Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013 Jan 9;131(0):171–174. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.12.016

Working Memory and Impulsivity Predict Marijuana-Related Problems Among Frequent Users

Anne M Day 1, Jane Metrik 1, Nichea S Spillane 1, Christopher W Kahler 1
PMCID: PMC3626751  NIHMSID: NIHMS430594  PMID: 23312340

Abstract

Background

Although marijuana is the most commonly used illicit substance in the US, only a small portion of users go on to develop dependence, suggesting that there are substantial individual differences in vulnerability to marijuana-related problems among users. Deficits in working memory and high trait impulsivity are two factors that may place marijuana users at increased risk for experiencing related problems.

Methods

Using baseline data from an experimental study that recruited 104 frequent marijuana users (M=71.86% of prior 60 days, SD=22%), we examined the associations of working memory and trait impulsivity with marijuana-related problems.

Results

Lower working memory, as measured by Trail Making Test B, but not short-term memory capacity, predicted more marijuana-related problems. Higher trait impulsivity scores were independently associated with greater number of problems.

Conclusions

Results suggest that marijuana users with reduced executive cognitive ability are more susceptible to developing problems related to their use. Trait impulsivity and executive working memory appear to be independent risk factors for experiencing marijuana-related problems.

Keywords: marijuana, working memory, problems, executive functioning, impulsivity

1. INTRODUCTION

Although marijuana is the most used illicit drug in the U.S. and the world (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2010), only about nine percent of those who have ever used become dependent (Anthony, 2006), indicating substantial individual differences in risk of progression to problem use (Anthony et al., 1994; Kendler and Prescott, 1998; Kleinman et al., 1998). Executive functioning deficits have been linked to the experience of behavioral (Wahlstedt et al., 2008) and emotional problems (Knouse et al., 2012), and play a critical role in the development and maintenance of drug addiction (Hester et al., 2010). These disturbances in prefrontally-mediated processes can represent both a pre-existing vulnerability to and a consequence of drug use (Yücel and Lubman, 2007).

Two facets of executive functioning that may have particular relevance to the development of marijuana problems are working memory and impulsivity. These constructs are inversely related and independently predict problem behaviors (Romer et al., 2011), including drug use (Albein-Urios et al., 2012). Those lower in working memory may have limited capacity to maintain task-relevant information during complex tasks, restricting ability to exert behavioral control when needed. Likewise, those high in impulsivity may be more likely to act upon maladaptive behavioral impulses without considering consequences. There is mixed evidence regarding whether marijuana use leads to deficits in working memory (Solowij and Pesa, 2010), and a dearth of information on the role of working memory in the experience of marijuana-related problems. By contrast, impulsivity has demonstrated a robust and consistent association with increased substance use (Littlefield and Sher, 2010), including marijuana use (Hale et al., 2003; Jaffe and Archer, 1987; Kollins, 2003; Satinder and Black, 1984; Vangsness, et al., 2005). Furthermore, trait impulsivity is positively associated with marijuana-related problems (Hayaki et al., 2011; Simons and Carey, 2002), although studies have been mostly limited to college samples with low levels of marijuana use (e.g., Simons et al., 2006).

The current study explored the association between working memory, impulsivity and marijuana-related problems among frequent users. We hypothesized that greater trait impulsivity and poorer working memory (but not short-term memory capacity) would independently predict greater number of marijuana-related problems.

2. METHODS

2.1. Participants

Data were drawn from a study investigating cannabinoid-related genetic variation and variability in marijuana’s acute and cue-elicited effects (Metrik et al., 2012a). The Institutional Review Board of Brown University approved the study. Participants were 104 marijuana smokers recruited through newspaper advertisements, flyers, and social media websites who met the following inclusion criteria: native English speakers, 18 to 44 years of age, non-Hispanic Caucasian (due to genetic aims of the parent study), marijuana use at least 2 days a week in the past month and at least weekly in the past 6 months, and self-reported ability to abstain from marijuana for 24 hours without withdrawal. Exclusion criteria were: intent to quit or receive treatment for cannabis abuse, use of other illicit drugs, pregnancy, nursing, past month affective or panic disorder, psychotic or suicidal state assessed by psychiatric interview, contraindicated medical issues by physical exam and BMI > 30, and smoking more than 20 tobacco cigarettes per day.

The sample was 36.5% female with a mean age of 21.29 (SD = 4.30, range = 18–42), with 64.3% of the sample reporting having completed college at least one year of, or being currently enrolled in, college (M=13.6 years of education, SD=1.7). Participants reported using marijuana a mean of 2.03 (SD = 1.22) times a day on 71.86% (SD = 22.02%) of the past 60 days (about 5 days per week). There were 44 tobacco cigarette smokers (42.3%), who smoked 4.08 cigarettes (SD = 3.77) per day. The 100 current drinkers drank 8.5 (SD=7.88) drinks per week consuming an average of 4.34 drinks (SD=2.44) on a drinking day.

2.2. Procedure

Participants completed self-report assessments and research assistants administered two executive functioning tests (Trail-Making Test and Digit Span). Participants abstained from marijuana and tobacco smoking for 12 hours before the session. Alveolar carbon-monoxide (CO) of ≤ 6 ppm was used to confirm no recent smoking (Cooper and Haney, 2009; Metrik et al., 2012b) with a Bedfont Scientific Smokelyzer and zero breath alcohol concentration was verified with an Alco-Sensor IV (Intoximeters, Inc., St Louis, MO, USA). Smokers smoked one cigarette after CO confirmation to prevent effects of nicotine withdrawal on task performance.

2.3. Measures

The well-validated calendar-assisted Timeline Follow-Back interview (TLFB; Dennis et al., 2004) was used to assess past 60-day marijuana use.

Trait impulsivity was measured with the mean composite score for the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11; Patton et al., 1995), a reliable and valid 30-item self-report measure of motor, attentional, and future planning impulsivity, scored on Likert scales, from 1 (rarely/never) to 4 (almost always).

Working memory was assessed with the Trails B (Sánchez-Cubillo et al., 2009) portion of the Trail-Making Test (TMT; Reitan and Wolfson, 1995). T-scores (Weaver et al., 2002; Reitan and Wolfson, 1995) adjusted for age, gender, and education were used.

Short-term memory capacity was measured with Digit Span subtests (forward and backward) of the WAIS-III (Wechsler, 1997) and was used as an indicator of the discriminant validity of working memory in the prediction of marijuana-related problems.

Marijuana-related problems were assessed with the 22-item Marijuana Problems Scale (MPS; Stephens, 2000), a reliable and valid measure of the number and severity of marijuana problems during the past 90 days. Total count of combined minor and serious problems was used. Internal validity for the BIS and MPS were fair (α=.68 and .76, respectively).

2.4. Data Analysis Plan

We first examined correlations among gender, age of onset of regular use of marijuana, marijuana use frequency, Digit Span Forward and Backward, Trails B, BIS, and marijuana-related problems. In a linear regression model, we tested the association of each variable with marijuana-related problems. Variables were centered to aid in interpretation of results. Age of initiation of regular marijuana use was included in the model as a covariate because it has been linked to negative outcomes including progression to cannabis dependence (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2010). This variable was dichotomized (under 16/16 and over; Gruber et al., 2012).

3. RESULTS

3.1. Marijuana-Related Problems

All participants reported at least one marijuana-related problem. The median number of problems was 3, with a range between 1 and 13 problems. Participants most frequently reported that marijuana caused them “to procrastinate” (53% of the sample) and “to have a lower energy level” (42.6%). No participants reported that marijuana had caused them to lose a job, and 1% reported experiencing medical problems as a result of marijuana. The remaining 18 problems were reported by between 2 and 37% of participants.

Means and standard deviations of variables, along with correlations, are listed in Table 1. As expected, number of marijuana problems was negatively correlated with age of initiation of regular use, Trails B, and BIS, but was uncorrelated with frequency of use or scores on both subtests of Digit Span. We found a nonsignificant relationship between gender and number of marijuana problems (p>.60), so gender was excluded from further analyses.

Table 1.

Correlations Among Executive Control and Marijuana Use Variables

Mean (SD) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. MJ Use Frequency 71.86% (22.02) --
2. Age of Regular −.19 --
3. Trails B 49.55 (10.21) −.22** .08 --
4. BIS 59.95 (8.21) .38** −.30** −.26** --
5. DS Forward 11.45 (2.06) −.03 .10 .36** −.03 --
6. DS Backward 7.99 (2.4) −.11 .20* .30** −.18 .30** --
7. MJ Problems 3.64 (2.94) .13 −.19* −.22** .34** −.01 −.003 --

Note. MJ = Marijuana; BIS = Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; DS = Digit Span. Use frequency is percent of days used in the past 60 days. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients reported for correlations for the dichotomous age of regular use variable; all other correlation statistics are based on Pearson’s Product-Moment correlation coefficients.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01.

3.2. Predicting Marijuana-Related Problems

Use frequency, age of initiation of regular use, and both subtests of Digit Span were not significantly associated with marijuana problems, while lower working memory (Trails B) and higher impulsivity (BIS) were significantly related to marijuana problems, predicting 6% and 4% of the variance, respectively (see Table 2). (The following interactions were all nonsignificant (ps>.45): use X BIS, use X Trails B, BIS X Trails B.)

Table 2.

Linear Regression Predicting Marijuana-Related Problems (N=104)


Predictor B SE (B) sr2 p
MJ Use Frequency −.01 .01 .001 .71
Age of Initiation −.1.21 .73 .27 .10
Digit Span Forward   .01 .14 .005 .50
Digit Span Backward   .16 .12 .02 .18
Trails B −.08 .03 .07 .009
BIS   .09 .04 .06 .016

Note. MJ = Marijuana. BIS = Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. MJ Use Frequency, Trails B and BIS are centered. Total R2=.154.

4. DISCUSSION

As predicted, lower working memory was associated with the experience of more marijuana-related problems, such as procrastination, lower energy and lower productivity, independent of frequency of use. Poor working memory has been implicated in difficulty limiting consumption of substances (Hofman et al., 2011), and in this sample, use and working memory were negatively correlated, indicating that those with greater use frequency performed more poorly on Trails B. Previous research indicates that marijuana users do perform worse than non-users on tests of executive functioning (Pope and Yurgelun-Todd, 1996; Solowij et al., 2002), although not necessarily worse on tasks of working memory (Bolla et al., 2002; Jager et al., 2006). Because of the cross-sectional nature of the study, it is impossible to tell whether frequency of use contributed to deficits in working memory, which then led to marijuana-related problems, or if pre-existing deficits in working memory led to greater marijuana use and marijuana-related problems. It is also possible that an underlying psychological variable (e.g., anxiety) that was not measured in the current study might have predicted both greater frequency of use and a greater number of problems.

Performance on Digit Span subtests was not predictive of problems associated with marijuana use. Digit Span Forward measures only short-term memory capacity, which reflects ability to store information for a short period of time (Conway et al., 2005). Digit Span Backward measures capacity to complete a transformation in short-term memory, which is a more complicated cognitive activity, but nonetheless does not assess working memory, which is utilized when an individual actively maintains task-relevant information in the face of ongoing distraction (Conway et al., 2005). Working memory and short-term memory capacity were correlated in this study, but working memory was uniquely related to the experience of problems, providing evidence that working memory and short-term memory capacity are indeed distinct.

Consistent with other studies (Hayaki et al., 2011; Simons and Carey, 2006), we found that trait impulsivity was related to marijuana-related problems. Future research might consider measuring impulsivity with behavioral tasks or completing prospective research to provide additional support for the direction of causality between impulsivity and the experience of marijuana-related problems.

In prior studies, frequent and intense use of marijuana has predicted negative academic and social outcomes (Brook et al., 2003, 2008). The present study may not have replicated these findings because participants were slightly older (M=22 years) than adolescents previously studied (ages 14 –15). We also had a restricted range of marijuana use in our sample, with participants using five days a week on average. At this level of use, other variables, such as facets of personality and executive function, may play a more critical role in the experience of problems.

One strength of this study is the community sample of regular marijuana users which reflects the age group (18–25 years) that has the highest rates of marijuana use (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2010). Another strength was measuring different elements of cognitive functioning (working memory and short-term memory capacity) and testing their independent effects. Other studies have utilized composite executive function scores (e.g., Glass et al., 2009; Thoma et al., 2011), which may fail to delineate which elements of executive control create a vulnerability to experiencing marijuana-related problems.

A limitation of these findings is the use of only one measure for short-term memory capacity and for working memory. Having a more comprehensive evaluation of each construct and other elements of executive functioning might further elucidate the links between executive functioning and marijuana-related problems.

The current study did not examine motives or expectancies related to marijuana use, which have been shown to mediate the relation between use and subsequent problems (Mirin, 1971; Patrick et al., 2011; Simons et al., 1998), and between other psychological risk factors and and problems (Buckner and Schmidt, 2008, 2009). In addition, negative expectancies about marijuana use have been shown to mediate the link between impulsivity and marijuana use (Vangsness et al., 2005), and as such, future studies might examine the role of motives expectancies in determining the link between impulsivity, executive functioning, use, and problems. Clinically, the results of the current study are valuable. Knowing that those high in impulsivity and low in working memory are at risk for developing negative consequences of marijuana use can guide efforts to develop targeted prevention and interventions efforts.

Acknowledgements

The authors gratefully acknowledge Dr. James Harper, III, Dr. Valerie Knopik, Amy Mochel, Samuel Fricchione, Netesha Reid, Suzanne Sales, and Timothy Souza for their contribution to the project.

Role of Funding Source

This study was supported by R03 DA027484 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse to Jane Metrik and Valerie Knopik, K08 DA029094 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse awarded to Nichea Spillane and T32 AA007459 from the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism awarded to Anne Day.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributors

Authors Day and Metrik designed this study and author Metrik designed, wrote the protocol, and collected the data for the laboratory study. Authors Metrik, Spillane, and Kahler undertook the statistical analysis, and authors Day, Metrik, and Spillane drafted the manuscript with editing by all authors. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.

REFERENCES

  1. Albein-Urios N, Martinez-González JM, Lozano Ó, Clark L, Verdejo-García A. Comparison of impulsivity and working memory in cocaine addiction and pathological gambling: implications for cocaine-induced neurotoxicity. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012;126:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anthony JC. The epidemiology of cannabis dependence. In: Roffman RA, Stephens RS, editors. Cannabis Dependence: Its Nature, Consequences, and Treatment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2006. pp. 58–95. [Google Scholar]
  3. Anthony JC, Warner LA, Kessler RC. Comparative epidemiology of dependence on tobacco, alcohol, controlled substances, and inhalants: Basic findings from the national comorbidity survey. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 1994;2:244–268. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bolla KI, Brown K, Eldreth D, Tate K, Cadet JL. Dose-related neurocognitive effects of marijuana use. Neurology. 2002;59:1337–1343. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000031422.66442.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brook JS, Brook DW, Rosen Z, Rabbitt CR. Earlier marijuana use and later problem behavior in colombian youths. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2003;42:485–492. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000037050.04952.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brook JS, Stimmel MA, Zhang C, Brook DW. The association between earlier marijuana use and subsequent academic achievement and health problems: a longitudinal study. Am. J. Addict. 2008;17:155–160. doi: 10.1080/10550490701860930. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buckner JD, Schmidt NB. Marijuana effect expectancies: relations to social anxiety and marijuana use problems. Addict. Behav. 2008;33:1477–1483. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.06.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Buckner JD, Schmidt NB. Social anxiety disorder and marijuana use problems: the mediating role of marijuana effect expectancies. Depress. Anxiety. 2009;26:864–870. doi: 10.1002/da.20567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Conway ARA, Kane MJ, Bunting MF, Hambrick DZ, Wilhelm O, Engle RW. Working memory span tasks: a methodological review and user's guide. Psychol. Bull. Rev. 2005;12:769–786. doi: 10.3758/bf03196772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cooper ZD, Haney M. Comparison of subjective, pharmacokinetic, and physiological effects of marijuana smoked as joints and blunts. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;103:107–113. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.01.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dennis ML, Funk R, Godley SH, Godley MD, Waldron HB. Cross validation of the alcohol and cannabis use measures in the Global Appraisal of Individual Needs (GAIN) and Timeline Followback (TLFB; Form 90) among adolescents in substance abuse treatment. Addiction. 2004;99:125–133. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00859.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Derzon JH, Lipsey MW. A synthesis of the relationship of marijuana use with delinquent and problem behaviors. School Psychol. Int. 1999;20:57–68. [Google Scholar]
  13. Glass JM, Buu A, Adams KM, Nigg JT, Puttler LI, Jester JM, Zucker RA. Effects of alcoholism severity and smoking on executive neurocognitive function. Addiction. 2009;104:38–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02415.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gruber SA, Sagar KA, Dahlgren MK, Racine M, Lukas SE. Age of onset of marijuana use and executive function. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2012;26:496–506. doi: 10.1037/a0026269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hale RL, Whiteman S, Muehl K, Faynberg E. Tridimensional personality traits of college student marijuana users. Psychol. Rep. 2003;92:661–666. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2003.92.2.661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hayaki J, Herman DS, Hagerty CE, de Dios MA, Anderson BJ, Stein MD. Expectancies and self-efficacy mediate the effects of impulsivity on marijuana use outcomes: an application of the acquired preparedness model. Addict Behav. 2011;36:389–396. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.12.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hester R, Lubman DI, Yücel M. The role of executive control in human drug addiction. Curr. Top Behav. Neurosci. 2010;3:301–318. doi: 10.1007/7854_2009_28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Friese M, Baddeley AD. Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2011. Working Memory and Self-Regulation; pp. 204–225. [Google Scholar]
  19. Jaffe LT, Archer RP. The prediction of drug use among college students from MMPI, MCMI, and sensation seeking scales. J. Pers. Assess. 1987;51:243–253. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5102_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Jager G, Kahn RS, Van DB, Van Ree JM, Ramsey NF. Long-term effects of frequent cannabis use on working memory and attention: an fMRI study. Psychopharmacology. 2006;185:358–368. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0298-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kendler KS, Prescott CA. Cannabis use, abuse, and dependence in a population-based sample of female twins. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1998;155:1016–1022. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.8.1016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kleinman PH, Wish ED, Deren S, Rainone G. Daily marijuana use and problem behaviors among adolescents. Int. J. Addict. 1988;23:87–107. doi: 10.3109/10826088809027492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Knouse LE, Barkley RA, Murphy KR. Does executive functioning (ef) predict depression in clinic-referred adults?: ef tests vs. rating scales. J. Affect. Disord. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.05.064. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kollins SH. Delay discounting is associated with substance use in college students. Addict. Behav. 2003;28:1167–1173. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(02)00220-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Littlefield AK, Sher KJ. The multiple, distinct ways that personality contributes to alcohol use disorders. Soc. Personal Psychol. Compass. 2010;4:767–782. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00296.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Metrik J, Kahler CW, Rohsenow DJ, McGeary JE, Knopik VS. Marijuana’s Acute Effects on Attentional Bias for Affective Cues. Poster presented at the CPDD Meeting; Palm Springs, CA. 2012a. [Google Scholar]
  27. Metrik J, Kahler CW, Reynolds B, McGeary JE, Monti PM, Haney M, deWit H, Rohsenow DJ. Balanced-placebo design with marijuana: pharmacological and expectancy effects on impulsivity and risk taking. Psychopharmacology. 2012b;223:489–499. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2740-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Mirin SM. Casual versus heavy use of marijuana: a redefinition of the marijuana problem. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1971;127:1134–1140. doi: 10.1176/ajp.127.9.1134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Patrick ME, Schulenberg JE, OMalley PM, Johnston LD, Bachman JG. Adolescents reported reasons for alcohol and marijuana use as predictors of substance use and problems in adulthood. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs. 2011;72:106–116. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2011.72.106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 1995;51:768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Pope HG, Yurgelun-Todd D. The residual cognitive effects of heavy marijuana use in college students. JAMA. 1996;275:521–527. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ranganathan M, D'Souza DC. The acute effects of cannabinoids on memory in humans: a review. Psychopharmacology. 2006;188:425–444. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0508-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Reitan RM, Wolfson D. Category test and trail making test as measures of frontal lobe functions. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1995;9:50–56. [Google Scholar]
  34. Romer D, Betancourt LM, Brodsky NL, Giannetta JM, Yang W, Hurt H. Does adolescent risk taking imply weak executive function? A prospective study of relations between working memory performance, impulsivity, and risk taking in early adolescence. Dev. Sci. 2011;14:1119–1133. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01061.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Sánchez-Cubillo I, Periáñez JA, Adrover-Roig D, Rodríguez-Sánchez JM, Ríos-Logo M, Tirapu J, Barceló F. Construct validity of the trail making test: role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2009;15:438–450. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Satinder KP, Black A. Cannabis use and sensation-seeking orientation. J. Psychol. 1984;116:101–105. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1984.9923623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Simons J, Correia CJ, Carey KB, Borsari BE. Validating a five-factor marijuana motives measure: relations with use, problems, and alcohol motives. J. Couns. Psychol. 1998;45:265–273. [Google Scholar]
  38. Simons JS, Carey KB. Risk and vulnerability for marijuana use problems: the role of affect dysregulation. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2002;16:72–75. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Simons JS, Carey KB. An affective and cognitive model of marijuana and alcohol problems. Addict. Behav. 2006;31:1578–1592. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Simons JS, Neal DJ, Gaher RM. Risk for marijuana-related problems among college students: an application of zero-inflated negative binomial regression. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2006;32:41–53. doi: 10.1080/00952990500328539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Solowij N, Pesa N. Cognitive abnormalities and cannabis use. Rev. Bras Psiquiatr. 2010;32:S31–S40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Solowij N, Stephens R, Roffman RA, Babor T. Does marijuana use cause long-term cognitive deficits? JAMA. 2002;287:2653–2654. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Stephens RS, Roffman RA, Curtin L. Comparison of extended versus brief treatments for marijuana use. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2000;68:898–908. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Rockville, MD: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration; Results from the 2010 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Summary of National Findings, NSDUH Series H-41. HHS Publication No. (SMA) 11–4658. 2011. [Google Scholar]
  45. Thoma P, Zalewski I, von Reventlow HG, Norra C, Juckel G, Daum I. Cognitive and affective empathy in depression linked to executive control. Psychiatry Res. 2011;189:373–378. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Vangsness L, Bry BH, LaBouvie EW. Impulsivity, negative expectancies, and marijuana use: a test of the acquired preparedness model. Addict. Behav. 2005;30:1071–1076. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Wagner S, Helmreich I, Dahmen N, Lieb K, Tadic A. Reliability of three alternate forms of the trail making tests A and B. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2011;26:314–321. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acr024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Wåhlstedt C, Thorell LB, Bohlin G. ADHD symptoms and executive function impairment: early predictors of later behavioral problems. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2008;33:160–178. doi: 10.1080/87565640701884253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Weaver LK, Hopkins RO, Chan KJ, Churchill S, Elliott CG, Clemmer TP, Orme JF, Thomas FO, Jr, Morris AH. Hyperbaric oxygen for acute carbon monoxide poisoning. N. Engl. J. Med. 2002;347:1057–1067. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa013121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3rd Edition. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment; 1997. (WAIS-3) [Google Scholar]
  51. Yücel M, Lubman DI. Neurocognitive and neuroimaging evidence of behavioural dysregulation in human drug addiction: Implications for diagnosis, treatment and prevention. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2007;26:33–39. doi: 10.1080/09595230601036978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES