Abstract
Functional brain activity and connectivity have been studied by calculating intersubject and seed-based correlations of hemodynamic data acquired with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). To inspect temporal dynamics, these correlation measures have been calculated over sliding time windows with necessary restrictions on the length of the temporal window that compromises the temporal resolution. Here, we show that it is possible to increase temporal resolution by using instantaneous phase synchronization (PS) as a measure of dynamic (time-varying) functional connectivity. We applied PS on an fMRI dataset obtained while 12 healthy volunteers watched a feature film. Narrow frequency band (0.04–0.07 Hz) was used in the PS analysis to avoid artifactual results. We defined three metrics for computing time-varying functional connectivity and time-varying intersubject reliability based on estimation of instantaneous PS across the subjects: (1) seed-based PS, (2) intersubject PS, and (3) intersubject seed-based PS. Our findings show that these PS-based metrics yield results consistent with both seed-based correlation and intersubject correlation methods when inspected over the whole time series, but provide an important advantage of maximal single-TR temporal resolution. These metrics can be applied both in studies with complex naturalistic stimuli (e.g., watching a movie or listening to music in the MRI scanner) and more controlled (e.g., event-related or blocked design) paradigms. A MATLAB toolbox FUNPSY (http://becs.aalto.fi/bml/software.html) is openly available for using these metrics in fMRI data analysis.
Key words: circular statistics, dynamic functional connectivity, fMRI, instantaneous, nonlinear time series analysis, phase synchronization
Introduction
In cognitive neuroscience, there is growing interest toward the use of highly complex naturalistic stimuli, for example, movies, narrated stories, and music, to increase ecological validity of neuroimaging studies and to enable new type of research on emotions and higher-order cognitive functions (Alluri et al., 2012; Brennan et al., 2010; Hasson et al., 2004; Jääskeläinen et al., 2008). In their seminal article, Hasson et al. (2004) showed that when subjects are watching a feature movie, blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal time series in various brain structures of different subjects exhibit strong similarity and quantified it with intersubject correlation (ISC). To assess the dynamics of ISC, time-varying ISC can be computed over sliding temporal windows—at the expense of reduced temporal resolution—to identify the triggering event in a rich dynamic stimulus.
Indeed, it can be argued that synchronization, which the ISC measure reflects, is one of the most common ways of sharing information between entities of a system (Mesulam, 1990; Strogatz, 2004). Synchronization has been studied with both linear (temporal correlation, spectral coherence) and nonlinear (mutual information, phase synchronization [PS]) methods (Quian Quiroga et al., 2002).
PS is a specific synchronization measure that was initially introduced in physics when studying the behavior of two weakly coupled oscillators (Rosenblum et al., 1996). The original idea was to compare two signals by first separating their instantaneous amplitude from instantaneous phase information and then compare only phase time series. This is achieved by converting the real signal into its complex analytic version (Boashash, 1992) with signal processing techniques, like the Hilbert transform, Gabor expansion, or Wavelet filtering (for comparisons see Le Van Quyen et al., 2001; Schack and Weiss, 2005; Sun and Small, 2009).
While PS is a relatively well-established tool in magnetoencephalography (MEG)/electroencephalography (EEG) research, its usefulness in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) analysis has been explored only in a handful of studies. Laird et al. (2002) used PS based on Hilbert transform to identify synchronization of BOLD responses during a finger tapping experiment, demonstrating the potential of the method compared with the general linear model. Deshmukh et al. (2004) also used PS analysis in a finger tapping fMRI experiment to determine clusters of functionally connected brain areas. However, neither of these studies applied narrowband filtering of data, which is a necessary requirement when working with instantaneous phases—this follows from the Bedrosian's theorem (see the Methods section of the present article and Sun and Small, 2009).
More recently, Kitzbichler et al. (2009) investigated the power-law scaling of synchronization metrics in the human brain for simulated and recorded MEG and fMRI “resting state” data. They explored the inter-regional temporal dynamics of PS derived from wavelet coefficients of BOLD signal. However, the authors failed to consider possible artifacts arising from pulse and breathing that contaminate the relatively high frequency bands of BOLD activity that were explored. Further, the choice of an arbitrary phase difference threshold (π/4) was not statistically justified. In addition there are three other studies that have explored the dynamics of the complex phase of the BOLD signal during rest using wavelet coherence phase (Chang and Glover, 2010; Müller et al., 2004) and empirical mode decomposition (Niazy et al., 2011).
Despite these attempts, instantaneous PS has not gained popularity in fMRI data analysis. One possible reason for this is that when conventional controlled paradigms (e.g., event related or blocked design) are used, it is customary to look at signal properties that remain stable over the entire block or several repetitions, rather than instantaneous activations. However, the instantaneous characteristic of PS becomes handy when high temporal resolution is critical, such as when inspecting which aspects of a highly complex and dynamic stimulus relate to hemodynamic brain responses.
Dynamic functional connectivity measured with fMRI
Characterizing connectivity in the brain is important not only to gain a better understanding of brain functions (Jirsa and McIntosh, 2007), but also to classify clinical populations (Lynall et al., 2010) and monitor their recovery (Nakamura et al., 2009). Compared with anatomical or with effective connectivity (Friston, 1994), functional connectivity is just a measure of the similarity between activity in two brain areas, allowing a data-driven approach in the analysis of the connections. Various model-free measures of functional connectivity based on fMRI data exist (Margulies et al., 2010; Smith et al., 2011) with seed-based correlation (SBC) being one of the most used and straightforward methods for studying connectivity with low amount of assumptions and parameters. Measures of fMRI functional connectivity are usually time independent, examining connectivity between regions of interest over the whole scanning session, by computing pairwise temporal correlation coefficient between BOLD signal time series from the regions. It is only recently that the temporal dynamics of resting state fMRI connectivity have been explored with computational models (Cabral et al., 2011; Honey et al., 2007) and with real data using sliding time window correlation (Majeed et al., 2011), sliding time window ICA (Kiviniemi et al., 2011), wavelet coherence (Chang and Glover, 2010), partial least squares (Grigg and Grady, 2010), and Kalman filtering (Kang et al., 2011).
It is also of interest to study how connectivity changes due to stimuli or task demands in a model-free manner. Sakoğlu et al. (2010) addressed the issue of dynamic functional connectivity during task modulation. The authors defined Dynamic Functional Network Connectivity as the functional connectivity over a sliding time window. Various time window widths were compared but it was not possible to reduce the temporal window under a critical sample size (64 samples in this case) without compromising the reliability of the resulting correlation value (Fisher, 1921); shorter temporal windows (i.e., too few samples) will produce biased estimates. Longer time windows increase reliability at the expense of temporal resolution.
PS as a measure of dynamic functional connectivity
Here, we hypothesized that it is possible to solve the time resolution/reliability trade-off by adopting PS as an instantaneous measure of dynamic functional connectivity during stimulation. We specifically hypothesized that instantaneous PS is a reliable measure comparable to correlation-based methods (SBC and ISC) but with maximal temporal resolution, enabling assessment of time-varying functional connectivity in a model-free fashion. The aim of this study is to introduce a set of metrics to evaluate functional connectivity of fMRI time series with PS, test the statistical significance of connections, and compare the PS metrics with established correlation-based methods. We expected that there are several advantages in using instantaneous phase as a measure of functional connectivity: (1) PS is not affected by intersubject amplitude variability (i.e., the phase captures the temporal dynamics); (2) PS instantly identifies full temporal dynamics without any need for time-windowed averaging; (3) PS is a nonlinear measure, which can be more suitable for identifying complex dynamic processes in the brain (Pereda et al., 2005); (4) PS is computationally faster than temporal correlation: with one transform we obtain the phase value for each time point for the whole brain, and PS is then a sum of complex numbers over the unit circle.
Methods
The analytic signal, instantaneous phase, and instantaneous frequency
The analytic representation of a real valued signal x(t) is a complex signal xa(t) with the same Fourier transform of x(t) but defined only for positive frequencies. The analytic signal can be built from the real signal by using the Hilbert transform:
(1) |
Where H[•] is the Hilbert transform and j is the imaginary unit. The main advantages of using the analytic signal are that, given some real data represented by one function of time, we can determine two functions of time to better access meaningful properties of the signal. We consider now a narrowband signal that can be written as an amplitude-modulated low-pass signal a(t) with carrier frequency expressed by φ(t):
(2) |
If the Bedrosian's theorem (Bedrosian, 1962) is respected by a(t) and cos[ϕ(t)] (i.e., if the Fourier transforms of the two signals have separate supports) then the analytic signal of a narrowband signal can be rewritten as the product of two meaningful components:
(3) |
Where a(t) is the instantaneous envelope and ϕ(t) the instantaneous phase. The Bedrosian's theorem has a clear implication: the narrower the bandwidth of the signal of interest, the better does the Hilbert transform produce an analytic signal with meaningful envelope and phase. Adopting a band-pass filtered version of the BOLD time series improves the separation between the phase and envelope spectra. The information from the original signal is hence redistributed in the phase component for frequencies in the pass-band and in the amplitude component at low frequencies out of pass-band. For an exhaustive review of the mathematical and practical aspects of the analytic signal and its applications please refer to Boashash (1992), and Sun and Small (2009).
BOLD signal as a narrowband signal
The frequency spectrum of fMRI BOLD signal has been extensively studied since the early days of fMRI (Weisskoff et al., 1993). In 1996 Biswal et al. studied the effects of cardiac and respiratory rates on the BOLD signal with typical frequencies around 1–2 Hz and 0.3 Hz, respectively. Since fMRI data are often collected with time to repetition (TR) in the order of 2 sec (0.25 Hz Nyquist frequency), respiratory and cardiac frequencies are out of band, causing aliasing in the higher frequency range of the BOLD signal (>0.1 Hz). The lower end of the BOLD spectrum (0.0–0.015 Hz) is also not immune to noise such as “low frequency drift” (Smith et al. 1999). It is nowadays common practice to band-pass filter the BOLD signal between 0.01–0.08 Hz (Biswal et al., 1995; Buckner et al., 2009; Zou et al., 2008), but even within this frequency range there is noise contamination. Wise et al. (2004) investigated the spontaneous fluctuations in arterial carbon dioxide level mainly affecting the BOLD signal in the frequency range 0.0–0.05 Hz. Beckmann et al. (2005) and Birn et al. (2006) reported a noise component around 0.03 Hz attributed to respiratory-related fluctuations when a slow TR is used. Recently Zuo et al. (2010) explored the BOLD signal at sub-bands relevant in electrophysiological DC and intracranial recordings (Penttonen and Buzsáki, 2003): slow-5 (0.01–0.027 Hz), slow-4 (0.027–0.073 Hz), slow-3 (0.073–0.198 Hz), and slow-2 (0.198–0.25 Hz). The slow-3 and slow-2 bands were mainly mapped to the white matter and attributed to aliased respiratory and cardiac signals. Slow-4 and slow-5 bands were mainly identified in the gray matter, with slow-4 more prominent in the thalamus, basal ganglia, and sensorimotor regions while slow-5 was more prominent in ventromedial cortical areas. Slow-4 was the most reliable sub-band with more widespread spatial distribution of reliable voxels. A similar band (0.03–0.06 Hz) was also reported to give greater small-world topology when considering resting state data with complex network tools (Achard et al., 2006).
Based on these studies, we chose the frequency range of ∼0.04–0.07 Hz, equivalent to the slow-4 band minus the 0.03 Hz critical frequency. Figure 1 shows a graphical summary of the frequency bands addressed in the mentioned literature and our chosen sub-band.
Metrics of PS
We define three different metrics to analyze the PS of fMRI data (see Fig. 2).
Seed-based phase synchronization (SBPS): within-subject functional synchrony;
Intersubject phase synchronization (IPS): between-subject synchrony;
Intersubject seed-based phase synchronization (ISBPS): within- and between-subject functional synchrony.
Seed-based PS
SBPS is a group measure of functional connectivity, taking into account possible individual differences within each subject.
Let ϕs,1(t) and ϕs,2(t) be the phase signals for subject s on two different brain regions (voxels or regions of interest [ROI]). We compute the differential phase for subject s as follows:
(4) |
We define the seed-based phase coefficient as follows:
(5) |
Where . Its absolute value—the resultant vector length (Jammalamadaka and Sengupta, 2001)—is a measure of circular dispersion of phases over the unit circle, which also corresponds to the phase locking value (Lachaux et al., 1999) commonly used in neuroscience. By looking at the magnitude of the complex signal SBPC(t) multiplied by the cosine of the mean angle we obtain the SBPS as follows:
(6) |
where
(7) |
Equation 7 is the group mean angle of all the individual phase differences. The range of SBPS is continuous between −1 and 1, that is, accounting also for possible anti-correlations. Equation 6 can be also re-written as the Real part of Equation 5.
Intersubject PS
IPS is a measure of reliability of the data. Being a measure of synchrony between subjects for each voxel, IPS gives similar results as ISC with the advantage that instantaneous information is intrinsically present.
First, the intersubject phase coefficient for a given voxel is computed.
(8) |
Where is the number of subjects. The absolute value of IPC gives the IPS in time.
(9) |
The range of IPS is continuous between 0 and 1. An alternative coefficient called pairwise phase consistency (PPC) (Vinck et al., 2010) can also be adopted to compute intersubject phase synchrony. PPC is comparable to the pairwise ISC coefficient described by Kauppi et al. (2010). It has the advantage that it is less affected by potential bias due to a small number of subjects. PPC is defined as follows:
(10) |
where
(11) |
with d(ϕj, ϕk) being the absolute angular distance between two angles. As noted in the original article (Vinck et al., 2010), one possible disadvantage of using PPC is that small negative values are possible; this is a consequence of the unbiasedness of the PPC.
Intersubject seed-based PS
ISBPS is a measure of functional synchrony, where we require all subjects to have the same phase value for the selected brain regions (voxels or ROIs).
Given
(12) |
Where is the number of subjects and is the number of brain regions (two regions only in our pairwise comparisons). The absolute value of ISBPC gives the ISBPS in time.
(13) |
The range of ISBPS is continuous between 0 and 1.
Statistical significance of fMRI phase synchrony
Statistical significance can be estimated with parametric and nonparametric tests. With parametric tests the null hypothesis is that the phases (or the phase differences for SBPS) have a uniform distribution around the unit circle, while the alternative hypothesis is that the values have a von Mises distribution, the circular equivalent of a normal distribution. Statistical significance for IPS and ISBPS is then computed with the Rayleigh test for each time point (Fisher, 1995; Mardia and Jupp, 2000). For SBPS, since the alternative hypothesis has a known mean phase equal to zero, the most suitable parametric test is the V test (Zar, 1999). However, the most reliable results are obtained with nonparametric permutation tests (Good, 2005; Nichols and Holmes, 2002), where no assumptions are made on the distributions of the null and alternative hypotheses, solving at the same time the multiple comparison problem.
In permutation testing, it is a common approach to create surrogate data by simulating or re-sampling the original time series, for example, by preserving the magnitude of the frequency spectrum of the time series while randomizing the Fourier phases (Dolan and Spano, 2001), or by re-sampling via wavelet as in Laird et al. (2002) or with twin surrogates (Thiel et al., 2006) specifically designed for phase synchrony tests. However, generating surrogate time series with similar spectra or similar statistics might destroy the actual complex nonlinear temporal dependencies between time samples. To circumvent this potential problem, it is possible to use bootstrap re-sampling, with surrogate data obtained by applying random circular shifting to the original time series for each permutation (Politis and Romano, 1992).
Temporal averages of phase synchrony metrics
While all the proposed metrics are time varying, it is also useful to infer average functional values from the dynamic data. The simplest scenario is by calculating a percentage of time points that are significantly phase synchronized over the whole duration of the scanning. This approach was adopted by Ville (1948) in the early days of studying analytic signal properties. He demonstrated that the average instantaneous frequency corresponds to the temporal average frequency of the signal. In the present case this implies that by adopting PS as a functional measure, we can also study global PS by considering the average PS over the whole time series.
Data and comparison of methods
To test the new metrics proposed in this study, we used a naturalistic fMRI dataset obtained from 12 healthy subjects who watched a feature movie in a 3T fMRI scanner with TR=2 sec. Standard preprocessing steps were performed with FSL (for more details, see Lahnakoski et al., 2012) and all subjects were co-registered to the MNI 152 two-millimeter template. We performed two types of analyses using ROIs and whole-brain data. For ROI analysis, we used a template with 264 functional areas (Power et al., 2011), with a sphere of radius of 4 mm for each ROI. Visualizations are done for two areas in the right visual cortex: V1/V2 (template area 140, MNI coordinates [8, −91, −7]) and MT/V5+(template area 161, MNI coordinates [42, −66, −8]).
IPS was compared with ISC measured with temporal sliding windows with lengths varying from 4 to 32 samples (i.e., 8 to 64 sec). The ISC curves were obtained by computing pairwise ISCs between subjects and then averaging the upper triangle entries of the correlation matrix. Whole-brain temporal average IPS was compared with ISC over the whole time series. We also computed the Euclidean distance between IPS and ISC time series for each voxel to localize the differences between the two approaches.
SBPS was compared with sliding window group temporal correlation between all pairs of regions of interest for a total of 34716 pairs. Group temporal correlation time series is obtained by first computing individual sliding window temporal correlation time series between the two regions, and then averaging the Fisher-transformed correlation values to obtain group data (for a similar approach, see Buckner et al., 2009). The center time point of each time window is the value of temporal reference for comparisons.
To quantify the effect of the size of the sliding windows versus the phase-based measures, we computed the average percentage of significant time points across all 264 regions in the ISC/IPS case and across all 34716 links for SBC/SBPS. Since ISBPS does not have a correlation-based equivalent, it was only compared with SBPS and IPS for the two selected regions. Further, to verify that the amplitude part of the analytic signal did not contain relevant information, we also computed ISC and SBC using the instantaneous envelope time series. All comparisons are carried out on band-pass filtered BOLD time series with frequencies 0.04–0.07 Hz, identical to the one used to compute the analytic signal. We adopted the Parks-McClellan algorithm (McClellan et al., 1973) for optimum design of linear phase FIR filter (0.01 weight in pass-band, attenuation of ∼30 dB in stop-band, and transition band of ∼0.015 Hz). Due to the group delay of the FIR filter and its transient, the filtered signal delay is compensated and the first N-1 filtered samples are discarded (Oppenheim and Schafer, 2009). All statistical analyses were carried out with nonparametric testing using bootstrap resampling of original time series to generate surrogate probability distributions for the null hypothesis estimated over at least 0.5 million points (equivalent to 1000 independent permutations for each time series of ∼500 time points). Correction for multiple comparisons was performed using maximal statistic combined with multistep tests (for details see Nichols and Holmes, 2002).
Results
Comparing intersubject PS with ISC
Figure 3A shows the comparison between average IPS and global ISC for the whole brain, thresholded at p<0.01. When calculated over the whole time series, IPS and ISC yield similar estimates of brain areas exhibiting intersubject synchronization. Figure 3B shows IPS and sliding window ISC time series for multiple window sizes in one region of interest (V5/MT+). ISC calculated over a very short four-sample time window shows dynamic changes similar to IPS, but is strongly affected by computational bias, with low mean percentage of significant time points (Fig. 3D). On the other hand, ISC calculated over longer time windows (16 and 32 samples) resembles a moving average of IPS, compromising temporal resolution for the sake of a reliable measure of correlation. ISC over eight samples is closest to IPS both in space (Fig. 3C) and in average number of significant time points (Fig. 3D). In contrast to phase, the instantaneous amplitude of the analytic signal did not seem to contain relevant intersubject information, giving very low percentages of significant time points (Fig. 3D, dot-dashed curves).
Comparing seed-based PS with sliding window SBC
Figure 4A shows the comparison of SBPS calculated over multiple window sizes and sliding window group temporal correlation (SBC) between V1/V2 and V5/MT+ regions of interest. SBC over short time windows approximately follows the SBPS curve despite the high amount of uncertainty due to the shortness of the time window. With longer time windows, correlation values are more reliable, but miss the temporal dynamics of functional connectivity and become more prone to statistical errors. For an example of the latter, the time interval of 600–625 is falsely statistically significant with SBC, because of the two peaks at 600 and 625. These two peaks (marked in Fig. 4A) corresponded to close-up scenes with movement (high SBPS) versus large-angle panoramic scenes (low SBPS) in the movie stimulus. As can be seen, SBPS clearly identifies intervals of synchronization, without suffering the bias of using very short temporal windows. In contrast, the correlation-based methods, when using longer time windows, fail to identify such rapid changes in synchronization, yielding high level of correlation for all temporal intervals.
In Figure 4B the effect of window sizes is quantified as the average percentage of significant time points across all links, compared with SBPS and SBC computed using the instantaneous amplitude of the analytic signal. To achieve a similar amount of significant time points with SBC, one needs to sacrifice temporal resolution using windows of at least 16 samples. Also in this case the amplitude of the analytic signal did not seem to carry relevant information.
Finally, Figure 4C shows a graphical comparison between ISBPS (red) and SBPS (black) for the two sample regions selected and the two IPS curves for each region. As previously noted, this metric is stricter than SBPS since we impose intersubject synchronization for both regions of interest. ISBPS is significantly high only during intervals of intersubject synchronization over the two ROIs (time points 400–420), while SBPS can increase even without ROI intersubject synchronization (time points 420–440).
Discussion
In the present study, we showed that instantaneous PS is a reliable measure comparable to correlation-based methods (SBC and ISC) but with maximal single-TR temporal resolution, thus enabling assessment of time-varying functional connectivity based on fMRI data in a model-free fashion. When comparing IPS with sliding time window ISC, we obtained similar results for windows of 8–10 TRs in length (Fig. 3D), with differences being located in those areas where intersubject synchrony can change rapidly (Fig. 3C). These happen to be the sensory cortical areas with short visual (Hasson et al., 2008) and auditory (Lerner et al., 2011) temporal receptive windows. When comparing SBPS with SBC, sliding windows of at least 16 samples/TRs are needed to reach similar performance (Fig. 4B). Evidently, PS-based methods significantly increase temporal resolution at which dynamic functional connectivity can be estimated. Further, comparison of ISBPS and SBPS demonstrates that SBPS allows detecting functional connectivity even when there is no significant intersubject synchronization within each area.
Improving time-varying analysis
The method proposed here is based on the general concept represented by recently introduced methods, such as ISC or multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA), that focus on spatiotemporal properties of the signal and not on activations over a specific baseline. ISC (Hasson et al., 2010), intuitive and elegant in its simplicity, was an important innovation in fMRI analysis methodology. ISC is a measure of reliability (i.e., how similar the BOLD signals of different subjects are in different parts of the brain when an identical stimulus is used as external source of synchrony). Moreover, ISC does not require a linear relationship between the external stimuli and the BOLD responses. Finally, due to its simplicity, when ISC is compared with more complex techniques (e.g., MVPA), we can still clearly understand its neuronal correlates by going back to the BOLD signal itself rather than looking at intricate multivoxel patterns. With ISC one may study, for instance, how complex stimulus can affect processing at multiple cognitive stages (Lerner et al., 2011) and how brain activity of one person affects brain activity of another person during social interaction (Stephens et al., 2010) without simplifications assumed in linear models. IPS extends the use of ISC by providing more accurate timing of brain events in a reliable way, with the additional requirement of band-pass filtering the BOLD signal. We argue, however, that the part of the BOLD signal not affected by artifacts is intrinsically narrowband, and thus well suited for PS analysis.
Enabling time-varying connectivity analysis
Another important and novel feature of our method is the ability to look at the intersubject reliability of the connectivity between two brain areas with the highest possible temporal resolution; this is necessary especially with complex continuous stimulation, where each volume is comparable to a different experimental condition. SBC groups together many volumes into a time window to compute the average connectivity, missing the complex event that triggered the connection. SBPS on the other hand is intrinsically instantaneous, providing connectivity information for each time point.
Importantly, this allows shifting of intersubjectivity estimates from individual voxels or brain areas (ISC) to connections between voxels and brain areas (SBPS), in other words, from the nodes to the links of the brain network, opening new opportunities to examine the relationship between brain areas with graph-theoretical tools (Sporns, 2010). Further, by adding the temporal accuracy of the PS measures, it is possible to progress from inspection of static to time-varying networks (Holme and Saramäki, 2012). PS is a dynamic measure of functional connectivity; we can evaluate which pairs of brain areas are significantly synchronizing across the subjects and we can also determine when they are synchronizing.
Limitations of phase synchrony approach
Frequency bands of BOLD signal
The necessity of dealing with narrowband signals to compute meaningful PS raises the important question of which BOLD frequency band best captures functionally relevant information. Sub-bands of BOLD have been explored in ISC (Kauppi et al., 2010) and functional connectivity studies (Chang and Glover, 2010). While ISC is more immune to physiological noises, functional connectivity can be greatly affected since within-subject time series are compared where there is a high possibility for synchronous artifacts. Removal of artifactual signals from BOLD time series (e.g., Särkkä et al., 2012) can allow multiple explorations across all the frequency bands of BOLD signals. PS could then be investigated over the whole spectrum with multiple narrowband filters, allowing exploration of within-band and between-band PS, in a similar vein as with EEG and MEG data (Tass et al., 1998). Studies with combined techniques (such as simultaneous fMRI and intracranial recordings) might provide additional information on the existence of meaningful slow sub-bands, corresponding to various processes or connectivity stages, as suggested by early animal studies (Leopold et al., 2003) and more recent resting-state research (Chang and Glover, 2010).
Phase synchrony as a group measure
The proposed PS metrics, like ISC, are measures of reliability over the subjects (i.e., they can only be computed at group level). Although in principle it is possible to compute individual PS connectivity, assessing its statistical significance is challenging due to the lower statistical power; in our measures, statistical power is increased by increasing the number of subjects. Further—like in ISC—when using intersubject group measures, all subjects must be exposed to the same external source of synchronization (i.e., stimulus).
On the instantaneous envelope of the analytic signal
In PS literature, the amplitude part is almost always discarded, as it is assumed to be irrelevant or uncorrelated with the actual phase dynamics. Indeed, we were able to confirm this assumption with our data. Temporal dynamics are contained in the phase part of the analytic signal. However, the issue is far from being resolved in physics and neuroscience. The effect of the amplitude part of the analytic signal has been studied analytically for two oscillators (Xiao-Wen and Zhi-Gang, 2007), concluding that when the envelopes are matched, there is no influence over PS from the amplitude part. A similar conclusion, relevant for neuroscience, was recently reached by Daffertshofer and van Wijk (2011). Using computational modeling, they studied how the amplitude part biases the PS functional connectivity compared with the effective structural connectivity. They also concluded that the amplitudes of the analytic signals do not affect the PS results if the two ROIs have comparable magnitudes. As a general rule, the instantaneous amplitude of the signals should be tested to ensure that it is not relevant to the temporal dynamics, especially when lower frequency bands are explored (e.g., for the slow-5 frequency band, where the spectrum of the phase starts to overlap with the spectrum of the envelope, possibly violating the Bedrosian's theorem).
Conclusions
Our proposed PS method addresses some of the current limitations of time-varying ISC and SBC, and creates the basis for bridging ISC with complex network analysis by shifting the interest toward intersubject synchronization of functional connectivity patterns, rather than properties of single voxels. Despite the necessary mathematical compromise of having to use a narrowband signal, the method proved to be reliable, yielding (1) similar results as ISC when inspected over the whole time course and (2) yielding significantly improved temporal resolution as compared with time-varying ISC and SBC functional connectivity measures. All the methods described in this article have been released publicly as a MATLAB toolbox (FUNPSY, http://becs.aalto.fi/bml/software.html) and an alternative version is available in the ISC toolbox (http://code.google.com/p/isc-toolbox/).
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the aivoAALTO project of the Aalto University and by the Academy of Finland (grant numbers: 129670, 130412, and 138145).
Author Disclosure Statement
No competing financial interests exist.
References
- Achard S. Salvador R. Whitcher B. Suckling J. Bullmore E. A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs. J Neurosci. 2006;26:63–72. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3874-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alluri V. Toiviainen P. Jääskeläinen IP. Glerean E. Sams M. Brattico E. Large-scale brain networks emerge from dynamic processing of musical timbre, key and rhythm. Neuroimage. 2012;59:3677–3689. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann CF. DeLuca M. Devlin JT. Smith SM. Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2005;360:1001–1013. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bedrosian E. A product theorem for Hilbert transforms. Proc IEEE. 1962;74:520–521. doi: 10.1109/PROC.1986.13495. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Birn RM. Smith MA. Bandettini PA. Diamond JB. Separating respiratory-variation-related fluctuations from neuronal-activity-related fluctuations in fMRI. Neuroimage. 2006;31:1536–1548. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biswal B. DeYoe aE. Hyde JS. Reduction of physiological fluctuations in fMRI using digital filters. Magn Reson Med. 1996;35:107–113. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910350114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biswal B. Yetkin FZ. Haughton VM. Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med. 1995;34:537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boashash B. Estimating and interpreting the instantaneous frequency of a signal-part 1: fundamentals. Proc IEEE. 1992;80:520–538. doi: 10.1109/5.135376. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brennan J. Nir Y. Hasson U. Malach R. Heeger DJ. Pylkkänen L. Syntactic structure building in the anterior temporal lobe during natural story listening. Brain Lang. 2010;120:163–173. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner RL. Sepulcre J. Talukdar T. Krienen FM. Liu H. Hedden T, et al. Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci. 2009;29:1860–1873. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5062-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabral J. Hugues E. Sporns O. Deco G. Role of local network oscillations in resting-state functional connectivity. Neuroimage. 2011;57:130–139. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang C. Glover GH. Time-frequency dynamics of resting-state brain connectivity measured with fMRI. Neuroimage. 2010;50:81–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daffertshofer A. van Wijk BCM. On the influence of amplitude on the connectivity between phases. Front Neuroinform. 2011;5:6. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2011.00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deshmukh AV. Shivhare V. Gadre VM. Patkar DP. Pungavkar S. A phase based method for investigating the functional connectivity in the fMRI data. Proc IEEE INDICON. 2004:272–277. doi: 10.1109/INDICO.2004.1497754. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dolan K. Spano M. Surrogate for nonlinear time series analysis. Phys Rev E. 2001;64:1–6. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.64.046128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher NI. Statistical Analysis of Circular Data. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1995. p. 277. [Google Scholar]
- Fisher RA. On the “probable error” of a coefficient of correlation deduced from a small sample. Metron. 1921;1:3–32. [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ. Functional and effective connectivity in neuroimaging: a synthesis. Hum Brain Mapp. 1994;2:56–78. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020107. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Good PI. Permutation, Parametric and Bootstrap Tests of Hypotheses. New York, NY: Springer; 2005. p. 376. [Google Scholar]
- Grigg O. Grady CL. Task-related effects on the temporal and spatial dynamics of resting-state functional connectivity in the default network. PLoS One. 2010;5:e13311. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasson U. Malach R. Heeger DJ. Reliability of cortical activity during natural stimulation. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14:40–48. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasson U. Nir Y. Levy I. Fuhrmann G. Malach R. Intersubject synchronization of cortical activity during natural vision. Science. 2004;303:1634–1640. doi: 10.1126/science.1089506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasson U. Yang E. Vallines I. Heeger DJ. Rubin N. A hierarchy of temporal receptive windows in human cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:2539–2550. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5487-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holme P. Saramäki J. Temporal networks. Phys Rep. 2012:1–25. doi: 10.1016/j.physrep.2012.03.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Honey CJ. Kötter R. Breakspear M. Sporns O. Network structure of cerebral cortex shapes functional connectivity on multiple time scales. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:10240–10245. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701519104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jammalamadaka SR. Sengupta A. Topics in Circular Statistics. Singapore City, Singapore: World Scientific Publishing Co. Pte. Ltd.; 2001. p. 322. [Google Scholar]
- Jirsa VK. McIntosh AR. Handbook of Brain Connectivity. Berlin, Germany: Springer; 2007. p. 528. [Google Scholar]
- Jääskeläinen IP. Koskentalo K. Balk MH. Autti T. Kauramäki J. Pomren C, et al. Inter-subject synchronization of prefrontal cortex hemodynamic activity during natural viewing. Open Neuroimag J. 2008;2:14–19. doi: 10.2174/1874440000802010014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kang J. Wang L. Yan C. Wang J. Liang X. He Y. Characterizing dynamic functional connectivity in the resting brain using variable parameter regression and Kalman filtering approaches. Neuroimage. 2011;56:1222–1234. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kauppi J. Jääskeläinen IP. Sams M. Tohka J. Inter-subject correlation of brain hemodynamic responses during watching a movie: localization in space and frequency. Front Neuroinform. 2010;4:1–10. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2010.00005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kitzbichler MG. Smith ML. Christensen SR. Bullmore E. Broadband criticality of human brain network synchronization. PLoS Comput Biol. 2009;5:e1000314. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiviniemi V. Vire T. Remes J. Elseoud AA. Starck T. Tervonen O, et al. A sliding time-window ica reveals spatial variability of the default mode network in time. Brain Connect. 2011;1:339–347. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lachaux JP. Rodriguez E. Martinerie J. Varela FJ. Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999;8:194–208. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4<194:AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-C. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahnakoski JM. Salmi J. Jääskeläinen IP. Lampinen J. Glerean E. Tikka P. Sams M. Stimulus-related independent component and voxel-wise analysis of human brain activity during free viewing of a feature film. PLoS One. 2012;7:e35215. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird AR. Rogers BP. Carew JD. Arfanakis K. Moritz CH. Meyerand ME. Characterizing instantaneous phase relationships in whole-brain fMRI activation data. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;16:71–80. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le van Quyen M. Foucher J. Lachaux JP. Rodriguez E. Lutz A. Martinerie J, et al. Comparison of Hilbert transform and wavelet methods for the analysis of neuronal synchrony. J Neurosci Methods. 2001;111:83–98. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(01)00372-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lerner Y. Honey CJ. Silbert LJ. Hasson U. Topographic mapping of a hierarchy of temporal receptive windows using a narrated story. J Neurosci. 2011;31:2906–2915. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3684-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leopold DA. Murayama Y. Logothetis NK. Very slow activity fluctuations in monkey visual cortex: implications for functional brain imaging. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:422–433. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.4.422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynall ME. Bassett DS. Kerwin R. McKenna PJ. Kitzbichler M. Muller U, et al. Functional connectivity and brain networks in schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 2010;30:9477–9487. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0333-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Majeed W. Magnuson M. Hasenkamp W. Schwarb H. Schumacher EH. Barsalou L, et al. Spatiotemporal dynamics of low frequency BOLD fluctuations in rats and humans. Neuroimage. 2011;54:1140–1150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mardia KV. Jupp PE. Directional Statistics. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons; 2000. p. 429. [Google Scholar]
- Margulies DS. Böttger J. Long X. Lv Y. Kelly C. Schäfer A, et al. Resting developments: a review of fMRI post-processing methodologies for spontaneous brain activity. MAGMA. 2010;23:289–307. doi: 10.1007/s10334-010-0228-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClellan J. Parks T. Rabiner L. A computer program for designing optimum FIR linear phase digital filters. IEEE Trans Audio Electroacoustics. 1973;21:506–526. doi: 10.1109/TAU.1973.1162525. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mesulam MM. Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Ann Neurol. 1990;28:597–613. doi: 10.1002/ana.410280502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller K. Lohmann G. Neumann J. Grigutsch M. Mildner T. von Cramon DY. Investigating the wavelet coherence phase of the BOLD signal. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2004;20:145–152. doi: 10.1002/jmri.20064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakamura T. Hillary FG. Biswal BB. Resting network plasticity following brain injury. PloS One. 2009;4:e8220. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niazy RK. Xie J. Miller K. Beckmann CF. Smith SM. Slow brain oscillations of sleep, resting state and vigilance. Prog Brain Res. 2011;193:259–76. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00017-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nichols TE. Holmes AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;15:1–25. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oppenheim AV. Schafer RW. Discrete-Time Signal Processing. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall Press; 2009. p. 1120. [Google Scholar]
- Penttonen M. Buzsáki G. Natural logarithmic relationship between brain oscillators. Thalamus Relat Syst. 2003;2:145–152. [Google Scholar]
- Pereda E. Quiroga RQ. Bhattacharya J. Nonlinear multivariate analysis of neurophysiological signals. Prog Neurobiol. 2005;77:1–37. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Politis DN. Romano JP. A circular block-resampling procedure for stationary data. In: LePage R, editor; Billard L, editor. Exploring the Limits of Bootstrap. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Interscience; 1992. pp. 263–270. [Google Scholar]
- Power JD. Cohen AL. Nelson SM. Wig GS. Barnes KA. Church JA, et al. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron. 2011;72:665–678. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quian Quiroga R. Kraskov A. Kreuz T. Grassberger P. Performance of different synchronization measures in real data: a case study on electroencephalographic signals. Phys Rev E. 2002;65:041903. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.65.041903. (14) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenblum M. Pikovsky A. Kurths J. Phase synchronization of chaotic oscillators. Phys Rev Lett. 1996;76:1804–1807. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.76.1804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakoğlu U. Pearlson GD. Kiehl KA. Wang YM. Michael AM. Calhoun VD. A method for evaluating dynamic functional network connectivity and task-modulation: application to schizophrenia. MAGMA. 2010;23:351–366. doi: 10.1007/s10334-010-0197-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schack B. Weiss S. Quantification of phase synchronization phenomena and their importance for verbal memory processes. Biol Cybern. 2005;92:275–287. doi: 10.1007/s00422-005-0555-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith AM. Lewis BK. Ruttimann UE. Ye FQ. Sinnwell TM. Yang Y, et al. Investigation of low frequency drift in fMRI signal. Neuroimage. 1999;9:526–533. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM. Miller KL. Salimi-Khorshidi G. Webster M. Beckmann CF. Nichols TE, et al. Network modelling methods for FMRI. Neuroimage. 2011;54:875–891. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sporns O. Networks of the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2010. p. 375. [Google Scholar]
- Stephens GJ. Silbert LJ. Hasson U. Speaker-listener neural coupling underlies successful communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:14425–14430. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008662107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strogatz SH. Sync: The Emerging Science of Spontaneous Order. New York, NY: Hyperion; 2004. p. 338. [Google Scholar]
- Sun J. Small M. Unified framework for detecting phase synchronization in coupled time series. Physi Rev E. 2009;80:1–11. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.80.046219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Särkkä S. Solin A. Nummenmaa A. Vehtari A. Auranen T. Vanni S, et al. Dynamic retrospective filtering of physiological noise in BOLD fMRI: DRIFTER. Neuroimage. 2012;60:1517–1527. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tass P. Rosenblum M. Weule J. Kurths J. Pikovsky A. Volkmann J, et al. Detection of n: m phase locking from noisy data: application to magnetoencephalography. Phys Rev Lett. 1998;81:3291–3294. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.81.3291. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Thiel M. Romano MC. Kurths J. Rolfs M. Kliegl R. Twin surrogates to test for complex synchronisation. Europhys Lett. 2006;75:535–541. doi: 10.1209/epl/i2006-10147-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ville J. Theorie et application de la notion de signal analytical. Câbles et Transmissions. 1948;2:61–74. [Google Scholar]
- Vinck M. van Wingerden M. Womelsdorf T. Fries P. Pennartz CMA. The pairwise phase consistency: a bias-free measure of rhythmic neuronal synchronization. Neuroimage. 2010;51:112–122. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weisskoff R. Baker J. Belliveau J. Davis T. Kwong K. Cohen M, et al. Power Spectrum Analysis of Functionally-Weighted MR Data: What's in the Noise. Proceedings of the 12th Annual Meeting of Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine; New York, New York, USA. 1993. p. 7. [Google Scholar]
- Wise RG. Ide K. Poulin MJ. Tracey I. Resting fluctuations in arterial carbon dioxide induce significant low frequency variations in BOLD signal. Neuroimage. 2004;21:1652–1664. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xiao-Wen L. Zhi-Gang Z. Phase synchronization of coupled rossler oscillators: amplitude effect. Commun Theor Phys. 2007;47:265. doi: 10.1088/0253-6102/47/2/016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zar JH. Biostatistical Analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1999. p. 662. [Google Scholar]
- Zou Q. Zhu C. Yang Y. Zuo X. Long X. Cao Q, et al. An improved approach to detection of amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (ALFF) for resting-state fMRI: fractional ALFF. J Neurosci Methods. 2008;172:137–141. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.04.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zuo X. Di Martino A. Kelly C. Shehzad ZE. Gee DG. Klein DF, et al. The oscillating brain: complex and reliable. Neuroimage. 2010;49:1432–1445. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]