Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2009 Feb 1;18(1):53–57. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01605.x

Infants’ grasp of others’ intentions

Amanda L Woodward 1
PMCID: PMC3640581  NIHMSID: NIHMS459178  PMID: 23645974

Abstract

The perception of others as intentional agents is fundamental to human experience and foundational to development. Recent research reveals that this cornerstone of social perception has its roots early in infancy, and that it draws structure from the universal, early emerging human experience of engaging in goal-directed action. Infants’ own action capabilities correlate with their emerging tendency to view others’ actions as organized by goals. Moreover, interventions that facilitate new goal-directed actions alter infants’ perception of those same actions in others. These effects seem to depend on the first-person aspects of infants’ experience. These findings open new questions about how doing leads to knowing in the social domain.


Fundamental to human experience is the perception that we live in a world of intentional agents. The apprehension of others’ intentions is both mundane and mysterious. A person’s bodily movements are physically concrete. The goals and states of attention they embody are entirely abstract, yet real to human observers. We see others’ actions not as raw physical movements, but rather as structured by intentional relations between agents and their goals and objects of attention. We can conceive of intentional relations from the very concrete (getting or wanting real objects) to the very abstract (imagining the future or regretting missed opportunities).

This cornerstone of social perception is pervasive in adults’ memory for, reasoning about, and communication of event information. It also plays a critical role in development. Much of what children learn in the first years of life they learn from other people, including language, culture, societal values, and collaborative activities. Learning in these domains depends on understanding others’ intentions (Tomasello et al., 2005). To illustrate, when young children learn new words, they do not simply relate the words they hear to the things they see. Instead, they look at the person who uttered the word, analyze her focus of attention and probable intentions, and use this information to interpret the word (Baldwin & Moses, 2001)

Research from across laboratories has shown that the perception of others’ actions as intentional begins to emerge in the first year of life. The earliest evidence for this ability involves actions that are directed at concrete goals, such as reaching. Consider the action in Figure 1A, a woman reaches toward and grasps a toy. This action could be represented in terms of its physical properties (e.g., the movement and angle of the arm). But, to mature observers, the event is more naturally described in terms of the relation between the agent and her goal (e.g., “She grasped the toy.”). Infants see it this way as well. To illustrate, in a visual habituation experiment, infants were first habituated to repeated examples of a person reaching for one of two objects, similar to the event in Figure 1A. Then, the objects’ positions were reversed and infants viewed two kinds of test trials: On new-goal trials, the person reached to the same location to grasp the new object; thus, her physical movements were the same, but her goal has changed. On new-side trials, the person reached to the other side to grasp the same object; thus, she moved in a new way, but still acted on the same goal. By 5 months, infants show increased looking (a response to novelty) on new-goal trials, but not on new-side trials in experiments like this one (Woodward, 1998, 2005).

Figure 1. Goal relations detected by infants in typical human actions.

Figure 1

The depicted events illustrate experimental stimuli presented to infants in habituation and imitation experiments, as described in the text. By 5–6 months infants represent grasping events (A) as organized by the relation between agent and object. By 9–12 months, infants represent looking events (B) in this way, and they also understand actions on an intermediary (the box in C) as directed at the ultimate goal (the toy inside the box), rather than at the box itself (reprinted from A. Woodward & A. Needham (eds.) (in press). Learning and the infant mind. Oxford University Press).

Critically, infants do not respond in this way when the moving entity is not readily identified as an agent (Woodward, 1998, Hofer et al., 2005), or when the action is ambiguous (Woodward, 2005). Further, when viewing novel events, infants respond differentially depending on the presence of cues indicating the animacy of the agent or goal-directedness of the action (Biro & Leslie, 2007; Luo & Baillargeon, 2005). Thus, infants’ response to goal changes is not readily explained by lower level factors, such as the association between the agents and the object or the way the action draws attention to the object. Rather, infants selectively attend to the relational structure of goal-directed actions.

This selective attention to goals is also evident in infants’ overt actions. When 7-month-old infants see an adult reach toward one of two toys, as in Figure 1A, they subsequently select that toy themselves (Hamlin et al., 2008). However, if infants see the adult direct an ambiguous action toward the toy, they choose randomly when given the choice between them. Even though both kinds of movements lead infants to attend to the toy, only one, the reach, is seen as goal-directed, and this interpretation drives infants’ responses. Like older children, 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce the goals of observed actions (Meltzoff, 2006).

DO INFANTS READ ACTIONS OR MINDS?

These findings show that infants represent actions as organized by the relation between agent and object. But how do infants understand this relation? Do they understand the mental connections expressed in concrete actions? Or do they understand the relational structure of action in more behavioral terms? There is currently active debate on this issue (for varying perspectives, see Gergely & Csibra, 2003; Luo & Baillargeon, 2005; Woodward, 2005). The absence of linguistic evidence from infants makes it particularly difficult to resolve.

Nevertheless, evidence indicates that by 9 to 12 months, if not before, infants discern relatively abstract aspects of action structure. To start, infants represent not only physical intentional relations, but also the entirely abstract relation between a person and the object of his or her attention, as in Figure 1B (Woodward, 2005). Infants also condition their expectations about instrumental actions based on the agent’s prior focus of attention (Luo & Johnson, in press; Phillips et al., 2002). For example, infants expect that a person will act on the object to which she has just attended (Phillips et al., 2002). Further, infants represent the higher order plans that structure sequences of individual actions. To illustrate, they understand that a person’s actions on a container or tool are directed at the goal it enables the user to attain rather than the tool or container itself, as in Figure 1C (Sommerville et al., in press; Sommerville & Woodward, 2005). These findings indicate that infants understand goals as distinct from particular, physical connections, seeing them instead as more abstract relations that organize physical actions. Finally, infants understand goals not as properties of events, but as residing in the person. Infants do not generalize goal information from one person to another, unless the action takes a conventional form, such as the use of a linguistic symbol (Buresh & Woodward, 2007).

Thus although it is unlikely that infants understand others’ mental lives in all the ways that adults do, infants understand intentions as existing independent of particular concrete actions and as residing within the individual. Each of these is essential to recovering intentions from observed actions and each is part of what it means to understand intention in psychological terms.

ORIGINS OF INFANTS’ GOAL PERCEPTION

The perception of others’ intentions is automatic and universal in adults. Because it is essential to human survival, it is reasonable to assume that it is the product of natural selection. Further, as just illustrated, the beginnings of this ability emerge in infancy. These considerations have led several theorists to stress the role of innate constraints in explaining the origins of goal perception (Biro & Leslie, 2007; Gergely & Csibra, 2003). However, early, universal abilities may also reflect the effects of early and universally available experience. Indeed, it is common for species-typical abilities to recruit information that is reliably present in the early environment. Often, the critical experiences are reliably present because they are produced by the developing organism itself. In our recent work, we have begun to test whether early, self-produced action contributes to infants’ perception of others’ goals.

The initial motivation for considering this possibility was both empirical and theoretical. Empirically, we noted that infants began to show systematic responses in laboratory measures of goal perception at around the same ages they gained control over the action themselves. Theoretically, it has long been hypothesized that first-person agentive experience provides insight into understanding others’ intentions. Recent interest in the role of embodied cognition in development has led to renewed attention to this possibility (Meltzoff, 2006; Shipley & Zacks, 2008; von Hofsten, 2004). We first approached this question by asking whether developmental variation in infants’ own actions correlates with their action perception. Then, we conducted intervention experiments to gain a clearer view of the influence of acting on action perception.

Developmental Variation in Action Production Correlates with Goal Perception

During the first year, infants become increasingly able to coordinate their goal-directed actions. If first-person experience provides insight into others’ goals, then these developments in infants’ own actions should correlate with their ability to discern goal structure in others’ actions. This prediction has been confirmed. At 9 months, infants who produce object-directed points understand others’ points as object-directed; infants who do not yet point do not (Brune & Woodward, 2007). At 10 months, infants who are skilled at producing means-end sequences represent the means-end structure of others’ actions, but those who are less skilled do not (Sommerville & Woodward, 2005). Of course, these correlations do not reveal the causal relations at play. Nevertheless, they provide an important test of ecological validity by showing that infants’ naturally emerging actions relate to their perception of others’ actions.

Acting Changes Infants’ Goal Perception

Our next step was to attempt to alter infants’ actions and then assess whether this rendered changes in their action perception. We began with infants who are very limited in both the production and perception of goal-directed actions, 3-month-olds. Although 3-month-old infants are not yet skilled at reaching, they can learn to apprehend objects by swiping at them while wearing Velcro-covered “sticky mittens” (Needham et al., 2002). We gave one group of infants practice with sticky mittens immediately prior to a habituation session assessing their perception of others’ mittened reaches as goal-directed (Sommerville et al., 2005). A control group, who viewed the habituation events without mittens training, did not respond systematically, indicating that infants did not spontaneously view the habituation events as goal-directed. In contrast, infants who had mittens training showed a selective novelty response on goal-change trials. Furthermore, there was a significant correlation between the extent to which infants engaged in object-directed actions with the mittens and their selective response on goal-change trials. Thus, producing in goal-directed actions led infants to subsequently view others’ actions as goal-directed (see Figure 2).

Figure 2. Effects of active mittens training on infants’ perception of others’ goals.

Figure 2

Three-month-old infants who underwent “sticky mittens” training subsequently responded to changes in the goal structure of observed reaches, whereas untrained infants did not (reprinted from Gerson & Woodward, in press; data from Sommerville, Woodward & Needham (2005).

These findings leave open the question of why infants’ actions had this effect. One possibility is that infants extract goal structure via visual analysis, and that they created for themselves a set of salient visual examples. In this case, infants’ learning from their own actions would be no different from their observational learning from others’ actions. Alternatively, first-person agency may provide unique information about action structure that could not be gleaned from observation alone. Goal-directed action requires that, at some level, the agent represent the goal that structures the action. This is the case even in young infants, who prospectively adjust their actions to apprehend objects (von Hofsten, 2004). The tacit knowledge that guides infants’ own goal-directed actions could provide a key for understanding the goal structure of others’ actions.

To distinguish these possibilities, Sarah Gerson and I replicated the active mittens training condition and compared it to a matched observational condition. In the observational condition, each infant viewed an adult producing mittened actions according to a script generated from an infant in the active training condition. Thus, infants in the two conditions were yoked in their amount of experience, but differed as to whether that experience was self-produced or observed. Our preliminary findings indicate that this difference was critical. As in the earlier study, active experience led infants to view others’ actions as goal-directed, and individual variation in infants’ level of mittened activity correlated with their response to goal-change trials. In contrast, observational experience did not have these effects.

Sommerville and her colleagues (in press) found similar patterns when they trained 10-month-old infants in producing a means-end action. Infants who received active training in pulling a cane to retrieve a toy subsequently discerned the means-end structure of another person’s cane-pulling actions. Infants who underwent observational training did not show this response. Thus, these findings suggest that first-person experience provides unique insights into the goal structure of others’ actions.

Initial Insights and Next Questions

Our findings indicate that action perception, like many other species-typical abilities, recruits information from self-produced experience during development. These findings highlight the role of learning and active engagement in early social cognitive development and in infant cognitive development more generally. They also raise pressing questions about the ways in which acting informs infants’ social perception.

To start, the mechanisms by which information is translated from acting to perceiving have not yet been identified. One interesting possibility is that no translation is needed because acting and perceiving share overlapping neurocognitive representations. This possibility is consistent with recent findings of “mirror neurons”--neurons that fire during both action production and action perception--in non-human primates, and similar neural mirroring in adult humans. However, there are many open questions about the existence of mirror representations in infants and their role in development (Gerson & Woodward, in press; Meltzoff, 2006).

Further, we do not yet know how far infants go with what they learn from acting. When infants engage in a particular intentional action, does this inform their understanding of only very similar actions in similar contexts, or does this knowledge generalize across contexts or broader classes of actions? The answers to this question may vary depending on general developmental changes as well as specific expertise. We have hypothesized that the general cognitive ability to create analogies may support the generalization of action knowledge (Gerson & Woodward, in press). In some cases, infants seem able to perceive goal structure in novel actions or very abstract events (Biro & Leslie, 2007; Gergely & Csibra, 2003; Luo & Baillargeon, 2005). One possibility is that these cases represent far analogical extensions of goal representations initially derived from action. Alternatively, these responses to abstract displays may reflect the output of a separate, perceptually based, system (Woodward, 2005).

This point raises a more general question: How does goal perception relate to other aspects of infants’ social knowledge? Infants see much more than goals in others’ actions. They are sensitive to biological patterns of motion, to emotional expressions, contingency, facial identify and expressions, social categories, and systematic patterns in the actions of their caretakers, among other classes of social information. One possibility is that these different aspects of social knowledge are integrated into a single system in infants. If so, then knowledge acquired in one local domain should readily connect with other kinds of social information. Alternatively, especially early on, infants may begin with relatively isolated pockets of social knowledge. These issues await investigation.

Acknowledgements

The research described here was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (HD35707) and the National Science Foundation (0634796). I thank Sarah Gerson, Harry Reis and two anonymous reviewers for their comments on prior versions of the paper.

References

  1. Baldwin DA, Moses JA. Links between social understanding and early word learning: Challenges to current accounts. Social Development. 2001;10:311–329. [Google Scholar]
  2. Biro S, Leslie AM. Infants' perception of goal-directed actions: Development through cue-based bootstrapping. Developmental Science. 2007;10:379–398. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00544.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Brune CW, Woodward AL. Social cognition and social responsiveness in 10-month-old infants. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2007;8:133–158. [Google Scholar]
  4. Buresh J, Woodward AL. Infants track action goals within and across agents. Cognition. 2007;104:287–314. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Gergely G, Csibra G. Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naive theory of rational action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7:287–292. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00128-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Gerson S, Woodward AL. Building intentional action knowledge with one's hands. In: Johnson SP, editor. Neo-constructivism. New York: Oxford University Press; (in press). [Google Scholar]
  7. Hamlin JK, Hallinan EV, Woodward AL. Do as I do: 8-month-old infants selectively reproduce others' goals. Developmental Science. 2008;11:487–494. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00694.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Hofer T, Hauf P, Aschersleben G. Infant's perception of goal-directed actions performed by a mechanical device. Infant Behavior & Development. 2005;28:466–480. [Google Scholar]
  9. Luo Y, Baillargeon R. Can a self-propelled box have a goal? Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychological Science. 2005;16:601–608. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01582.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Luo Y, Johnson SC. Recognizing thee role of perception in action at 6 months. Developmental Science. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00741.x. (in press). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Meltzoff AN. The "like me" framework for recognizing and becoming an intentional agent. Acta Psychologica. 2006;124:26–43. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Needham A, Barrett T, Peterman K. A pick-me-up for infants’ exploratory skills. Infant Behavior and Development. 2002;25:279–295. [Google Scholar]
  13. Phillips AT, Wellman HM, Spelke ES. Infants' ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action. Cognition. 2002;85:53–78. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00073-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Sommerville JA, Hildebrand EA, Crane CC. Experience matters: The impact of doing versus watching on infants’ subsequent perception of tool use events. Developmental Psychology. doi: 10.1037/a0012296. (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Sommerville JA, Woodward AL. Pulling out the intentional structure of human action: The relation between action production and processing in infancy. Cognition. 2005;95:1–30. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Sommerville JA, Woodward AL, Needham A. Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others’ actions. Cognition. 2005;96:B1–B11. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2005;28:675–735. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. von Hofsten C. An action perspective on motor development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004;8:266–272. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Woodward AL. Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition. 1998;69:1–34. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(98)00058-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Woodward AL. The infant origins of intentional understanding. In: Kail RV, editor. Advances in child development and behavior. Vol. 33. Oxford: Elsevier; 2005. pp. 229–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Recommended readings

  1. Meltzoff AN, Prinz W. The imitative mind: Development, evolution and brain bases. Eds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2002. This volume provides a broad, cross-disciplinary overview of the relation between action production and action perception.
  2. Shipley TF, Zacks JM. Understanding events: From perception to action. Eds. New York: Oxford University Press; 2008. This impressively comprehensive volume distills current cognitive scientific insights into how the human mind represents actions and events.
  3. Sommerville, Woodward, Needham Cited in reference list. This study provided the first evidence that changes in infants’ own actions render changes in their action perception. 2005 [Google Scholar]
  4. Woodward Cited in reference list. This chapter offers a comprehensive review of the new and burgeoning body of research investigating infants’ action knowledge. 2005 [Google Scholar]
  5. Woodward A, Needham A. Learning and the infant mind. Eds. New York: Oxford University Press; (in press). This volume highlights cutting-edge investigations into the contributions of experience and learning to infants’ conceptual development.

RESOURCES