.17)† |
White | −0.23 | −0.20 (−0.38 to −0.03)* |
Asian/Pacific Islander | 0.21 | 0.28 (0.10–0.47)**,‡ |
Mixed/other | −0.01 | 0.02 (−0.22–0.25) |
Mother’s education | ||
≤high school | 0.07 | ref |
>high school | −0.12 | −0.14 (−0.25 to −0.03)* |
Has any children | ||
No | 0.00 | ref |
Yes | 0.01 | 0.03 (−0.11–0.18) |
School/employment status | ||
Neither in school nor employed | −0.03 | ref |
In school or employed | 0.13 | 0.21 (0.06–0.35)** |
Baseline contraceptive method | ||
Pill | −0.05 | ref |
Patch | 0.06 | 0.12 (−0.02–0.26) |
Ring | 0.13 | −0.04 (−0.19–0.12) |
Injectable | −0.16 | 0.20 (0.05–0.34)**,§ |
p≤.05.
p≤.01.
p≤.001.
Differs from white and Asian/Pacific Islander at p≤.01.
Differs from white at p≤.001.
Differs from ring at p≤.01.
Notes: Scores were standardized for regression analyses (mean, 0; standard deviation, 1; range, −3.1–4.3). The model controls for recruitment clinic and interview mode; scores did not differ by these variables. na=not applicable. ref=reference group.
Over the course of the study, 75% of women who completed at least one follow-up visit discontinued their baseline hormonal contraceptive method, including 41% who discontinued within the first three months and 62% within six months. The discontinuation rate was 82 women per 100 person-years.27 In an unadjusted proportional hazards model, as BOC score increased, the rate of contraceptive discontinuation increased (hazard ratio, 1.1; p≤.01). Discontinuation rates generally did not vary by feelings about a potential pregnancy, but women who would be very pleased if they became pregnant had a higher discontinuation rate than those who would be very upset (1.5; p≤.05).
In the model adjusting for social and demographic variables, the association between BOC score and contraceptive discontinuation was only marginally significant (p=.07), and anticipating feeling very pleased about a potential pregnancy was no longer significant (Table 3). The contraceptive discontinuation rate decreased by age, and white women experienced a lower discontinuation rate than both black women and women of mixed or other races. Women who chose the patch, the ring or the injectable at baseline discontinued use at higher rates than women who selected the pill; patch users discontinued at higher rates than women using the ring or injectable. When we repeated analyses using women’s pattern of contraceptive use as the outcome (not shown), results were generally unchanged; however, women who would be very pleased about a pregnancy had less consistent patterns of use than those who would be very upset (odds ratio, 0.5; p≤.05).
TABLE 3.
Characteristic | Contraceptive discontinuation | Pregnancy |
---|---|---|
Perceived benefits of childbearing | 1.07 (0.99–1.14) | 1.17 (1.02–1.34)* |
Feeling if became pregnant | ||
Very upset (ref) | 1.00 | 1.00 |
Somewhat upset | 1.00 (0.85–1.18) | 1.66 (1.23–2.22)***,§ |
Wouldn’t care/don’t know | 1.01 (0.85–1.22) | 1.43 (1.02–2.00)* |
Somewhat pleased | 0.93 (0.73–1.18) | 1.02 (0.63–1.64) |
Very pleased | 1.19 (0.84–1.69) | 2.09 (1.21–3.62)** |
Age | 0.96 (0.93–0.99)** | 0.98 (0.93–1.03) |
Race/ethnicity | ||
Black (ref) | 1.00 | 1.00 |
Latina | 0.92 (0.78–1.08) | 0.85 (0.63–1.13) |
White | 0.73 (0.59–0.92)**,† | 0.67 (0.42–1.07) |
Asian/Pacific Islander | 0.87 (0.69–1.09) | 0.35 (0.19–0.63)***,†† |
Mixed/other | 1.05 (0.79–1.38) | 1.02 (0.61–1.70) |
Mother’s education >high school | 1.12 (0.98–1.28) | 1.19 (0.93–1.53) |
Has any children | 1.13 (0.95–1.34) | 1.60 (1.19–2.14)** |
In school or employed | 1.15 (0.96–1.38) | 1.16 (0.85–1.57) |
Baseline contraceptive method | ||
Pill (ref) | 1.00 | 1.00 |
Patch | 1.97 (1.66–2.34)***,‡ | 1.74 (1.27–2.39)***,‡‡ |
Ring | 1.24 (1.02–1.52)* | 2.00 (1.40–2.84)***,‡‡ |
Injectable | 1.29 (1.08–1.55)** | 0.82 (0.55–1.22) |
p≤.05.
p≤.01.
p≤.001.
Differs from mixed/other at p≤.05.
Differs from ring and injectable at p≤.001.
Differs from somewhat pleased at p≤.05.
Differs from Latina and mixed/other at p≤.01.
Differs from injectable at p≤.001.
Notes: Tests of significance are two-tailed. The models control for recruitment clinic and interview mode.
Overall, 22% of participants who completed a follow-up visit became pregnant during the study period, including 4% who became pregnant by three months and 11% by six months. The pregnancy rate was 23 per 100 person-years.27 In an unadjusted model, as BOC score increased, so did the one-year pregnancy rate (hazard ratio, 1.2; p≤.01). The pregnancy rate was higher for women who would feel very pleased by a pregnancy (2.6), who would not care or did not know (1.7), or who would feel somewhat upset (1.8) than for those who stated they would feel very upset by a pregnancy (p≤.001 for each comparison); rates did not differ among these three groups.
The BOC was still associated with pregnancy after feelings about becoming pregnant and social and demographic variables were controlled for (hazard ratio, 1.2—Table 3). Compared with the rates for those who would be very upset by a pregnancy, pregnancy rates were higher among those who would feel very pleased (2.1), those would not care or did not know (1.4), and, unexpectedly, those who would feel somewhat upset (1.7). Women who would be somewhat upset by a pregnancy had a higher pregnancy rate than women who would be somewhat pleased.
Asians and Pacific Islanders experienced a lower pregnancy rate than black women, Latinas, and women of mixed or other races. Women who already had children had a higher pregnancy rate than those without children. Finally, women using the patch or the ring experienced higher pregnancy rates than both pill and injectable users.
Contraceptive use pattern was strongly associated with pregnancy hazard (p≤.001—not shown). One-year risk of pregnancy declined as consistency of contraceptive use increased. Thirty-five percent of women who discontinued their baseline method and did not adopt another effective method became pregnant over a year, as did 25% of those who switched to another effective method, with breaks; 13% of those who switched to another method, with no breaks; 11% of those who continued the baseline method with breaks; and 6% of those who continued the baseline method with no breaks.
DISCUSSION
In this cohort of adolescent and young women adopting a hormonal contraceptive method, Benefits of Childbearing scale scores were positively associated with pregnancy, independent of women’s verbalized feelings about their potentially becoming pregnant. Young women who perceive benefits to having a child may engage in behaviors that put them at risk for pregnancy, even if they state that they do not want to become pregnant and they seek hormonal contraceptives. This finding lends support to viewing pregnancy-related behaviors from more than a strict planned behavior perspective.2,23 To better assess risk of pregnancy among young women initiating hormonal contraception, it may be useful for providers to examine women’s perceptions about childbearing, in addition to their stated pregnancy intentions.
It is intriguing that perceived benefits of childbearing and feelings about a potential pregnancy were associated with subsequent pregnancy, but they were not associated with contraceptive discontinuation in adjusted models. For women initiating a new contraceptive method, the primary risk factors for contraceptive discontinuation and actual pregnancy may be different. Even when they do not feel favorably about the prospect of pregnancy, women may discontinue methods more for reasons related to the methods themselves (e.g., side effects or beliefs about safety) than because of favorable attitudes about pregnancy or childbearing. Indeed, only 2% of women in this cohort who stopped using their selected method stated that they did so because they desired pregnancy; most cited side effects or access and cost barriers as their main reason.27 It is also possible that our measures of contraceptive use may not have captured the complexity of inconsistent use and method switching; however, the strong negative association we observed between consistency of contraceptive use and pregnancy risk supports the validity of our contraceptive use pattern measure.
Although perceived benefits of childbearing were positively correlated with feelings about a potential pregnancy, BOC scores varied widely among women with similar feelings about pregnancy. For instance, even among those stating they would be very upset about a pregnancy, some agreed with all of the BOC items. Perceived benefits of childbearing and feelings about a potential pregnancy are likely related but distinct constructs—one representing theoretical beliefs about the benefits of having a baby, and the other reflecting hypothetical feelings about becoming pregnant. Interestingly, although the women in this study were initiating hormonal contraceptive use, more than one in six would not care or did not know how they would feel if they became pregnant. Our results add to existing evidence that young women’s feelings about having a baby can be complex, encompassing a range of potentially ambivalent attitudes.2,12
BOC scores were elevated among nonwhites, women whose mothers had relatively little education and adolescents—subgroups who are at elevated risk of unintended pregnancy and abortion in the United States.1 That Latinas and black women perceived more benefits to childbearing than white women is consistent with findings of racial and ethnic differences in young women’s attitudes toward childbearing33 and pregnancy.34–36 Research is needed to examine the roles that differences in attitudes about childbearing and pregnancy among racial, ethnic and socioeconomic groups might play in disparities in adolescent and unintended pregnancy.
We were somewhat surprised to find that perceptions of the benefits of childbearing declined as age increased, even though they did not differ between women with and without children. The only quantitative study we are aware of that has examined perceived advantages and disadvantages of childbearing by age included only teenagers, and it found no significant differences by age.14 A qualitative study among adolescents seeking prenatal care in Rhode Island found that those aged 17 and under tended to be more likely to view pregnancy as a way to enhance connections with others, while those aged 18–19 focused more on practical benefits of teenage motherhood versus having children later.13 Research has fairly consistently found age to be positively associated with favorable attitudes toward the prospect of pregnancy.34–36 Perhaps younger women, with less life experience, hold more idealistic views than their older counterparts of the benefits of childbearing, even if they do not want to become pregnant. Given that the proportion of pregnancies that are unintended is highest among adolescents,1 examination of perceived benefits of childbearing might be particularly salient in this group.
BOC scores in this study were the same for women with and without children. However, for women at the same overall BOC score level, those with children were less likely to agree that having a baby would strengthen the relationship with the father than those without children (see appendix). Experience bearing and raising children may indicate that having a baby does not necessarily make women’s relationships with their partners stronger. A more in-depth assessment of how benefits of childbearing items function differentially between women with and without children could be useful for identifying potentially unrealistic expectations about the benefits of having a child.
Limitations and Strengths
Several methodological factors limited our analyses. The initial list of BOC items included only two regarding potential drawbacks of childbearing. In another study, participants cited disadvantages and advantages of early childbearing,13 and contraceptive behavior and pregnancy risk may be shaped as much by disincentives to become pregnant as by perceived benefits.37 In addition, because no prospective measures of pregnancy intention or attitudes have been developed,2 we were limited to using an individual item to assess feelings about potentially becoming pregnant. The women in our cohort both did not desire pregnancy and were initiating hormonal contraceptive use. Our results may not be generalizable to women who are not presenting to clinics or initiating a new hormonal method, or to women outside the San Francisco Bay Area. Finally, some participants may have changed their attitudes toward becoming pregnant over time.
In spite of these limitations, our study has several strengths. The BOC scale was based on extensive qualitative research and analyzed using rigorous psychometric methods (see appendix). Although studies among U.S. teenagers have examined the relationships between pregnancy intentions and subsequent sexual behavior or pregnancy (with mixed results),2,3,29,36 we are not aware of other work that has prospectively assessed the predictive ability of attitudes about childbearing. Studies that have investigated the correlation between perceived benefits of childbearing and reproductive outcomes have been cross-sectional, examining attitudes about childbearing after sex14 or pregnancy19 has occurred; participants’ feelings about the benefits of childbearing likely were influenced by these events. By assessing perceived benefits of childbearing and feelings about a potential pregnancy prospectively, we have established that the attitudes existed prior to pregnancy and were not influenced by the pregnancy itself. Our study is also unique in that it assessed the independent associations between perceived benefits of childbearing and our outcomes.
Conclusion
Our findings highlight the complexity of helping young women prevent pregnancy. Efforts to increase contraceptive adoption and continuation may be thwarted if we fail to acknowledge that young women hold not only explicit pregnancy desires, but also beliefs about the benefits of having a child, which may influence sexual behavior and pregnancy risk. Future research might investigate the performance of a tool like the BOC in clinical settings to determine if it might inform interventions that can help young women achieve their goals of delayed or prevented pregnancy.
Benefits of Childbearing scale items.
Final scale items Having a baby would give me someone to love. Having a baby would make me feel important. Having a baby would help me keep the baby’s father around. Having a baby would help me get money from the baby’s father. Having a baby could get me out of a bad situation. Having a baby would make me a woman. Having a baby would make other people think I am important. Having a baby would make my relationship with the baby’s father stronger. Having a baby means somebody will love me. Items considered but removed Being a mother is special. A baby is a blessing. A baby is a lot of work. Babies take a lot of time and cost a lot. Having a baby would help me get money from the government. If I get pregnant and I love the guy, I would have his baby. |
Note: Participants rated each item on a Likert scale with response options of “strongly agree,” “agree,” “neither disagree nor agree,” “disagree” and “strongly disagree.”
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by grant R01 HD045480 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. The authors thank Signy Judd for her skillful leadership of focus groups; Abby Sokoloff and Beth Brown for their dedication and effort in directing the longitudinal study; Maggie Chen for her input regarding attitudes toward childbearing; and Anne Foster-Rosales and the staff of the Planned Parenthood Shasta Pacific clinics who contributed to study implementation activities. Results from this analysis were presented at the annual meeting of the American Public Health Association, San Francisco, Oct. 27–31, 2012.
APPENDIX
Our aim was to develop a measure of childbearing attitudes that is reliable, valid and appropriate for use among diverse populations of young U.S. women at high risk of unintended pregnancy. We implemented a three-stage design: item development, initial item selection and final measure performance assessment. Analyses included the 1,377 cohort members who completed the BOC items. Analyses were conducted using ACER ConQuest, version 2.0, and were consistent with guidelines for psychometric testing of a new instrument.38
Item Development
We used methods based on item response theory to evaluate the scale. Item response theory uses logistic random intercept models to determine the properties of scale items, assess scale performance and place individuals along a continuum of the latent variable.39–41 The idea behind this theory is that individuals respond to items on the basis of their attitude level. For example, the more benefits of childbearing a woman perceives, the higher her probability of agreeing with an item naming a potential benefit of childbearing. Item response theory offers advantages over traditional scale evaluation methods that are based on classical test theory.41–43 For instance, it allows for variation in the distances between response categories for an item (e.g., the difference between “agree” and “disagree” can be greater than the difference between “strongly agree” and “agree”) and between items (a partial credit model).44 Differential item functioning between groups of individuals can be examined to identify potentially biased items.
Benefits of childbearing items were developed on the basis of formative qualitative research; 16 focus groups were conducted with 113 women aged 15–26 of multiple races.28 We also included items from the Positive Orientation Toward Early Motherhood scale19 and the Perceived Consequences of Teenage Childbearing scale14 if the attitudes they assessed were prevalent in the focus groups. We developed 15 Likert-scaled items for potential use in the BOC (see box); total scores ranged from 0 (least favorable beliefs about the benefits of childbearing) to 36 (most favorable).
Initial Item Selection
We followed several steps to select from the 15 original items those that would remain in the scale. We fitted items to a unidimensional partial credit item response model44 and assessed their fit; a weighted mean square statistic of less than 1.33 was considered an acceptable fit. To examine whether items might better be treated multidimensionally, we also fitted a multidimensional item response model, dividing items by whether they reflected emotional or practical benefits. The fit was not substantially improved, and we used the more parsimonious unidimensional model.
The scale’s internal consistency was assessed with the separation reliability coefficient, which is analogous to Cronbach’s alpha. For initial item selection, we ranked items by fit and incrementally added items to the model, starting with the best fitting ones, until reliability no longer increased.
To assess internal structure validity, we plotted women’s BOC scores on a scale next to “item-threshold levels,” representing the level of perceived benefits of childbearing a woman would need to have a 50% chance of selecting a response category falling on either side of the threshold (e.g., “strongly disagree” vs. any other response). We examined the plot to be sure that items captured the range of participants’ attitudes. We ensured that women endorsing each increasing response option on each item had increasing scores on the scale. Finally, we plotted the frequency with which each item’s response categories were selected along the range of overall BOC scores (item characteristic curves). Items with response categories that were the most common along large ranges of the—scale or those that were never the most common were—removed.
All items fit the item response model and exhibited good internal structure validity: Women endorsing each increasing response option on each item had increasing scores on the BOC scale. However, six items were removed from the final scale. “Being a mother is special” and “a baby is a blessing” were removed because few participants disagreed; thus, most thresholds fell below participant levels. “Babies take a lot of time and cost a lot” and “a baby is a lot of work” were removed because the large majority of respondents agreed. When we added these four items, as well as “having a baby would help me get money from the government” and “if…I love the guy, I would have his baby,” to the scale, the reliability of the scale was reduced. We thus removed the items.
Final Measure Performance Assessment
We established the final scale’s psychometric properties by fitting the nine final items to a new item response model and assessing item fit, reliability and internal structure validity using the same steps outlined above. In addition, we assessed differential item functioning, which indicates that an item in a scale performs differently among groups of women who otherwise score similarly on the scale. The presence of differential item functioning can indicate that an item is biased, but it can also provide insight into group differences on an item. To assess differential item functioning, we introduced interaction terms between items and each social and demographic variable to nested models with a random intercept for the social or demographic subgroup alone. We determined, a priori, that a 0.33 logit difference in item-by-trait coefficients between groups represented a meaningful difference and warranted removal of the item from the final scale.45,46
Participants’ raw BOC scores covered the full range of the scale (0–36), with a unimodal, right-skewed distribution and a cluster of individuals at zero. On the scale generated by item response theory, scores ranged from −5 to 5 (mean, −0.9). Items and their response categories covered the range of participants’ childbearing attitudes, indicating that the items were appropriate for the population being studied. All nine items fit the model.
The separation reliability of the final scale was high (0.82). Most criteria for internal structure validity were met. Participants selecting increasing response options on each item had increasing scores on the BOC scale. For instance, women responding “strongly disagree” to each item had lower average overall BOC scores than those responding “disagree.” In general, each possible response option was the most common response among women at the appropriate range of the BOC scale, i.e. “strongly disagree” was the most frequent response among women with the lowest BOC scores and “strongly agree” was most common among those with the highest BOC scores. However, for four items, “strongly agree” and “agree” were not the most endorsed responses for women at the corresponding range of the scale, largely because few women agreed with these statements. These items reflected the belief that having a baby would help the woman keep the baby’s father around, would help her get money from him, could get her out of a bad situation and would make other people think she is important.
We detected no differential item functioning for any items by age-group, race or ethnicity, or maternal education. Items performed nondifferentially between women with and without children, with one exception: Women with children were less likely than women without children who had the same overall BOC score to agree that having a baby would strengthen the relationship with the father. We speculate that this finding is due to these women’s relationship experiences after having a child rather than to a meaningful difference in how women conceptualize childbearing and motherhood.
Because we conducted our psychometric analysis of the BOC within a longitudinal study designed to answer the primary research questions, we were unable to assess short-term test-retest reliability, and we used the same study population for initial item selection11 and the final psychometric analysis. Analyses should be repeated in other populations to verify our results. However, because the BOC was developed in a racially diverse population of adolescents and women aged 20–24, the scale may be better suited to capture childbearing attitudes in nonadolescent and racially diverse populations than are scales that were developed among adolescents only and focused on one racial or ethnic group.14,19
Footnotes
FN A
Analyses assessing group differences in BOC scores by feelings about a potential pregnancy and demographic characteristics were also conducted by fitting explanatory item response models including terms for each group variable. Wald tests were used to determine the significance of group differences (source: De Boeck P and Wilson M, eds., Explanatory Item Response Models: A Generalized Linear and Nonlinear Approach, New York: Springer-Verlag, 2004). Results were unchanged from those presented.
Contributor Information
Corinne H. Rocca, Bixby Center for Global Reproductive Health, Department of Obstetrics, Gynecology and Reproductive Sciences, School of Medicine, University of California, San Francisco.
Cynthia C. Harper, Bixby Center for Global Reproductive Health, Department of Obstetrics, Gynecology and Reproductive Sciences, School of Medicine, University of California, San Francisco.
Tina R. Raine-Bennett, Women’s Health Research Institute, Division of Research, Kaiser Permanente Northern California, Oakland.
References
- 1.Finer LB, Zolna MR. Unintended pregnancy in the United States: incidence and disparities, 2006. Contraception. 2011;84(5):478–485. doi: 10.1016/j.contraception.2011.07.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Rocca CH, et al. Predictive ability and stability of adolescents’ pregnancy intentions in a predominantly Latino community. Studies in Family Planning. 2010;41(3):179–192. doi: 10.1111/j.1728-4465.2010.00242.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bartz D, et al. Pregnancy intentions and contraceptive behaviors among adolescent women: a coital event level analysis. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2007;41(3):271–276. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Trussell J, Vaughan B, Stanford J. Are all contraceptive failures unintended pregnancies? Evidence from the 1995 National Survey of Family Growth. Family Planning Perspectives. 1999;31(5):246–247. 260. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kaye K, Suellentrop K, Sloup C. The Fog Zone: How Misperceptions, Magical Thinking, and Ambivalence Put Young Adults at Risk for Unplanned Pregnancy. Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent Teen and Unplanned Pregnancy; 2009. [accessed July 20, 2010.]. < http://www.thenationalcampaign.org/fogzone/>. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mosher WD, Jones J. Use of contraception in the United States: 1982–2008. Vital and Health Statistics. 2010;23(29) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gipson JD, Koenig MA, Hindin MJ. The effects of unintended pregnancy on infant, child, and parental health: a review of the literature. Studies in Family Planning. 2008;39(1):18–38. doi: 10.1111/j.1728-4465.2008.00148.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gilliam ML, et al. Concerns about contraceptive side effects among young Latinas: a focus-group approach. Contraception. 2004;70(4):299–305. doi: 10.1016/j.contraception.2004.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Dennis A, Grossman D. Barriers to contraception and interest in over-the-counter access among low-income women: a qualitative study. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health. 2012;44(2):84–91. doi: 10.1363/4408412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Petersen R, et al. How contraceptive use patterns differ by pregnancy intention: implications for counseling. Women’s Health Issues. 2001;11(5):427–435. doi: 10.1016/s1049-3867(01)00090-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Sheeder J, et al. Adolescent childbearing ambivalence: Is it the sum of its parts? Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent Gynecology. 2010;23(2):86–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jpag.2009.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kendall C, et al. Understanding pregnancy in a population of inner-city women in New Orleans—results of qualitative research. Social Science & Medicine. 2005;60(2):297–311. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rosengard C, et al. Concepts of the advantages and disadvantages of teenage childbearing among pregnant adolescents: a qualitative analysis. Pediatrics. 2006;118(2):503–510. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-3058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Unger JB, Molina GB, Teran L. Perceived consequences of teenage childbearing among adolescent girls in an urban sample. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2000;26(3):205–212. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(99)00067-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Condon JT, Donovan J, Corkindale CJ. Australian adolescents’ attitudes and beliefs concerning pregnancy, childbirth and parenthood: the development, psychometric testing and results of a new scale. Journal of Adolescence. 2001;24(6):729–742. doi: 10.1006/jado.2001.0439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hare-Mustin RT, Bennett SK, Broderick PC. Attitude toward motherhood: gender, generational and religious comparisons. Sex Roles. 1983;9(5):643–661. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Holton S, Fisher J, Rowe H. Attitudes toward women and motherhood: their role in Australian women’s childbearing behaviour. Sex Roles. 2009;61(9–10):677–687. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Warner R, et al. Attitudes toward motherhood in postnatal depression: development of the Maternal Attitudes Questionnaire. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1997;43(4):351–358. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(97)00128-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Afable-Munsuz A, et al. A positive orientation toward early motherhood is associated with unintended pregnancy among New Orleans youth. Maternal and Child Health Journal. 2006;10(3):265–276. doi: 10.1007/s10995-005-0049-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gerson MJ. A scale for motivation for parenthood: the Index of Parenthood Motivation. Journal of Psychology. 1983;113(2):211–220. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lawson KL. Development and psychometric properties of the Perceptions of Parenting Inventory. Journal of Psychology. 2004;138(5):433–455. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ajzen I. From intentions to actions: a theory of planned behavior. In: Kuhl J, Beckmann J, editors. Action Control: from Cognition to Behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1985. pp. 11–39. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Johnson-Hanks J. Demographic transitions and modernity. Annual Review of Anthropology. 2008;37:301–315. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Moos MK, et al. Pregnant women’s perspectives on intendedness of pregnancy. Women’s Health Issues. 1997;7(6):385–392. doi: 10.1016/s1049-3867(97)00081-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Esacove A. Making sense of sex: rethinking intentionality. Culture, Health & Sexuality. 2008;10(4):377–390. doi: 10.1080/13691050801898869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sable MR. Pregnancy intentions may not be a useful measure for research on maternal and child health outcomes. Family Planning Perspectives. 1999;31(5):249–250. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Raine TR, et al. One-year contraceptive continuation and pregnancy in adolescent girls and women initiating hormonal contraceptives. Obstetrics & Gynecology. 2011;(2 Pt 1):363–371. doi: 10.1097/AOG.0b013e31820563d3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Raine TR, et al. Attitudes toward the vaginal ring and transdermal patch among adolescents and young women. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2009;45(3):262–267. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.02.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Rosengard C, et al. Adolescent pregnancy intentions and pregnancy outcomes: a longitudinal examination. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2004;35(6):453–461. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Driscoll AK, et al. Adolescent Latino reproductive health: a review of the literature. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences. 2001;23(3):255–326. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kleinbaum DG, Kupper LL, Morgenstern H. Epidemiologic Research: Principles in Quantitative Methods. Belmont, CA: Lifetime Learning Publications; 1982. pp. 104–105. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Cohen J. A power primer. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112(1):155–159. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.112.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rocca CH, Harper CC. Do racial and ethnic differences in contraceptive attitudes and knowledge explain disparities in method use? Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health. 2012;44(3):150–158. doi: 10.1363/4415012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Schwarz EB, et al. Prevalence and correlates of ambivalence towards pregnancy among nonpregnant women. Contraception. 2007;75(4):305–310. doi: 10.1016/j.contraception.2006.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Abma JC, Martinez GM, Copen CE. Teenagers in the United States: sexual activity, contraceptive use and childbearing, National Survey of Family Growth 2006–2008. Vital and Health Statistics. 2010;23(30) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Jaccard J, Dodge T, Dittus P. Do adolescents want to avoid pregnancy? Attitudes toward pregnancy as predictors of pregnancy. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2003;33(2):79–83. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(03)00134-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Stevens-Simon C, Beach RK, Klerman LV. To be rather than not to be—that is the problem with the questions we ask adolescents about their childbearing intentions. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 2001;155(12):1298–1300. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.155.12.1298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.American Educational Research Association. Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1999. American Psychological Association and National Council for Measurement in Psychology. [Google Scholar]
- 39.De Boeck P, Wilson M, editors. Explanatory Item Response Models: A Generalized Linear and Nonlinear Approach. New York: Springer-Verlag; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Embretson SE, Reise SP. Item Response Theory for Psychologists. 2. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Wilson M, Allen DD, Li JC. Improving measurement in health education and health behavior research using item response modeling: comparison with the classical test theory approach. Health Education Research. 2006;21(Suppl 1):i19–i32. doi: 10.1093/her/cyl053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hays RD, Morales LS, Reise SP. Item response theory and health outcomes measurement in the 21st century. Medical Care. 2000;38(9 Suppl):II28–II42. doi: 10.1097/00005650-200009002-00007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Embretson SE. The new rules of measurement. Psychological Assessment. 1996;8(4):341–349. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Masters GN. A Rasch model for partial credit scoring. Psychometrika. 1982;47(2):149–174. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Steinberg L, Thissen D. Using effect sizes for research reporting: examples using item response theory to analyze differential item functioning. Psychological Methods. 2006;11(4):402–415. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.4.402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Uebelacker LA, et al. Use of item response theory to understand differential functioning of DSM-IV major depression symptoms by race, ethnicity and gender. Psychological Medicine. 2009;39(4):591–601. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]