Abstract
Shame has been shown to predict sexual HIV transmission risk behavior, medication non-adherence, symptomatic HIV or AIDS, and symptoms of depression and PTSD. However, there remains a dearth of tools to measure the specific constructs of HIV-related and sexual abuse-related shame. To ameliorate this gap, we present a 31-item measure that assesses HIV and sexual abuse-related shame, and the impact of shame on HIV-related health behaviors. A diverse sample of 271 HIV-positive men and women who were sexually abused as children completed the HIV and Abuse Related Shame Inventory (HARSI) among other measures. An exploratory factor analysis supported the retention of three-factors, explaining 56.7% of the sample variance. These internally consistent factors showed good test–retest reliability, and sound convergent and divergent validity using eight well-established HIV specific and general psychosocial criterion measures. Unlike stigma or discrimination, shame is potentially alterable through individually-focused interventions, making the measurement of shame clinically meaningful.
Keywords: HIV-related shame, Abuse-related shame, Exploratory factor analysis, Childhood sexual abuse, HIV/AIDS
Introduction
Approximately 1.1 million people in the United States are living with HIV/AIDS [1, 2]. As many as 33 to 53% of these individuals have also experienced childhood sexual abuse (CSA) [3–6], rates at least double those observed in the general population [7–10]. Individuals living with both HIV and a history of CSA face diverse challenges, including high rates of mental health problems such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), depression, anxiety, and borderline personality disorder; substance use problems; adult revictimization; homelessness and incarceration [4, 11, 12]. Further, people living with HIV who have a history of CSA demonstrate higher rates of HIV transmission risk behavior such as unprotected sex with HIV-negative or serostatus unknown partners [13, 14] and exchanging sex for money, drugs, or shelter [15] over those who do not have such a history.
People living with HIV and people who have experienced CSA have each separately been noted as being at risk for experiencing shame [16–19]. Shame is a painful emotion resulting from negative self-evaluation following a perceived deviation from a social or moral code [20–22], and is accompanied by feelings of worthlessness, rejection, isolation, and the desire to disappear. The globalized sense of self-inferiority experienced in shame is thought to make individuals more prone to psychological maladjustment than other emotions [21, 22]. In fact, shame has been shown to have negative impacts on behavioral, physical, clinical, and psychosocial outcomes [19, 21, 22]. HIV-related shame has been shown to be a predictor of sexual HIV transmission risk behavior [23], HIV medication non-adherence [24], reduced health-related quality of life [19], as well as non-attendance in clinical trials [25]. Similarly, sexual abuse-related shame has been related to greater depression and PTSD symptoms and decreased self-esteem [17, 18], adult revictimization [26, 27], dissociation [28], and greater verbal and physical intimate partner and family conflict [29]. Finally, those with higher levels of CSA-related shame were less likely to improve in group therapy [30].
Despite this body of work, the measurement of HIV-related and CSA-related shame is still nascent. Rather than evaluating HIV-related shame as a unique factor, HIV-related shame is often included as a component of HIV-related stigma or with other related but distinct constructs such as blame, judgment, or social isolation [31, 32]. While shame and internalized stigma are sometimes considered equivalent, we consider them to be related but distinct concepts. Stigma involves the experience of blame, exclusion, or rejection due to another’s negative social judgment [33], and thus is a socially-constructed concept derived from culturally embedded power structures to mark a perceived defect or disgrace, making stigma resistant to change [34–36]. On the other hand, shame is an internally-constructed emotional response which may be influenced, but not controlled, by stigmatizing attitudes perceived to be present in the community or internalized by the individual [19]. A consequence of this conceptualization is that shame, unlike stigma and discrimination, is more amenable to change through psychosocial interventions [21, 37]. Indeed, research supports this view of shame and stigma as related but separate constructs [37–39].
We are only aware of one measure which separately assesses HIV-related shame—a three-item subscale from a measure of HIV-related stigma, which has not been tested for reliability or validity, and contains only a marginally acceptable number of items to be considered for factor analysis [40]. In the CSA literature, many studies have used generalized measures of shame instead of abuse-specific shame [26, 28, 29]. However, this is problematic as correlations between abuse specific shame and general shame are low [16], and measures of general shame have been shown to have diminished validity in clinical samples [41]. We are aware of only two measures of CSA-related shame—a four-item measure for children [17, 18], and the 18-item abuse-related beliefs questionnaire (ARBQ) [42], which contains subscales measuring guilt, shame, and resilience, but has only been tested on women. Further, despite the high prevalence of CSA among those living with HIV, and the negative impact of shame resulting from each, to our knowledge there are no measures which (a) assess HIV-related and CSA-related shame concurrently; (b) directly ask individuals how shame might impact behaviors such as HIV transmission risk behavior, medication non-adherence, or serostatus disclosure; or (c) assess how HIV-related shame might affect shame experienced due to membership in another marginalized group, such as being gay, engaging in sex work, or using intravenous drugs.
There is a clear need for more extensive measurement of shame specific to HIV infection and to CSA. Therefore, the purpose of this study is to describe the development of a measure of shame regarding sexual abuse and HIV infection, including evaluating the factor structure and establishing the reliability and validity of HIV and Abuse Related Shame Inventory (HARSI). The development of the HARSI was influenced by shame-based PTSD [43], which describes shame as both a primary and secondary emotion. Shame as a primary emotion results from the perceived loss of social value and serves as a mechanism for maintaining social rank and order, leads to submissive behaviors and reinforces perceptions of the self as damaged. Shame as a secondary emotion results from a cognitive appraisal of oneself as being weak or acting contrary to social values, and can lead to intrusive and negative thoughts and avoidant behavior. Feelings of shame can influence avoidant behaviors, withdrawal from social networks, negative self-image, reduced self-care and increased health risk behavior, and is therefore a critical variable to understand among people living with HIV. While the HARSI was not designed to differentiate between primary and secondary shame, it was designed to bridge the gaps noted in the literature above by: (a) assessing both HIV-related and CSA-related shame, allowing for relative comparison of the two specific types of shame in populations which are dually impacted; (b) reflecting multiple aspects of shame, such as rejection, self-reproach, and the desire to withdraw; (c) assessing the impact of shame on HIV-related health behaviors (e.g., disclosure, medication adherence, treatment utilization); and (d) assessing the effect of concurrent sources of shame.
Methods
Participants
As part of a randomized, controlled trial for a group intervention for coping with HIV and CSA [11, 14], HIV-positive adults (age 18 or older) who had experienced sexual abuse as a child and/or adolescent were recruited between March 2002 and January 2004 from community organizations and health care clinics serving those with HIV/AIDS in New York City. Flyers and printed recruitment material advertised a group intervention for people living with HIV who had experienced CSA. Participants were included in the study if they had experienced sexual abuse as a child (age 12 years and under) and/or adolescent (age 13–17 years), defined as any unwanted touching of a sexual nature by an adult or by someone at least 5 years older than the participant when the incident occurred. Participants were excluded if they presented with impaired mental status or active psychosis. Further, to prevent participants in need of immediate care from being randomly assigned to a waitlist condition in the larger trial, those with acute distress due to sexual revictimization within the past month or severe depressive symptoms (Beck Depression Inventory [BDI] score >30) [44] were also excluded. All participants provided informed consent and procedures were approved by an institutional review board.
Of the 333 potential participants that were screened for eligibility, 21 did not meet the aforementioned criteria (seven had not been sexually abused, one had experienced sexual revictimization in the past month, six were cognitively impaired, and seven were severely depressed) and 41 participants were not enrolled in the study for assorted reasons (23 could not be located following screening, 13 were unable to continue, three were incarcerated, and two died). Two hundred and seventy-one individuals (137 male, 130 female, four transgendered) were eligible for the study and returned to complete the baseline assessment. For all analyses, the four transgendered participants were categorized according to their self-identification (three female, one male). Participants were diverse based on sexual orientation (men, 75.4% gay/homosexual; women, 75.6% heterosexual) and ethnicity (68% African American, 17% Hispanic/Latino, 10% Caucasian, and 5% other), and had experienced high levels of stress and trauma in their lifetimes. The level of sexual abuse was severe: 90% had experienced penetrative anal or vaginal abuse prior to the age of 18, 58% were sexually abused during both childhood and adolescence, and 55% were sexually revictimized as adults. Additionally, 38% of study participants met DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for PTSD, 64% of participants had been homeless, 42% had been incarcerated, and 49% had traded sex for money or drugs.
Procedures
All interested participants were screened for sexual abuse history using a structured clinical interview based on a modified and expanded version of the traumatic experiences questionnaire [45]. Eligible participants returned to complete a baseline assessment using a computer assisted personal interview, after which they were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: the HIV and trauma coping group intervention, a time-matched support group intervention, or a waitlist control condition. Both groups provided a supportive environment for participants to address issues of HIV and trauma [46]. Participants randomly assigned to the waitlist control condition were subsequently re-administered the baseline assessment and then randomly assigned to one of the two treatment conditions after 4 months. Thus, all study participants were ultimately randomly assigned one of the two active treatment conditions. Any participant who experienced an extreme level of distress, including suicidal intention, was referred for evaluation and additional services. Post assessments were administered using a computer assisted personal interview within 2 weeks following completion of the group interventions. Participants received $35 for completion of the baseline assessment and $45 for completion of the post assessment.
Shame Measure
HIV and Abuse Related Shame Inventory (HARSI)
Based on existing literature linking shame with traumatic stress [43, 47], we developed a conceptual model of the construct of shame, and, with the goal of developing a brief measure, identified components of shame relevant to both HIV infection and CSA. These include: difficulties with disclosure; decreased social standing and rejection; self-reproach; believing one deserves to be mistreated; a desire to hide or disappear; and feelings of defect, worthlessness, and shame. Additionally, we wanted to identify the impact of shame on HIV-specific health behaviors. Again, based on existing literature, we identified four key components of health behavior that may be particularly impacted by shame: HIV risk behavior, healthcare and service utilization, HIV serostatus disclosure, and interacting with others. Finally, items were created to assess how individuals might feel shame as a result of being HIV-positive in conjunction with membership in another marginalized group, including: being gay, having been sexually abused, being a drug user, or engaging in transactional sex.
We created 47 items to assess shame related to HIV infection and shame related to sexual abuse which were divided into four scales: (A) HIV-related shame, (B) Impact of HIV-related shame on behavior, (C) Sexual abuse-related shame, and (D) Shame interaction effects. note that scales A (HIV-related shame) and B (impact of shame on behavior) were thought of as general scales that could have applicability across a range of groups living with HIV, while scale C (sexual abuse-related shame) was considered a more focused measure to compare and differentiate shame related to sexual abuse from shame related to living with HIV.
The original 47 items were pilot tested with 39 people living with HIV who had experienced CSA. Based on Cronbach’s alphas and item–total correlations, we eliminated four items from the HIV-related shame scale (scale A). All four of these items were reverse coded, leaving one reverse coded item in the final version of this scale. We also rewrote one item on the HIV-related shame scale to make its intention clearer. Changes made to the impact of HIV-related shame on behavior scale (scale B) included eliminating one item that referred to using a needle exchange that had low frequency of endorsement, merging two pairs of items with similar content that showed response patterns suggesting redundancy, and rewriting two other items to make their intention clearer. Finally, we eliminated two reverse coded items from the sexual abuse-related shame scale based on reliability data.
Abbreviated versions of the 38 HARSI items included in this study are listed by scale on Table 1; full items are available from the authors. For each item, participants were asked to indicate how true each statement was for them during the past month on a 5-point Likert scale (0 = not at all, 1 = a little bit, 2 = somewhat, 3 = quite a bit, 4 = very much).
Table 1.
Factor
|
% Data missing | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | ||
Scale A: HIV-related shame | |||||
A1: Hard to tell people about my infection | 0.812 | 0.076 | − 0.080 | − 0.335 | 0 |
A2: Failed to live up to my expectations | 0.684 | − 0.020 | 0.029 | − 0.042 | 0 |
A3: Expect people to think less of me | 0.750 | − 0.068 | 0.157 | − 0.051 | 0.4 |
A4: Put myself down for having HIV | 0.708 | − 0.016 | 0.207 | 0.167 | 0 |
A5: Feel defective because of HIV | 0.792 | − 0.108 | 0.085 | 0.258 | 0 |
A6: Ashamed HIV + | 0.835 | − 0.042 | 0.082 | 0.148 | 0.4 |
A7: Expect rejection when others find out | 0.758 | − 0.121 | 0.175 | 0.084 | 0.4 |
A8: Feel worthless because HIV | 0.659 | − 0.001 | 0.211 | 0.232 | 0 |
A9: Ashamed by HIV symptoms | 0.682 | 0.019 | 0.102 | 0.230 | 0.4 |
A10: Hide infection from others | 0.824 | 0.199 | − 0.067 | − 0.116 | 0 |
A11: Dread others will find out HIV status | 0.741 | 0.152 | − 0.050 | 0.087 | 0 |
A12: Deserve to be mistreated | 0.210 | 0.559 | 0.182 | − 0.068 | 0 |
A13R: I accept myself as an HIV+ person | 0.190 | 0.137 | 0.137 | − 0.071 | 0.4 |
A14: HIV makes me want to disappear | 0.518 | 0.143 | 0.231 | 0.144 | 0 |
Scale B: Impact of HIV-related shame on behavior | |||||
B1: Kept me from using condom | 0.233 | 0.572 | − 0.119 | 0.023 | 7.7 |
B2: Kept me from cleaning needles | − 0.292 | 0.866 | − 0.078 | 0.247 | 30.6 |
B3: Kept me from interacting with others | 0.279 | 0.590 | − 0.092 | − 0.005 | 2.2 |
B4: Kept me from applying for services | 0.202 | 0.641 | 0.143 | − 0.057 | 0.7 |
B5: Kept me from telling status to family | 0.618 | 0.316 | − 0.111 | − 0.135 | 1.1 |
B6: Kept me from telling status to sex partner | 0.396 | 0.567 | − 0.169 | − 0.006 | 7.4 |
B7: Kept me from telling status to drug partner | 0.003 | 0.846 | 0.045 | 0.207 | 34.7 |
B8: Delayed me from getting healthcare | 0.202 | 0.611 | 0.159 | 0.050 | 2.6 |
B9: Kept me from adhering To HIV treatment | − 0.048 | 0.760 | 0.222 | − 0.129 | 1.5 |
B10: Kept me from getting info on HIV | − 0.020 | 0.794 | 0.238 | − 0.184 | 0.7 |
Scale C: Sexual abuse-related shame | |||||
C1: Hard to tell people about abuse | 0.061 | 0.025 | 0.755 | − 0.188 | 0 |
C2: Think less of me because of abuse | 0.009 | 0.048 | 0.829 | − 0.061 | 0.4 |
C3: Ashamed of abuse | 0.146 | − 0.065 | 0.837 | − 0.076 | 0 |
C4: Feel worthless because of abuse | 0.070 | − 0.035 | 0.884 | − 0.014 | 0 |
C5: Hide my abuse from others | 0.085 | 0.069 | 0.775 | − 0.181 | 0 |
C6: Put self down for abuse | − 0.073 | 0.025 | 0.875 | 0.151 | 0 |
C7: Abuse makes me feel defective | − 0.020 | 0.012 | 0.883 | 0.117 | 0 |
C8: Others reject me when they find out | − 0.078 | 0.038 | 0.865 | 0.091 | 1.1 |
C9: Abuse makes me want to disappear | 0.017 | 0.028 | 0.779 | 0.124 | 0 |
Scale D: Shame interaction effects | |||||
D1: Interaction gay/HIV + shame | 0.211 | 0.253 | 0.307 | 0.115 | 21.8 |
D2: Interaction abuse/HIV + shame | 0.112 | 0.156 | 0.632 | 0.138 | 0.7 |
D3: Interaction IDU/HIV + shame | 0.233 | 0.235 | − 0.045 | 0.622 | 33.9 |
D4: Interaction drug use/HIV + shame | 0.154 | 0.094 | 0.181 | 0.767 | 18.1 |
D5: Interaction sex work/HIV + shame | 0.162 | 0.041 | 0.143 | 0.709 | 27.7 |
Measures for Convergent and Divergent Validity
As shame has been associated with symptomatic HIV or AIDS [40], HIV transmission risk behavior [23], and psychological distress [48], the following measures were used to estimate convergent and divergent validity.
HIV Physical Symptoms [49]
HIV physical symptoms were assessed over a 4 month retrospective period using a 20-item self report measure. Participants were asked to rate the severity of 12 symptoms found in HIV disease and eight more general symptoms using a 5-point Likert scale (0 = not present, 1 = mild, 2 = moderate, 3 = severe, 4 = very severe). Items are summed to generate a total score, with higher scores reflecting greater symptom severity. This scale exhibited strong internal consistency with the current sample (Chronbach’s alpha = 0.93).
HIV-Related Stressors [50]
This 13-item measure was used to measure HIV-specific stressors (e.g., AIDS discrimination, drug regimen, HIV illness of friends or relatives). Respondents rated how often they felt stressed by each of these items during the past 4 months on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (always). A total score was calculated by summing the score for each item. In the current sample the internal consistency for this measure was good (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.89).
Sexual HIV Transmission Risk Behavior [23]
Participants reported the number of times they engaged in oral, anal, and vaginal intercourse with and without condoms in the past 4 months. They also reported the HIV status of each partner with whom they engaged in each behavior in the previous 4 months. As sexual behavior data formed highly skewed and zero-inflated distributions, dichotomous coding was used to categorize participant responses into a dichotomous variable representing sexual HIV transmission risk behavior. This variable was coded affirmatively if the participant had engaged in any unprotected anal or vaginal sexual behavior with a HIV-negative or serostatus unknown partner in the previous 4 months.
Traumatic Symptom Inventory (TSI) [51]
The TSI is a 100-item self-report measure that assesses acute and chronic posttraumatic symptoms. Each item reflects a potential trauma symptom and was rated according to its frequency of occurrence over the preceding 6 months on a 4-point Likert scale (0 = never, 3 = often). The TSI consists of ten clinical scales that can be categorized into three broad symptom clusters labeled: trauma-related symptoms, mood and anxiety symptoms, and behavioral difficulties. The trauma-related symptoms cluster assesses trauma-specific symptom domains and contains the following four scales: intrusive experiences, defensive avoidance, dissociation, and impaired self-reference. The mood and anxiety symptoms cluster assesses symptom domains that are frequently associated with trauma and contains the following three scales: anger/irritability, depression, and anxious arousal. The behavioral difficulties cluster assesses common behavioral disruptions that are associated with trauma, particularly sexual trauma, and contains the following three scales: sexual concerns, dysfunctional sexual behavior, and tension reduction behavior. The clinical scales of the TSI are internally consistent (Cronbach’s alphas = 0.81–0.91, current sample).
Symptom Checklist 90—Revised (SCL-90-R) [52]
The SCL-90-R has been used extensively to assess both global psychiatric distress and specific types of distress symptoms. Participants are asked to rate how much they were distressed by each of 90 psychiatric symptoms on a 5-point Likert scale over the past 7 days (0 = not at all, 4 = extremely). The mean score across the 90-items produces a Global Severity Index (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.98, current sample).
Perceived Availability of Social Support (PASS) [53]
Perceived availability of social support (PASS) was measured using a subscale (seven items) of the social relationship scale. The PASS assesses whether an individual believes he or she would have support from others given various situations, such as inability to get out of bed for several weeks. Responses are given on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (definitely no) to 5 (definitely yes). Good internal consistency was observed in the current sample (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.85).
To evaluate convergent and divergent validity, Pearson correlations between each Shame scale and each validity measure at baseline were computed, except for sexual HIV transmission risk behavior, where a Spearmen–Brown correlation was computed instead. It was hypothesized that the shame scales would be positively correlated with HIV-specific measures, with HIV-related shame having the strongest relationships with these variables, except for sexual HIV transmission risk behavior, which would have the strongest relationship with impact of HIV-related shame on behavior. It was also hypothesized that Shame scales would be positively correlated with all of the psychosocial measures except perceived availability of social support, which would be negatively related to shame. Further, it was hypothesized that sexual abuse-related shame would have the strongest correlations with psychosocial measures.
Statistical Analysis
Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA)
Factor analysis was confined to EFA, as the sample size restricted us from splitting the data into validation and confirmatory samples [54], as is ideal. To validate our constructs, EFA was performed in Mplus version 5.21 [55] using the robust WLS mean- and variance-adjusted χ2 test (WLSMV), which has been shown to have good statistical properties in testing model fit with relatively small sample sizes (N = 200) and ordinal outcomes [56, 57]. Additionally, full-information maximum likelihood estimation was chosen to account for data missing at random, also minimizing parameter biases [58]. An oblique quartimin rotation was performed, allowing extracted factors to correlate [59], as was our expectation. To determine the number of factors to retain in the solution, Glorfeld’s [60] extension of Horn’s parallel analysis [61] was used—one of the most accurate methods available. Glorfeld’s extension of parallel analysis compares the eigenvalues of extracted factors to eigenvalues generated using a Monte Carlo simulation, with factors having eigenvalues larger than expected by chance retained. Thus, components were kept when their eigenvalues were larger than those from the 95th percentile in multiple simulations using random data. Finally, we considered several other important item-level features to increase interpretability of the factors, including: (a) amount of missing data, as some questions were not applicable to the full sample; (b) residual variances (i.e., no high negative values) [62]; and (c) factor loadings exceeding the critical cutoff of 0.4, [54] with minimal cross-loadings. After an initial EFA, results were evaluated and poorly performing items were removed. A subsequent EFA with the remaining items was then performed.
Reliability and Validity
Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were calculated to assess internal consistency. Test–retest reliability over a 4 month period was assessed using data from a non-treated group from the larger trial. This group was comprised of 48 participants randomized to the waitlist control condition who completed both baseline and post assessments, and 18 individuals who were unable to start group therapy due to scheduling difficulties and who were later re-administered the baseline assessment prior to starting a subsequent group, for a total of 66 participants. Convergent and divergent validity was assessed using the measures described above. Finally, t-tests comparing mean differences of shame scales by gender found no significant differences (t-values ranged from 0.21 to 1.69, with P values all greater than 0.05). These calculations were performed on the final subscales using SPSS 14.0.
Results
Exploratory Factor Analysis
An initial parallel analysis with all 38 original shame items supported the retention of four factors, conforming to our theoretical expectations (see Table 1 for factor loadings). Thus, an initial EFA was performed retaining four factors. Applying our item-level criteria as described above, the following items were dropped prior to repeating the analysis: (a) items B2, B7, D1, and D3-5 due to substantial missing data (>18%); (b) item B2 due to a high residual variance and cross-loading; and (c) item A13 due to a poor factor loading.
With this reduced set of 31 items, a parallel analysis suggested the retention of three-factors (see Table 2 for factor loadings). This final model explained 56.7% of the sample variance, however, because factors were correlated, the amount of unique variance accounted for by individual factors was not examined. Only three items loaded contrary to our theoretically-derived subscales, with A12 (“I feel I deserve to be mistreated by others because of my HIV”) loading on the Impact of Shame on Behavior subscale instead of the HIV-related shame subscale, item B5 (“Feelings of shame that I’m HIV + have kept me from telling my HIV status to a friend or family member”) loading on the HIV-related shame subscale instead of the impact of shame on behavior subscale, and item D2 (“Shame I feel about being sexually abused or raped makes me more ashamed about being HIV+”) loading on the sexual abuse-related shame subscale instead of the shame interaction effects subscale. In fact, the shame interactions effects subscale was eliminated due to the removal of four of the five original items because of substantial missing data due to participants not endorsing being gay, a drug user or a sex worker. The three remaining subscales, HIV-related shame, the impact of shame on behavior, and sexual abuse-related shame were retained and assessed for internal consistency, temporal stability, and convergent and divergent validity.
Table 2.
Factor
|
|||
---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | |
Scale A: HIV-related shame | |||
A1: Hard to tell people about my infection | 0.720 | 0.161 | − 0.219 |
A2: Failed to live up to my expectations | 0.699 | 0.003 | 0.001 |
A3: Expect people to think less of me | 0.758 | − 0.040 | 0.096 |
A4: Put myself down for having HIV | 0.767 | − 0.030 | 0.218 |
A5: Feel defective because Of HIV | 0.879 | − 0.139 | 0.123 |
A6: Ashamed HIV + | 0.886 | − 0.050 | 0.081 |
A7: Expect rejection when others find out | 0.796 | − 0.100 | 0.158 |
A8: Feel worthless because HIV | 0.728 | − 0.008 | 0.237 |
A9: Ashamed by HIV symptoms | 0.751 | − 0.006 | 0.133 |
A10: Hide infection from others | 0.784 | 0.257 | − 0.164 |
A11: Dread others will find out HIV status | 0.746 | 0.176 | − 0.069 |
A12: Deserve to be mistreated | 0.165 | 0.551 | 0.153 |
A14: HIV makes me want to disappear | 0.540 | 0.166 | 0.240 |
Scale B: Impact of HIV-related shame on behavior | |||
B1: Kept me from using condom | 0.204 | 0.554 | − 0.130 |
B3: Kept me from interacting with others | 0.213 | 0.634 | − 0.124 |
B4: Kept me from applying for services | 0.158 | 0.642 | 0.103 |
B5: Kept me from telling status to family | 0.557 | 0.365 | − 0.203 |
B6: Kept me from telling status to sex partner | 0.361 | 0.539 | − 0.200 |
B8: Delayed me from getting healthcare | 0.168 | 0.625 | 0.144 |
B9: Kept me from adhering to HIV treatment | − 0.169 | 0.847 | 0.166 |
B10: Kept me from getting info on HIV | − 0.111 | 0.878 | 0.164 |
Scale C: Sexual abuse-related shame | |||
C1: Hard to tell people about abuse | 0.021 | 0.082 | 0.689 |
C2: Think less of me because of abuse | 0.041 | 0.017 | 0.796 |
C3: Ashamed of abuse | 0.147 | − 0.032 | 0.798 |
C4: Feel worthless because of abuse | 0.089 | − 0.003 | 0.858 |
C5: Hide my abuse from others | 0.038 | 0.134 | 0.707 |
C6: Put self down for abuse | − 0.025 | 0.039 | 0.901 |
C7: Abuse makes me feel defective | 0.021 | 0.038 | 0.898 |
C8: Others reject me when they find out | − 0.024 | 0.022 | 0.874 |
C9: Abuse makes me want to disappear | 0.059 | 0.052 | 0.794 |
D2: Interaction abuse/HIV + shame | 0.162 | 0.115 | 0.641 |
Reliability estimatesa | |||
Cronbach’s alpha (N = 271) | 0.93 | 0.77 | 0.93 |
4-Month test–retest correlation (N = 66) | 0.83 | 0.37 | 0.72 |
Reliability estimates include all items in bold for each factor (column)
Reliability and Validity
Items for each of the three scales suggested by the EFA were further evaluated for internal consistency. Both item-scale total correlations, Cronbach’s alpha for the scale with each item removed, and overall Cronbach’s alpha were evaluated and supported the final solution of the EFA. Scale items were then summed to form total scale scores for each of the three scales, and test–retest reliability was evaluated by computing Pearson correlations for each scale at baseline with scores on each scale 4 months later in the sample of participants who were not exposed to a group intervention during the 4 month wait period. For internal consistency and test–retest reliability, Cronbach’s alphas and Pearson correlations are respectively listed on the bottom rows of Table 2.
Table 3 presents descriptive data for each of the final shame subscales, and for each of the validity measures examined in this study. Correlations demonstrating convergent and divergent validity are shown in Table 4. For the most part, correlations followed our hypothesized patterns as outlined on p. 11. However, only the impact of HIV-related shame on behavior was correlated with sexual HIV transmission risk behavior. While all three shame scales were significantly related to HIV physical symptoms, sexual abuse-related shame actually had the strongest relationship. Finally, while all of the shame scales were negatively associated with perceived availability of social support, the impact of HIV-related shame on behavior had the strongest relationship with this variable.
Table 3.
Measure | N | Mean | SD | Range |
---|---|---|---|---|
HARSI scales | ||||
HIV-related shame | 271 | 17.1 | 13.4 | 0–50 |
Impact of HIV-related shame on behavior | 271 | 3.1 | 4.6 | 0–14 |
Sexual abuse-related shame | 271 | 14.6 | 11.2 | 0–40 |
HIV specific criterion measures | ||||
HIV physical symptoms | 267 | 11.8 | 10.2 | 0–54 |
HIV-related stressors | 267 | 32.3 | 11.8 | 13–65 |
Sexual HIV transmission risk behavior | 255 | 53a | 20.8%b | – |
Psychosocial criterion measures | ||||
TSI: trauma-related symptoms | 268 | 31.3 | 17.7 | 0–99 |
TSI: mood and anxiety symptoms | 268 | 29.7 | 17.6 | 0–73 |
TSI: behavioral difficulties | 268 | 30.9 | 22.6 | 0–75 |
SCL-90-R global severity index | 271 | 1.03 | 0.67 | 0–3.5 |
Perceived availability of social support | 271 | 28.5 | 5.5 | 7–35 |
Note: Ns vary from 267 to 271 as a result of missing data, except sexual HIV transmission behavior N = 255 due to missing data. Sexual HIV transmission behavior (unprotected anal or vaginal intercourse with an HIV negative or serostatus unknown partner in the past 4 months) is a dichotomous indicator, with zero indicating no transmission risk and one indicating transmission risk in the past 4 months. For all continuous measures, higher scores indicate more symptoms, except perceived availability of social support where higher scores indicate higher levels of support
Number of participants in sample displaying sexual transmission risk behavior
Percent of sample displaying sexual transmission risk behavior
Table 4.
HIV-related shame | Effect of HIV-related shame on behavior | Sexual abuse-related shame | |
---|---|---|---|
HARSI scales | |||
HIV-related shame | – | – | – |
Impact of HIV-related shame on behavior | 0.559** | – | – |
Sexual abuse-related shame | 0.488** | 0.339** | – |
HIV specific criterion measures | |||
HIV physical symptoms | 0.213** | 0.215** | 0.287** |
HIV-related stressors | 0.533** | 0.326** | 0.367** |
Sexual HIV transmission risk behavior | 0.120 | 0.226** | 0.081 |
Psychosocial criterion measures | |||
TSI: trauma-related symptoms | 0.416** | 0.192** | 0.504** |
TSI: mood and anxiety symptoms | 0.442** | 0.225** | 0.452** |
TSI: behavioral difficulties | 0.394** | 0.268** | 0.448** |
SCL-90-R global severity index | 0.493** | 0.301** | 0.562** |
Perceived availability of social support | −.154* | −.194** | −.146* |
Note: Ns vary from 267 to 271 as a result of missing data, except sexual HIV transmission behavior N = 256 due to missing data
Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)
Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)
Discussion
While the negative consequences of stigma on people living with HIV have been recognized [34], the role of shame has received much less attention. Research suggests, however, that shame does have negative consequences on mental and physical health [21], has been directly linked to PTSD [43, 47], and results in avoidance that may impact HIV health behaviors such as disclosure and health care utilization. Further, among people living with HIV, shame may be related to several factors, including living with HIV and the experience of CSA. Thus we created the HARSI to measure components of shame relevant to both HIV infection and CSA, including the impact of shame on HIV-specific health behaviors.
The HARSI consists of three internally-consistent sub-scales: HIV-related shame, the impact of shame on behavior, and sexual abuse-related shame. The measure was developed for and tested in a highly traumatized population of people living with HIV infection who had experienced sexual abuse as children or adolescents. The sample was diverse across gender, sexual orientation, ethnicity, and socio-economic status. The results of the exploratory factor analysis presented here and the moderate correlations observed between subscales support the inclusion of three subscales in a single measure. Four month test–retest reliability was good for the HIV-related and sexual abuse-related shame subscales. While test–retest reliability was low for the impact of shame on behavior subscale, this is understandable given that many of the behaviors on this subscale may be impacted by opportunity (such as the presence of a sex partner or interaction with family members), or need for services and information, which may change over time.
The HARSI demonstrated sound convergent and divergent validity using eight well-established HIV-specific and broader psychosocial criterion measures. As desired, correlations were moderate (0.5) at best, indicating the HARSI is measuring related but distinct constructs. While most measures performed as hypothesized, deviations occurred in three areas: (1) only the impact of shame on behavior scale was correlated with sexual HIV transmission risk behavior. This does make sense as the HIV-related and sexual abuse related shame scales focus more on emotions while the impact of shame on behavior scale focuses on behavior. However, the relationship between HIV-related shame and sexual HIV transmission risk did trend toward significance. Further, there were instances of unprotected sex occurring within serodiscordant couples where shame is likely not a factor, which dilutes the relationship between shame and risk. Finally, we would anticipate that HIV-related and sexual abuse-related shame would more strongly impact serostatus disclosure; however, we did not assess disclosure in relation to unprotected sex in this study. (2) While all three shame scales were significantly related to HIV physical symptoms, surprisingly sexual abuse-related shame had the strongest relationship with this variable, although the difference was marginal. However, to some degree this may reflect the negative health consequences of traumatic stress [63]. (3) Finally, the impact of shame on behavior scale had the strongest relationship with perceived availability of social support. This is understandable given that this subscale contains several items on social interaction.
The HARSI items generally formed distinct factors as expected, with the following exceptions: seven of the initial items were dropped from the measure, six which pertained to illicit drugs, sex work, and being gay, and one due to poor factor loading. Only 43.1% of our sample identified as being gay or lesbian; in the past 4 months, only 3.3% had engaged in injection drug use (43.5% any illicit drug use); and 12.2% had engaged in sex work. Thus, in the future, it would be beneficial to assess these items using samples specifically drawn from these groups. The final item to be dropped was the sole positively-worded item (“I accept myself as an HIV + person”) due to poor factor loading. While more positively-worded items in the measure would have been beneficial, it is not clear that self-acceptance is the opposite of shame such that the item could be reverse scored and included in a scale with items reflecting shame. Indeed, other measures of shame are comprised entirely of negatively-worded items [16, 37, 40, 64, 65]. In the final analysis, there were just two cross-loadings: items A12 and B5, which loaded instead on the impact of shame on behavior and the HIV-related shame subscale, respectively. Future work should assess the fit of these items to determine whether this is purely a sample-specific artifact.
The HARSI was developed using a very specialized population, adults living with HIV infection who experienced CSA. While we anticipate scales A and B will have utility for measuring HIV-related shame and the impact of shame on behavior among broader samples of people living with HIV, this was not tested in the current study. Additionally, while we measured the impact of HIV-related shame on HIV health behaviors, we did not ask about the impact of abuse-related shame on behavior. Further, we did not have a sufficient sample size to confirm the results of exploratory factor analyses. Future work with larger sample sizes could confirm the robustness of the factors by performing a confirmatory factor analysis, and if possible, separately looking at the effects of factors such as gender and sexual orientation. Previous work has shown that women are more prone to feel shame than men [66], HIV-related shame has been associated with female gender [40], and CSA-related shame has been found to be greater in girls at abuse discovery (but not a year following the abuse) [18]. However, our results do not indicate that separate analysis by gender would significantly change the factor structure of the HARSI, and there are no differences in level of shame by gender or sexual orientation. Additionally, by making HARSI scales A and C parallel rather than specific, we can compare the relative impact of shame resulting from HIV or CSA, but we may be missing some important phenomenological aspects of shame specific to living with HIV or experiencing sexual abuse. A final limitation is that the current study did not include an established criterion measure of shame, such as the abuse-related beliefs questionnaire (ARBQ). While our measure has good reliability, face validity, and evidence for construct (convergent and divergent) validity, comparison with an established criterion is desirable.
Given the high prevalence of CSA among those living with HIV and the documented negative impact of shame on behavioral, physical, clinical, and psychosocial outcomes, there is a great need for reliable and valid measurement instruments to assess shame. Thus the HARSI adds substantially to the current literature, which is surprisingly lacking in robust measures specific to HIV-related and CSA-related shame. The HARSI is the only measure we are aware of that (1) addresses both HIV-related and sexual abuse-related shame concurrently, (2) captures multiple emotional and behavioral components of shame, and (3) includes a subscale assessing the impact of shame on behaviors such as healthcare service utilization, risk behavior, medication non-adherence, and serostatus disclosure, all of which have been influenced by shame [24, 40]. In fact, this measure has already been used to demonstrate the negative impact of shame on sexual HIV transmission risk behavior [23] and on health related quality of life [19]. Additionally, unlike stigma and discrimination, shame is a potentially modifiable variable at the individual level, and has been shown to be responsive to therapy [48, 67]. Finally, shame may prevent patients from fully disclosing their symptoms, resulting in poorer treatment outcomes [30, 68]. Thus addressing patient shame may be critical in overcoming barriers to care and achieving optimal physical and mental health outcomes.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by grants R01-MH62965, K23-MH076671, and P30-MH62294 (Center for Interdisciplinary Research on AIDS; CIRA) from the National Institute of Mental Health. The authors gratefully acknowledge our community collaboration with Callen-Lorde Community Health Center in New York City.
Footnotes
Disclosure No competing financial interests exist.
Contributor Information
Sharon A. S. Neufeld, Department of Social and Developmental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Kathleen J. Sikkema, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
Rachel S. Lee, Clinician in Private Practice, New York, NY, USA
Arlene Kochman, Duke Global Health Institute, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA.
Nathan B. Hansen, Email: nathan.hansen@yale.edu, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, The Consultation Center, 389 Whitney Avenue, New Haven, CT 06511, USA
References
- 1.Centers for disease control and prevention. [Accessed 28 July 2010.];HIV/AIDS statistics and surveillance: basic statistics. 2010 http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/topics/surveillance/basic.htm#hivest.
- 2.Centers for disease control and prevention. [Accessed 14 June 2011.];Diagnoses of HIV infection and AIDS in the United States and dependent areas. 2008 HIV surveillance report 2010. http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/surveillance/resources/reports/2008report.
- 3.Bedimo AL, Kissinger P, Bessinger R. History of sexual abuse among HIV-infected women. Int J STD AIDS. 1997;8:332–5. doi: 10.1258/0956462971920046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kalichman SC, Sikkema KJ, DiFonzo K, Luke W, Austin J. Emotional adjustment in survivors of sexual assault living with HIV/AIDS. J Trauma Stress. 2002;15(4):189–296. doi: 10.1023/A:1016247727498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Liebschutz JM, Feinman G, Sullivan L, Stein M, Samet J. Physical and sexual abuse in women infected with the human immunodeficiency virus: increased illness and health care organization. Arch Intern Med. 2000;160:1659–64. doi: 10.1001/archinte.160.11.1659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Schiff M, El-Bassel N, Engstrom M, Gilbert L. Psychological distress and intimate physical and sexual abuse among women in methadone maintenance treatment programs. Soc Serv Rev. 2002;76:302–20. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Briere J, Elliott DM. Prevalence and psychological sequelae of self-reported childhood physical and sexual abuse in a general population sample of men and women. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27:1205–22. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kimerling R, Armistead L, Forehand R. Victimization experiences and HIV infection in women: association with serostatus, psychological symptoms, and health status. J Trauma Stress. 1999;12:41–58. doi: 10.1023/A:1024790131267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Finkelhor D, Hotaling G, Lewis IA, Smith C. Sexual abuse in a national survey of adult men and women: prevalence, characteristics, and risk factors. Child Abuse Negl. 1990;14:19–28. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(90)90077-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Allers CT, Benjack KJ, White J, Rousey JT. HIV vulnerability and the adult survivor of childhood sexual abuse. Child Abuse Negl. 1993;17:291–8. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(93)90048-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Sikkema KJ, Hansen NB, Kochman A, Tarakeshwar N, Neufeld S, Meade CS, et al. Outcomes from a group intervention for coping with HIV/AIDS and childhood sexual abuse: reductions in traumatic stress. AIDS Behav. 2007;11:49–60. doi: 10.1007/s10461-006-9149-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Myers HF, Wyatt GE, Loeb TB, Carmona JV, Warda U, Longshore D, et al. Severity of child sexual abuse, post-traumatic stress and risky sexual behaviors among HIV-positive women. AIDS Behav. 2006;10:191–9. doi: 10.1007/s10461-005-9054-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.O’Leary A, Purcell D, Remien RH, Gomez C. Childhood sexual abuse and sexual transmission risk behaviour among HIV-positive men who have sex with men. AIDS Care. 2003;15:17–26. doi: 10.1080/0954012021000039725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sikkema KJ, Wilson PA, Hansen NB, Kochman A, Neufeld S, Ghebremichael M, et al. Effects of a coping intervention on transmission risk behavior among people living with HIV/AIDS and a history of childhood sexual abuse. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2008;47:506–13. doi: 10.1097/QAI.0b013e318160d727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Henny KD, Kidder DP, Stall R, Wolitski RJ. Physical and sexual abuse among homeless and unstably housed adults living with HIV: prevalence and associated risks. AIDS Behav. 2007;11:842–53. doi: 10.1007/s10461-007-9251-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Feiring C, Taska LS. The persistence of shame following childhood sexual abuse: a longitudinal look at risk and recovery. Child Maltreat. 2005;10:337–49. doi: 10.1177/1077559505276686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Feiring C, Taska LS, Chen K. Trying to understand why horrible things happen: attribution, shame, and symptom development following sexual abuse. Child Maltreat. 2002;7:26–41. doi: 10.1177/1077559502007001003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Feiring C, Taska LS, Lewis M. Adjustment following sexual abuse discovery: the role of shame and attribution style. Dev Psychol. 2002;38:79–92. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.1.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Persons E, Kershaw T, Sikkema KJ, Hansen NB. The impact of shame on health-related quality of life among HIV-positive adults with a history of childhood sexual abuse. AIDS Patient Care and STDs. 2010;24:571–80. doi: 10.1089/apc.2009.0209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Wilson JP, Drozdek B, Turkovic S. Posttraumatic shame and guilt. Trauma Viol Abuse. 2006;7:122–41. doi: 10.1177/1524838005285914. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Dickerson SS, Gruenewald TL, Kemeny ME. When the social self is threatened: shame, physiology, and health. J Pers. 2004;72:1191–216. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00295.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Tangney JP. Recent advances in the empirical study of shame and guilt. Am Behav Sci. 1995;38:1132–45. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Sikkema KJ, Hansen NB, Meade CS, Kochman A, Fox AM. Psychosocial predictors of sexual HIV transmission risk behavior among HIV-positive adults with a sexual abuse history in childhood. Arch Sex Behav. 2009;38:121–34. doi: 10.1007/s10508-007-9238-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Konkle-Parker DJ, Erlen JA, Dubbert PM. Barriers and facilitators to medication adherence in a southern minority population with HIV disease. J Assoc Nurses AIDS Care. 2008;19:98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.jana.2007.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Zuniga ML, Blanco E, Martinez P, Strathdee SA, Gifford AL. Perceptions of barriers and facilitators to participation in clinical trials in HIV-positive Latinas: a pilot study. J Women’s Health. 2007;16:1322–30. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2006.0234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kessler BL, Bieschke KJ. A retrospective analysis of shame, dissociation, and adult victimization in survivors of childhood sexual abuse. J Couns Psychol. 1999;46:335–41. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Whiffen VE, MacIntosh HB. Mediators of the link between childhood sexual abuse and emotional distress: a critical review. Trauma Viol Abuse. 2005;6:24–39. doi: 10.1177/1524838004272543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Talbot JA, Talbot NL, Tu X. Shame-proneness as a diathesis for dissociation in women with histories of childhood sexual abuse. J Trauma Stress. 2004;17:445–8. doi: 10.1023/B:JOTS.0000048959.29766.ae. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Jungmeen K, Talbot NL, Cicchetti D. Childhood abuse and current interpersonal conflict: the role of shame. Child Abuse Negl. 2009;33:362–71. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hansen NB, Kershaw TS, Kochman A, Sikkema KJ. A classification and regression tree (CART) analysis predicting treatment outcome following a group RCT for HIV + adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Psychother Res. 2007;17:404–15. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Zelaya CE, Sivaram S, Johnson SC, Srikrishnan AK, Solomon S, Celentano DD. HIV/AIDS stigma: reliability and validity on a new measurement instrument in Chennai, India. AIDS Behav. 2008;12:781–8. doi: 10.1007/s10461-007-9331-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Genberg BL, Kawichai S, Chingono A, Sendah M, Chariyalertsak S, Konda K, et al. Assessing HIV/AIDS stigma and discrimination in developing countries. AIDS Behav. 2008;12:772–80. doi: 10.1007/s10461-007-9340-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Scambler G. Health-related stigma. Sociol Health Illn. 2009;31:441–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9566.2009.01161.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Parker R, Aggleton P. HIV and AIDS-related stigma and discrimination: a conceptual framework and implications for action. Soc Sci Med. 2003;57:13–24. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00304-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Earnshaw VA, Chaudoir SR. From conceptualizing to measuring HIV stigma: a review of HIV stigma mechanism measures. AIDS Behav. 2009;13:1160–77. doi: 10.1007/s10461-009-9593-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Link BG, Phelan JC. Conceptualizing stigma. Annu Rev Sociol. 2001;27:363–85. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Fortenberry JD, McFarlane M, Bleakley A, Bull S, Fishbein M, Grimley DM, et al. Relationships of stigma and shame to gonorrhea and HIV screening. Am J Public Health. 2002;92:378–81. doi: 10.2105/ajph.92.3.378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Sayles JN, Hays RD, Sarkisian CA, Mahajan AP, Spritzer KL, Cunningham WE. Development and psychometric assessment of a multidimensional measure of internalized HIV stigma in a sample of HIV-positive adults. AIDS Behav. 2008;12:748–58. doi: 10.1007/s10461-008-9375-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Cunningham SD, Tschann J, Gurvey JE, Fortunberry JD, Ellen JM. Attitudes about sexual disclosure and perceptions of stigma and shame. Sex Trans Infect. 2002;78:334–8. doi: 10.1136/sti.78.5.334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Swendeman D, Rotheram-Borus MJ, Comulada S, Weiss R, Ramos ME. Predictors of HIV-related stigma among young people living with HIV. Health Psychol. 2006;25:501–9. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.4.501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Rusch N, Corrigan PW, Bohus M, Jacob GA, Brueck R, Lieb K. Measuring shame and guilt by self-report questionnaires: a validation study. Psychiatry Res. 2007;150:313–25. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ginzburg K, Arnow B, Hart S, Gardner W, Koopman C, Classen CC, et al. The abuse-related beliefs questionnaire for survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Child Abuse Negl. 2006;30:929–43. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lee DA, Scragg P, Turner S. The role of shame and guilt in traumatic events: a clinical model of shame-based and guilt-based PTSD. Br J Med Psychol. 2001;74:451–66. doi: 10.1348/000711201161109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Beck AT, Steer RA. BDI: Beck depression inventory manual. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kaplan M, Asnis GM, Lipschitz DS, Chorney P. Suicidal behavior and abuse in psychiatric outpatients. Compr Psychiatry. 1995;36:229–35. doi: 10.1016/0010-440x(95)90087-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Sikkema KJ, Hansen NB, Tarakeshwar N, Kochman A, Tate DC, Lee RS. The clinical significance of change in trauma-related symptoms following a pilot group intervention for coping with HIV-AIDS and childhood sexual trauma. AIDS Behav. 2004;8:277–91. doi: 10.1023/B:AIBE.0000044075.12845.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Andrews B, Brewin CR, Rose S, Kirk M. Predicting PTSD symptoms in victims of violent crime: the role of shame, anger, and childhood abuse. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109:69–73. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.109.1.69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Ginzburg K, Butler LD, Giese-Davis J, Cavanaugh CE, Neri E, Koopman C, et al. Shame, guilt, and posttraumatic stress disorder in adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse at risk for human immunodeficiency virus: outcomes of a randomized clinical trial of a group psychotherapy treatment. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2009;197:536–42. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181ab2ebd. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Folkman S, Chesney M, Collette L, Boccellari A, Cooke M. Postbereavement depressive mood and its prebereavement predictors in HIV+ and HIV− gay men. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;70:336–48. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.70.2.336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.DeMarco FJ, Ostrow DG, DiFranceisco W. General and AIDS-specific stress, coping, and psychological distress in the biracial coping and change study cohort of gay men. AIDS Behav. 1999;3:177–86. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Briere J. Traumatic symptom inventory professional manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Derogatis LR. SCL-90-R: administration, scoring and procedures manual-II. Towson: Clinical Psychometric Research; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 53.O’Brien K, Wortman CB, Kessler RC, Joseph JG. Social relationships of men at risk for AIDS. Soc Sci Med. 1993;36:1161–7. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(93)90236-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Floyd FJ, Widaman KF. Factor analysis in the development and refinement of clinical assessment instruments. Psychol Assess. 1995;7:286–99. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. 5. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén;; 1998–2008. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Flora DB, Curran PJ. An empirical evaluation of alternative methods of estimation for confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data. Psychol Methods. 2004;9:466–91. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.9.4.466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Nussbeck FW, Eid M, Lischetzke T. Analysing multitrait-multi-method data with structural equation models for ordinal variables applying the WLSMV estimator: what sample size is needed for valid results? Br J Math Stat Psychol. 2006;59:195–213. doi: 10.1348/000711005X67490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Enders CK, Bandalos DL. The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Struct Equ Model. 2001;8:430–57. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Costello AB, Osborne JW. Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Pract Assess Res Eval. 2005;10:7. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Glorfeld LW. An improvement on Horn’s parallel analysis methodology for selecting the correct number of factors to retain. Educ Psychol Measur. 1995;53:377–93. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Horn JL. A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika. 1965;30:179–85. doi: 10.1007/BF02289447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Osman A, Kopper BA, Barrios F, Gutierrez PM, Bagge CL. Reliability and validity of the Beck depression inventory-II with adolescent psychiatric inpatients. Psychol Assess. 2004;16:120–32. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.16.2.120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Schnurr PP, Green BL, editors. Trauma and health: physical health consequences of exposure to extreme stress. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Andrews B, Qian M, Valentine JD. Predicting depressive symptoms with a new measure of shame: the experience of shame scale. Br J Clin Psychol. 2002;41:29–42. doi: 10.1348/014466502163778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Harder DW, Greenwald DF. Further validation of the shame and guilt scales of the Harder personal feelings questionnaire-2. Psychol Rep. 1999;85:271–81. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1999.85.1.271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Woien SL, Ernst HA, Patock-Peckham JA, Nagoshi CT. Validation of the TOSCA to measure shame and guilt. Pers Individ Differ. 2003;35:313–26. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Deblinger E, Runyon MK. Understanding and treating feelings of shame in children who have experienced maltreatment. Child Maltreat. 2005;10:364–76. doi: 10.1177/1077559505279306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Hook A, Andrews B. The relationship of non-disclosure in therapy to shame and depression. Br J Clin Psychol. 2005;44:425–38. doi: 10.1348/014466505X34165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]