Skip to main content
Schizophrenia Bulletin logoLink to Schizophrenia Bulletin
. 2012 Feb 29;39(3):592–600. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs008

Birth Weight and Neurocognition in Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders

David Freedman 1,*, Yuanyuan Bao 2, William S Kremen 3,4, Sophia Vinogradov 5, Ian W McKeague 6, Alan S Brown 1,2
PMCID: PMC3627778  PMID: 22378899

Abstract

Low birth weight is associated with both schizophrenia and neurocognitive impairment. Yet, to our knowledge, no previous study has examined the relationship between lower birth weight and neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSD). In this preliminary study, we investigated the relationship using a broad neuropsychological battery in cases with SSD and matched control subjects. The sample consisted of all subjects in the Developmental Insult and Brain Anomaly in Schizophrenia study, a nested case-control investigation developed from a large birth cohort, which followed subjects longitudinally. Case ascertainment was based on computerized record linkages between the birth cohort members and the Kaiser Permanente Medical Care Plan, and all diagnoses were confirmed by consensus diagnosis following the Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies. Lower birth weight was associated with impairment in executive function, working memory, generalized intellectual function, and neuromotor function in cases with SSD but not in control subjects. No deficits were observed in verbal memory for either group. These results support the hypothesis that lower birth weight plays a role in neuropsychological disruptions in SSD and that the antecedents of lower birth weight may have a greater impact on these disruptions in SSD than in controls. These data may facilitate a better understanding of the etiopathogenesis of the cognitive underpinnings of SSD.

Keywords: executive functioning, working memory, epidemiology, risk factors, obstetric complications

Introduction

Low birth weight (LBW) is a widely replicated risk factor for schizophrenia. In a meta-analysis of population-based prospective studies by Cannon et al,1 birth weight below 2500 g, and 2000 g, was related to 1.7-fold and 4-fold increases, respectively, in the risk of schizophrenia. Jones et al,2 in a follow-up of the Northern Finland birth cohort, found a greater than 2-fold increased risk of schizophrenia for subjects with birth weight below 2500 g. In the largest such study to date, in which Abel et al3 combined Danish and Swedish national population–based cohorts, a dose-response relationship between birth weight and schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSD) was reported. Other studies, however, have been less conclusive,2,4,5 with some effects either becoming attenuated or disappearing following control for potential confounders.6

Neurocognitive deficits represent a core phenotype of SSD, with multiple areas of impairment observed in the prodrome, at first episode, and chronically.711 These deficits generally do not improve with existing treatments and lead to substantial disability. In numerous studies of nonschizophrenia samples, associations between LBW and neurocognitive deficits have been consistently reported in children and adolescents.1215 Functional areas of impairment have included motor, visual-motor, language, academic, and executive performance.14 LBW was also shown to be related to IQ in a dose-dependent fashion.12

Seidman et al,16 in the New England cohorts of the National Collaborative Perinatal Project, specifically sought to address potential etiologic mechanisms by comparing birth weight, hypoxia-ischemic complications, and chronic hypoxia as risks for neurocognitive deficits in a population-based study. After controlling for sociodemographic confounders, the authors found that LBW was associated with lower cognitive performance (academic-achievement, verbal-conceptual, and perceptual-motor skill functions) at age 7; the LBW effect was stronger than hypoxia-ischemic complications or chronic hypoxia, with little co-occurrence, suggesting that neurocognitive impairment is relatively specific to LBW among these factors.16

In summary, LBW has been associated with both SSD and neurocognitive deficits. Yet, to our knowledge, no previous study has examined the relationship between LBW and neurocognitive deficits in SSD. Such work may shed light on the mechanisms by which LBW is related to SSD and begins to elucidate the underpinnings of neurocognitive deficits in SSD. Hence, the aim of the present study was to examine the relationship between birth weight and neurocognition in SSD and in matched controls in a well-defined population-based birth cohort. We hypothesized that lower birth weight would be related to neurocognitive deficits in SSD cases and that this association would be attenuated in controls.

Methods

Subjects

The sample consisted of all subjects in the Developmental Insult and Brain Anomaly in Schizophrenia (DIBS) study, one of the aims of which was to examine the relationship between early developmental risk factors and neurocognition in SSD cases and controls using a comprehensive neuropsychological protocol.17 The methods of the DIBS study, including a flow diagram of the ascertainment, recruitment, and selection of subjects, have been extensively described in a previous publication.1719 Subjects were derived from a follow-up study of SSD among offspring of mothers who were enrolled in the Child Health and Development Study (CHDS), a large birth cohort, from 1959–1966.18,20 Nearly, every pregnant woman received obstetric care from the Kaiser Permanente Medical Care Plan (KPMCP) in Alameda County, California.

Ascertainment and Diagnosis of Cases

SSD was defined as schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, delusional disorder, psychotic disorder not otherwise specified, and schizotypal personality disorder, in accord with previous studies.21 Case ascertainment was based on computerized record linkage between the CHDS and KPMCP identifiers from inpatient, outpatient, and pharmacy databases, and all diagnoses of SSD were confirmed by consensus diagnosis of 3 research psychiatrists following assessment with the Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies22 by clinicians with a minimum of a master’s degree in a mental health field who were trained to reliability. Diagnostic chart reviews were conducted for potential cases who were not interviewed, and all diagnoses were confirmed by a research psychiatrist (A.S.B.). These procedures resulted in 71 total SSD cases.

Ascertainment of the DIBS Study Sample

The DIBS study is a nested case-control study based on the larger schizophrenia follow-up of the CHDS cohort described above. All subjects who met eligibility criteria were targeted for neuropsychological assessments. Exclusion criteria have been detailed previously.17

Twenty-six SSD case subjects (13 with schizophrenia, 7 with schizoaffective disorder, and 6 with other SSD) and 25 control subjects matched to cases in a 1:1 ratio on Kaiser Permanente membership at the time of case ascertainment, date of birth (±28 days), and availability of maternal serum were enrolled and assessed using a comprehensive neuropsychological battery (see next section). Cases and controls in the DIBS were similar to subjects in the overall sample with regard to maternal age, race, education, and parity.17

All subjects provided written informed consent. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the New York State Psychiatric Institute, the Kaiser Foundation Research Institute, the University of California, San Francisco, and the San Francisco Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center.

Neuropsychological Assessment

The comprehensive neuropsychological assessment was designed to obtain global neurocognitive functioning data as well as functional domains thought to be most important in SSD.17,23 The battery included a number of tests in each functional area: executive function (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Trail Making Test Part B, Ruff Figural Fluency Test, and Verbal Fluency); motor function (Grooved Peg Board and Finger Tapping); working memory (Digit Span Forward, Digit Span Backward, Letter-Number Sequencing [LNS], and Auditory N-Back Test); IQ (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III [WAIS] subtests: Information, Digit Symbol, and Picture Completion); and verbal episodic memory (California Verbal Learning Test [CVLT]). The battery was administered over 2 days by graduate students (master’s level or higher) in a mental health–related field and supervised by 2 senior research neuropsychologists.

We also constructed composite scores for executive functioning, working memory, and verbal memory (see “Statistical Analysis” section). We utilized composite scores for these 3 domains to provide overall indicators of functioning among the major domains and to minimize the potential for spurious findings from multiple comparisons.

At the time of neuropsychological testing, case subjects were an average of 39.4 years of age (SD = 1.8), and control subjects were 40.4 years (SD = 1.8).

Birth Weight

Birth weight was measured at the time of delivery by Kaiser medical staff, documented in the newborn’s medical record, and extracted into the CHDS database.24

Statistical Analysis

We conducted stratified within group analyses of the relationship between birth weight, case status, and neurocognitive outcomes. Generalized linear models (GLM) were utilized for all analyses because these are a flexible parametric class of models appropriate for small data sets in which the conditional distribution of the response given the predictor is determined, making use of the structure of the outcome variable. Each neuropsychological test has a specific outcome structure that helps suggest the particular GLM model; the canonical link function is used in each case, as described in a previous publication on neuropsychological outcomes in the DIBS.17 Data from the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, which consists of a series of trials with binary correct/incorrect responses, was analyzed using binomial regression, as were Digit Span and LNS. For the Trail Making Test (hereinafter Trails A and Trails B) and Grooved Peg Board, gamma distributions are a natural choice because the outcomes (time) are nonnegative. For the Verbal Fluency and the Ruff Figural Fluency tests, in which the outcome is the number of correct responses and which have no predetermined upper bound, Poisson models were applied. Gaussian models were applied to the CVLT, Auditory N-Back Test, and WAIS subtests. In order to obtain composite scores for each functional area, sums of standard scores for the tests of each domain were constructed and Gaussian modeling was used.

Birth weight was defined as a continuous variable. The use of a continuous measure of birth weight was predicated on 3 reasons. First, previous studies indicate that neurocognitive performance generally increases in a linear fashion across the continuum of increasing birth weights.3,12 Second, the sample had very few newborns below the traditional 2500 g threshold for LBW (n = 4 subjects, 2 cases, and 2 controls). Mean birth weight for cases was 3420 g (SD = 602) and for controls was 3209 g (SD = 545). Third, use of a continuous variable maximizes statistical power. Therefore, we refer to the primary exposure as “lower birth weight.”

Potential Confounders

Maternal age, maternal race, maternal education (a marker of socioeconomic status), maternal smoking, off-spring sex, and use of medication at the time of neuropsychological testing were considered as potential confounders. Variables were considered a priori as potential confounders based on associations with birth weight and with neurocognition at a statistical significance threshold of P < .1. We found no association between birth weight and any of these covariates, and they were therefore not included in the statistical models to avoid inducing bias.25,26

In order to better approximate small for gestational age status, gestational age was controlled for in all neuropsychological outcome analyses (table 1).

Table 1.

Characteristics of Case and Control Subjects and Birth Weight in Ounces

Case Subjects (N = 26) Comparison Subjects (N = 25)
Birth Weight Birth Weight
Number (%) Mean (SD) P value Number (%) Mean (SD) P value
Maternal age .131 .170
    ≤29 12 (46) 127.5 (19.5) 13 (52) 108.1 (16.2)
    >29 14 (54) 114.8 (21.6) 12 (48) 118.8 (21.4)
Maternal race .655 .456
    White 14 (54) 123.2 (15.8) 14 (56) 117.4 (19.6)
    Black 10 (38) 115.8 (28.5) 8 (32) 106.6 (19.0)
    Other 2 (8) 127.0 (18.4) 3 (12) 111.0 (19.0)
Maternal education .948 .440
    ≤High school grad/trade school 16 (67) 119.9 (19.3) 14 (56) 110.5 (18.1)
    Some college/college grad/RN 8 (33) 119.4 (21.0) 11 (44) 116.6 (20.9)
Maternal smoking .853 .152
    Exposeda 11 119.7 (20.0) 10 105.7 (16.4)
    Unexposed 10 121.4 (20.9) 14 117.2 (20.2)
Offspring sex .966 .206
    Male 18 (69) 120.8 (23.5) 17 (68) 116.6 (18.4)
    Female 8 (31) 120.4 (16.6) 8 (32) 106.0 (20.2)
Antipsychotic use among casesb .29
    Used 8 (31) 127.4 (13.9)
    Never used 18 (69) 117.7 (23.5)
Stimulant and anticholinergic use among casesc .24
    Used 3 (12) 122.4 (19.8)
    Never used 23 (88) 107.0 (32.2)
a

Exposure to maternal smoking is defined as smoking at the time of pregnancy or until the pregnancy; no exposure to maternal smoking is defined as never smoking or not currently smoking.

b

Antipsychotic medications case subjects reported using at the time of testing: Clozaril, Haldol, Navane, Risperdal, Seroquel, Stelazine, Thorazine, Trilafon, and Zyprexa.

c

Stimulant and anticholinergic medications case subjects reported using at the time of testing: benztropine and methylphenidate.

Results

Table 2 presents a comparison of mean scores on each neuropsychological test for all subjects. Case subjects performed significantly worse on every test administered except for the CVLT total learning slope and the 0-Back component of the N-Back; on these 2 measures, diminished performance did not reach statistical significance.

Table 2.

Comparison of Neuropsychological Performance Between Cases and Controlsa (N = 51)

Case (n = 26) Control (n = 25) P Value from GLM
Mean SD Mean SD
Executive function
    Composite score −3.00 5.07 2.96 2.75 <.0001
    Wisconsin Card Sort Test
        Total errors 14.96 11.62 6.42 5.72 <.0001
        Perseverative errors 5.62 6.78 1.67 2.46 <.0001
        Nonperseverative errors 9.34 6.88 4.75 3.70 <.0001
    Trail Making Test
        Trails A time 41.73 17.46 22.63 5.39 <.0001
        Trails B time 104.39 50.79 52.92 12.36 <.0001
        Trails B regressed on Trails A 1.10 29.62 −8.27 10.64 .0401
    Verbal Fluency Test
        Letter fluency (total correct) 12.23 3.67 14.46 2.96 .0431
        Category fluency (total correct) 18.20 5.23 22.42 5.50 .0006
    Ruff Figural Fluency Test
        Ruff Figural Fluency (total correct) 61.88 24.09 89.25 28.65 <.0001
Working memory
    Composite score −1.03 2.28 1.07 2.03 .002
    WAIS Digit Span
        Forward last correct 5.08 1.38 5.92 1.02 .0263
        Backward last correct 3.31 1.29 4.04 1.33 .0322
    Letter-Number Sequencing (LNS)
        LNS scaled score 7.69 2.90 10.21 3.02 .0003
    Auditory N-Back
        0-Back d-prime 0.92 0.13 0.94 0.05 .8667
        2-Back d-prime 0.62 0.28 0.81 0.14 .0145
Neuromotor function
    Grooved Peg Board dominant hand time 98.27 44.63 70.04 9.48 .0007
    Finger Tap total taps for trials 1–3 85.15 32.40 116.43 22.85 <.0001
WAIS
    Full-scale IQ Estimate 93.42 14.43 107.75 13.91 <.0001
    WAIS info total scaled score 15.67 5.46 18.96 5.42 .0258
    WAIS picture completion total scaled   score 16.81 4.53 20.67 4.37 .0026
Verbal memory
    Composite score −1.35 2.88 0.36 1.64 .0569
        CVLTb trial 5 −1.21 1.31 −0.09 0.90 .0039
        CVLT total learning slope trials 1–5 −0.13 1.66 0.44 0.92 .3690

Note: Bolded values are statistically significant at the P = .05 level; GLM, generalized linear models; CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test; WAIS III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III.

a

All adjusted for gestational age.

b

All CVLT scores are standard scores.

Birth Weight and Neurocognition in Case Subjects

Lower birth weight was positively associated with impairment on several neurocognitive outcomes among those with SSD (table 3). The findings were most clearly observed on executive function performance, with significant deficits on the composite measure of executive functioning, all 3 components of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (total errors, perseverative errors, and nonperseverative errors), Trails B, and the Ruff Figural Fluency Test.

Table 3.

Birth Weight and Neuropsychological Performance in Case Subjectsa

Domain/Measure Parameter Estimate Parameter 95% CI P Value from GLM
Executive function
    Composite score 0.096 b 0.016 to 0.177 .019
    Wisconsin Card Sort Test
        Total errors −0.022 b −0.028 to −0.015 <.001
        Perseverative errors −0.016 b −0.025 to −0.007 <.001
        Nonperseverative errors −0.027 b −0.036 to −0.018 <.001
    Trail Making Test
        Trails A time 0.0000 −0.0002 to 0.0002 .778
        Trails B time 0.0001 b 0.0000 to 0.0002 <.001
        Trails B regressed on Trails A −0.628 b −1.272 to 0.017 0.056
    Verbal Fluency Test
        Letter fluency (total correct) 0.001 −0.004 to 0.007 .672
        Category fluency (total correct) 0.0005 −0.004 to 0.005 .811
    Ruff Figural Fluency Test
        Ruff Figural Fluency (total correct) 0.005 b 0.002 to 0.007 <.001
Working memory
    Composite score 0.030b −0.012 to 0.072 .168
    WAIS Digit Span
        Forward last correct 0.012b −0.002 to 0.026 .091
        Backward last correct 0.009 −0.006 to 0.023 .249
    Letter-Number Sequencing (LNS)
        LNS scaled score 0.011b 0.002 to 0.020 .012
    Auditory N-Back
        0-Back d-prime −0.002 −0.004 to −0.000 .037
        2-Back d-prime −0.001 −0.007 to 0.004 .640
Neuromotor function
    Grooved Peg Board dominant hand time 0.00003 −0.00004 to 0.00009 .408
    Finger Tap total taps for trials 1–3 0.003b 0.001 to 0.006 <.001
WAIS
    Full-scale IQ estimate 0.327b 0.093 to 0.560 .006
    WAIS info total scaled score 0.083 −0.024 to 0.191 .128
    WAIS picture completion total scaled   score 0.083b −0.003 to 0.169 .060
Verbal memory
    Composite score −0.017 −0.067 to 0.034 .526
  CVLTc trial 5 −0.008 −0.031 to 0.016 .511
        CVLT total learning slope trials 1–5 −0.009 −0.038 to 0.021 .565

Note: Bolded values are statistically significant at the P = .05 level; GLM, generalized linear models; CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test; WAIS III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III.

a

All adjusted for gestational age.

b

Parameter indicates worse performance for lower birth weight.

c

All CVLT scores are standard scores.

With regard to working memory, lower birth weight was associated with significant impairment on the LNS test. Birth weight was not significantly related to the other working memory measures (composite score, Auditory 2-Back, or Digit Span Backward). For general intellectual function, lower birth weight was significantly associated with decreased full-scale IQ. Additionally, lower birth weight was related to diminished performance on the Finger Tapping Test (total taps). Verbal memory and verbal fluency were not associated with birth weight.

Birth Weight and Neurocognition in Control Subjects

Birth weight was not significantly associated with neurocognitive performance among the controls. Statistical trends were observed between lower birth weight and worse performance on the information (P = .06) and Digit Span Backward (P = .10) subtests of the WAIS, and the long delay free recall condition of the CVLT (P = .08) (table 4).

Table 4.

Birth Weight and Neuropsychological Performance in Control Subjectsa

Domain/Measure Parameter Estimate Parameter 95% CI P Value from GLM
Executive function
    Composite score 0.007 −0.059 to 0.074 .828
    Wisconsin Card Sort Test
        Total errors −0.005 −0.016 to 0.005 .331
        Perseverative errors 0.006 −0.014 to 0.026 .538
        Nonperseverative errors −0.010 −0.023 to 0.003 .132
    Trail Making Test
        Trails A time −0.0001 −0.0003 to 0.0001 .548
        Trails B time −0.00002 −0.00011 to 0.00007 .672
        Trails B regressed on Trails A 0.003 −0.242 to 0.247 .983
    Verbal Fluency Test
        Letter fluency (total correct) −0.003 −0.009 to 0.003 .362
        Category fluency (total correct) 0.004 −0.001 to 0.009 .106
    Ruff Figural Fluency Test
        Ruff Figural Fluency (total correct) −0.0003 −0.0027 to 0.0022 .832
Working memory
    Composite score 0.026 −0.026 to 0.078 .323
    WAIS Digit Span
        Forward last correct 0.001 −0.018 to 0.020 .901
        Backward last correct 0.016b −0.003 to 0.034 .099
    Letter-Number Sequencing (LNS)
        LNS scaled score 0.003 −0.007 to 0.013 .600
 Auditory N-Back
  0-Back d-prime −0.000 −0.001 to 0.001 .998
  2-Back d-prime 0.000 −0.003 to 0.003 .831
Neuromotor function
    Grooved Peg Board dominant hand time −0.00002 −0.00006 to 0.00002 .398
    Finger Tap total taps for trials 1–3 0.0001 −0.0020 to 0.0022 .906
WAIS
    Full-scale IQ Estimate 0.228 −0.058 to 0.514 .119
        WAIS info total scaled score 0.106b −0.006 to 0.218 .063
        WAIS picture completion total scaled    score 0.017 −0.083 to 0.117 .738
Verbal memory
    Composite score 0.006 −0.032 to 0.045 .748
  CVLTc trial 5 0.006 −0.015 to 0.027 .574
        CVLT total learning slope trials 1–5 0.000 −0.021 to 0.022 .976

Note: Bolded values are statistically significant at the P = .05 level; GLM, generalized linear models; CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test; WAIS III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III.

a

All adjusted for gestational age.

b

Parameter indicates worse performance for lower birth weight.

c

All CVLT scores are standard scores.

Discussion

Lower birth weight was consistently associated with impairment in executive function, working memory, general intellectual function, and neuromotor performance for case subjects but not for control subjects. To our knowledge, this is the first time that birth weight has been examined in relation to neurocognition in a sample of adult patients with SSD and controls. Given that most of the cases were well within the “normal” weight range, with few births below the traditional 2500 g threshold, our findings extend those of Abel et al,3 who demonstrated that decreasing birth weight acts incrementally to increase SSD risk.

Performance on executive functioning was impaired on a number of tests in this domain. The findings suggest that lower birth weight is related to impairment in cognitive switching (set shifting) and abstract reasoning, as indicated by performance on the Trails B and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; and perseveration, as indicated by performance on the Ruff Figural Fluency Test and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Nonverbal fluency also appeared to be associated with lower birth weight.

Interestingly, lower birth weight cases also evidenced a statistical trend for diminished performance on the WAIS picture completion subtest (P = .06). Together with the results on the Ruff Figural Fluency Test, these findings may indicate broad visuospatial deficits. Visuospatial impairment has been observed in offspring with very LBW in previous studies.2729 Moreover, no relationship was found between lower birth weight and verbal fluency in cases, supporting possible specificity to figural fluency. It is also possible that diminished performance on the Ruff Figural Fluency Test may be explained by the fact that self-generation of novel designs, which this instrument assesses, requires abstraction and greater strategic resources as compared with word generation, which is measured by verbal fluency tests. Consistent with this notion, picture completion also requires some level of abstract reasoning to properly sequence the pictures.

The working memory results varied by functional ability area as well. Within the case group, lower birth weight was significantly related to worse performance on LNS. While similar to Digit Span Backward, LNS assesses visuospatial working memory, which Digit Span does not.30,31 The 2-Back component of the N-Back, which was also not associated with lower birth weight, is believed to be more specific to working memory updating. In light of the differences captured by these working memory tests and the observed pattern of findings, lower birth weight may therefore be related to impaired visuospatial working memory. Direct tests of visuospatial working memory would, of course, be needed to confirm this assertion.

Similarly, the relationship between lower birth weight and neuromotor performance differed between the 2 measures assessed, with a significant relationship for Finger Tapping but a nonsignificant relationship for the Grooved Peg Board Test. Slower Finger Tapping, a measure of self-directed manual motor speed captures a different functional ability than manual precision, as measured by the Grooved Peg Board Test.32

The finding of no association between birth weight and Verbal Fluency, combined with the results of the CVLT, suggest that birth weight may spare verbal ability. This may also help explain the nonsignificant result on the WAIS information subtest, which tends to draw on verbal skills.

Lower birth weight can result from a host of risks, including maternal infection, genetic factors, congenital anomalies, inadequate nutrition, environmental toxins, and placental factors.33 Hence, each, or some combination, of these risk factors may account for the observed findings. While the present sample is not sufficiently powered to test for relationships between these antecedents and neurocognitive dysfunction, we believe that this can be accomplished in future research studies with larger sample sizes.

It is worth emphasizing that, with the exception of a few scattered statistical trends, there were no relationships between lower birth weight and neurocognition among controls. This lack of association suggests that SSD cases may be more vulnerable than controls to the neurodevelopmental effects on neurocognitive functioning associated with lower birth weight or its antecedent exposures. We therefore postulate that the predisposition for neurocognitive deficits likely requires additional environmental risk factors or genetic mutations that are related to SSD vulnerability and which interact with the determinants of lower birth weight. Given the exploratory nature of the study, however, further research on these questions is warranted.

Longitudinal studies of LBW and neurocognition in nonpsychiatric populations suggest that as LBW children age, their neurocognitive performance often catches up to within a normal range of scores compared with their peers. This process is often referred to as “normalization.” For example, the Port Pirie birth cohort utilized age appropriate IQ assessments at aged 2, 4, 7, and 11–13 years old, concluding that differences in cognition became progressively attenuated as the cohort aged until there were no differences at ages 11–13.13 It is possible that the observed findings in those who develop SSD reflect a premorbid neurocognitive process that interferes with the neural plasticity that could explain normalization. Our subjects were tested at age 40, meaning that any normalization of performance, which might have occurred, would have already been completed by the time of the testing. This would tend to “flatten” the relationship between performance and birth weight in our subjects, making it less likely that we would observe an effect of birth weight on neurocognitive functioning. Limited neural plasticity is thought to explain the lack of normalization in extremely LBW infants (below 750 g) who do not experience normalization over development but remain substantially impaired throughout the life course.34 We speculate that a similar process occurs for those who later develop SSD. This process may interact with already impaired cognitive reserve found in schizophrenia to give rise to diminished normalization of cognitive impairment.

More broadly, these findings provide further support for the “fetal programming hypothesis,” which is based on a body of literature indicating that the risk for several chronic adult-onset diseases, including depression, increases as birth weight decreases.4,3437 This phenomenon has been attributed to a number of risk factors and mechanisms including nutritional deprivation and hormonal alterations and a possible interaction with genetic vulnerabilities. It is plausible that similar mechanisms could account for the associations between neurodevelopmental exposures and later onset of neurocognitive deficits in SSD.

Strengths and Limitations

The study had several notable strengths, including the fact that it is based on a well-defined birth cohort, utilized prospective, objective measures of birth weight, and included controls that were representative of the source population that gave rise to the cases. There were also some limitations. First, the sample size was modest. As a result, we are unable to definitively conclude that the nonsignificant findings are not the result of limited power. Second, the multiple comparisons may have led to inflated probability of Type I error. It is worth noting, though, that lower birth weight was significantly related to diminished performance on executive functioning in SSD cases as measured by the composite score, and this resulted from a convergence of individual test findings within this domain. Additionally, within domains, a limited number of tests were administered, and composite scores were analyzed at the domain level, reducing chance associations. We also observed consistency for specific functional abilities on some measures within a domain (eg, cognitive switching on Trails B and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Tests) but not on others that assess different abilities within a domain (eg, visuospatial ability on LNS but no association for Digit Span which does not assess that ability), diminishing the probability that these findings result from chance.38,39

Third, subjects were assessed in adulthood and intervening risks, which may have moderated the outcome, were not measured over the life course. This will need to await further studies that collect longitudinal data on neurocognition and moderating variables over the life course. Even so, this is a prospective nested case-control study drawn from a well-defined cohort and followed into adulthood, with birth record data.

Furthermore, lower birth weight was related to deficits in several additional domains of neurocognitive function, including working memory, neuromotor function, and general intellect, which are each adversely affected in SSD. The lack of associations between lower birth weight and these domains in controls, and the biological plausibility of this risk factor in neurocognitive deficits based on studies of SSD and of neuropsychological function, add further to the credence of these results.

Conclusions

In summary, this exploratory analysis of the relationship between birth weight and neurocognition in SSD, in a well-defined, representative birth cohort revealed that lower birth weight is associated with impairments in executive function and certain aspects of working memory, neuromotor function, and generalized intellectual function in patients with schizophrenia. These findings were not observed in controls, suggesting that neurocognitive development in patients with SSD may be more vulnerable to prenatal factors or genetic variants that give rise to lower birth weight. Given the modest sample size, these findings are suggestive, but not definitive, and require independent replications in larger samples. Moreover, since the cognitive measures were obtained at only one point in time, it would be enlightening to test relationships between birth weight and the trajectory of cognitive function in samples with longitudinal data as well as potential mediating or moderating factors over the life course. Finally, it would be of value to assess whether the observed associations are found in other psychiatric syndromes, such as bipolar disorder.

The focus on neurocognitive phenotypes in SSD may improve our ability to detect risk factors for these disorders that may not be readily apparent in studies in which the clinical phenotype is the sole outcome. This may lead to new preventive strategies and a better understanding of the etiopathogenic mechanisms that underlie the cognitive deficits in this disorder, a question of critical relevance given their marked impact on occupational and social functioning, and the lack of effective treatments for this dimension of SSD.

Funding

National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH ) (1RO1MH-60249 and 1KO2-MH65422 to A.S.B.); NIMH Training (5T32MH-13043-40 to D.F.); National Institute on Child Health and Development (N01-HD-1-3334 and NO1-HD-6-3258 to B.A. Cohn); National Institute on Aging (R01AG-018363, R01AG-022381, and RO1AG-22982 to W.S.K.).

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Catherine Schaefer, PhD, Barbara Cohn, PhD, and John Poole, PhD for their contributions to this work. The authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study.

References

  • 1.Cannon M, Jones PB, Murray RM. Obstetric complications and schizophrenia: historical and meta-analytic review. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1080–1092. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Moilanen K, Jokelainen J, Jones PB, Hartikainen AL, Jarvelin MR, Isohanni M. Deviant intrauterine growth and risk of schizophrenia: a 34-year follow-up of the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort. Schizophr Res. 2010;124:223–230. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Abel KM, Wicks S, Susser ES, et al. Birth weight, schizophrenia, and adult mental disorder is risk confined to the smallest babies? Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:923–930. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Welham J, Isohanni M, Jones P, McGrath J. The antecedents of schizophrenia: a review of birth cohort studies. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:603–623. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gunnell D, Harrison G, Whitley E, Lewis G, Tynelius P, Rasmussen F. The association of fetal and childhood growth with risk of schizophrenia. Cohort study of 720,000 Swedish men and women. Schizophr Res. 2005;79:315–322. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.07.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Byrne M, Agerbo E, Bennedsen B, Eaton WW, Mortensen PB. Obstetric conditions and risk of first admission with schizophrenia: a Danish national register based study. Schizophr Res. 2007;97:51–59. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Heinrichs RW. The primacy of cognition in schizophrenia. Am Psychol. 2005;60:229–242. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.3.229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mesholam-Gately RI, Giuliano AJ, Goff KP, Faraone SV, Seidman LJ. Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:315–336. doi: 10.1037/a0014708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Frangou S. Cognitive function in early onset schizophrenia: a selective review. Front Hum Neurosci. 2010;3 doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.079.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kurtz MM. Neurocognitive impairment across the lifespan in schizophrenia: an update. Schizophr Res. 2005;74:15–26. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Elvevag B, Goldberg TE. Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia is the core of the disorder. Crit Rev Neurobiol. 2000;14:1–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Shenkin SD, Starr JM, Deary IJ. Birth weight and cognitive ability in childhood: a systematic review. Psychol Bull. 2004;130:989–1013. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Tong S. Birthweight and cognitive development during childhood. J Paediatr Child Health. 2006;42:98–103. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2006.00805.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Aylward GP. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of infants born prematurely. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2005;26:427–440. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200512000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Aarnoudse-Moens CSH, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J. Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics. 2009;124:717–728. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Seidman LJ, Buka SL, Goldstein JM, Horton NJ, Rieder RO, Tsuang MT. The relationship of prenatal and perinatal complications to cognitive functioning at age 7 in the New England cohorts of the National Collaborative Perinatal Project. Schizophr Bull. 2000;26:309–321. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Brown AS, Vinogradov S, Kremen WS, et al. Prenatal exposure to maternal infection and executive dysfunction in adult schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166:683–690. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08010089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Susser ES, Schaefer CA, Brown AS, Begg MD, Wyatt RJ. The design of the prenatal determinants of schizophrenia study. Schizophr Bull. 2000;26:257–273. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Brown AS, Begg MD, Gravenstein S, et al. Serologic evidence of prenatal influenza in the etiology of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:774–780. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.van de Berg BJ. The California Child Health and Developmental Studies, in Handbook. New York, NY: Praeger; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kendler KS, Neale MC, Walsh D. Evaluating the spectrum concept of schizophrenia in the Roscommon Family Study. Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152:749–754. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.5.749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Nurnberger JI, Jr, Blehar MC, Kaufmann CA, et al. Diagnostic interview for genetic studies. Rationale, unique features, and training. NIMH Genetics Initiative. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51:849–859. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950110009002. discussion 863–1844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kremen WS, Vinogradov S, Poole JH, et al. Cognitive decline in schizophrenia from childhood to midlife: a 33-year longitudinal birth cohort study. Schizophr Res. 2010;118:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.van den Berg BJ. Morbidity of low birth weight and/or preterm children compared to that of the “mature”. Pediatrics. 1968;42:590. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Susser ES, Schwartz S, Morabia A, Bromet EJ. Psychiatric Epidemiology: Searching for the Causes of Mental Disorders. Oxford, UK: New York; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL. Modern Epidemiology. 3rd ed. Philadelphia, PA: Wolters Kluwer Health/Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Taylor HG, Minich N, Bangert B, Filpek PA, Hack M. Long-term neuropsychological outcomes of very low birth weight: associations with early risks for periventricular brain insults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:987–1004. doi: 10.1017/s1355617704107078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Downie ALS, Jakobson LS, Frisk V, Ushycky I. Periventricular brain injury, visual motion processing, and reading and spelling abilities in children who were extremely low birthweight. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003;9:440–449. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703930098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Aylward GP. Cognitive and neuropsychological outcomes: more than IQ scores. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2002;8:234–240. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.10043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Crowe SF. Does the letter number sequencing task measure anything more than digit span? Assessment. 2000;7:113–117. doi: 10.1177/107319110000700202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition, Technical Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. 3rd ed. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Creasy RK, Resnik R, Iams JD. Maternal-Fetal Medicine. 5th ed. Philadelphia, PA: W.B. Saunders Co.; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Taylor HG, Minich NM, Klein N, Hack M. Longitudinal outcomes of very low birth weight: neuropsychological findings. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:149–163. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704102038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Thompson C, Syddall H, Rodin I, Osmond C, Barker DJP. Birth weight and the risk of depressive disorder in late life. Br J Psychiatry. 2001;179:450–455. doi: 10.1192/bjp.179.5.450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Wiles NJ, Peters TJ, Leon DA, Lewis G. Birth weight and psychological distress at age 45-51 years - Results from the Aberdeen Children of the 1950s cohort study. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;187:21–28. doi: 10.1192/bjp.187.1.21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Barker DJP, Osmond C, Kajantie E, Eriksson JG. Growth and chronic disease: findings in the Helsinki Birth Cohort. Ann Hum Biol. 2009;36:445–458. doi: 10.1080/03014460902980295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Groth-Marnat G. Neuropsychological Assessment in Clinical Practice: A Guide to Test Interpretation and Integration. New York, NY: Wiley; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Schizophrenia Bulletin are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES