Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2013 Jun;13(2):211–224. doi: 10.3758/s13415-012-0139-1

Can theories of visual representation help to explain asymmetries in amygdala function?

Brenton W McMenamin 1, Chad J Marsolek 1
PMCID: PMC3621008  NIHMSID: NIHMS429059  PMID: 23239022

Abstract

Emotional processing differs between the left and right hemispheres of the brain, and functional differences have been reported more specifically between the left amygdala and right amygdala, subcortical structures heavily implicated in emotional processing. However, the empirical pattern of amygdalar asymmetries is inconsistent with extant theories of emotional asymmetries. Here we review this discrepancy, and we hypothesize that hemispheric differences in visual object processing help to explain the previously reported functional differences between the left and right amygdalae. The implication that perceptual factors play a large role in determining amygdalar asymmetries may help to explain amygdalar dysfunction in the development and maintenance of post-traumatic stress disorder.


The cerebral hemispheres differ in their processing of emotional information, and modern neuroimaging techniques have begun to explore precisely which neural structures are involved in these hemispheric asymmetries. The amygdalae are subcortical structures located bilaterally in the temporal lobes that play roles in diverse emotional processes (Zald, 2003). Recently, several meta-analyses of amygdalar activation confirmed that the left and right amygdalae function differently, but these findings were not consistent with previous theories for emotional asymmetries in general or previous theories of amygdalar asymmetries in particular (Table 1). We propose that bottom-up perceptual processes heavily influence the amygdalae, and that hemispheric asymmetries in visual object processing can help to provide a parsimonious explanation of amygdalar asymmetries.

Table 1.

Previous descriptions of functional hemispheric asymmetries in the human amygdalae

Asymmetry type Left amygdala function Right amygdala function Reference(s)
Language-related Activated by language-based stimuli Activated by image-based stimuli Markowitsch (1998), Glascher and Adolphs (2003)
Language-related Verbally instructed threat Image-apparent threat Phelps et al. (2001)
Masking-related Detailed, sustained analysis Shallow, rapid analysis Markowitsch (1998), Glascher and Adolphs (2003)
Masking-related Used for explicit/conscious evaluation of emotion Used for implicit evaluation of emotion Markowitsch (1998), Glascher and Adolphs (2003)
Habituation-rate Slow neural habituation Fast neural habituation Wright et al. (2001)
Gender-related More active in females More active in males Cahill (2006)

This review is organized into five sections: the first section reviews differences in emotional processing between the two cerebral hemispheres (including the amygdala), the second reviews functional asymmetries specific to the amygdalae, and the third reviews hemispheric asymmetries in visual object processing. Section four describes ways in which the visual object processing asymmetries may help to explain the amygdalar asymmetries, and section five provides an example of how a consideration of perceptual factors can provide insight regarding amygdalar dysfunction in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).

I. Theories of Emotion Asymmetry

Emotional asymmetries in humans

For a century, researchers have hypothesized that the left and right hemispheres of the brain process emotion differently (Mills, 1912, cited in Murphy, Nimmo-Smith, & Lawrence, 2003). Three major theories have been proposed to characterize the differences in emotional processing between hemispheres in humans: right-hemisphere dominance, valence lateralization, and motivation lateralization.

Right-hemisphere dominance theory

The earliest theories of emotional lateralization proposed that the right hemisphere (RH) is more efficient at affective processing than the left hemisphere (LH), providing a complement to the LH’s language and ‘cognitive’ abilities (Harrington, 1995). A variety of experimental paradigms and measures have been used to provide evidence for the RH dominance theory. For example, the left half of a face – whose musculature is controlled by the right hemisphere -- is more expressive (Sackeim, Gur, & Saucy, 1978), affective prosody in speech is more easily detected when presented to the left ear (hence mostly to the RH) than to the right ear (hence mostly to the LH; Erhan, Borod, Tenke, & Bruder, 1998), and emotional stimuli elicit stronger physiological responses when presented to the left visual field (LVF; hence directly to the RH) than to the right visual field (RVF; hence directly to the LH; Spence, Shapiro, & Zaidel, 1996).

Valence lateralization theory

Eventually, evidence accumulated that the LH also plays a prominent role in emotional processing. The valence lateralization theory asserts that the LH is more efficient at processing positively valenced affect than the RH, and that the RH is more efficient at processing negatively valenced affect than the LH (Davidson, 1992). Lesions of the LH prefrontal cortex (PFC) or LH basal ganglia correspond to an increased likelihood of depressive symptoms (Morris, Robinson, Raphael, & Hopwood, 1996), whereas a lesion of the RH frontal operculum (Starkstein et al., 1989), a complete RH hemispherectomy or lesions to unspecified locations in the RH (Sackeim et al., 1982) increase the likelihood of cheerfulness and euphoric symptoms. Similarly, in non-lesioned patients, individual differences in threat sensitivity are positively associated with resting-state neural activity, as indexed by the inverse of alpha-band electroencephalography [EEG]) at right anterior electrodes (Coan & Allen, 2004; Sutton & Davidson, 1997). Source-estimate analysis indicates this EEG effect is due to activation of right dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC; Shackman, McMenamin, Maxwell, Greischar, & Davidson, 2009). Conversely, individual differences in reward sensitivity are positively associated with neural activity at left anterior electrodes (Coan & Allen, 2004; Sutton & Davidson, 1997), which are presumably linked to activation within left PFC. Lastly, divided visual field paradigms – which use lateralized stimulus presentation to facilitate processing in the RH (via brief presentation in the LVF) or LH (via brief presentation in the RVF) – provide behavioral evidence that the speed with which either hemisphere processes positively or negatively valenced emotional images is consistent with valence lateralization (Davidson, Mednick, Moss, Saron, & Schaffer, 1987; Maxwell, Shackman, & Davidson, 2005).

Motivation lateralization theory

More recently, the valence lateralization theory has been modified to a motivation lateralization theory, by which the LH is more efficient at processing approach-related affect and the RH is more efficient at processing withdrawal-related affect (Harmon-Jones, 2003). This theory is largely indistinguishable from the valence lateralization theory because approach and withdrawal behaviors are typically elicited by stimuli with positive and negative valences, respectively. However, Berkman and Lieberman (2010) deconfounded stimulus valence and motivation with a novel task in a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study. Participants read about the fictional Nochmani culture that enjoyed eating insects but were disgusted by eating meat, and then categorized pictures of food as edible or inedible to the Nochmani. In this task, stimuli could have a positive valence to the participant but evoke a withdrawal action (e.g., indicating that a pleasing picture of meat is inedible) or have a negative valence and an approach response (e.g., indicating that a displeasing picture of an insect is edible). Activation in left dlPFC was greater for trials that emphasized approach-related behavior (i.e., “edible” objects, regardless of valence), and activation was stronger in right dlPFC for trials that emphasized withdrawal-related behavior (i.e., “inedible” objects, regardless of valence), supporting the motivation lateralization theory. Additional support for the motivation lateralization theory comes from studying anger because it is negatively valenced but approach related. An association between RH function and anger would provide evidence for the valence lateralization hypothesis; however, Harmon-Jones (Harmon-Jones, 2004a, 2004b) reported that anger was associated with LH EEG activity, providing evidence that the asymmetries in emotional function are more closely linked to motivation than to valence.

Emotional asymmetries in non-humans

The lateralization of affective processes is not unique to humans or primates. Pigeons (Güntürkün & Kesch, 1987), chicks (Rogers, 2000), black-winged stilts (Ventolini et al., 2005), and several species of toad (Robins & Rogers, 2004; Vallortigara, Rogers, Bisazza, Lippolis, & Robins, 1998) are more likely to initiate feeding behaviors for food stimuli processed in the LH. Conversely, toads (Lippolis, Bisazza, Rogers, & Vallortigara, 2002; Vallortigara et al., 1998), chameleons (Deckel, 1998), chicks (Rogers, 2000), and baboons (Casperd & Dunbar, 1996) are more likely to exhibit defensive behaviors when threats are processed in the RH1.

This particular pattern of emotional asymmetry is found in quite diverse species, suggesting that it provides an important general benefit and is not a peculiarity of the primate cortex that may be attributable to a specific environmental niche or language abilities. However, one could question how asymmetrically organized emotional systems are capable of benefiting an organism. In fact, one may intuit that a brain with highly lateralized emotional systems would perform sub-optimally because, for example, it diminishes the reward-responsiveness in one visual field and threat-responsiveness in the other. However, Güntürkün et al. (2000) report that pigeons with greater visual-field asymmetries for feeding (i.e., better performance distinguishing grain from grit when using the RVF/LH than the LVF/RH) were also more efficient at foraging. This suggests that pigeons with less lateralization in feeding behaviors were less efficient at foraging overall. Rogers, Zucca, and Vallortigara (2004) manipulated the degree of brain lateralization in chicks by varying their exposure to light or dark prior to hatching – the chick embryo is positioned such that the left eye is occluded late in development, and exposure to light at this time results in stronger lateralization. The light-exposed chicks were more efficient at foraging, and more accurate at detecting a predator while foraging. This indicates that lateralization facilitates foraging behavior and facilitates the simultaneous operation of approach- and withdrawal-related processes (e.g., foraging for food while remaining vigilant to threat).

II. Asymmetries in Amygdala Activation

There is support for all three of the major theories of emotional asymmetry, so it is important to determine which regions of the brain may be responsible for which asymmetry patterns. Simply specifying that a process differs between the left and right hemispheres does not provide a desirable level of anatomical specificity given that meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies can identify asymmetries in emotional processing in specific brain regions within each hemisphere (Wager et al., 2003). Particular emphasis has been placed on exploring the asymmetries in the amygdala because of its involvement in many emotional processes (Zald, 2003). The amygdala’s reputation as a “fear-center” or “threat-detector” would imply greater involvement in processing negative affect and/or withdrawal-related behavior, so each of the three aforementioned theories of emotional asymmetry should predict relatively greater responsiveness of the RH amygdala than the LH amygdala. However, neuroimaging meta-analyses indicate that effects are reported more frequently within the left amygdala than the right amygdala (Fusar-Poli et al., 2009; Murphy et al., 2003; Wager et al., 2003). If traditional theories of emotional asymmetries fail to explain differences in functional activity between the two amygdalae, what could provide an explanation?

Hypotheses for hemispheric asymmetries in amygdalar function

Previous hypotheses of functional differences between the left and right amygdalae (Table 1) can be grouped into four general families: language-related differences, masking-related differences, habituation-rate differences, and gender effects. It is important to note that these hypotheses are not mutually exclusive, and some researchers suggest that asymmetries are actually due to a combination of these differences (Glascher & Adolphs, 2003; Markowitsch, 1998).

Language-related differences

One hypothesis is that the left amygdala is more responsive than the right amygdala to stimuli requiring linguistic processing (Markowitsch, 1998). This would imply that the left amygdala is more easily activated by written or spoken words and initiating an emotional response to stimuli whose emotional value was learned linguistically rather than through experience. Phelps et al. (2001) instructed participants that a particular stimulus was predictive of future punishment, and they found that this instructed-threat stimulus evoked greater activity in the left amygdala, in contrast to traditional fear conditioning studies that have implicated regions throughout the right-hemisphere (Hugdahl, 1995).

Masking-related differences

Markowitsch (1998) also proposed that the left amygdala is involved in “explicit, language-related or feature-extracting processes”, and the right amygdala is involved in “imagery-related, pictorial and fast or shallow” processing (Markowitsch, 1998, p. 240). These functional differences could be described generally as a difference in the temporal characteristics of processing, where the left amygdala is involved with slower processes and the right amygdala is involved with faster processes. This is consistent with lesion data in which damage to the left amygdala reduced the correlation between skin-conductance response (SCR) magnitude of the concious rating of an image’s arousal, whereas damage to the right amygdala was associated with reduced SCR to simply viewing emotional images (Glascher & Adolphs, 2003). This theoretical account proposes that the left amygdalae is more involved with slower, explicit emotion appraisal processes and the right amygdala is more involved with faster, implicit ‘threat detection’ types of processes. A common experimental manipulation for testing the temporal characteristics of processing is to limit the processing time for a stimulus by applying a mask. The right amygdala is more active than the left amygdala for emotional visual stimuli that have been masked to limit concious awareness and/or explicit processing (Morris, Öhman & Dolan, 1998; Morris, Öhman & Dolan, 1999).

Habituation-rate differences

An alternative explanation is that different rates of neural habituation occur for the two amygdalae. Wright, Fischer, Whalen, McInerney, Shin and Rauch (2001) repeatedly presented the same emotionally expressive face (happy of fearful) to participants while in an fMRI scanner, and measured the evoked amygdalar response as a function of stimulus repetition. The activity evoked by the fearful face relative to the happy face did not change with repetition in the left amygdala, whereas the evoked fearful response decreased with repetition in the RH amygdala (i.e., the response habituated). It may be important to note that this study used facial stimuli exclusively, which rely more on RH representation (Haxby, Hoffman, & Gobbini, 2000; Herrington, Taylor, Grupe, Curby & Schultz, 2012), so the absence of left amygdala habituation could be attributed to a less robust perceptual input from ipsilateral visual areas. Moreover, the habituation was not linked to a behavioral or physiological change, making the interpretation difficult. If physiological arousal evoked by the stimulus did not change after habituation, the implication would be that the RH amygdala was developing a more efficient representation for a frequently encountered stimulus. Alternatively, if arousal decreased during the habituation, additional tests would be necessary after a period of rest to determine whether the effect was due to a transient fatigue effect or a more permanent “unlearning” of emotional significance.

Gender-based asymmetries

Finally, amygdalar asymmetries may depend in part on gender (Cahill, 2006). In females, the left amygdala is activated more than the right amygdala at rest (Kilpatrick, Zald, Pardo, & Cahill, 2006) or during the encoding of emotional memories (Cahill, 2001), and the converse is true for males. Savic and Lindström (2008) extended this finding by demonstrating an interaction with sexual orientation. Heterosexual females and homosexual males have greater resting-state connectivity between the left amygdala and outside areas than between the right amygdala and outside areas, whereas heterosexual males and homosexual females have greater resting-state connectivity between the right amygdala and outside areas than between the left amygdala and outside areas. This pattern of different amygdala lateralization for males and females has also been found in rats (Sullivan, Dufresne & Waldron, 2009).

Meta-analyses of amygdalar asymmetries

Several meta-analyses of neuroimaging results have been performed, and they can be used to test which theories of amygdalar asymmetry are supported by the body of published neuroimaging data. The number of studies, the meta-analytic techniques, and the statistical tests differed for each report, so the methods and results from each meta-analysis are reviewed separately.

Wager, Phan, Liberzon, and Taylor (2003)

This meta-analysis did not focus specifically on the amygdala or the theories of emotional asymmetry in Table 1, but it did pioneer the use of meta-analysis of neuroimaging results to investigate emotional processes. It used a vote-counting technique to determine the relative frequency of significant effects in 65 fMRI and PET studies. Vote-counting is a simple technique for meta-analysis performed by tallying the number of times researchers report significant activity in a particular brain region, and then testing whether significant effects are more or less likely under different experimental conditions.

Studies investigating withdrawal-related affect reported significance more frequently in both amygdalae relative to studies investigating approach-related affect. Moreover, studies using withdrawal-related or negative emotional content reported significant activity in the left amygdala more frequently than the right amygdala, contradicting the predictions from the right-hemisphere dominance, valence lateralization, and motivation lateralization theories. Across all brain regions, males had more lateralizalized responses to emotional stimuli than females, but the predicted gender differences in lateralization were not found between the amygdalae per se. Rather, the sublenticular area surrounding the amygdala demonstrated the rightward asymmetry for males and a leftward asymmetry for females predicted by the gender asymmetry theory.

Baas, Aleman, and Kahn (2004)

This meta-analysis focused specifically on amygdalar asymmetries and testing several of the hypothesized functional differences in Table 1. Fifty-four fMRI and PET studies were analyzed with a vote counting approach. The stimuli used in each of the experiments were classified as pictorial, language-related, or “other” (i.e., memory recall, non-linguistic sounds) to test the role of linguistic content on each amygdala. The uniformity of stimuli was used to test effects of neural habituation – if the same stimuli were repeated over the course of the task, habituation should be greater than in a study in which different stimuli are used. The task demands for each experiment were classified as elaborative if one had to read words to ascertain emotional value, if mood was induced, or if emotional reappraisal was required. Experiments that instructed participants to attend to the emotional aspects of the task were labeled as explicit; otherwise, the experiment was categorized as containing implicitly processed emotion. Baas et al. (2004) found no evidence for the language-related, masking-related, or habituation-rate hypotheses. However, they did find that significant amygdalar activation was more frequent in the left amygdala than the right overall, but none of the differences in experimental stimuli or task parameters affected that asymmetry.

Costafreda, Brammer, David, and Fu (2008)

The non-significant effects reported for most factors on amygdalar asymmetries in Wager et al. (2003) and Baas et al. (2004) may have been due to methodological factors that limited statistical power. Costafreda et al. (2008) performed a more sensitive meta-analysis by expanding the corpus of data to include 385 fMRI and PET studies and using a logistic regression to test for differences in the probability of significant effects. This regression approach is a significant improvement over the previous methods because it accounts for the relationships between experimental parameters to reduce potential confounds (e.g., in Baas et al.’s analysis, seven out of ten studies using elaboration also had uniform stimuli). With the benefit of additional studies and this improved methodology, the researchers tested whether stimulus masking or linguistic demands affected the likelihood of asymmetric amygdalar activity. Costafreda et al found that activation was more likely in the left amygdala than the right, consistent with an overall LH amygdala bias. But if visual stimuli were masked to limit explicit or sustained processing, the right amygdala was more likely to be activated than the left. If linguistic stimuli were used (i.e., stimuli were written words, spoken or internal dialogue), the left amygdala was more likely to be activated than the right.

Sergerie, Chochol, and Armony (2008)

The previous meta-analyses relied on vote-counting, which is used to measure how certain experimental parameters affect the likelihood of statistical significance but not how experimental parameters affect the strength of activation (i.e. effect size). Sergerie, Chochol, and Armony (2008) performed a meta-analysis to replicate Wager et al. (2003) and Baas et al. (2004) with a larger corpus of data (148 fMRI and PET studies), however the vote-counting technique was replaced with an approach that compared effect-sizes across studies.

First, significant effects overall were more likely in the left amygdala than the right, consistent with the previous meta-analyses, however, the effect size did not differ between the left and right amygdalae. Second, a comparison of blocked and event-related designs indicated hemispheric differences in amygdalar activation. Significant activity was reported more frequently in the left amygdala than the right when using blocked designs, but not when using event-related designs. This may suggest different effects of task set on the two amygdalae because blocked designs require less task or process switching over time than do event-related designs. Alternatively, event-related designs may minimize the opportunity for slow and sustained activation in the left amygdala. Third, language-related stimuli evoked marginally stronger effects in the LH amygdala than did pictorial stimuli, consistent with the analyses by Costafreda et al. (2008). Lastly, a positive correlation between the effect-size of amygdala activation and the proportion of the participants that were male suggested that amygdala activation is strong in males, even though statistically significant effects were not more common in studies with a higher proportion of males. However, no tests of gender-based lateralization were reported.

Summary of amygdalar meta-analyses

Left hemisphere bias in amygdala activation

The meta-analyses indicate that there are reliable differences in the activation of the left and right amygdalae (summarized in Table 2), but the only consistent finding across all of the meta-analyses was that significant effects were more common in the left amygdala than the right amygdala overall (Baas et al., 2004; Costafreda et al., 2008; Sergerie et al., 2008; Wager et al., 2003; for additional evidence, see also Fusar-Poli et al., 2009; Murphy et al., 2003). This is inconsistent with the right-hemisphere dominance theory of emotion asymmetry, and several analyses performed specific tests to demonstrate that the activation of the amygdalae are not consistent with the valence or motivation lateralization theories (Sergerie et al., 2008; Wager et al., 2003).

Table 2.

Summary of neuroimaging meta-analyses of hemispheric asymmetries in amygdalar function.

Meta-analysis Found evidence for language differences Found evidence for masking differences Found evidence for habituation-rate differences Found evidence for gender effects Found evidence for a left amygdala bias
Wager, Phan, Liberzon and Taylor (2003)
Baas, Aleman and Kahn (2004)
Costafreda, Brammer, David and Fu (2008)
Sergerie, Chochol and Armony (2008)

Language-related asymmetries

Costafreda et al. (2008) and Sergerie et al. (2008) concluded that LH amygdala activity was more likely than RH amygdala activity during language-based emotional processing, consistent with the hypotheses formed by Markowitsch (1998) and Glascher and Adolphs (2003). The tests of language-based effects in these meta-analyses were restricted to experiments in which a participant read or listened to words, thus it is also informative that experiments with verbally-instructed threats result in greater left than right amygdala activity (Phelps et al., 2001).

Masking-related asymmetries

Costafreda et al. (2008) reported that masking a visual stimulus resulted in a greater likelihood of activation of the right amygdala relative to the left amygdala. This suggests that masks that degrade the stimulus or limit the amount of time available for processing disrupt activation in the right amygdala to a lesser degree than activation in the left amygdala.

Habituation-rate asymmetries

The Baas et al (2004) analysis tested whether the use of repeated stimuli affected amygdalar asymmetries, but did not find a significant effect. However, Sergerie et al. (2008) reported that blocked designs, in which amygdalar processing would be sustained in emotional conditions, evoke more activity from the left amygdala than the right. This is consistent with the prediction that the activity in the right amygdala habituates more rapidly than activity in the left amygdala when this is enabled by a blocked design.

Gender-based asymmetries

Wager et al. (2003) provided evidence of gender differences in amygdalae lateralization consistent with those hypothesized by Cahill (2001, 2006). Females exhibit more activity near the left amygdala and males exhibit greater activity near the right amygdala. The only other meta-analysis to address gender effects did not test whether gender interacted with amygdala lateralization (Sergerie, et al., 2008). However, this should not be interpreted as evidence against the existence of gender-based asymmetries in amygdala functioning. The gender-based amygdala asymmetry is generally reported in studies of long-term memory, so it may be inappropriate to expect it to appear in a general meta-analysis. Moreover, the effect may be obscured by experiments that do not explicitly account for important variables relevant to the gender effect (e.g., sexual orientation, see Lavric & Lindström, 2008).

Conclusions

This review indicates that amygdalae activation does not correspond to any of the traditional theories of hemispheric asymmetry for emotion (i.e., right-hemisphere dominance, valence lateralization, or motivation lateralization). However, partial support is found for each of the hypothesized functional differences between the amygdalae (i.e., language-related differences, masking-related differences, habituation-rate differences, and gender differences), and a strong consensus indicates that the left amygdala is more frequently activated than the right overall. An unparsimonious explanation for these findings is that several hypotheses are each credited with partially explaining some of the observed asymmetries. Alternatively in the rest of this article, a unifying hypothesis is proposed. Specifically, hemispheric asymmetries in perceptual processing may be responsible for hemispheric asymmetries in the amygdalae, in a manner that may provide a parsimonious explanation for the observed results.

III. Asymmetries in Visual Object Processing

The human amygdalae process information from most sensory modalities (Zald, 2003), mapping perceptual inputs to affective values (Rolls, 2005), however they are particularly responsive to visual input (Phan, Wager, Taylor, & Liberzon, 2002). Visual information enters through the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA) via projections from the thalamus and the anterior portion of inferotemporal cortex (IT; Freese & Amaral, 2009), and the medial portion of the BLA projects back onto the ventral visual stream to modulate ongoing processing (Freese & Amaral, 2009; Sabatinelli, Lang, Bradley, Costa, & Keil, 2009; Vuilleumier, 2005). The BLA receives most visual input from the ipsilateral hemisphere (McDonald, 1998), so the existence of a hemispheric asymmetry in the ventral visual stream’s perceptual representations could easily manifest itself “downstream” in the amygdala. This section reviews asymmetries in visual object processing, and the subsequent section describes how these asymmetries may account for the observed functional differences between the left and right amygdalae.

Hemispheric asymmetries in recognizing categories and identifying exemplars

Object perception is complicated by the conflicting demands to be able to both categorize and individuate objects effectively. In many situations, the ability to recognize the abstract category to which a visual object belongs is important (e.g., when scanning a desk to quickly find any pen to write with). In other situations, the ability to identify the specific exemplar to which a visual object corresponds is paramount (e.g., when trying to find the individual pen that was a gift from a friend). These abilities place contradictory demands on the visual system. For efficient abstract category recognition, it is useful to learn the visual features that maximize between-category variations and minimize within-category variations. In contrast, for efficient specific exemplar identification, it is useful to learn the visual information that maximizes within-category variation for distinguishing exemplars.

A resolution to the contradictory demands is that dissociable neural subsystems may underlie the ability to abstractly categorize objects and the ability to specifically identify exemplars (Marsolek, 1999, 2003; Marsolek & Burgund, 1997, 2008). An abstract-category subsystem operates more effectively in the LH than in the RH, and a specific-exemplar subsystem operates more effectively in the RH than in the LH. This pattern of asymmetries has been observed in divided-visual-field experiments (e.g., Marsolek, 1999; Marsolek, Andresen, & Nicholas, 2002; Marsolek & Burgund, 2008), fMRI studies (Koutstaal et al., 2001; Simons, Koutstaal, Prince, Wagner, & Schacter, 2003), performance following unilateral brain damage (e.g., Beeri, Vakil, Adonsky, & Levenkron, 2004; Vaidya, Gabrieli, Verfaellie, Fleischman, & Askari, 1998), differential effects of the neuromodulator serotonin (Burgund, Marsolek & Luciana, 2003), and selective disruption via repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (Pobric, Schweinberger & Lavidor, 2007). It is important to note that these asymmetries in visual category recognition and exemplar identification can affect the object representations used by other cognitive processes. For example, the visual asymmetries extend to how categories and exemplars are stored in working memory (Marsolek & Burgund, 2008).

Parts-based and whole-based processing strategies

The abstract-category (AC) and specific-exemplar (SE) subsystems utilize different processing strategies to accomplish their different goals. To effectively categorize objects, an abstract-category subsystem uses a parts-based processing strategy to represent the smaller features of larger whole objects that are diagnostic of an object’s category, even when visually dissimilar objects belong to the same category (Marsolek, 1995; Marsolek & Burgund, 2003). In contrast, to effectively individuate objects, a specific-exemplar subsystem uses a whole-based strategy to represent the visually distinctive whole configurations that distinguish even very similar objects (Marsolek, Schacter, & Nicholas, 1996; Marsolek & Burgund, 2003).

A virtue of this theory is that both relatively parts-based and relatively whole-based representations are posited, because both kinds of representations enable benefits for different purposes. It has been argued that the two kinds of representations provide two complementary methods for representation in any system, with the parts-based strategy enabling effective generalization to novel objects and the whole-based strategy enabling effective discrimination of similar inputs (Hummel, 2000, 2003). In addition, the effects of expertise have been investigated in studies in which participants learn new categories of pre-experimentally novel artificial figures (e.g., Greebles). After expertise has been gained for effectively distinguishing visually similar shapes, whole-based representations are used which enable greater automaticity in performance (Gauthier & Tarr, 1997; Gauthier, Williams, Tarr & Tanaka, 1998; Gauthier & Tarr, 2002). Neuroimaging evidence supports the hypothesis that the two kinds of representations are asymmetric. Attending to local image features (i.e., the small parts in hierarchical figures, such as the small letter S’s that form a larger letter H) increases activity in the LH, but attending to global image features (i.e., the wholes in hierarchical figures, such as the larger letter H that is formed by small letter S’s) increases activity in the RH (Fink, Marshall, Halligan, Frith, Frackowiak & Dolan, 1996; Han et al., 2002).

Effects of stimulus masking

The effects of stimulus degradation (e.g., masking) on object processing are more pronounced in the LH than the RH (Christman, 1989; Sergent & Hellige, 1986), which may be related to the different representational strategies used by the AC and SE subsystems. The whole-based representations used in a SE subsystem are more distributed than the parts-based representations used in an AC subsystem (Marsolek & Burgund, 1997), which could confer greater robustness to stimulus noise and degradation to the representations in the RH (Marsolek & Burgund, 2003). Repetition priming effects measured in a divided-visual-field paradigm support the hypothesis that an AC subsystem operates more effectively in the LH than in the RH, and this asymmetry is influenced by stimulus degradation (Marsolek, 1999; Marsolek & Hudson, 1999). Moreover, an effect similar to masking and stimulus degradation may occur in situations when attention is heavily restricted. Consistent with the findings that the LH is more sensitive than the RH to stimulus degradation, priming within an AC subsystem is reduced when attention is directed away from the prime stimulus relative to when attention is directed toward the prime stimulus (Stankiewicz et al., 1998; Hummel, 2000, 2003; Stankiewicz & Hummel, 2002).

IV. Relationship Between Visual and Amygdalar Asymmetries

Processing in sensory cortices can be modulated by emotional factors (Curby, Johnson & Tyson, 2012; Diamond & Weinberger, 1986; Edeline & Weinberger, 1992; Vuilleumier, 2005; Maxwell, Shackman, McMenamin, Greischar & Davidson, 2011), and perceptual factors are relevant to understanding emotional processes (Vuilleumier, Armony, Driver & Dolan, 2003; Larson, Aronoff & Stearns, 2007; Larson, Aronoff, Sarinopoulos & Zhu, 2009; McMenamin, Trask, Radue, Huskamp, Kersten & Marsolek, in press). Despite the reciprocal influences between emotional and perceptual systems, relatively few attempts have been made to connect the hemispheric asymmetries in emotion to the hemispheric asymmetries in perception.

Kensinger and Choi (2009) presented emotional and non-emotional object images in the left and right visual fields and then tested memory for the objects in a surprise memory test. Combining the observed asymmetries for processing visual object categories and exemplars with the asymmetries for processing affective valence or motivation, one can predict that (a) memory for object exemplars should be greater when they are presented directly to the RH than to the LH, particularly for objects with negative or withdrawal-related emotional value, and (b) memory for object categories should be greater when they are presented directly to the LH than to the RH, particularly for positive or approach-related stimuli. The hypothesized results were found, indicating that asymmetries in object representation and emotional processing interact during memory encoding.

Cahill and van Stregen (2003) linked gender-based asymmetries in amygdalar function to hemispheric differences in local/global processing by testing whether the way in which emotion modulates memory is relatively more local in females and relatively more global in males. Participants listened to a short emotion-provoking story accompanied by a slide show, and then their memory for the story was assessed in an unexpected memory test. Memory for local information was assessed as memory for details that were peripheral to the central aspects of the narrative, and memory for global information was assessed as memory for the central aspects of the narrative. Half of the participants received a beta-blocker (a substance that blocks the amygdala’s memory modulating function) prior to the story to reduce the effect of the emotionality of the stories on memory for them. Compared against a placebo group, participants in the beta-blocker group exhibited impaired memory for the local information when they were female participants and impaired memory for the global information when they were male participants.

These studies connected emotional and perceptual asymmetries, but neither tested the specific question of whether amygdalar asymmetries can be explained by asymmetries in object representation. Section II reviewed hypotheses of amygdalar asymmetry and grouped them into five families—an overall LH activation bias, language-related effects, masking-related effects, habituation-rate effects, and gender-based asymmetries. Each of these hypotheses was partially supported by neuroimaging meta-analyses. Below, we revisit each of these hypotheses and explore how each may arise because of differences in object representation across hemispheres.

Overall LH bias and language-related asymmetries explained by perceptual input

Emotional associations formed for object categories – which are represented effectively in the LH – may generalize to novel stimuli and situations, whereas emotional associations formed for object exemplars – which are represented effectively in the RH – may not generalize as widely. This may explain why significant activation was reported more frequently in the LH amygdala than in the RH amygdala in all of the meta-analyses. However, Costafreda et al. (2008) specifically reported that linguistic stimuli result in more frequent activation of the LH amygdala than the RH amygdala, consistent with the leftward lateralization for the visual analysis of linguistic stimuli (e.g., the visual word-form area; Vigneau, Jobard, Mazoyer, & Tzourio-Mazoyer, 2005). In addition, the perceptual processing of linguistic stimuli is usually categorical in nature, in that the goal is to recognize the word category to which each input belongs, not the specific exemplar to which each input corresponds (e.g., a word in a particular font and style; Marsolek, 2004). Moreover, the activation of the LH amygdala for verbally instructed threats (Phelps et al., 2001) may depend on the ability of analytic representations in the LH to effectively generalize to novel (i.e., not previously seen) objects.

Masking-based asymmetries explained by perceptual input

From their meta-analysis, Costafreda et al.’s (2008) reported that the RH amygdala is more frequently activated than the LH amygdala in studies in which visual stimuli are masked. This supports the previous hypothesis that the RH amygdala is specialized for rapid or implicit emotional processing (Glascher & Adolphs, 2003; Markowitsch, 1998). The cause of this asymmetry may arise from asymmetries on the effects of stimulus masking during visual object processing. In particular, the finding that stimulus degradation has a greater effect on stimuli presented to the LH than to the RH suggests that masking disrupts visual processing in the LH and the subsequent left amygdala more than visual processing in the RH and the subsequent right amygdala.

Habituation-rate asymmetries explained by perceptual input

Different habituation rates have also been hypothesized as critical for the functional differences between the amygdalae, such that the RH amygdala habituates faster than the LH amygdala (Sergerie et al., 2008; Wright et al., 2001b). Why would this be found? Reduced activation in the amygdalae after repeated stimulus presentation may be a consequence of repetition priming in perceptual systems, which generally manifests as reductions in neural activity (Grill-Spector, Henson, & Martin, 2006; but see also Marsolek et al., 2010). Repetition-priming effects for the SE subsystem are larger behaviorally (Marsolek, 1999) and more widespread neurally (Koutstaal et al., 2001; Simons et al., 2003) than for the AC subsystem, so the reduced activity in the RH amygdala after repeated presentations may be due to a larger degree of suppressed input from RH visual areas. Alternatively, the habituation effects may reflect an un-learning of emotional relevance because reward or punishment is not delivered with the emotional stimulus in a habituation study; less evidence is needed to learn that a particular object exemplar is irrelevant, but learning irrelevance for an entire object category should take longer. Therefore, the observation that repeated stimulus presentation reduces activity in the RH amygdala, and not in the LH amygdala, may reflect faster unlearning of threat in a SE subsystem than in an AC subsystem.

Gender-based asymmetries explained by perceptual input

Gender differences in amygdala lateralization were partially confirmed by Wager et al.’s (2004) meta-analysis. Regions surrounding the LH amygdala were activated more frequently than regions surrounding the RH amygdala in female participants, and vice versa in male participants. As discussed in Cahill and van Stregen (2003), this may not reflect differences in amygdalae function, but instead reflect different biases toward local and global feature processing in females and males, respectively (Kramer, Ellenberg, Leonard, & Share, 1996; Roalf, Lowery, & Turetsky, 2006). If so, the explanation for gender-based asymmetries is in line with perceptual asymmetries that parts-based representations are used in an AC subsystem in the LH and whole-based representations are used in a SE subsystem in the RH. These perceptual asymmetries combined with different gender biases may explain the gender-based asymmetries in amygdala activation.

V. Effects of Perceptual Asymmetry on Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder

If amygdalar function is partially determined by perceptual factors, it is reasonable to explore whether perceptual factors also play a role in amygdalar dysfunction. As an example, this section illustrates how consideration of perceptual factors may help to form novel ideas about the origin and maintenance of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) occurs for some individuals following exposure to a traumatic event and manifests itself as a vivid re-experiencing of the trauma (e.g., flashbacks), emotional numbing or avoidance, and hyperarousal (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). A study of Vietnam War veterans with (n = 193) and without (n = 52) head trauma reported that none of the veterans with amygdalar damage (0 out of 15) developed PTSD, but 48% of those without head trauma (25 out of 52) did develop PTSD (Koenigs et al., 2007). This suggests that intact amygdalae are critical for the development and/or maintenance of PTSD, complementing reports that indicate increased amygdalar activity at rest and during a variety of cognitive tasks for individuals with PTSD (Bremner, 2007).

Increased amygdalar activity in PTSD may be due to reduced top-down inhibition from the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), corresponding to impaired fear extinction (Koenigs & Grafman, 2009). However, the acquisition and maintenance of PTSD is also dependant upon bottom-up associative learning in the amygdalae (Ehlers & Clark, 2000), suggesting a critical role for perceptual representations in the development of PTSD symptoms. This section advances the hypothesis that the over-generalization of fear responses in PTSD, and possibly other affective disorders (Lissek et al., 2008; Lissek et al., 2009; Schechtman, Laufer & Paz, 2010), may be partially explained by an over-reliance on the abstract-category object recognition subsystem during the traumatic event.

Parts-based representation for traumatic memory

Traumatic memories are different from typical episodic memories because they are difficult to recall voluntarily, but when triggered, are experienced as a fragmented sensory experience that approximates “re-living” the trauma (i.e., flashback). These memories are predominately visual and highly disorganized, making it difficult to verbalize until many flashbacks allow the fragmented events to be combined into a coherent narrative (Foa, Molnar, & Cashman, 1995; van der Kolk & Fisler, 1995; van der Kolk, Hopper, & Osterman, 2001; Koss, Figueredo, Bell, Tharan, & Tromp, 1996). The inability to voluntarily recall traumatic memories, and the fact that their content is accurate and stable over time (van der Kolk & Fisler, 1995), distinguish them from ”flashbulb memories” for emotional events, which are easily recalled voluntarily, but are often inaccurate and unstable (Talarico & Rubin, 2003).

Experiencing peritraumatic psychological dissociation—described as “… a compartmentalization of experience: elements of the experience are not integrated into a unitary whole, but stored in memory as isolated fragments…” (van der Kolk & Fisler, 1995, p. 6)—is strongly associated with the strength of subsequent PTSD symptoms (Koopman, Classen, Cardenta, & Spiegel, 1995; Marshall & Schell, 2002). Brewin (2001) proposes two distinct representations for traumatic memory—a situationally accessible memory (SAM) stores the original “dissociated” sensory impressions, and a verbally accessible memory (VAM). Flashbacks are triggered whenever a stimulus matches a sensory fragment in the SAM, but the spatial and temporal structure of the SAM contents are learned during flashbacks and stored as more complex, structured memories in the VAM. Eventually, the simple sensory fragments in the SAM that trigger flashbacks are coalesced into more complex memories in the VAM, providing a consciously accessible narrative of the trauma.

The present theory may help to explain these findings. The fragmented quality of traumatic memory and the phenomenology of peritraumatic dissociation suggests that traumatic memories may rely on parts-based perceptual representations with poorly bound features, possibly like those that are normally used in an AC subsystem.

Left-hemisphere involvement in PTSD

Provocation of PTSD symptoms activates the LH amygdala more than the RH amygdala (Liberzon et al., 1999; Shin et al., 2004), and it activates the LH ventral visual stream more than the RH ventral visual stream (Shin et al., 1999). These findings are consistent with the hypothesized over-use of an AC subsystem. Also consistent is the finding that the prevalence of PTSD among females—who have more left-lateralized amygdala function than men (Cahill, 2006)—is roughly twice that of men (Stein, McQuaid, Pedrelli, Lenox, & McCahill, 2000). In addition, fMRI data indicate that females with PTSD have greater LH amygdala activation during the acquisition phase of fear conditioning than non-PTSD females (Bremner et al., 2005).

Three lines of research seem to provide putative evidence against the importance of the LH for the development of PTSD. First, Smith, Abou-Khalil and Zald (2008) performed a case study on a single individual that had her LH amygdala lesioned to treat epilepsy, but developed PTSD after trauma later in life. This study may not invalidate the hypothesis that AC representations are critical for the development of PTSD because it is unknown whether the typical asymmetrical organization of AC and SE subsystems was preserved in that single case study after years of severe epilepsy and a unilateral lesion.

Second, deficits in verbal – but not spatial – explicit memory accompany PTSD (Bremner, 2007). Verbal and spatial working memory systems tend to be lateralized to the LH and RH, respectively (Smith, Jonides, & Koeppe, 1996), so the verbal working memory deficit suggests that LH processing is deficient. One could argue that this LH deficit contradicts the proposed over-reliance on LH perceptual representations in PTSD, however, working memory systems can operate independently of the implicit long-term memory systems (Gabrieli, Fleischman, Keane, Reminger, & Morrell, 1995) and fear memory systems (Bechara et al., 1995) involved in PTSD development.

Lastly, PTSD patients sometimes demonstrate changes in RH amygdala activity that correspond to the severity of PTSD symptoms. Rauch et al (2000) measured amygdalar activity for masked fearful versus happy faces in participants with and without PTSD. Collapsed across groups, a difference in activity elicited by fearful and happy faces was observed in the LH amygdala, but a difference between PTSD and non-PTSD groups was restricted to the RH amygdala. Moreover, the magnitude of the fear-minus-happy effect in the RH amygdala correlated positively with PTSD symptom severity. The correlation between symptom severity and RH amygdala function was replicated in two of the aforementioned symptom provocation studies (Liberzon et al., 1999; Shin et al., 2004) and an fMRI study (Armony, Corbo, Clement, & Brunet, 2005). Armony et al. (2005) presented masked or unmasked emotional faces, and the fearful minus happy effect in the RH amygdala positively correlated with PTSD symptom severity for masked faces; however, the RH amygdala effect for unmasked faces was negatively correlated with symptom severity. These findings may not contradict the proposal that an AC subsystem is critical to the development and expression PTSD, instead they may illustrate that participants with strong symptoms are likely to show effects bilaterally. And, perhaps more importantly, they may indicate that the use of facial stimuli (which engage more SE processing than other objects) likely biases the results away from LH involvement.

Serotonergic involvement in the abstract-category system and PTSD

A final link between visual asymmetries and PTSD may be made via the neuromodulator serotonin. By our hypothesis, provoking PTSD symptoms over-activates the LH amygdala in part because the AC subsystem is over-used in PTSD patients. Increasing brain serotonin levels helps to treat PTSD symptoms (Hidalgo & Davidson, 2000; Seedat et al., 2002) by reducing amygdalar activity (Harmer, Mackay, Reid, Cowen, & Goodwin, 2006). The reduction in amygdalar activity may be direct or it may be due in part to a reduced use of the AC subsystem, according to our hypothesis. If so, increasing serotonin levels in normal participants may also decrease the use of the AC subsystem. Evidence in line with this hypothesis has been observed in a repetition priming study (Burgund et al., 2003). Participants with increased serotonin levels (i.e., tryptophan augmentation) exhibited only specific-exemplar priming effects, but participants with decreased serotonin levels (i.e., tryptophan depletion) exhibited only abstract priming effects. This suggests that over-activation of the LH amygdala in PTSD may be alleviated by increased serotonin in part through a reduction of the over-reliance on an AC subsystem in the LH.

Future directions

Amygdalar hyperactivation is common for individuals with PTSD, accompanying symptoms that include fragmented, parts-based memories of the trauma and a reliance on LH processing. These symptoms could stem from serotonergic depletions that result in over-reliance on parts-based representations in an AC subsystem. Future research is needed to test whether individual differences in the strength of AC and SE processing subsystems mediate the relationships between 5-HT, dissociative processing, fragmented traumatic memories, and overgeneralization of fearful stimuli. Testing these hypotheses may help identify additional risk factors, diagnostic tools, and treatment methods for PTSD.

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to Daniel Kersten, Shmuel Lissek and Angus W. MacDonald for feedback on earlier drafts of this paper. BWM was supported by T32-HD007151.

Footnotes

1

Some reports indicate a RH benefit for approach-related behaviors that require the discrimination of conspecifics, such as agonistic contact in toads (Vallortigara et al., 1998) and courtship behavior in black-winged stilts (Ventolini et al., 2005). Subsequent sections provide an account of how this may occur due to a RH perceptual benefit for discriminating object exemplars (i.e., conspecifics).

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  2. Archambault A, Gosselin F, Schyns P. A natural bias for the basic level. Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society; 2000. pp. 585–590. [Google Scholar]
  3. Armony JL, Corbo V, Clement MH, Brunet A. Amygdala response in patients with acute PTSD to masked and unmasked emotional facial expressions. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162(10):1961. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baas D, Aleman A, Kahn RS. Lateralization of amygdala activation: a systematic review of functional neuroimaging studies. Brain Research Reviews. 2004;45(2):96–103. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Adolphs R, Rockland C, Damasio A. Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science. 1995;269(5227):1115–1118. doi: 10.1126/science.7652558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beeri MS, Vakil E, Adonsky A, Levenkron S. The role of the cerebral hemispheres in specific versus abstract priming. Laterality. 2004;9:313–323. doi: 10.1080/13576500342000176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berkman ET, Lieberman MD. Approaching the bad and avoiding the good: Lateral prefrontal cortical asymmetry distinguishes between action and valence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2010;22(9):1970–1979. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Biederman I. Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological review. 1987;94(2):115–147. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.2.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boggan AL, Bartlett J, Krawczyk DC. Chess masters show a hallmark of face processing with chess. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2012;141:37–42. doi: 10.1037/a0024236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bonnasse-Gahot L, Nadal JP. Neural coding of categories: information efficiency and optimal population codes. Journal of Computational Neuroscience. 2008;25(1):169–187. doi: 10.1007/s10827-007-0071-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bowers JS. On the biological plausibility of grandmother cells: Implications for neural network theories in psychology and neuroscience. Psychological Review. 2009;116(1):220–251. doi: 10.1037/a0014462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bremner JD. Neuroimaging in posttraumatic stress disorder and other stress-related disorders. Neuroimaging Clinics of North America. 2007;17(4):523. doi: 10.1016/j.nic.2007.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bremner JD, Vermetten E, Schmahl C, Vaccarino V, Vythilingam M, Afzal N, Grillon C, et al. Positron emission tomographic imaging of neural correlates of a fear acquisition and extinction paradigm in women with childhood sexual-abuse-related post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychological medicine. 2005;35(06):791–806. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704003290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brewin CR. Memory processes in post-traumatic stress disorder. International Review of Psychiatry. 2001;13(3):159–163. doi: 10.1080/09540260120074019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bülthoff HH, Edelman S. Psychophysical support for a two-dimensional view interpolation theory of object recognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1992;89(1):60–64. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.1.60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Burgund ED, Marsolek CJ. Viewpoint-invariant and viewpoint-dependent object recognition in dissociable neural subsystems. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2000;7(3):480–489. doi: 10.3758/bf03214360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Burgund ED, Marsolek CJ, Luciana M. Serotonin levels influence patterns of repetition priming. Neuropsychology. 2003;17(1):161–170. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cahill L. Sex-Related Difference in Amygdala Activity during Emotionally Influenced Memory Storage. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2001;75(1):1–9. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2000.3999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cahill L, van Stegeren A. Sex-related impairment of memory for emotional events with [beta]-adrenergic blockade. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2003;79(1):81–88. doi: 10.1016/s1074-7427(02)00019-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cahill L. Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7(6):477–484. doi: 10.1038/nrn1909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Casperd JM, Dunbar RIM. Asymmetries in the visual processing of emotional cues during agonistic interactions by gelada baboons. Behavioural Processes. 1996;37(1):57–65. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(95)00075-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Christman S. Perceptual characteristics in visual laterality research. Brain and Cognition. 1989;11:238–257. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(89)90020-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Coan J, Allen JB. Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion. Biological Psychology. 2004;67(1–2):7–50. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Collier DA, Stöber G, Li T, Heils A, Catalano M, Di Bella D, Arranz MJ, et al. A novel functional polymorphism within the promoter of the serotonin transporter gene: possible role in susceptibility to affective disorders. Molecular Psychiatry. 1996;1(6):453–460. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cools R, Calder AJ, Lawrence AD, Clark L, Bullmore E, Robbins TW. Individual differences in threat sensitivity predict serotonergic modulation of amygdala response to fearful faces. Psychopharmacology. 2005;180(4):670–679. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2215-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Costafreda SG, Brammer MJ, David AS, Fu CH. Predictors of amygdala activation during the processing of emotional stimuli: a meta-analysis of 385 PET and fMRI studies. Brain Research Reviews. 2008;58(1):57–70. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Curby KM, Hayward G, Gauthier I. Laterality effects in the recognition of depth-rotated novel objects. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2004;4(1):100–111. doi: 10.3758/cabn.4.1.100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Curby KM, Johnson KJ, Tyson A. Face to face with emotion: Holistic face processing is modulated by emotional state. Cognition & Emotion. 2012;26(1):93–102. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.555752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Davidson RJ, Mednick D, Moss E, Saron C, Schaffer CE. Ratings of emotion in faces are influenced by the visual field to which stimuli are presented. Brain and Cognition. 1987;6(4):403–411. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(87)90136-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Davidson RJ. Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion. Brain and Cognition. 1992;20:125–151. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(92)90065-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Deckel A. Effects of serotonergic drugs on lateralized aggression and aggressive displays in Anolis carolinensis. Behavioural Brain Research. 1998;95(2):227–232. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00048-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Diamond DM, Weinberger NM. Classical conditioning rapidly induces specific changes in frequency receptive fields of single neurons in secondary and ventral ectosylvian auditory cortical fields. Brain Research. 1986;372(2):357–360. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91144-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Edeline JM, Weinberger NM. Associative retuning in the thalamic source of input to the amygdala and auditory cortex: receptive field plasticity in the medial division of the medial geniculate body. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1992;106(1):81–105. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.1.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Ehlers A, Clark DM. A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour research and therapy. 2000;38(4):319–345. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00123-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Erhan H, Borod JC, Tenke CE, Bruder GE. Identification of emotion in a dichotic listening task: event-related brain potential and behavioral findings. Brain and cognition. 1998;37(2):286–307. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1998.0984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Eurich CW, Wilke SD. Multidimensional encoding strategy of spiking neurons. Neural Computation. 2000;12(7):1519–1529. doi: 10.1162/089976600300015240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fink GR, Halligan PW, Marshall JC, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS, Dolan RJ. Where in the brain does visual attention select the forest and the trees? Nature. 1996;382(6592):626–628. doi: 10.1038/382626a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Foa EB, Molnar C, Cashman L. Change in rape narratives during exposure therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1995;8(4):675–690. doi: 10.1007/BF02102894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Freese J, Amaral D. Neuroanatomy of the primate amygdala. In: Whalen P, Phelps EA, editors. The human amygdala. 2009. pp. 3–42. [Google Scholar]
  40. Fusar-Poli P, Placentino A, Carletti F, Allen P, Landi P, Abbamonte M, Barale F, et al. Laterality effect on emotional faces processing: ALE meta-analysis of evidence. Neuroscience letters. 2009;452(3):262–267. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.01.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gabrieli JD, Fleischman DA, Keane MM, Reminger SL, Morrell F. Double Dissociation Between Memory Systems Underlying Explicit and Implicit Memory in the Human Brain. Psychological Science. 1995;6(2):76–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1467–9280.1995.tb00310.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gauthier I, Tarr MJ. Becoming a ‘Greeble’ expert: Exploring mechanisms for face recognition. Vision Research. 1997;37:1673–1681. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(96)00286-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gauthier I, Tarr MJ. Unraveling mechanisms for expert 32 object recognition: bridging Brain activity and behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2002;28:431–446. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Gauthier I, Williams P, Tarr MJ, Tanaka J. Training ‘greeble’ experts: A framework for studying expert object recognition processes. Vision Research. 1998;38:2401–2428. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00442-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Glascher J, Adolphs R. Processing of the arousal of subliminal and supraliminal emotional stimuli by the human amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23(32):10274. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-32-10274.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Grill-Spector K, Henson R, Martin A. Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006;10(1):14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Güntürkün O, Kesch S. Visual lateralization during feeding in pigeons. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1987;101(3):433–435. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.101.3.433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Güntürkün O, Diekamp B, Manns M, Nottelmann F, Prior H, Schwarz A, Skiba M. Asymmetry pays: visual lateralization improves discrimination success in pigeons. Current Biology. 2000;10(17):1079–1081. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(00)00671-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Han S, Weaver JA, Murray SO, Kang X, Yund EW, Woods DL. Hemispheric asymmetry in global/local processing: Effects of stimulus position and spatial frequency. NeuroImage. 2002;17(3):1290–1299. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Harmer CJ, Mackay CE, Reid CB, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM. Antidepressant drug treatment modifies the neural processing of nonconscious threat cues. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;59(9):816–820. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Harmon-Jones E. On the relationship of frontal brain activity and anger: Examining the role of attitude toward anger. Cognition & Emotion. 2004a;18(3):337–361. [Google Scholar]
  52. Harmon-Jones E. Contributions from research on anger and cognitive dissonance to understanding the motivational functions of asymmetrical frontal brain activity. Biological Psychology. 2004b;67(1–2):51–76. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Harmon-Jones E. Clarifying the emotive functions of asymmetrical frontal cortical activity. Psychophysiology. 2003;40(6):838–848. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Harrington A. Unfinished business: Models of laterality in the nineteenth century. In: Davidson RJ, Hughdahl K, editors. Brain asymmetry. Cambridge, MA, US: The MIT Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  55. Haxby J, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI. The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2000;4(6):223–233. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01482-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Herrington JD, Taylor JM, Grupe DW, Curby KM, Schultz RT. Bidirectional communication between amygdala and fusiform gyrus during facial recognition. NeuroImage. 2012;56:2348–2355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Hidalgo RB, Davidson JRT. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2000;14(1):70–76. doi: 10.1177/026988110001400110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Holyoak KJ, Hummel JE. The Proper Treatment of Symbols in a Connectionist Architecture. In: Dietrich E, Markman AB, editors. Cognitive dynamics: Conceptual and representational change in humans and machines. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2000. pp. 229–263. [Google Scholar]
  59. Hugdahl K. Classical conditions and implicit learning: The right hemisphere hypothesis. In: Davidson RJ, Hughdahl K, editors. Brain asymmetry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1995. pp. 235–267. [Google Scholar]
  60. Hummel JE. Where view-based theories break down: The role of structure in shape perception and object recognition. Cognitive dynamics: Conceptual change in humans and machines. 2000:157–185. [Google Scholar]
  61. Hummel JE. The Complementary Properties of Holistic and Analytic Representations of Shape. Perception of faces, objects, and scenes: Analytic and holistic processes. 2003:212. [Google Scholar]
  62. Jolicoeur P, Gluck MA, Kosslyn SM. Pictures and names: Making the connection. Cognitive Psychology. 1984;16(2):243–275. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(84)90009-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kensinger EA, Choi ES. When Side Matters: Hemispheric Processing and the Visual Specificity of Emotional Memories. Learning, Memory. 2009;35(1):247–253. doi: 10.1037/a0013414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Kilpatrick LA, Zald DH, Pardo JV, Cahill LF. Sex-related differences in amygdala functional connectivity during resting conditions. Neuroimage. 2006;30(2):452–461. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Kim JG, Biederman I, Lescroart MD, Hayworth KJ. Adaptation to objects in the lateral occipital complex (LOC): shape or semantics? Vision Research. 2009;49(18):2297–2305. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.06.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Koenigs M, Grafman J. Post-traumatic stress disorder: The role of medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala. The Neuroscientist: a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry. 2009;15(5):540. doi: 10.1177/1073858409333072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Koenigs M, Huey ED, Raymont V, Cheon B, Solomon J, Wassermann EM, Grafman J. Focal brain damage protects against post-traumatic stress disorder in combat veterans. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;11(2):232–237. doi: 10.1038/nn2032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. van der Kolk BA, Fisler R. Dissociation and the fragmentary nature of traumatic memories: Overview and exploratory study. Journal of traumatic stress. 1995;8(4):505–525. doi: 10.1007/BF02102887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. van der Kolk B, Hopper J, Osterman J. Exploring the Nature of Traumatic Memory. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma. 2001;4(2):9–31. doi: 10.1300/J146v04n02_02. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. Koopman C, Classen C, Cardenta E, Spiegel D. When disaster strikes, acute stress disorder may follow. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1995;8(1):29–46. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490080103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Koss MP, Figueredo AJ, Bell I, Tharan M, Tromp S. Traumatic memory characteristics: a cross-validated mediational model of response to rape among employed women. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1996;105(3):421–432. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.105.3.421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Koutstaal W, Wagner AD, Rotte M, Maril A, Buckner RL, Schacter DL. Perceptual specificity in visual object priming: functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a laterality difference in fusiform cortex. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39(2):184–199. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00087-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Kramer JH, Ellenberg L, Leonard J, Share LJ. Developmental sex differences in global-local perceptual bias. Neuropsychology. 1996;10(3):402–407. [Google Scholar]
  74. Large M, Aldcroft A, Vilis T. Task-related laterality effects in the lateral occipital complex. Brain Research. 2007;1128(1):130–138. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.10.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Larson CL, Aronoff J, Stearns JJ. The shape of threat: Simple geometric forms evoke rapid and sustained capture of attention. Emotion. 2007;7(3):526–534. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Larson CL, Aronoff J, Sarinopoulos IC, Zhu DC. Recognizing threat: A simple geometric shape activates neural circuitry for threat detection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009:1–13. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21111. (Early Access) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Lee HJ, Lee MS, Kang RH, Kim H, Kim SD, Kee BS, Kim YH, et al. Influence of the serotonin transporter promoter gene polymorphism on susceptibility to posttraumatic stress disorder. Depression and Anxiety. 2005;21(3):135–139. doi: 10.1002/da.20064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Liberzon I, Taylor SF, Amdur R, Jung TD, Chamberlain KR, Minoshima S, Koeppe RA, et al. Brain activation in PTSD in response to trauma-related stimuli. Biological Psychiatry. 1999;45(7):817–826. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00246-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Lippolis G, Bisazza A, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G. Lateralisation of predator avoidance responses in three species of toads. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition. 2002;7(2):163–183. doi: 10.1080/13576500143000221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Lissek S, Biggs AL, Rabin SJ, Cornwell BR, Alvarez RP, Pine DS, Grillon C. Generalization of conditioned fear-potentiated startle in humans: Experimental validation and clinical relevance. Behavior Research and Therapy. 2008;46:678–687. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Lissek S, Rabin SJ, McDowell DJ, Dvir S, Bradford DE, Geraci M, Pine DS, Grillon C. Impaired discriminative fear-conditioning resulting from elevated fear responding to learned safety cues among individuals with panic disorder. Behavior Research and Therapy. 2009;47:111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Logothetis NK, Sheinberg DL. Visual object recognition. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1996;19:577–621. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.19.030196.003045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Markowitsch HJ. Differential contribution of right and left amygdala to affective information processing. Behavioural Neurology. 1998;11(4):233–244. doi: 10.1155/1999/180434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Marshall GN, Schell TL. Reappraising the link between peritraumatic dissociation and PTSD symptom severity: Evidence from a longitudinal study of community violence survivors. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111(4):626–636. doi: 10.1037//0021-843X.111.4.626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Marsolek CJ. Abstract visual-form representations in the left cerebral hemisphere. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance. 1995;21(2):375–386. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.21.2.375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Marsolek CJ. Dissociable neural subsystems underlie abstract and specific object recognition. Psychological Science. 1999;10(2):111–118. [Google Scholar]
  87. Marsolek CJ. Abstractionist versus exemplar-based theories of visual word priming: A subsystems resolution. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2004;57:1233–1259. doi: 10.1080/02724980343000747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Marsolek CJ, Burgund ED. Computational analyses and hemispheric asymmetries in visual-form recognition. In: Christman S, editor. Cerebral asymmetries in sensory and perceptual processing. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1997. pp. 125–158. [Google Scholar]
  89. Marsolek CJ, Burgund ED. Visual recognition and priming of incomplete objects: The influence of stimulus and task demands. In: Bowers JS, Marsolek CJ, editors. Rethinking implicit memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  90. Marsolek CJ, Burgund ED. Dissociable neural subsystems underlie visual working memory for abstract categories and specific exemplars. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2008;8(1):17–24. doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.1.17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Marsolek CJ, Hudson TE. Task and stimulus demands influence letter-case specific priming in the right cerebral hemisphere. Laterality. 1999;4:127–147. doi: 10.1080/713754331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Marsolek CJ, Nicholas CD, Andresen DR. Interhemispheric communication of abstract and specific visual-form information. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:1983–1999. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00065-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Marsolek CJ, Schacter DL, Nicholas CD. Form-specific visual priming for new associations in the right cerebral hemisphere. Memory and Cognition. 1996;24:539–556. doi: 10.3758/bf03201082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Maxwell JS, Shackman AJ, Davidson RJ. Unattended Facial Expressions Asymmetrically Bias the Concurrent Processing of Nonemotional Information. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2005;17(9):1386–1395. doi: 10.1162/0898929054985437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Maxwell JS, Shackman AJ, McMenamin BW, Greischar LL, Davidson RJ. Stress potentiates early and attenuates late stages of visual processing. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:1156–1161. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3384-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. McDonald AJ. Cortical pathways to the mammalian amygdala. Progress in Neurobiology. 1998;55(3):257–332. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(98)00003-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. McMenamin BW, Trask J, Radue J, Huskamp K, Kerstn DK, Marsolek CJ. The diagnosticity of color for emotional objects. Motivation & Emotion. doi: 10.1007/s11031-012-9319-0. (In Press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Mervis CB, Rosch E. Categorization of natural objects. Annual review of psychology. 1981;32(1):89–115. [Google Scholar]
  99. Mervis CB, Crisafi MA. Order of Acquisition of Subordinate-, Basic-, and Superordinate-Level Categories. Child Development. 1982;53:258–266. [Google Scholar]
  100. Morris JS, Öhman A, Dolan RJ. Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature. 1998;393(6684):467–470. doi: 10.1038/30976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Morris JS, Öhman A, Dolan RJ. A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1999;96(4):1680–1685. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.4.1680. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Morris PL, Robinson RG, Raphael B, Hopwood MJ. Lesion location and poststroke depression. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 1996;8(4):399. doi: 10.1176/jnp.8.4.399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Murphy FC, Nimmo-Smith I, Lawrence AD. Functional neuroanatomy of emotions: A meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;3(3):207–233. doi: 10.3758/CABN.3.3.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Palmeri TJ. Exemplar similarity and the development of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 1997;23:324–354. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.23.2.324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage. 2002;16(2):331–348. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Phelps EA, O’Connor KJ, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Grillon C, Davis M. Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear. Nature Neuroscience. 2001;4(4):437–441. doi: 10.1038/86110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Pobric G, Schweinberger SR, Lavidor M. Magnetic stimulation of the right visual cortex impairs form-specific priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2007;19:1013–1020. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.1013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Rainnie DG, Ressler KJ. Physiology of the Amygdala: Implications for PTSD. In: Shiromani PJ, LeDoux JE, Keane TM, editors. Post-traumatic stress disorder: basic science and clinical practice. 2009. [Google Scholar]
  109. Rauch SL, Whalen PJ, Shin LM, McInerney SC, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, Orr SP, et al. Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: A functional MRI study. Psychiatry. 2000;47:769–776. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00828-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Roalf D, Lowery N, Turetsky BI. Behavioral and physiological findings of gender differences in global-local visual processing. Brain and Cognition. 2006;60(1):32–42. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Robins A, Rogers LJ. Lateralized prey-catching responses in the cane toad, Bufo marinus: analysis of complex visual stimuli. Animal Behaviour. 2004;68(4):767–775. [Google Scholar]
  112. Rogers L. Evolution of Hemispheric Specialization: Advantages and Disadvantages. Brain and Language. 2000;73(2):236–253. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Rogers LJ, Zucca P, Vallortigara G. Advantages of having a lateralized brain. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2004;271(Suppl_6):S420–S422. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2004.0200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Rolls E. Emotion explained. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  115. Rosch E. Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1975;104(3):192–233. [Google Scholar]
  116. Rosch E, Mervis CB. Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive psychology. 1975;7(4):573–605. [Google Scholar]
  117. Rosch E, Mervis CB, Gray WD, Johnson DM, Boyes-Braem P. Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive psychology. 1976;8(3):382–439. [Google Scholar]
  118. Sabatinelli D, Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Costa VD, Keil A. The timing of emotional discrimination in human amygdala and ventral visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(47):14864. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3278-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Sackeim HA, Greenberg MS, Weiman AL, Gur RC, Hungerbuhler JP, Geschwind N. Hemispheric asymmetry in the expression of positive and negative emotions: neurologic evidence. Archives of Neurology. 1982;39(4):210. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1982.00510160016003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Sackeim HA, Gur RC, Saucy MC. Emotions are expressed more intensely on the left side of the face. Science. 1978;202(4366):434–6. doi: 10.1126/science.705335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Savic I, Lindström P. PET and MRI show differences in cerebral asymmetry and functional connectivity between homo- and heterosexual subjects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2008;105(27):9403–9408. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801566105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Schechtman E, Laufer O, Paz R. Negative valence widens generalization of learning. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(31):10460–4. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2377-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Seedat S, Stein DJ, Ziervogel C, Middleton T, Kaminer D, Emsley RA, Rossouw W. Comparison of response to a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor in children, adolescents, and adults with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology. 2002;12(1):37–46. doi: 10.1089/10445460252943551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Sergerie K, Chochol C, Armony JL. The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008;32(4):811–830. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Shackman AJ, McMenamin BW, Maxwell JS, Greischar LL, Davidson RJ. Right Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortical Activity and Behavioral Inhibition. Psychological Science. 2009;20(12):1500–1506. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02476.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Shin LM, McNally RJ, Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Rauch SL, Alpert NM, Metzger LJ, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow during script-driven imagery in childhood sexual abuse-related PTSD: a PET investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156(4):575. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.4.575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Shin LM, Orr SP, Carson MA, Rauch SL, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, Peters PM, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow in the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex during traumatic imagery in male and female Vietnam veterans with PTSD. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61(2):168. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.2.168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Simons JS, Koutstaal W, Prince S, Wagner AD, Schacter DL. Neural mechanisms of visual object priming: Evidence for perceptual and semantic distinctions in fusiform cortex. Neuroimage. 2003;19(3):613–626. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00096-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Smith EE, Jonides J, Koeppe RA. Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY: 1991) 1996;6(1):11–20. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.1.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Smith SD, Abou-Khalil B, Zald DH. Posttraumatic stress disorder in a patient with no left amygdala. Journal of abnormal psychology. 2008;117(2):479. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Spence S, Shapiro D, Zaidel E. The role of the right hemisphere in the physiological and cognitive components of emotional processing. Psychophysiology. 1996;33(2):112–122. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb02115.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Stankiewicz BJ, Hummel JE. Automatic priming for translation-and scale-invariant representations of object shape. Visual Cognition. 2002;9(6):719–739. [Google Scholar]
  133. Stankiewicz BJ, Hummel JE, Cooper EE. The role of attention in priming for left-right reflections of object images: Evidence for a dual representation of object shape. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance. 1998;24(3):732–744. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.24.3.732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Starkstein SE, Robinson RG, Honig MA, Parikh RM, Joselyn J, Price TR. Mood changes after right-hemisphere lesions. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 1989;155(1):79. doi: 10.1192/bjp.155.1.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Stein MB, McQuaid JR, Pedrelli P, Lenox R, McCahill ME. Posttraumatic stress disorder in the primary care medical setting. General Hospital Psychiatry. 2000;22(4):261–269. doi: 10.1016/s0163-8343(00)00080-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Stutzmann GE, LeDoux JE. GABAergic antagonists block the inhibitory effects of serotonin in the lateral amygdala: a mechanism for modulation of sensory inputs related to fear conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19(11):8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-j0005.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Sullivan RM, Dufresne MM, Waldron J. Lateralized sex difference in stress-induced dopamine release in the rat. NeuroReport. 2009;20(3):229. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283196b3e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Sutton SK, Davidson RJ. Prefrontal Brain Asymmetry: A Biological Substrate of the Behavioral Approach and Inhibition Systems. Psychological Science. 1997;8(3):204–210. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00413.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  139. Talarico JM, Rubin DC. Confidence, not consistency, characterizes flashbulb memories. Psychological Science. 2003;14(5):455. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.02453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Tanaka JW, Taylor M. Object categories and expertise: Is the basic level in the eye of the beholder? Cognitive Psychology. 1991;23:457–482. [Google Scholar]
  141. Tanaka JW. The entry point of face recognition: Evidence for face expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2001;130:534–543. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.130.3.534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Ullman S. High-level vision. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  143. Vaidya CJ, Gabrieli JDE, Verfaellie M, Fleischman D, Askari N. Font-specific priming following global amnesia and occipital lobe damage. Neuropsychology. 1998;12:183–192. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.2.183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ, Bisazza A, Lippolis G, Robins A. Complementary right and left hemifield use for predatory and agonistic behaviour in toads. Neuroreport. 1998;9(14):3341–3344. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199810050-00035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Ventolini N, Ferrero EA, Sponza S, Della Chiesa A, Zucca P, Vallortigara G. Laterality in the wild: preferential hemifield use during predatory and sexual behaviour in the black-winged stilt. Animal Behaviour. 2005;69(5):1077–1084. [Google Scholar]
  146. Vigneau M, Jobard G, Mazoyer B, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Word and non-word reading: what role for the Visual Word Form Area? NeuroImage. 2005;27(3):694–705. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nature neuroscience. 2003;6(6):624–631. doi: 10.1038/nn1057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Vuilleumier P, Henson RN, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Multiple levels of visual object constancy revealed by event-related fMRI of repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience. 2002;5(5):491–499. doi: 10.1038/nn839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Vuilleumier P. How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9(12):585–594. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Wager TD, Phan KL, Liberzon I, Taylor SF. Valence, gender, and lateralization of functional brain anatomy in emotion: a meta-analysis of findings from neuroimaging. Neuroimage. 2003;19(3):513–531. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00078-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Wong ACN, Palmeri TJ, Gauthier I. Conditions for facelike expertise with objects: Becoming a Ziggerin expert - but which type? Psychological Science. 2009;20:1108–1117. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02430.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Wright CI, Fischer H, Whalen PJ, McInerney SC, Shin LM, Rauch SL. Differential prefrontal cortex and amygdala habituation to repeatedly presented emotional stimuli. Neuroreport. 2001;12(2):379. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200102120-00039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Zald DH. The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Research Reviews. 2003;41(1):88–123. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(02)00248-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES