Skip to main content
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience logoLink to Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
editorial
. 2012 Nov 17;8(4):359–361. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss111

Imaging depletion: fMRI provides new insights into the processes underlying ego depletion*

Elliot T Berkman 1, Jordan S Miller-Ziegler 1
PMCID: PMC3624962  PMID: 22997053

Abstract

‘Ego depletion’ refers to the phenomenon of diminished ability to enact self-regulation with repeated efforts. Several models offer process accounts of how ego depletion works, but few studies directly investigate these processes. A study in this issue of Social, Cognitive, & Affective Neuroscience by Wagner and Heatherton is among the first to do so. Their results substantiate one possible mechanism of ego depletion and, more broadly, illustrate how neuroscience data can further social psychological theory.


Performance on a task requiring self-control reduces performance on subsequent self-control tasks—an effect that psychologists have dubbed ‘ego depletion’—but the mechanisms behind ego depletion are largely unknown. In an article in this issue of Social, Cognitive, & Affective Neuroscience, Wagner and Heatherton (this issue) provide some of the first insights into the neural processes the give rise to ego-depletion effects, and what they found has major implications for theories of self-control.

This article comes at an exciting time in ego-depletion research. Extensive evidence for ego depletion (e.g. Hagger et al., 2010) has led to strong consensus that it occurs in a variety of contexts, so the field is now turning to the question of mechanism—how does ego depletion work?

Three main theories are currently under investigation. Each accounts for some but not all of the available data, perhaps because each focuses on one piece of a complicated, multifaceted process. The first holds that ego depletion happens because various forms of self-control all rely upon a common physiological resource—blood glucose—that is quickly consumed by effortful self-control attempts, and subsequent self-control efforts are diminished when glucose levels are low or appear low to a glucose monitoring system. Evidence in favor of this hypothesis shows that the ego-depletion effect is mitigated by intake of a high-glucose drink following the first task, but not by intake of a low-glucose but otherwise similar control drink (Gailliot et al., 2007). Nonetheless, a handful of recent findings run counter to the notion that self-control relies upon a limited physiological resource (Clarkson et al., 2010; Kurzban, 2010). For example, Job et al. (2010) found that only those who believed that self-control was a limited resource showed the depletion effect; those with an unlimited belief about self-control resources did not evince ego depletion following a self-control task (regardless of whether the belief existed prior to the experiment or was induced by an experimenter). These findings imply that the relationship between glucose and self-control is not one-to-one and begin to explore other factors that alter the relationship between resources (glucose or some other type) and self-control effort.

Two additional accounts of depletion have emerged that complement the resource model and account for some of the recent data. One idea is that engaging in self-control heightens reactivity to affective stimuli in general (K.D. Vohs, R.F. Baumeister, N.L. Mead, S. Ramanathan, W. Hofmann, B.J. Schmeichel, submitted for publication). This hypothesis is grounded in the idea of a trade-off between affective evaluation and executive function, or, more specifically, between emotion reactivity and emotion regulation (Heatherton and Wagner, 2011), whereby weakening one system intensifies the other. For example, exerting effort on a cold pressor task (which requires a large amount self-control) will increase subjective reactivity to emotional stimuli making a subsequent emotion regulation task more difficult. In this ‘trade-off’ model, stronger emotions or impulses, not necessarily reduced self-control resources, cause the decrements in performance on sequential self-control tasks in ego-depletion studies.

Another explanation of ego-depletion effects is that self-control exertion generates concurrent shifts of motivation away from self-control and toward self-gratification and of attention away from control-related cues and toward gratification-related cues (Inzlicht and Schmeichel, 2012). In this model, self-control resources are not depleted so much as re-allocated based on context, needs and priorities; self-control efforts hamper subsequent performance because of reduced motivation to engage in further control (e.g. self-licensing to take a break) and/or a lack of attention to cues signaling the need for control (e.g. diminished error monitoring). Consistent with other work on the importance of beliefs about (Job et al., 2010) or perceptions of (Clarkson et al., 2010) resources, this model hypothesizes that the motivation to exert self-control varies with appraisals of available resources, and that appraisals are only loosely connected to actual resources. A variety of factors such as beliefs, recent exertion and glucose levels can affect appraisals of resources, which in turn determine self-control allocation.

The ego-depletion literature is thus blessed with ample theory about process and literally hundreds of empirical studies demonstrating the effect (Hagger et al., 2010), but lacks almost any data that speak directly to its underlying processes. Thus, the article by Wagner and Heatherton in this issue is a significant step forward because it provides some of the first data on the neural processes of depletion. The same article also makes a more broad contribution by serving as an excellent model of how neuroscience data can inform social psychological theory.

So what did they do? The experimenters employed a standard sequential task paradigm to induce ego depletion, or not, in their participants (Wagner and Heatherton, this issue). What’s new is that the participants’ brain activation was measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) before and after the depletion induction (or control), which allowed the experimenters to look at the changes in activation caused by earlier self-control efforts. Participants first viewed a series of highly emotional positive and negative images, in addition to neutral images, and made an indoor/outdoor decision about each. They then viewed a video overlaid with a series of distracting words, and were told either that they should ignore the words (the depletion condition) or that they could read them if they chose to do so (the control condition). This manipulation has reliably produced ego-depletion effects in participants in several previous studies. Finally, participants viewed a new series of emotional and neutral images and completed the same scene discrimination task. By comparing the depletion with the control group in the amount of change in neural activation between the first and second emotional tasks, this design allowed the researchers to examine the effects of a class ego depletion induction on neural activation.

The pattern of results was fascinating, and provided some support for each of the new theories. First, depleted participants showed increased activation in the left amygdala from before to after the depletion condition, whereas controls showed no change or possibly a slight decrease. In other words, control participants may have habituated to the images, but depleted participants were more reactive to the images on a neural level following self-control exertion. On its own, this result is consistent with both the trade-off model, which predicts increased reactivity following self-control exertion, and the motivation–attention model, which predicts that subjects will be less motivated to regulate and more attentive to emotional stimuli after exertion. However, there was only an increase in amygdala reactivity in response to negative, and not to positive or neutral images (see figure 3 in Wagner and Heatherton, this issue). This result runs counter to the prediction made by the trade-off model of increased overall emotional reactivity, but can be explained by the motivation–attention model in terms of reduced motivation to engage in regulation, which is presumably engaged more during negative than other kinds of stimuli. The finding that regions known to be involved in emotion regulation, such as the right inferior frontal gyrus, were more active during viewing of negative than positive or neutral images across both time points also fits this explanation (see table 1 and figure 2 in Wagner and Heatherton, this issue).

The second main finding was that the control group displayed increased functional connectivity between left amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)—a region associated with incidental or spontaneous emotion regulation (i.e. regulation that was not prompted by the experimenter; Berkman and Lieberman, 2009)—while viewing negative images, but the depletion group showed no change in the coupling between these regions (see figure 4 in Wagner and Heatherton, this issue). It is as though the control group participants increased their spontaneous regulatory efforts during the second run of the emotion reactivity task, but the group difference emerged because the depletion group participants merely failed to ramp up their regulatory efforts (as opposed to a depletion-like pattern of reduced regulation). Like the previous one, this result is also in line with motivation–attention model, which would suggest the interpretation that the ‘depleted’ participants could but simply did not deploy self-control.

This pattern of results, together with knowledge gained from cognitive neuroscience studies, can directly inform social psychological theories of the ego-depletion process. On the one hand, the motivation–attention model suggests that participants in the depletion condition may have been less motivated to engage in control, less attentive to cues to engage control or both. On the other, we have the result that depleted participants failed to show increased vmPFC–amygdala connectivity whereas controls did not. What could the role of the vmPFC be here? That region is involved in a number of processes including self-related processing, emotion and long-term memory (Wager et al., 2007), but in the context of emotional stimuli, several studies have found the vmPFC to be involved in the modulation of the perceived relevance, valence or value of a stimulus with respect to current goals (Phelps et al., 2004; Hare et al., 2009). In the Wagner and Heatherton study, it is possible that the lack of regulation in the depletion group reflects a diminished value of self-control in that group—perhaps those participants simply abandoned the goal to regulate following the first emotional task.

From the broader perspective of the field of social neuroscience, this article provides an excellent example of how neuroimaging can complement existing social or cognitive measures in theory development and testing. This is particularly true in research areas such as this one that have several competing theories to explain a phenomenon, with each theory predicting the involvement of a different set of mental processes (e.g. emotional reactivity, self-regulation, attentional control, motivation, etc.). At this point, cognitive neuroscience studies have amassed sufficient knowledge about the neural systems involved in many of these processes to begin to attach relative likelihoods to various mental processes given a set of activations (Yarkoni et al., 2011; Berkman and Falk, in press). Here, for example, the involvement of vmPFC is more likely to reflect motivational than attentional processes based on what we know about those two systems. In addition, neuroimaging methods can be especially helpful where other measures are unreliable, unavailable or inaccessible to subjective report. For instance, ego depletion produces a small effect on self-reported negative affect that only has been detectable with large samples or meta-analyses (Hagger et al., 2010), and researchers have been unable to identify any other subjective measures that reliably indicate a depleted state. Similarly, research on implicit motives shows that people may not have a strong sense of their own motivational state at a given moment (McClelland et al., 1989), and people spontaneously engage in emotion regulation without necessarily being aware of it (Berkman and Lieberman, 2009), so verbal reports of motivation and emotion regulation can be unreliable. In these cases, neuroimaging data have the potential to provide a unique way of testing the theory.

The study by Wagner and Heatherton is an excellent first step toward understanding the neural systems of ego depletion and exemplary in its use of fMRI for testing social psychological theory more broadly. But the field is still well short of a complete mechanistic explanation of how one act of self-control influences the next, which is why more neuroscience research is so critical in this area. We are delighted to see neuroscience used in these ways, and optimistic that this article presages many future studies clarifying how motivation, emotion and attention all fit together during self-control.

REFERENCES

  1. Berkman ET, Falk EB. Beyond brain mapping: using the brain to predict real-world outcomes. Current Directions in Psychological Science. in press doi: 10.1177/0963721412469394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Berkman ET, Lieberman MD. Using neuroscience to broaden emotion regulation: theoretical and methodological considerations. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2009;3(4):475–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00186.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Clarkson JJ, Hirt ER, Jia L, Alexander MB. When perception is more than reality: the effects of perceived versus actual resource depletion on self-regulatory behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2010;98(1):29–46. doi: 10.1037/a0017539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Gailliot MT, Baumeister RF, DeWall CN, et al. Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2007;92(2):325–36. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Hagger MS, Wood C, Stiff C, Chatzisarantis NLD. Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2010;136(4):495–525. doi: 10.1037/a0019486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Hare TA, Camerer CF, Rangel A. Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science. 2009;324(5927):646–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1168450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Heatherton TF, Wagner DD. Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15(3):132–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Inzlicht M, Schmeichel BJ. What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2012;7(5):450–63. doi: 10.1177/1745691612454134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Job V, Dweck CS, Walton GM. Ego depletion—is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychological Science. 2010;21(11):1686–93. doi: 10.1177/0956797610384745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Kurzban R. Does the brain consume additional glucose during self-control tasks? Evolutionary Psychology. 2010;8(2):244–59. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. McClelland DC, Koestner R, Weinberger J. How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review. 1989;96(4):690–702. [Google Scholar]
  12. Phelps EA, Delgado MR, Nearing KI, LeDoux JE. Extinction learning in humans: role of the amygdala and vmPFC. Neuron. 2004;43(6):897–905. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Wager TD, Lindquist M, Kaplan L. Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data: current and future directions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2007;2(2):150–8. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Wagner DD, Heatherton DF. Self-regulatory depletion increases emotional reactivity in the amygdala. Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience. in press doi: 10.1093/scan/nss082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE, Van Essen DC, Wager TD. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nature Methods. 2011;8(8):665–70. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES