Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 May 7.
Published in final edited form as: Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2010 Oct 31;35(2):395–409. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.10.019

Psychomotor retardation in depression: Biological underpinnings, measurement, and treatment

Jeylan S Buyukdura a,1, Shawn M McClintock a,b,2, Paul E Croarkin a,*
PMCID: PMC3646325  NIHMSID: NIHMS461542  PMID: 21044654

Abstract

Psychomotor retardation is a long established component of depression that can have significant clinical and therapeutic implications for treatment. Due to its negative impact on overall function in depressed patients, we review its biological correlates, optimal methods of measurement, and relevance in the context of therapeutic interventions. The aim of the paper is to provide a synthesis of the literature on psychomotor retardation in depression with the goal of enhanced awareness for clinicians and researchers. Increased knowledge and understanding of psychomotor retardation in major depressive disorder may lead to further research and better informed diagnosis in regards to psychomotor retardation. Manifestations of psychomotor retardation include slowed speech, decreased movement, and impaired cognitive function. It is common in patients with melancholic depression and those with psychotic features. Biological correlates may include abnormalities in the basal ganglia and dopaminergic pathways. Neurophysiologic tools such as neuroimaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation may play a role in the study of this symptom in the future. At present, there are three objective scales to evaluate psychomotor retardation severity. Studies examining the impact of psychomotor retardation on clinical outcome have found differential results. However, available evidence suggests that depressed patients with psychomotor retardation may respond well to electroconvulsive therapy (ECT). Current literature regarding antidepressants is inconclusive, though tricyclic antidepressants may be considered for treatment of patients with psychomotor retardation. Future work examining this objective aspect of major depressive disorder (MDD) is essential. This could further elucidate the biological underpinnings of depression and optimize its treatment.

Keywords: Major depressive disorder, Neuropsychological measures, Psychomotor retardation, Rating scales

1. Introduction

Psychomotor retardation has been characterized as a major feature of depression since antiquity. Hippocrates and Aretaeus of Cappadocia both described psychomotor retardation as a characteristic of depression (Sobin and Sackeim, 1997; Whitwell, 1936; Zilboorg, 1944). Darwin also discussed visible psychomotor symptoms and depressed patients who “no longer wish for action but remain motionless and passive, or may occasionally rock themselves to and fro” (Dantchev and Widlocher, 1998; Greden and Carroll, 1981). In the proceeding decades, authors such as Kraepelin expanded on psycho-motor retardation, building upon the knowledge of this noteworthy phenomenon by describing how it was more prominent than depressed mood and involved constrained speech, thought, and behavior (Greden and Carroll, 1981; Sobin and Sackeim, 1997).

Presently, psychomotor retardation is regarded as a key aspect of major depressive disorder (MDD) (American Psychiatric Association, 2000; Greden and Carroll, 1981; Widlocher, 1983). In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Fourth Edition-Text Revision (DSM IV-TR), it is one of the 9 core symptoms identified to diagnose MDD (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Psychomotor retardation is also a principal symptom of MDD with melancholic features (American Psychiatric Association, 2000; Parker, 2005). Despite its long observed prevalence in MDD, the characterization and clinical significance of psychomotor retardation are poorly understood (Greden and Carroll, 1981; Sobin and Sackeim, 1997). This review will examine the biological correlates, measurement, and treatment implications of psychomotor retardation.

The aim of the paper is to provide a synthesis of the literature on psychomotor retardation in depression with the goal of enhanced awareness for clinicians and researchers. Increased knowledge and understanding of psychomotor retardation in major depressive disorder may lead to further research and better informed diagnosis in regards to psychomotor retardation. To carry out a systematic review, the lead author (JSB) performed independent searches in PubMed (1900–2010) database with the following terms: psycho-motor retardation, major depressive disorder, motor, speech, melancholia, antidepressant, electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), and scale. The systematic review included only the articles that mentioned psycho-motor retardation and at minimum, one other key search term in the abstract. Reference sections were also reviewed for additional sources. A total of 154 articles (English language literature) were included in this review. These studies were between the dates of 1936–2010, from Australia, Austria, Belgium, Canada, Denmark, France, Germany, Ireland, Italy, Japan, Mexico, Netherlands, New Zealand, Switzerland, United Kingdom, and United States; the methodologies and study designs varied.

2. Characteristics of psychomotor retardation

2.1. Observable characteristics

Psychomotor retardation is unique in regards to depressive symptomatology as it is assessed through direct behavioral observations of speech, facial expression, eye movements, self-touching, posture, and speed and degree of movements (Jones and Pansa, 1979; Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996; Sobin and Sackeim, 1997; Widlocher, 1983). Speech has been extensively studied in the context of depression and psychomotor retardation (Greden et al., 1981; Hardy et al., 1984; Sobin and Sackeim, 1997; Szabadi et al., 1976). Specifically, investigations have involved observations of pause and speech times, volume, tone, infection, articulation, and response length (Greden et al., 1981; Greden and Carroll, 1981; Hardy et al., 1984; Sobin and Sackeim, 1997; Szabadi et al., 1976). Clinicians can easily assess marked speech abnormalities such as gross changes in volume and prosody (Greden, 1993). Characteristic eye movements of patients with psychomotor retardation are fixed gaze and poor maintenance of eye contact (Sobin et al., 1998; Widlocher, 1983). Another characteristic symptom is gross psychomotor slowing, including movement of the hands, legs, torso, and head (Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996; Sobin et al., 1998; Widlocher, 1983). Slumped posture is also a manifestation of psychomotor retardation (Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996; Sobin et al., 1998; Widlocher, 1983). In addition, patients with psychomotor retardation have been found to engage in increased self-touching, especially of the face (Sobin and Sackeim, 1997).

Objective tools have utility in quantifying these subtle symptoms. For example, a tape-recorder and oscilloscope are used to record speech abnormalities such as lengthy pauses and lowered volume of speech (Hardy et al., 1984; Szabadi et al., 1976). Similarly, the symptom of flat facial expression can be observed by clinicians (Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996; Widlocher, 1983), and the use of electromyography (EMG) can increase the sensitivity of its reliable and valid documentation (Greden and Carroll, 1981). Electrooculography (EOG) is also a sensitive measurement for recording eye movements (Schmid-Priscoveanu and Allum, 1999). In addition to these observations and measurements during interviews (Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996; Sobin et al., 1998; Widlocher, 1983), slowed movement has been quantified by other methods including response speed, time to draw, and gross motor movement (Bezzi et al., 1981; Iverson, 2004; van Hoof et al., 1993). Psychomotor scales are a reliable way for clinicians to assess the degree of retardation by observation during an interview (Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996; Sobin et al., 1998; Widlocher, 1983). An outline of the characteristics, presentation, and method of observation is in Table 1.

Table 1.

Characteristics of psychomotor retardation.

Item Presentation in psychomotor retardation Assessed by Reference
Speech Increased pauses, decreased volume, reduced articulation, reduced tone and infection, delayed response Subtle abnormalities — tape-recorder and oscilloscope
Gross changes — observation by clinician
Greden et al. (1981a), Greden and Carroll (1981b), Greden (1993), Hardy et al. (1984), Sobin and Sackeim (1997), Szabadi et al. (1976)
Eye movement Fixed gaze, poor eye contact Subtle abnormalities — EOG
Gross changes — observation by clinician
Schmid-Priscoveanu and Allum (1999), Sobin et al. (1998), Widlocher (1983)
Gross movement Decreased and/or slowed movement of hands, legs, torso, head Subtle abnormalities — reaction time, drawing times
Gross changes — observation by clinician
Bezzi et al. (1981), Iverson (2004), Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic (1996), Sobin et al. (1998), van Hoof et al. (1993), Widlocher (1983)
Posture Slumped while sitting or standing Observation by clinician Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic (1996), Sobin et al. (1998), Widlocher (1983)
Self-touching Increased self-touching, especially face Observation by clinician Sobin and Sackeim (1997)
Facial expression Flat expression Subtle abnormalities — EMG
Gross changes — observation by clinician
Greden and Carroll (1981), Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic (1996), Widlocher (1983)

2.2. Melancholic depression

Psychomotor retardation is considered a main feature of melancholic depression (Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996; Parker et al., 2010; Sobin and Sackeim, 1997). The CORE measurement, which classifies depressed patients as melancholic or non-melancholic, focuses on psychomotor symptoms (Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996). Parker and colleagues hypothesized that psychomotor retardation is pathognomonic for melancholia (Parker, 2005). Nonetheless, psychomotor retardation has been found to be present in other subtypes of depression (e.g., atypical depression) (Benazzi, 2002; Blackburn, 1975; Gupta, 2009; Niculescu and Akiskal, 2001; Schrijvers et al., 2009; Smith et al., 1995; Widlocher, 1983), suggesting that it may not be unique to melancholia (Greden and Carroll, 1981; Gupta, 2009; Moffoot et al., 1994; Niculescu and Akiskal, 2001; Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996; Sobin and Sackeim, 1997; Widlocher, 1983).

2.3. Depression severity

There is no consensus regarding if depression severity is associated with the presence or degree of psychomotor retardation. Multiple studies have demonstrated that depression severity and psychomotor retardation are correlated (Blewett, 1992; Lemke et al., 1999). The Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) has a positive correlation (r=0.69, p=0.010) with the Salpetriere Retardation Rating Scale (SRRS), a symptom severity scale specific to psychomotor retardation (Pier et al., 2004; Sabbe et al., 1999; van Hoof et al., 1993). Other studies assert that neuropsychological measures have a higher correlation with psychomotor retardation than depression severity (Loo et al., 2008; Shah et al., 1997). For example, the SRRS was found in a study to be significantly correlated (r=0.67, p<0.005) with Posner’s covert orientation of visual attention test, motor tasks (p<0.02), and other neuropsychological measures of psychomotor retardation (Pier et al., 2004; Smith et al., 1995). In one study, patients with MDD relative to healthy controls displayed greater psychomotor retardation, while patients with dysthymic disorder did not show such features. However, results indicated that subjects with melancholic features were more likely to have psychomotor retardation than subjects with severe depressive symptoms (Pier et al., 2004). The discrepant findings between studies which show a correlation with psychomotor retardation to depression severity or neuropsychological measures may relate to differences in methodology, sample sizes, and measurement methods of psychomotor retardation.

3. Biological correlates of psychomotor retardation in MDD

Previous work has examined the possible pathophysiology of psychomotor changes in mood disorders. It is appealing to consider other neuropsychiatric disorders with psychomotor changes such as schizophrenia, Parkinson’s disease and Huntington’s disease as these disorders have high incidences of depressive symptomatology. This would also suggest that the basal ganglia play a critical role in psychomotor retardation in mood disorders. The basal ganglia encompass the caudate nucleus, lentiform nucleus (the putamen and globus pallidus), subthalamic nucleus, and substantia nigra which form an intricate center of the extrapyramidal motor system. The striatum (which is composed of the caudate nucleus, putamen, and nucleus accumbens) receives cortical input via the thalamus and has projections to the prefrontal, premotor, and supplementary motor areas. These areas have a key role in motor planning (Herrero et al., 2002). Severe diseases of the basal ganglia result in significant movement disorders such as Huntington’s disease (Graveland et al., 1985), Parkinson’s disease (Vidailhet et al., 1994), progressive supranuclear palsy (Litvan et al., 2000), and motor stereotypes (Canales and Graybiel, 2000). However, further investigations are required to confirm the basal ganglia’s role in this objective symptom of MDD (Schrijvers et al., 2008b). Experts have also postulated that psychomotor changes in depression correlate with specific neurocircuitry in the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia (Sobin and Sackeim, 1997).

3.1. Neuroimaging and neurophysiologic studies

Structural imaging studies suggest that patients with depression have frontostriatal abnormalities such as white matter changes in the basal ganglia and decreased volumes of the prefrontal cortex, caudate, and putamen. These deficits may be more profound in the presence of psychomotor retardation (Hickie et al., 1996b; Naismith et al., 2002; Steffens and Krishnan, 1998). Other work with structural MRI, found that white matter hyperintensities were associated with onset of depression after age 50, and with psychomotor retardation. These white matter hyperintensities were able to predict poor treatment response (r=−0.44, p<0.01), for both ECT (r=−0.42, p=0.06) and pharmacotherapy (r=−0.49, p<0.05) (Hickie et al., 1995).

Functional neuroimaging research has demonstrated that psycho-motor retardation in depression is associated with decreased blood flow in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, left prefrontal cortex, angular gyrus, and the anterior cingulate (Bench et al., 1993; Brody et al., 2001; Mayberg et al., 1994; Narita et al., 2004; Videbech et al., 2002).

An early positron emission tomography (PET) study examined subjects with both depression and Huntington’s disease compared to subjects with only Huntington’s disease and healthy controls. Regional cerebral glucose metabolism was measured using 2-[18F]-fluoro-2-deoxy-D-glucose. The results indicated that subjects with both MDD and Huntington’s had orbital frontal-inferior prefrontal cortex hypometabolism compared to the other subjects. This metabolic pattern is similar to that in patients with both MDD and Parkinson’s disease. These findings suggest that the paralimbic regions of the frontal lobes may be associated with mood and movement disorders (Mayberg et al., 1992). A later study with single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) with 99mTc-hexamethylpropylene amine oxime, found that in 13 subjects with severe depression, the severity of psychomotor retardation was negatively correlated with prefrontal, frontal and temporal perfusion (r=−0.54, p<0.03) (Mayberg et al., 1994). Hickie et al. found that the left neo-striatum regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) was inversely correlated with psychomotor retardation. In this investigation, subjects with MDD were injected with technetium-99m hexamethylpropylene amine oxime (99mTc-HMPAO) and were tested for psychomotor retardation using reaction times. During these reaction time tests, brain SPECT was administered, which measured regional cerebral blood flow in the left and right caudate and putamen. The change in rCBF was negatively correlated with reaction time (r=−0.48, p<0.05) (Hickie et al., 1999).

Martinot and colleagues used structural MRI and positron emission tomography with [18F]fluorodopa ([18F]DOPA) and found that patients with psychomotor retardation had significantly lower [18F]DOPA uptake K(i) values in the left caudate than controls (U=2, p=0.002). This finding suggests that decreased function of dopamine in the left caudate may contribute to psychomotor retardation and that dopamine enhancing antidepressants might prove to be beneficial for this subgroup of patients (Martinot et al., 2001).

In 1998, Bange and Bathien compared P300 event related potentials (ERPs) elicited by two visual tasks (the visual oddball paradigm and the simple visual paradigm) in 12 subjects with MDD, 11 subjects with bipolar depression and 20 matched controls. It was proposed that P300 ERPs might serve as a marker for decreased central processing in depressed subjects. With this model, peak latency reflects the time taken for stimulation evaluation, while amplitude reflects perceptual functioning and processing of the visual stimuli. When 18 of the depressed subjects reached remission, these ERPs were recorded again. Depressed subjects were significantly less accurate with the visual oddball paradigm based on reaction time and accuracy. Subjects with bipolar depression had increased P3 latencies but no difference in P300 amplitude compared to control subjects. At remission, subjects from the MDD and bipolar group demonstrated slowed reaction times and reduced P300 peak latencies. With regard to psychomotor retardation, these findings suggest that unipolar and bipolar subjects both had a motor component but that only bipolar subjects had a cognitive component (Bange and Bathien, 1998).

A novel study by Loo and colleagues employed transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to examine motor cortical physiology and functioning. In this investigation, 19 subjects with depression and 10 healthy control subjects underwent motor evoked potential measurements with EMG monitoring of the biceps brachii muscle both at rest and during voluntary contraction. On the day of TMS testing, depression and psychomotor retardation were assessed with the CORE and MADRS scales by two independent raters. Of the depressed subjects, 8 were characterized as having significant psychomotor retardation. Transcranial magnetic stimulation paradigms included resting threshold, active threshold with weak contractions (estimated 10% of maximum force), cortical silent period with weak contractions (estimated 10% of maximum force), intracortical inhibition (a paired pulse measure with an interstimulus interval of 2 ms), intracortical facilitation (interstimulus interval of 12 ms), and single pulses during four 15 second maximum voluntary contractions with the idea of testing a “fatiguing” paradigm. For this later paradigm, data collections include voluntary force, any change in force resulting from TMS, motor evoked potential size, and duration of the cortical silent period. At the culmination of testing, subjects also rated perceived effort to produce maximum voluntary force with a modified Borg scale. The depressed subjects with psychomotor retardation endorsed lower effort levels than the control group (p<0.05). Further the CORE scores correlated with maximum voluntary contractions as a percentage of expected force (r=−0.7, p<0.001) and changes in cortical silent period throughout the fourth contraction (r=−0.56, p<0.05). Conversely, there were no correlations with the MADRS. Depressed subjects with psychomotor retardation also produced less maximum motor force than other patients with depression and controls (p=0.027). Depression severity did not play a role in these findings. The investigators postulated that psychomotor retardation in depressed subjects involves an impaired ability to drive or activate the motor cortex (Loo et al., 2008).

3.2. Neurotransmitters

Neurochemical theories propose that dysfunctional dopaminergic neurotransmission is an underlying feature of psychomotor changes in patients with melancholic depression. Studies of plasma dopamine precursors and cerebrospinal fluid levels of dopamine metabolites such as homomvanillic acid support this model (Kapur and Mann, 1992; Schrijvers et al., 2008b; Winograd-Gurvich et al., 2006).

One study measured dopamine binding, with SPECT with D2/3 ligand 123I-IBZM in 15 subjects with depression and 15 healthy volunteers. Regional uptake, as shown by iodobenzamide (IBZM), in the left and right striatum was associated with psychomotor retardation as measured by reaction time and verbal fluency. Notably, severity of depression was not significantly correlated with IBSZM uptake (Shah et al., 1997).

Conversely one study yielded no evidence to support the hypothesis that patients with psychomotor retardation have decreased dopaminergic functioning. The study used injections of apomorphine, a dopamine agonist, in patients with melancholia and controls. This had no overall affect on performance of tasks assessing psychomotor retardation. However, the study was limited in that it had a small sample size and used melancholic patients with only mild psychomotor retardation (Austin et al., 2000).

Serotonin is also hypothesized to play a role in depression and psychomotor retardation. In 2001, Sabbe and colleagues administered meta-chlorophenylpiperazine (mCPP), a 5-HT receptor agonist to 14 healthy, adult, male subjects. During reaction time and copying psychomotor tasks, the males who received mCPP had cognitive slowing, but not motor slowing. These findings suggest that serotonin may play a role in the cognitive showing in psychomotor retardation (Sabbe et al., 2001).

3.3. HPA axis

Psychomotor symptoms in mood disorder are also thought to have a relationship with hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis overactivity (de Winter et al., 2003; van Londen et al., 1997, 1998). This was first demonstrated in 1984 by Klein and colleagues, who conducted dexamethasone suppression tests (DSTs) on 102 consecutive inpatients with MDD (Klein et al., 1984). The In-patient Multidimensional Psychiatric Scale (IMPS) was among the measures collected (Lorr et al., 1966). In this sample, there were 16 subjects classified as non-suppressors (defined as a postdexamethasone cortisol level greater than 6 micrograms per deciliter). These non-suppressors also displayed more psychomotor retardation as assessed by the IMPS and postdexamethasone cortisol displayed a correlation with the IMPS score of retarded depression (r=0.394, p<0.001) (Klein et al., 1984). Subsequent work in 1988 by Smith and colleagues examined the DST in 52 psychiatric inpatients that were “diagnostically heterogeneous” (Smith et al., 1988). There was no relationship between postdexamethasone serum cortisol levels and HRSD or anxiety scale scores. However, symptoms of psychomotor retardation were related to postdexamethasone serum cortisol levels (r=0.38, p<0.01) (Smith et al., 1988). This idea was further supported by the work of Mitchell and colleagues in 1996. In this study, 114 consecutive inpatients with MDD were enrolled and rated with the CORE scale and Hamilton Depression Severity scale. The majority of these patients were taking antipsychotic or antidepressant medications. Baseline, predexamethasone cortisol levels were drawn at 4:00 PM on day 1. Subsequently, 1 mg of dexamethasone was administered at 11:00 PM that night. On the next day, blood was drawn at 8:00 AM and 4:00 PM for cortisol and dexamethasone levels. Cortisol levels were also drawn were also drawn at 11:00 PM. The CORE scale scores had a significant correlation with the 8:00 AM postdexamethasone cortisol levels after the effects of age, dexamethasone concentrations, and basal cortisol levels were examined with partial correlations (p≤0.01) (Mitchell et al., 1996).

Arginine vasopressin (AVP) is produced by parvocellular and magnocellular hypothalamic neurons and is known to activate the HPA axis (Antoni, 1993). Studies have also examined the possible relationship between plasma AVP levels in patients with MDD and psychomotor retardation. In 1997, van Londen and colleagues collected plasma AVP levels in 52 subjects with MDD and 37 healthy controls. In this case, the mean plasma AVP levels displayed a modest correlation with psychomotor retardation as assessed with the Widlocher scale (r=0.2982, p=0.032) (van Londen et al., 1997). In a follow-up study, AVP concentrations, daytime wrist activity and nighttime wrist activity were assessed in 48 subjects with MDD and 30 healthy controls over 5 consecutive 24 hour periods. There was an inverse relationship between plasma AVP levels and motor activity in both groups (van Londen et al., 1998). In 2003, other investigators examined plasma AVP and cortisol levels in 66 subjects with MDD. Subjects that were diagnostically classified with anxious-retarded depression had a significant AVP-cortisol correlation (r=0.56, p=0.004). This group concluded that anxious-retarded depression may represent a phenotypic category of depression worthy of further study related to AVP and HPA axis dysregulation (de Winter et al., 2003).

4. Objective measurements of psychomotor retardation

Neuropsychological measures of psychomotor retardation assess its associated cognitive and motor behaviors to provide an objective approach for its quantification (Szabadi et al., 1976). Circadian rhythms and medication are important confounding factors that should be accounted for when measuring psychomotor retardation (Joffe et al., 1987; Moffoot et al., 1994; Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996; Sabbe et al., 1997). For example, psychomotor retardation is typically more pronounced in the morning than in the evening (Greden and Carroll, 1981; Moffoot et al., 1994; Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996). Psychotropic medications also impact the presentation of psychomotor retardation in depressed patients by decreasing its observed severity (Joffe et al., 1987; Sabbe et al., 1997). Subject effort is another important consideration, though research with the Borg self-rating scale has indicated that depressed patients perceive themselves to perform within normal limits on neuropsychological tasks despite significant motor and cognitive deficits (Steele et al., 2000). Table 2 outlines variables and results of different neuropsychological measures used in studies of psychomotor retardation.

Table 2.

List of objective measures and outcomes.

Category Test Variable Measure Finding P-value Study reference
Drawing tasks Figure copying Total time (for simple figures) Time Depressed>controls <0.001 van Hoof et al. (1993)
Drawing tasks Figure copying Total time (for complex figures) Time Depressed>controls <0.01 van Hoof et al. (1993)
Drawing tasks Figure copying Initiation time (for simple figures) Time Depressed>controls <0.01 van Hoof et al. (1993)
Drawing tasks Figure copying Movement time (for simple figures) Time Depressed>controls <0.01 van Hoof et al. (1993)
Drawing tasks Figure copying Movement time (for complex figures) Time Depressed>controls <0.001 van Hoof et al. (1993)
Drawing tasks Drawing task Movement time per line (trials 6–9) Time Depressed>controls <0.001 Sabbe et al. (1999)
Drawing tasks Drawing task Movement time between line/pause time (trials 6–9) Time Depressed>controls <0.001 Sabbe et al. (1999)
Drawing tasks Drawing task Mean velocity per line (trials 6–9) Time/length Depressed< controls <0.001 Sabbe et al. (1999)
Drawing tasks Figure copying Initiation time Time Depressed>dysthymic controls 0.022 Pier et al. (2004)
Drawing tasks Figure copying Movement time Time Depressed>dysthymic controls <0.05 Pier et al. (2004)
Drawing tasks Figure copying Pause time Time Depressed>dysthymic controls Pier et al. (2004)
Drawing tasks Figure copying Reinspection time Time Depressed>dysthymic controls Pier et al. (2004)
Drawing tasks Trail making test Drawing time Time Depressed>dysthymic controls Pier et al. (2004)
Drawing tasks Symbol digit substitution task Matching time Time Depressed>dysthymic controls <0.05 Pier et al. (2004)
Drawing tasks Symbol digit substitution task Writing time Time Depressed>dysthymic controls <0.001 Pier et al. (2004)
Drawing tasks Digit symbol substitution test Writing time (morning) Time Depressed>controls 0.004 Moffoot et al. (1994)
Drawing tasks Digit symbol substitution test Writing time (evening) Time Depressed controls NS Moffoot et al. (1994)
Drawing tasks Gibson Spiral Maze Total time to finish (with external distraction) Time Depressed>recovered depressed <0.05 Blackburn (1975)
Motor and cognitive CANTAB computerized psychometric testing battery Response initiation time Time Depressed>controls 0.09 Shah et al. (1997)
Motor and cognitive CANTAB computerized psychometric testing battery Movement time time Depressed>controls 0.05 Shah et al. (1997)
Motor and cognitive Two-choice, fixed foreperiod reaction time task Decision time Time Depressed>controls 0.04 Knott and Lapierre (1987)
Motor and cognitive Two-choice, fixed foreperiod reaction time task Movement time Time Depressed>controls 0.007 Knott and Lapierre (1987)
Cognitive Nufferno Speed Test Speed Time Depressed recovered depressed NS Blackburn (1975)
Cognitive Speed and Capacity of Language Processing Test Speed of processing Time Depressed< controls 0.001 Steele et al. (2000)
Cognitive Posner’s covert orientation of visual attention test Response time Time Depressed>controls <0.001 Smith et al. (1995)
Motor Heart and activity monitor Horizontal movement Amount of movement in 24 h Severely depressed>controls 0.046 Iverson (2004)
Motor Heart and activity monitor Horizontal movement Amount of movement in 24 h Severely depressed>less severely depressed 0.018 Iverson (2004)
Motor Finger tapping test Time to tap 210 times Total time Depressed>controls 0.001 Steele et al. (2000)
Motor Serial choice reaction test Reaction time Time Depressed>controls Bezzi et al. (1981)
Motor Serial choice reaction test Number of reactions Number of reactions Depressed< controls Bezzi et al. (1981)
Motor Maximum grip strength Grip strength (morning and evening) Strength Depressed< controls 0.05 Moffoot et al. (1994)
Motor Facial EMG monitoring Patterns of EMG for specific emotions Sadness Depressed>controls <0.01 Greden and Carroll (1981), Schwartz et al. (1976)
Speech Automatic speech Pause time Time Unmedicated depressed>medicated depressed <0.01 Szabadi et al. (1976)
Speech Automatic speech Phonation time Time Unmedicated depressed medicated depressed NS Szabadi et al. (1976)
Speech Automatic speech Pause time Time Unmedicated depressed>medicated depressed <0.001 Hardy et al. (1984)
Speech Automatic speech Phonation time Time Unmedicated depressed medicated depressed NS Hardy et al. (1984)
Speech Timing speech Pause time Time High depressed>low depressed <0.005 Greden and Carroll (1981), Pope et al. (1970)
Speech Timing speech Silent quotient Time High depressed>low depressed <0.005 Greden and Carroll (1981), Pope et al. (1970)
Speech Fluency task Generation of words or lists Number of words Depressed< controls <0.05 van Hoof et al. (1993)
Biological Pain threshold Pain threshold Threshold reflex Retarded depressed>controls depressed Bezzi et al. (1981)
Biological Transcranial magnetic stimulation Silent period Time Depressed>controls 0.04 Steele et al. (2000)
Biological Transcranial magnetic stimulation Motor threshold (in right hemisphere) Intensity for motor evoked potential Depressed< controls <0.0001 Bajbouj et al. (2006)
Biological Transcranial magnetic stimulation Cortical silent period (in right hemisphere) Time Depressed< controls 0.002 Bajbouj et al. (2006)
Biological Transcranial magnetic stimulation Paired pulse (in right hemisphere) Motor evoked potential Depressed< controls 0.004 Bajbouj et al. (2006)
Biological Urine MHPG level Urine MHPG level MHPG level Retarded depressed< controls <0.004 Samson et al. (1994)

Depressed is the measure from patients with diagnosis of depression.

Controls is the measure from the normal controls.

High depressed and low depressed are the measures from the same patient on days where they were more depressed and less depressed, respectively.

Dysthymic is the measure from patients with diagnosis of dysthymia.

Severely depressed and less severely depressed refer to the measures from patients with severe and less severe depression, respectively.

Retarded depressed and severely depressed refer to the measures from depressed patients who scored high on the SRRS and Montgomery and Asberg Depression Scale, respectively.

Unmedicated depressed and medicated depressed are the measures from the same patient before and after receiving medication as depression treatment, respectively.

Symbols:

<,> refer to the measures being significantly greater or less than.

refers to the measures being non-significantly different.

4.1. Drawing tasks

Drawing tasks are frequently used to measure psychomotor retardation (Pier et al., 2004; Sabbe et al., 1999; van Hoof et al., 1993). Patients and healthy cohorts are asked to copy simple or complex geometric figures (Pier et al., 2004; Sabbe et al., 1999; van Hoof et al., 1993). For tasks that involve drawing simple figures, subjects are instructed to copy a picture of a line or connect two circles with a line. The line-drawing tasks measure only motor aspects of psychomotor retardation (Pier et al., 2004). Complex figure copying tasks are slightly more challenging and can measure both motor and cognitive features (Pier et al., 2004; van Hoof et al., 1993). Preoccupation with precision is an important confounding variable to consider which could impact accuracy and anxiety levels in the subjects (Morgan et al., 1994). For instance, a group of healthy subjects may have varying degrees of speed while drawing due to handwriting styles, level of precision to detail, and test taking related anxiety.

There are a number of other drawing tests that can measure psychomotor retardation. The trail making test (TMT) is one such measure in which subjects must connect 25 circles that contain either numbers or a combination of numbers and letters in ascending order (Neu et al., 2001). This test was originally designed to test processing speed (TMT Part A) or cognitive flexibility (TMT Part B) (Misdraji and Gass, 2009). The digit symbol substitution test (DSST) involves graphomotor abilities and thinking, thus, it can measure both motor and cognitive aspects of psychomotor retardation. It is a time sensitive test in which subjects must draw a symbol that corresponds with a specific stimulus. The number of correct symbols within the time period (e.g. 90 or 120 s) is counted (Moffoot et al., 1994; Pier et al., 2004). One limitation of the DSST is that it involves incidental memory, so the cognitive portion of the task may not measure pure cognitive processing speed. Another test that involves graphomotor ability is the Gibson Spiral Maze (GSM). This specific maze task was designed to assess only psychomotor speed, and is not influenced by cognitive abilities. Subjects who complete the GSM must correctly trace through a spiral maze from a starting point to an end point without touching bordering lines (Blackburn, 1975).

4.2. Combined motor and cognitive measures

Other tests that measure both motor and cognitive aspects of psychomotor retardation include the Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB) computerized measures (Moffoot et al., 1994; Shah et al., 1997) and the two-choice fixed period reaction task (Knott and Lapierre, 1987). The CANTAB includes measures that require the subjects to place their finger in a specified place on a touch screen after being prompted by a specific stimulus. There are multiple measures of the CANTAB that can be performed, and can measure both motor and cognitive features (Moffoot et al., 1994; Shah et al., 1997). The CANTAB was designed to be engaging to the subjects, which helps ensure motivational factors do not detract from the subjects’ performance. However, it was not specifically designed to measure psychomotor retardation, thus, the results may not be specific to that construct (Sahakian and Owen, 1992).

4.3. Cognitive measures

A number of neuropsychological measures evaluate only the cognitive portion of psychomotor retardation. For example, the Nufferno Speed Test is designed to measure only cognitive processing speed. This test involves the subjects completing letter series under stressed and unstressed conditions (Blackburn, 1975). The Speed and Capacity of Language Processing Test (SCOLP) is a similar method of semantic cognitive speed. The subjects are first given simple statements, to which they respond true or false as quickly as possible. They then are given two words, one a true word and one not, and must quickly identify which is the true word. The combination of the two tests allows for the measurement of cognitive speed while controlling for vocabulary and poor language skills (Saxton et al., 2001; Steele et al., 2000). A benefit to the SCOLP test is that it was designed to account for the age of the examinee (Steele et al., 2000).

Another cognitive speed test is the Posner’s covert orientation of visual attention test (COVA) in which the subjects are shown a stimulus that will specify which side of a computer screen the subjects are to focus on. Subsequently, a second stimulus appears on the screen and the subjects respond by pressing a response key (Smith et al., 1995). A limitation to the COVA is that it requires attention, so subjects that have attention deficits may be mistaken for having psychomotor retardation.

4.4. Motor measures

Certain neuropsychological measures are designed to assess only the motor component of psychomotor retardation. The benefit of using these measures is that they allow researchers to assess the extent to which motor processing speed contributes to psychomotor retardation (Blackburn, 1975; Smith et al., 1995). One such method is actometry, which is a detection of acceleration that can be used to measure movement. Actometry can be measured by an actimeter or actometer, which are devices worn on the wrist or belt that measure gross activity levels during a 24 hour period (Iverson, 2004; Sobin and Sackeim, 1997). The data can be broken into different time periods of the day that can be helpful in determining when psychomotor retardation is most severe (Foster and Kupfer, 1975; Sobin and Sackeim, 1997). However, cautious interpretation of the data is warranted as the actometry measurement can be confounded by certain daytime activities including recreational sports and household chores. One way to control for confounds of actometry is to measure the subjects’ pulse (Iverson, 2004) with a device called LifeShirt (Minassian et al., 2010). This device uses software to calculate movement by summing movement on both the x and y axes while controlling for gravity. Minassian and colleagues hypothesized that LifeShirt could lead to the “development of a phenotypic signature” for specific diseases, such as mania in bipolar disorder or psychomotor retardation in MDD (Minassian et al., 2010).

The finger tapping test is also a measure of pure motor speed (Steele et al., 2000; Szabadi et al., 1976). To perform this task, subjects are asked to press a level as fast as possible for a specified time (Szabadi et al., 1976). In addition, the Serial Choice Reaction Test, also known as the Bjerner test, is a timed reaction test that measures motor speed. Subjects are instructed to move a lever either up or down depending on an auditory stimulus. The reaction time and the number of movements within 12 min are recorded (Bezzi et al., 1981). Maximum grip strength, measured by squeezing of a dynamometer, has also been used as a measure of psychomotor retardation (Moffoot et al., 1994). Although it has been shown that patients with psychomotor retardation have less motor force than healthy controls, this measure is affected by gender and muscle strength. Facial EMG monitoring is another means to measure psychomotor retardation (Greden and Carroll, 1981). Flat facial affect has been identified as a marker of psychomotor retardation (Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996), and EMG monitors can pick up on facial expressions that may go unnoticed by the clinician (Greden and Carroll, 1981). A limitation to facial EMG monitoring is that it can be variable to mood state and does not provide a global severity assessment of psychomotor retardation (Greden and Carroll, 1981).

4.5. Speech measures

Analysis of speech can also be an indicator of psychomotor retardation. Specific speech variables include pause and phonation time, silent quotient, and tone (Hardy et al., 1984; Pope et al., 1970; Szabadi et al., 1976). Recording of some of these variables, such as pause time and silent quotient, can be performed with a stopwatch or tape-recorder (Hardy et al., 1984; Pope et al., 1970; Szabadi et al., 1976). Other variables (e.g., phonation time) should be recorded with a tape-recorder (Hardy et al., 1984; Szabadi et al., 1976). When a tape-recorder is used, an oscilloscope can be employed to analyze verbal recordings (Hardy et al., 1984; Szabadi et al., 1976). It is beneficial to have the subject speak naturally (e.g., count from 1 to 10 at a natural pace) (Hardy et al., 1984; Szabadi et al., 1976). Speech during conversation may be affected by the quality of the conversation or the subject’s personality, but using a task like counting helps eliminate these confounds (Szabadi et al., 1976). Similar speech tasks such as verbal fluency challenges (e.g., say as many types of furniture as possible within 60 s) involve generating words based on semantic categories, which assesses cognitive processing speed and lower-order executive functions, instead of retardation in speech (van Hoof et al., 1993).

4.6. Biological measures

Biological measures are an objective way to identify psychomotor retardation, and may provide insight into the biological underpinnings of psychomotor retardation. One study (Bezzi et al., 1981) examined pain threshold as a marker of psychomotor retardation. To measure pain threshold, electrical pulses of increasing intensity were administered to the subject’s hand. An EMG on the subject’s biceps femori muscles measured the response to the stimulus. Patients with psychomotor retardation had higher pain tolerance than controls and depressed patients without psychomotor retardation (Bezzi et al., 1981).

Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is a method of measuring cortical inhibition and excitability through specific paradigms. These TMS paradigms provide information regarding GABAergic and glutamatergic neurotransmission which impacts motor functioning (Bajbouj et al., 2006; Greden, 1993; Loo et al., 2008; Niculescu and Akiskal, 2001; Steele et al., 2000). These TMS paradigms involve placing a coil over the subject’s motor cortex and subsequently stimulating this area with magnetic pulses, that results in motor movement. This movement, referred to as a motor evoked potential (MEP) can be recorded with EMG (Loo et al., 2008; Steele et al., 2000). The cortical silent period (CSP) and intracrotical inhibition (ICI) index cortical inhibition. Motor threshold (MT) and intracortical facilitation (ICF) index cortical excitability (Bajbouj et al., 2006; Loo et al., 2008; Steele et al., 2000). The motor threshold is the minimum amount of stimulus intensity that evokes an MEP of 50 μV or more. The cortical silent period involves the application of suprathreshold stimulation while the subjects produce sustained, submaximum contraction of their hand muscles. This produces a period of electrical quiescence after the MEP. Paired pulse TMS involves pairing a subthreshold stimulus with a suprathreshold stimulus that is administered with a variable interstimulus interval (ISI). Brief ISIs (such as 5 ms or less) inhibit cortical activity, while longer ISIs facilitate cortical activity. Investigators have attempted to examine TMS paradigms to quantify psychomotor retardation, postulating that it is correlated with motor cortical inhibition (Bajbouj et al., 2006; Loo et al., 2008; Steele et al., 2000). Another possible measure correlated to psychomotor retardation is 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol (MHPG) level. Levels of MHPG, which are thought to index noradrenergic activity, can be measured in plasma, urine, and saliva. Urine MHPG levels have been positively correlated with psychomotor retardation in patients with MDD (Samson et al., 1994; Yoshimura et al., 2004).

The measures for psychomotor retardation can be useful tools in diagnosing the presence of such features in patients. If the classification can be done in an efficient and valid method, then there could be a multitude of benefits for both the patient and clinician. However, more research is needed regarding the reliability and validity of psychomotor retardation measures in clinical populations.

5. Psychomotor retardation scales

Increasingly, psychiatrists in research and clinical practice use depression rating scales that assess the severity of depressive symptoms in patients. However, most depression scales have a minimal number of questions that assess psychomotor retardation. For example, the Montgomery–Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS), a scale commonly used depression severity measure in research studies assess “lassitude” but no other items address psychomotor retardation (Fantino and Moore, 2009). The Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression is also a commonly used scale to assess depression severity. The Hamilton scale contains only one item that assesses overall psychomotor retardation (Snaith, 1977). Since psychomotor retardation is an established component of depression and may potentially have treatment implications, its measurement would be beneficial for clinical practice and research. The three major scales currently available are the Salpetriere Retardation Rating Scale (SRRS) (Dantchev and Widlocher, 1998), also known as the Widlocher Depressive Retardation Rating Scale or the Depressive Retardation Rating Scale (DRRS) (Widlocher, 1983), the Motor Agitation and Retardation Scale (Sobin et al., 1998), and the CORE measure (Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996). Each has a unique focus and allows the clinician and researcher to specifically choose what they want to evaluate (Bonin-Guillaume et al., 2008; Dantchev and Widlocher, 1998; Parker, 2005; Sobin et al., 1998; Widlocher, 1983).

5.1. Salpetriere Retardation Rating Scale (SRRS)

The developers of the SRRS aimed to isolate psychomotor retardation from other depressive symptoms. Psychomotor retardation is thought to have both cognitive and motor aspects, which are both measured in the scale. There are a total of fifteen items on the SRRS. The first three measure movement, specifically the quality of stride and slowness of limb, trunk, head, and neck movement. The next three items focus on speech including verbal flow, tone of voice, and length of response. Two items are designed to objectively measure cognitive function. These questions are based on the interview conversation and measure the patient’s ability to approach and expand on topics. Five items are subjective and assess fatigue, level of interest, perception of time, memory, and concentration. The last item of the scale is an overall assessment of the patient’s psychomotor retardation. The items are scaled from 0 (symptom absence) to 4 (severe) based on the severity of the presenting symptom, for a total score range of 0 to 60. Each item has a structured format to qualify the level of severity (Dantchev and Widlocher, 1998). The designated cutoff score separating patients with psychomotor retardation from those without is 20 (Pier et al., 2004).

The SRSS has been used in a number of studies to gauge severity of psychomotor retardation. The SRRS has been found to be separately correlated with both cognitive (r=0.67, p<0.005) and motor aspects of psychomotor retardation (p<0.01) (Schrijvers et al., 2008a; Smith et al., 1995), as well as cognitive variables such as latency of response, line-drawing tasks, speech pause time, and figure copying tasks (Brebion et al., 1995; Dantchev and Widlocher, 1998; Hardy et al., 1984; Hoffmann et al., 1985; Pier et al., 2004). In addition to patients with MDD, the SRRS has been used to assess patients with dysthymic disorder. Overall, patients with dysthymic disorder had high scores on the SRSS cognitive items, but only half of the patients with dysthymia had a total score above the cutoff for psychomotor retardation (Pier et al., 2004).

5.2. Short Version of Retardation Rating Scale for Elderly Patients (RRS-4)

The Depressive Retardation Rating Scale has been made into a short version to evaluate psychomotor retardation in geriatric patients. There were some modifications with the items on the scale, to tailor it to geriatric populations. The RRS-4 consists of 14 items with a total score range from 0 to 56. Each item is scored on a 5-point scale from 0 (normal) to 4 (severe) (Bonin-Guillaume et al., 2008). Compared to the DRRS, the modified scale has one additional item rating motility and one less item rating speech. It also lacks the item for overall impression, but has the same number of cognitive and subjective experience items (Bonin-Guillaume et al., 2008; Widlocher, 1983).

5.3. The CORE measure

The CORE measure was designed with the intent to classify melancholic and non-melancholic subtypes of depression. For the purpose of this review, the CORE measure is considered a psychomotor retardation scale because its items focus on that construct. The assumption for this scale is that psychomotor retardation is not only a requirement for melancholic subtype, but if severe enough, is adequate in itself for the diagnosis of melancholic depression. Therefore, there are no endogenous symptom based items (e.g., anhedonia) for melancholia on the scale. In order for a clinician or researcher to use the CORE measure, it is recommended that they have experience in interviewing depressed patients. The scale is intended for use during a clinical interview with a patient and is rated by observation. Most items are designed for naturalistic observation, but some require the clinician to ask the patient perform certain tasks, such as smile, and then rate the completion of the task. It is recommended that the interview be conducted in the morning, because psychomotor disturbances are more profound early in the day. In addition, the scale should be used at least twenty minutes into the interview to allow the patient to get comfortable, and ensure that psychomotor movements are not heavily influenced by anxiety. The exception to this is to observe the patient when walking into the interview for posture and slowed movement. Nevertheless, it is recommended that a check be done when the patient is leaving the room. There are a total of 18 items on the CORE measure. The scale is divided into three subscales, which consist of 6 non-interactiveness scale items, 7 retardation scale items, and 5 agitation scale items. The noninteractiveness scale items include level of interactiveness, reactivity, attentiveness, richness of associations, willingness to converse, and length of responses. The retardation scale items include speed of movement, level of facial and body immobility, posture, amount of delay in motor movement and verbal responses, and speech rate. The items on the agitation subscale include facial expression, movement, and repetitive speech. Each item is scored on 4-point scale based on severity from 0 (absence of symptom) to 3 (severe) with a total score of 0–54. A patient with a score of 21 or higher is considered to have melancholic depression (Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996), and some studies use a cutoff score of 8 to classify psychomotor depression (Loo et al., 2008). The CORE measure has been found in many studies to have good psychometric properties including high reliability and validity (Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996).

5.4. Motor Agitation and Retardation Scale (MARS)

The Motor Agitation and Retardation Scale was designed to assess only the motor disturbances in depressed patients, without taking into account cognitive effects. The estimated time to complete the assessment is approximately 10 to 15 min. The instructions and explanation of the items are intended to be uncomplicated and practical. Psychomotor disturbances were divided into five major body categories including eyes, face, voice, limbs, and trunk. There are a total of 19 items on the scale. Items that fall into the eyes category include direction of gaze, amount of blinking, staring, and eye movement. Items that are associated with the face category include facial expression and facial expressivity. The category of voice has items that include volume, slurring, tone and time for onset. Items under the limbs category include hand, foot, and leg movement, stride, motor slowness, and tension in hands. The trunk category items include posture, immobility, and axial movement. The severity of each item ranges from a 1 to a 4, 4 being the most severe. There are two continuums of severity, the use of which is dictated by if the item is discrete or comprehensive. For discrete items such as erratic eye movement, the severity is scaled by: none, rare, periodic, and continual. For comprehensive items such as monotone speech, the severity is scaled by: none, mild, moderate, and severe (Sobin et al., 1998). The MARS has been found to have exceptional psychometric properties and is associated with the CORE scale’s motor items (Sobin et al., 1998).

Each of the retardation scales mentioned have different strengths and weaknesses, leading to situations in which one may be preferred over others. For clinicians, the MARS scale is designed to be quick and easy to use in a clinical session (Sobin et al., 1998). Throughout the literature, the SRRS has been a common scale used by researchers to measure psychomotor retardation (Schrijvers et al., 2008a; Smith et al., 1995). The SRRS is also a good scale for clinicians to use, and the modified version is optimal to use on elderly patients (Bonin-Guillaume et al., 2008). The CORE scale is primarily used for the diagnosis of melancholia by clinicians or the identification of melancholia for research purposes (Parker and Hadzi-Pavlovic, 1996).

6. Predictive value of psychomotor retardation to clinical outcome with antidepressant pharmacotherapy

Pharmacotherapy for depressed patients is guided by treatment algorithms, clinical judgment, and existing evidence from clinical trials. It is the hope that translational genetics and neuroimaging research will one day provide individualized treatment plans. Currently, there is a dearth of scientific evidence to guide individual antidepressant treatment planning (Taylor et al., 2006). Generally, the first line of treatment involves selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). Atypical antidepressants, tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs), and monoamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs) are considered subsequent options (Mitchell P.B., 1997; Taylor et al., 2006). This is a fairly effective method; nevertheless, only about 50% of patients respond to their first antidepressant treatment and less than 40% achieve remission (Gorlyn et al., 2008; Kemp et al., 2008; Trivedi et al., 2006). Treatment failures produce substantial financial and quality of life burdens for these individuals and society (Rasmussen-Torvik and McAlpine, 2007).

Reliable predictive factors could assist with medication choice and treatment algorithms (Mallinckrodt et al., 2007). It is important to note that lack of overall difference in efficacy of two medications does not ipso facto establish equivalency. One medication may be significantly more effective in a specific subgroup (Mallinckrodt et al., 2007). While there is “disappointingly little [known] about the prediction of response” to antidepressants; there have been a number of studies that investigated the predictive validity that baseline psychomotor retardation has for different types of antidepressant medication (Burns et al., 1995). However, these studies were fairly divided for which types of antidepressants psychomotor retardation is a successful predictor of response. There is also controversy as to whether findings can be generalized to other antidepressants of the same class (1986; 1990; Aman and Turbott, 1991; Amsterdam, 1998; Brown, 2007; Burns et al., 1995; Caligiuri et al., 2003; Del Zompo et al., 1990; Flament et al., 1999; Gorlyn et al., 2008; Guiard et al., 2009; Hegerl et al., 2001; Herrera-Guzman et al., 2008; Higuchi et al., 2008a,b; Hordern et al., 1963, 1964; Joffe et al., 1987; Joyce et al., 2002; Kemp et al., 2008; Mallinckrodt et al., 2005, 2007; Mitchell Philip B., 1995; Mitchell P.B., 1997; Mulder et al., 2006; Ranelli and Miller, 1981; Raoux et al., 1994; Rasmussen-Torvik and McAlpine, 2007; Roose et al., 1994; Sabbe et al., 1997; Sobin and Sackeim, 1997; Taylor et al., 2006; Thase et al., 1995; White and White, 1986; Yoshimura et al., 2004; Zarate et al., 1996). See Table 3 for a comprehensive list of the specific drugs, drug types, number of subjects, method of psychomotor measurement, psychomotor retardation’s predictive value, and significance.

Table 3.

Prediction of response to antidepressants by psychomotor retardation.

Drug name Drug type Predictor of response/nonresponse Number of subjects Method of psychomotor measurement Significance Reference
Fluoxetine SSRI Not predictor of response (found PMR improved with treatment) 44 Computer aided simple drawing tasks Sabbe et al. (1997)
Fluoxetine SSRI Nonresponse 76 Observationa P<0.001 Burns et al. (1995)
Sertraline SSRI Response Melancholia diagnosis P<0.05 Flament et al. (1999)
Fluoxetine SSRI Response 51 Melancholia diagnosis P=0.002 Heiligenstein et al. (1994)
Citalopram SSRI Nonresponse 2875 Melancholia diagnosis P=0.045 McGrath et al. (2008)
Duloxetine SNRI Not predictor of response 1913 (meta-analysis) Melancholia diagnosis Mallinckrodt et al. (2005)
Amitriptyline Tricyclic Response 36 Observation P<0.05 Ranelli and Miller (1981)
Imipramine Tricyclic Response (Meta-analysis) CSF MHPG P<0.05b Yoshimura et al. (2004)
Clomipramine, Maprotiline, Trimipramine Tricyclics Not predictor of response (found PMR improved with treatment) 26 Wrist actigraph and SSRS Raoux et al. (1994)
Lofepramine Tricyclic Response 68 Observation P<0.001 Burns et al. (1995)
Duloxetine SNRI Not predictor of response 1913 (meta-analysis) Melancholia diagnosis Mallinckrodt et al. (2005)
Milnacipran SNRI Response (Meta-analysis) Plasma MHPG P<0.05 Yoshimura et al. (2004)
Carbamazepine Mood stabilizer Not predictor of response (found PMR improved with treatment) 19 Wrist actigraph Joffe et al. (1987)
Imipramine, Clomipramine, Nortriptyline, Maprotiline, Fluoxitine Tricyclic, Tricyclic, Tricyclic, Tetracyclic, SSRI Response (Meta-analysis) Urine MHPG P<0.05b Yoshimura et al. (2004)
Sertraline, Phenelzine, Bupropion SSRI, MAOI, NDRI Nonresponse 28 Reaction time, velocity scaling, and observation P<0.01 Caligiuri et al. (2003)
Bupropion NDRI Response 20 Visual memory and mental processing speed P=0.009
P=0.006
Herrera-Guzman et al. (2008)
Citalopram vs Clomipramine SSRI vs TCA Response to Clomipramine over Citalopram 114 Endogenous depression diagnosis P<0.005 Danish University Antidepressant Group (1986)
Amitriptyline vs Fluoxetine TCA vs SSRI No difference 25 Retarded depression Laakmann et al. (1988)
Paroxetine vs Clomipramine SSRI vs TCA Response to Clomipramine over Paroxetine 102 Endogenous depression diagnosis P<0.001 Danish University Antidepressant Group (1990)
Fluoxetine vs Nortriptyline SSRI vs TCA Response to Nortriptyline over Fluoxetine 34 Melancholia diagnosisc P<0.001 Roose et al. (1994)
Fluoxetine vs Nortriptyline SSRI vs TCA Response to Fluoxetine over Nortriptyline 145 Score of >14 on CORE scale P=0.057 Joyce et al. (2002)
Fluoxetine vs Nortriptyline SSRI vs TCA No difference 145 Score of 8–13 on CORE scale or melancholia diagnosis Joyce et al. (2002)
Duloxetine vs Fluoxetine, Paroxetine, Escitalopram SNRI vs SSRIs Response to Duloxetine over SSRIs 2463 (meta-analysis) Observation P=0.002 Mallinckrodt et al. (2007)
Minaprin vs Amitriptyline MAOA vs tricyclic Not predictor of response (found PMR improved with treatment) 60 Observation Del Zompo et al. (1990)
Moclobemide vs Clomipramine MAOI vs TCA No difference 115 Psychomotor retardation from HDRS Danish University Antidepressant Group (1993)
a

Observation refers to using a question from a clinical scale such as HAM-D or MDRS, or assessing certain observable characteristics of psychomotor retardation.

b

Significant, but specific P-value unknown.

c

Patients were elderly and had concurrent cardiovascular disease.

6.1. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors

The SSRIs have been studied most frequently in patients with psychomotor retardation (1986; 1990; Amsterdam, 1998; Burns et al., 1995; Caligiuri et al., 2003; Flament et al., 1999; Gorlyn et al., 2008; Hegerl et al., 2001; Higuchi et al., 2008a; Joyce et al., 2002; Kemp et al., 2008; Mallinckrodt et al., 2007; McGrath et al., 2008; Mitchell Philip B., 1995; Mitchell P.B., 1997; Rasmussen-Torvik and McAlpine, 2007; Roose et al., 1994; Sabbe et al., 1997; Taylor et al., 2006; Yoshimura et al., 2004). Some studies made the argument that SSRIs would not work as well for patients with psychomotor retardation because it is likely these patients have a dopamine and/or norepinephrine imbalance in addition to or instead of a serotonin imbalance (Amsterdam, 1998; Caligiuri et al., 2003; Flament et al., 1999; Herrera-Guzman et al., 2008; Kemp et al., 2008; Taylor et al., 2006). These studies hypothesized that tricyclics (TCAs), serotonin–norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs), and norepinephrine and dopamine reuptake inhibitors (NDRIs) would be more effective (Amsterdam, 1998; Caligiuri et al., 2003; Flament et al., 1999; Herrera-Guzman et al., 2008; Kemp et al., 2008; Taylor et al., 2006). However, results are mixed. Studies found that melancholia predicted response to fluoxetine and sertraline (Flament et al., 1999; Heiligenstein et al., 1994), but nonresponse to Citalopram (McGrath et al., 2008). Psychomotor retardation, diagnosed by clinical observation predicted nonresponse to fluoxetine (Burns et al., 1995). Other work found that psychomotor retardation failed to predict response or nonresponse for SSRIs (Sabbe et al., 1997). Two studies hypothesized that sertraline would be the optimum SSRI for patients with psychomotor retardation because it has the greatest dopaminergic activity, compared to other SSRIs (Amsterdam, 1998; Flament et al., 1999). One study found that melancholia predicted response to sertraline (Flament et al., 1999), and another found that patients who performed poorly on psychomotor tests were more likely to have nonresponse to sertraline (Caligiuri et al., 2003). However, agents with broad pharmacologic actions may be more efficacious for patients with psychomotor retardation (Parker et al., 2010). Out of five studies that compared SSRIs to TCAs, four of them found that psychomotor retardation was a better predictor of response to TCAs (1986; 1990; Joyce et al., 2002; Laakmann et al., 1988; Mitchell Philip B., 1995; Roose et al., 1994). One of these studies found that the definition of psychomotor retardation could change the predictive value of TCAs being more effective than SSRIs (Joyce et al., 2002). Therefore discrepancies in results of the studies may be attributable to methodological differences or to differing definitions of psychomotor retardation (Joyce et al., 2002). Other work demonstrated that psychomotor retardation predicted response for SNRIs more so than for SSRIs (Mallinckrodt et al., 2007).

6.2. Tricyclics

The second most frequently studied class of antidepressant in relation to psychomotor retardation’s predictive value is the TCAs. Of the five studies found that examine TCAs, three concluded that psychomotor retardation predicted response, and two found that there was no predictive value for either response or nonresponse. No research has concluded that psychomotor retardation predicts nonresponse to TCAs (Ranelli and Miller, 1981; Raoux et al., 1994; Yoshimura et al., 2004). This suggests that TCAs may be considered in patients with psychomotor retardation and treatment resistance (1986; 1990; Joyce et al., 2002; Mitchell Philip B., 1995; Ranelli and Miller, 1981; Raoux et al., 1994; Yoshimura et al., 2004).

6.3. Monoamine oxidase inhibitors

MAOIs have studies with conflicting results. There are an even number of studies that found psychomotor retardation predicts response, nonresponse, and neither for MAOIs (Caligiuri et al., 2003; Del Zompo et al., 1990; White and White, 1986). One study found that an MAOI (Moclobemide) was equally as effective as a TCA (Clomipramine), however, psychomotor retardation was classified based on two questions on the HDRS and may not be completely accurate. The same study found that the TCA was more effective than the MAOI for patients with melancholia (1993). Therefore, nothing can be definitively concluded about how beneficial it is to prescribe MAOIs to patients with psychomotor retardation.

6.4. Other classes of antidepressants

Other classes of antidepressants studied for depressed patients with psychomotor retardation include NDRIs, SNRIs, Tetracyclics (TeCAs), and mood stabilizers. The class of antidepressants, NDRIs, has one study that found that they predict response, and one study that found that they predict nonresponse (Caligiuri et al., 2003; Herrera-Guzman et al., 2008). The SNRIs, TeCAs, and mood stabilizers all do not have any studies in which psychomotor retardation predicted nonresponse. However, they also do not have an abundance of studies that found that psychomotor retardation predicts response. SNRIs had more studies that found psychomotor retardation neither predicted response nor nonresponse than studies that found psychomotor retardation had predictive value for response (Higuchi et al., 2008b; Mallinckrodt et al., 2005; Yoshimura et al., 2004). Nevertheless, SNRIs were found to be better for patients with psychomotor retardation than SSRIs (Mallinckrodt et al., 2007). TeCAs and mood stabilizers each had only one study that examined psychomotor retardation’s predictive validity for them. The lack of abundance of evidence for these classes leads to uncertainty as to how affective they are for depressed patients with psychomotor retardation.

There are a number of factors that could explain why there is such disparity within the literature. One major reason could be that the studies do not classify patients and psychomotor retardation in a uniform manner. Studies used a variety of different tools to determine if a patient has psychomotor retardation, some of which include: the CORE rating scale, MHPG levels, reaction time, speech measures, drawing tasks, actigraph, and specific parts of depression severity scales (1986; 1990; Aman and Turbott, 1991; Amsterdam, 1998; Brown, 2007; Burns et al., 1995; Caligiuri et al., 2003; Del Zompo et al., 1990; Flament et al., 1999; Gorlyn et al., 2008; Guiard et al., 2009; Hegerl et al., 2001; Herrera-Guzman et al., 2008; Higuchi et al., 2008a,b; Hordern et al., 1963, 1964; Joffe et al., 1987; Joyce et al., 2002; Kemp et al., 2008; Mallinckrodt et al., 2005, 2007; Mitchell Philip B., 1995; Mitchell P.B., 1997; Mulder et al., 2006; Ranelli and Miller, 1981; Raoux et al., 1994; Rasmussen-Torvik and McAlpine, 2007; Roose et al., 1994; Sabbe et al., 1997; Sobin and Sackeim, 1997; Taylor et al., 2006; Thase et al., 1995; White and White, 1986; Yoshimura et al., 2004; Zarate et al., 1996). Although all these measures are related to psychomotor retardation, they are considerably different, and could potentially lead researchers to study a heterogeneous group of patients. In order to be able to truly compare the studies’ results, a uniform measure for psychomotor retardation should be used. Another reason that discrepancy could occur is due to the different drugs within a class that were studied and the differing dosages given to the patients. It is possible that specific drugs within a class may not have the same efficacy as the rest of the drugs within that class. In addition, it is common for antidepressants like TCAs to be prescribed at too low of a dose (Mitchell P.B., 1997). If this occurred in any of the studies, then the potential efficacy of the drug may not have been shown. Genetic, neuroimaging, and transcranial magnetic stimulation studies may provide further data regarding this question in the future (Loo et al., 2008; Rasmussen-Torvik and McAlpine, 2007).

7. Predictive value of psychomotor retardation to clinical outcome with electroconvulsive therapy

The use of electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) dates back to the 1930s; however, there is still much to learn regarding which subgroups of patients with MDD respond best to the treatment (Buchan et al., 1992; Hickie et al., 1990, 1996a; Mendels, 1965a,b,c; Rasmussen, 2003; Weller and Weller, 2000). ECT is typically reserved for patients who do not benefit from other antidepressant treatments, are acutely suicidal or psychotic, or who present with catatonic symptoms (Mendels, 1965a,b,c; Rasmussen, 2003).

Most studies that examined prediction of response to ECT found that psychomotor retardation, psychotic symptoms, catatonia, and older age were factors associated with better clinical outcome (1984; Buchan et al., 1992; Carney et al., 1965; Daniels, 2009; Fink et al., 2007; Gill and Lambourn, 1979; Hickie et al., 1990, 1996a,b; Hobson, 1953; Mendels, 1965a,b,c; Petrides et al., 2001; Rush and Weissenburger, 1994). Multiple investigations employing psychomotor retardation measures (e.g., CORE and SRRS) have found the construct of psychomotor retardation to be a positive predictor of beneficial clinical and functional outcome with ECT (Hickie et al., 1990, 1996a,b). One study utilized survey methods and found expert consensus in support of the association between the presence of psychomotor retardation and good clinical outcome (Gill and Lambourn, 1979). Retrospective chart reviews and weighted list factor methods have also substantiated the predictive beneficial outcome of psychomotor retardation with ECT (Mendels, 1965a,b,c).

Although most studies noted that psychomotor retardation was a predictive factor of clinical response and remission to ECT relative to other factors, some noted that psychotic symptoms or psychomotor agitation may be better predictors of response (The Northwick Park ECT trial, 1984; Avery and Silverman, 1984; Gill and Lambourn, 1979; Strian et al., 1979). This does not abase the validity of the conclusion that patients with psychomotor retardation are likely to respond well to ECT. However, it does suggest that there may be other subgroups of patents with depression that may benefit from ECT.

8. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) treatment effect on psychomotor retardation

Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation is a treatment option for depression, with increasing use in clinical practice (Schonfeldt-Lecuona et al., 2010). Treatment with rTMS involves using a changing magnetic field to depolarize neurons and trigger action potentials (Ruohonen and Karhu, 2010). In 2008, the FDA cleared the use of rTMS to treat adults with MDD, who have failed at one trial of an antidepressant medication (Ruohonen and Karhu, 2010).

Although currently there is no literature on the predictive value of psychomotor retardation on the efficacy of rTMS, there are studies that show rTMS helps decrease the severity of psychomotor retardation. Two studies found that although treatment with rTMS did not decrease depression scores on the HDRS, it significantly decreased scores on the psychomotor retardation scales SRRS (P<0.01) and MARS (P=0.023) (Baeken et al., 2010; Hoppner et al., 2003). However, another study found that neither depression nor psychomotor retardation improved with rTMS treatment (Hoeppner et al., 2010). The discrepancy between the studies was cited to potentially be due to different patient populations, intensity of stimulation, coil placement, and use of pharmacological antidepressants (Baeken et al., 2010).

One study found that TMS measures of CSP and MEP may have the potential to predict response to treatment with rTMS in depressed patients, including patients with high CORE scores (Fitzgerald et al., 2004). If measures such as CSP and MEP can be solidly correlated with psychomotor retardation, as some studies have investigated (Bajbouj et al., 2006; Steele et al., 2000), then there may be potential for psychomotor retardation to predict response to rTMS. Further research must be done on this relatively new and potentially beneficial treatment with regard to psychomotor retardation in depression.

9. Conclusion

The study of psychomotor retardation has significantly broadened the understanding and recognition of this unique symptom far beyond that of Darwin and Kraepelin. Motor symptoms, such as moving and speaking slowly, can now be analyzed quantitatively and studied in further depth. Emerging neuropsychiatric tools may further the understanding of psychomotor aspects of depression (Bajbouj et al., 2006; Loo et al., 2008; Steele et al., 2000). In addition, psychometric assessment scales exist that allow for the systematic evaluation of psychomotor retardation beyond that of noting the presence of marked psychomotor slowing (Bonin-Guillaume et al., 2008; Dantchev and Widlocher, 1998; Parker, 2005; Sobin et al., 1998).

Investigations into psychomotor retardation in the context of diseases co-morbid with MDD could bring increased understanding to its biological underpinnings and lead to better diagnosis. Psychomotor retardation is a symptom of MDD that can be affected by and overlap with symptoms of other chronic illnesses (Demet et al., 2002; Simon and Von Korff, 2006; Sobel et al., 2005; Starkstein et al., 2008). For example, the Sequenced Treatment Alternatives to Relieve Depression (STAR*D) trial found that patients with both MDD and diabetes mellitus or both MDD and premenstrual exacerbation (PME) had a higher incidence of psychomotor retardation than patients with only MDD (P=0.02, P=0.009) (Bryan et al., 2008; Kornstein et al., 2005). Patients with bipolar depression also have a high incidence of psychomotor retardation, similar to that of patients with MDD (Mitchell and Malhi, 2004; Sobin and Sackeim, 1997). One recent study, which used the Mood Spectrum Self-Report Questionnaire (MOODS-SR), found that patients with high lifetime psychomotor retardation factor (LPR) scores were more likely to have a longer duration of illness, suicide attempts, an earlier age of onset, more depressive episodes, and higher indicators of bipolarity. Therefore it was concluded that lifetime psychomotor retardation is associated with both severity of depression and indicators for mania and bipolar disorder (Calugi et al., 2011). This overlap of symptomatology poses challenges in diagnosis (Simon and Von Korff, 2006; Starkstein et al., 2008). However, continued research on patients with psychomotor retardation with both MDD and another illness, such as Parkinson’s disease, may assist in understanding this complex symptom (Winograd-Gurvich et al., 2006).

The relation to depression severity and to melancholia remains in question (Benazzi, 2002; Carlson and Kashani, 1988; Parker, 2005; Parker et al., 2010; Shah et al., 1997; Smith et al., 1995) and there are varying results regarding the predictive ability of psychomotor retardation for the efficacy of psychotropic medication and ECT (Gill and Lambourn, 1979; Mallinckrodt et al., 2007). Future investigations of psychomotor retardation could produce numerous benefits such as further insights regarding the biology of mood disorders and enhanced treatment planning for patients with psychomotor retardation.

Acknowledgments

This publication was supported by Grant Number R25 MH74675 (PI: Graham Emslie) and K23 MH087739 (PI: Shawn McClintock) from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), and Grant Number UL1 RR024982 (PI: Milton Packer) from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), a component of the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

Abbreviations

99mTc-HMPAO

technetium-99m hexamethylpropylene amine oxime

AVP

arginine vasopressin

CANTAB

Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery

COVA

Posner’s covert orientation of visual attention test

CSP

cortical silent period

DST

dexamethasone suppression tests

DMS

delayed matched to sample

DRRS

Depressive Retardation Rating Scale

DSM IV-TR

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Fourth Edition-Text Revision

DSST

symbol substitution test

ECT

electroconvulsive therapy

EEG

electroencephalography

EMG

electromyography

EOG

electro-oculogram

ERP

event related potential

GSM

Gibson Spiral Maze

HDRS

Hamilton Depression Rating Scale

HPA

hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal

IBZM

iodobenzamide

ICF

intracortical facilitation

ICI

intracortical inhibition

IMPS

In-patient Multidimensional Psychiatric Scale

LPR

lifetime psychomotor retardation

MADRS

Montgomery–Asberg Depression Rating Scale

MAOI

monoamine oxidase inhibitor

MARS

Motor Agitation and Retardation Scale

mCPP

meta-chlorophenylpiperazine

MDD

major depressive disorder

MEP

motor evoked potential

MHPG

3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol

MOODS-SR

Mood Spectrum Self-Report Questionnaire

MT

motor threshold

NDRI

norepinephrine and dopamine reuptake inhibitors

PALT

paired associate learning subtest from the Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale

PME

premenstrual exacerbation

RRS-4

Short Version of Retardation Rating Scale for Elderly Patients

SCOLP

Speed and Capacity of Language Processing Test

SNRI

serotonin–norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor

SPECT

single photon emission computed tomography

SRRS

Salpetriere Retardation Rating Scale

SSRI

selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor

STAR*D

Sequenced Treatment Alternatives to Relieve Depression

TCA

tricyclic

TeCA

tetracyclic

TMS

transcranial magnetic stimulation

TMT

trail making test

Contributor Information

Jeylan S. Buyukdura, Email: jeylan.buyukdura@utsouthwestern.edu.

Shawn M. McClintock, Email: shawn.mcclintock@utsouthwestern.edu.

Paul E. Croarkin, Email: paul.croarkin@utsouthwestern.edu.

References

  1. Aman MG, Turbott SH. Prediction of clinical response in children taking methylphenidate. J Autism Dev Disord. 1991;21(2):211–28. doi: 10.1007/BF02284761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amsterdam JD. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor efficacy in severe and melancholic depression. J Psychopharmacol. 1998;12(3 Suppl B):S99–S111. doi: 10.1177/0269881198012003061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Antoni FA. Vasopressinergic control of pituitary adrenocorticotropin secretion comes of age. Front Neuroendocrinol. 1993;14(2):76–122. doi: 10.1006/frne.1993.1004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Austin MP, Mitchell P, Hadzi-Pavlovic D, Hickie I, Parker G, Chan J, et al. Effect of apomorphine on motor and cognitive function in melancholic patients: a preliminary report. Psychiatry Res. 2000;97(2–3):207–15. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(00)00222-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Avery D, Silverman J. Psychomotor retardation and agitation in depression. Relationship to age, sex, and response to treatment. J Affect Disord. 1984;7(1):67–76. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(84)90066-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baeken C, De Raedt R, Santermans L, Zeeuws D, Vanderhasselt MA, Meers M, et al. HF-rTMS treatment decreases psychomotor retardation in medication-resistant melancholic depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2010;34 (4):684–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.03.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bajbouj M, Lisanby SH, Lang UE, Danker-Hopfe H, Heuser I, Neu P. Evidence for impaired cortical inhibition in patients with unipolar major depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59(5):395–400. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.07.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bange F, Bathien N. Visual cognitive dysfunction in depression: an event-related potential study. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1998;108(5):472–81. doi: 10.1016/s0168-5597(98)00024-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Benazzi F. Psychomotor changes in melancholic and atypical depression: unipolar and bipolar-II subtypes. Psychiatry Res. 2002;112(3):211–20. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(02)00241-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bench CJ, Friston KJ, Brown RG, Frackowiak RS, Dolan RJ. Regional cerebral blood flow in depression measured by positron emission tomography: the relationship with clinical dimensions. Psychol Med. 1993;23(3):579–90. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700025368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bezzi G, Pinelli P, Tosca P. Motor reactivity, pain threshold and effects of sleep deprivation in unipolar depressives. Psychiatr Clin (Basel) 1981;14(3):150–60. doi: 10.1159/000283911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Blackburn IM. Mental and psychomotor speed in depression and mania. Br J Psychiatry. 1975;126:329–35. doi: 10.1192/bjp.126.4.329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Blewett AE. Abnormal subjective time experience in depression. Br J Psychiatry. 1992;161:195–200. doi: 10.1192/bjp.161.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bonin-Guillaume S, Jouve E, Sautel L, Fakra E, Blin O. RRS-4: short version of the retardation rating scale to screen for depression in elderly inpatients. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;16(4):331–5. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e318165dbe0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Brebion G, Smith MJ, Allilaire JF. Psychometric characteristics of ideational retardation in depressives. Br J Clin Psychol. 1995;34(Pt 3):371–81. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1995.tb01472.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brody AL, Barsom MW, Bota RG, Saxena S. Prefrontal-subcortical and limbic circuit mediation of major depressive disorder. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry. 2001;6(2):102–12. doi: 10.1053/scnp.2001.21837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brown WA. Treatment response in melancholia. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 2007;433:125–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.00970.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bryan CJ, Songer TJ, Brooks MM, Thase ME, Gaynes BN, Klinkman M, et al. A comparison of baseline sociodemographic and clinical characteristics between major depressive disorder patients with and without diabetes: a STAR*D report. J Affect Disord. 2008;108(1–2):113–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Buchan H, Johnstone E, McPherson K, Palmer RL, Crow TJ, Brandon S. Who benefits from electroconvulsive therapy? Combined results of the Leicester and Northwick Park trials. Br J Psychiatry. 1992;160:355–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.160.3.355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Burns RA, Lock T, Edwards DR, Katona CL, Harrison DA, Robertson MM, et al. Predictors of response to amine-specific antidepressants. J Affect Disord. 1995;35(3):97–106. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(95)00039-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Caligiuri MP, Gentili V, Eberson S, Kelsoe J, Rapaport M, Gillin JC. A quantitative neuromotor predictor of antidepressant non-response in patients with major depression. J Affect Disord. 2003;77(2):135–41. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(02)00107-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Calugi S, Cassano GB, Litta A, Rucci P, Benvenuti A, Miniati M, et al. Does psychomotor retardation define a clinically relevant phenotype of unipolar depression? J Affect Disord. 2011;129(1–3):296–300. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Canales JJ, Graybiel AM. A measure of striatal function predicts motor stereotypy. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3(4):377–83. doi: 10.1038/73949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Carlson GA, Kashani JH. Phenomenology of major depression from childhood through adulthood: analysis of three studies. Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145(10):1222–5. doi: 10.1176/ajp.145.10.1222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Carney MW, Roth M, Garside RF. The diagnosis of depressive syndromes and the prediction of E.C.T. response. Br J Psychiatry. 1965;111:659–74. doi: 10.1192/bjp.111.477.659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Daniels J. Catatonia: clinical aspects and neurobiological correlates. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2009;21(4):371–80. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2009.21.4.371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Danish University Antidepressant Group. Citalopram: clinical effect profile in comparison with clomipramine. A controlled multicenter study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1986;90(1):131–8. doi: 10.1007/BF00172884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Danish University Antidepressant Group. Paroxetine: a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor showing better tolerance, but weaker antidepressant effect than clomipramine in a controlled multicenter study. J Affect Disord. 1990;18(4):289–99. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(90)90081-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Danish University Antidepressant Group. Moclobemide: a reversible MAO-A-inhibitor showing weaker antidepressant effect than clomipramine in a controlled multicenter study. J Affect Disord. 1993;28(2):105–16. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(93)90039-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dantchev N, Widlocher DJ. The measurement of retardation in depression. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59(Suppl 14):19–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. de Winter RF, van Hemert AM, DeRijk RH, Zwinderman KH, Frankhuijzen-Sierevogel AC, Wiegant VM, et al. Anxious-retarded depression: relation with plasma vasopressin and cortisol. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28(1):140–7. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Del Zompo M, Bernardi F, Burrai C, Bocchetta A. A double-blind study of minaprine versus amitriptyline in major depression. Neuropsychobiology. 1990;24(2):79–83. doi: 10.1159/000119465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Demet MM, Ozmen B, Deveci A, Boyvada S, Adiguzel H, Aydemir O. Depression and anxiety in hyperthyroidism. Arch Med Res. 2002;33(6):552–6. doi: 10.1016/s0188-4409(02)00410-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. Washington, DC: Author: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. text revision. [Google Scholar]
  35. Fantino B, Moore N. The self-reported Montgomery–Asberg Depression Rating Scale is a useful evaluative tool in major depressive disorder. BMC Psychiatry. 2009;9:26. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-9-26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Fink M, Rush AJ, Knapp R, Rasmussen K, Mueller M, Rummans TA, et al. DSM melancholic features are unreliable predictors of ECT response: a CORE publication. J ECT. 2007;23(3):139–46. doi: 10.1097/yct.0b013e3180337344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fitzgerald PB, Brown TL, Marston NA, Daskalakis ZJ, de Castella A, Bradshaw JL, et al. Motor cortical excitability and clinical response to rTMS in depression. J Affect Disord. 2004;82(1):71–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2003.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Flament MF, Lane RM, Zhu R, Ying Z. Predictors of an acute antidepressant response to fluoxetine and sertraline. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 1999;14(5):259–75. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Foster FG, Kupfer DJ. Psychomotor activity as a correlate of depression and sleep in acutely disturbed psychiatric inpatients. Am J Psychiatry. 1975;132(9):928–31. doi: 10.1176/ajp.132.9.928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gill D, Lambourn J. Indications for electric convulsion therapy and its use by senior psychiatrists. Br Med J. 1979;1(6172):1169–71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.1.6172.1169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gorlyn M, Keilp JG, Grunebaum MF, Taylor BP, Oquendo MA, Bruder GE, et al. Neuropsychological characteristics as predictors of SSRI treatment response in depressed subjects. J Neural Transm. 2008;115(8):1213–9. doi: 10.1007/s00702-008-0084-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Graveland GA, Williams RS, DiFiglia M. Evidence for degenerative and regenerative changes in neostriatal spiny neurons in Huntington’s disease. Science. 1985;227 (4688):770–3. doi: 10.1126/science.3155875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Greden JF. Psychomotor monitoring: a promise being fulfilled? J Psychiatr Res. 1993;27 (3):285–7. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(93)90039-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Greden JF, Carroll BJ. Psychomotor function in affective disorders: an overview of new monitoring techniques. Am J Psychiatry. 1981;138(11):1441–8. doi: 10.1176/ajp.138.11.1441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Greden JF, Albala AA, Smokler IA, Gardner R, Carroll BJ. Speech pause time: a marker of psychomotor retardation among endogenous depressives. Biol Psychiatry. 1981;16 (9):851–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Guiard BP, El Mansari M, Blier P. Prospect of a dopamine contribution in the next generation of antidepressant drugs: the triple reuptake inhibitors. Curr Drug Targets. 2009;10(11):1069–s84. doi: 10.2174/138945009789735156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Gupta RK. Major depression: an illness with objective physical signs. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2009:1–6. doi: 10.1080/15622970902812072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hardy P, Jouvent R, Widlocher D. Speech pause time and the retardation rating scale for depression (ERD). Towards a reciprocal validation. J Affect Disord. 1984;6(1):123–7. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(84)90014-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hegerl U, Gallinat J, Juckel G. Event-related potentials. Do they reflect central serotonergic neurotransmission and do they predict clinical response to serotonin agonists? J Affect Disord. 2001;62(1–2):93–100. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00353-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Heiligenstein JH, Tollefson GD, Faries DE. Response patterns of depressed outpatients with and without melancholia: a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of fluoxetine versus placebo. J Affect Disord. 1994;30(3):163–73. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(94)90077-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Herrera-Guzman I, Gudayol-Ferre E, Lira-Mandujano J, Herrera-Abarca J, Herrera-Guzman D, Montoya-Perez K, et al. Cognitive predictors of treatment response to bupropion and cognitive effects of bupropion in patients with major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2008;160(1):72–82. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Herrero MT, Barcia C, Navarro JM. Functional anatomy of thalamus and basal ganglia. Childs Nerv Syst. 2002;18(8):386–404. doi: 10.1007/s00381-002-0604-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hickie I, Parsonage B, Parker G. Prediction of response to electroconvulsive therapy. Preliminary validation of a sign-based typology of depression. Br J Psychiatry. 1990;157:65–71. doi: 10.1192/bjp.157.1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hickie I, Scott E, Mitchell P, Wilhelm K, Austin MP, Bennett B. Subcortical hyperintensities on magnetic resonance imaging: clinical correlates and prognostic significance in patients with severe depression. Biol Psychiatry. 1995;37(3):151–60. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)00174-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hickie I, Mason C, Parker G. Comparative validity of two measures of psychomotor function in patients with severe depression. J Affect Disord. 1996a;37(2–3):143–9. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(95)00087-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Hickie I, Mason C, Parker G, Brodaty H. Prediction of ECT response: validation of a refined sign-based (CORE) system for defining melancholia. Br J Psychiatry. 1996b;169(1):68–74. doi: 10.1192/bjp.169.1.68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Hickie I, Ward P, Scott E, Haindl W, Walker B, Dixon J, et al. Neostriatal rCBF correlates of psychomotor slowing in patients with major depression. Psychiatry Res. 1999;92 (2–3):75–81. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(99)00038-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Higuchi H, Sato K, Yoshida K, Takahashi H, Kamata M, Otani K, et al. Predictors of antidepressant response to fluvoxamine obtained using the three-factor structures of the Montgomery and Asberg Depression Rating Scale for major depressive disorders in Japanese patients. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2008a;62(3):301–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2008.01797.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Higuchi H, Sato K, Yoshida K, Takahashi H, Kamata M, Otani K, et al. No predictors of antidepressant patient response to milnacipran were obtained using the three-factor structures of the Montgomery and Asberg Depression Rating Scale in Japanese patients with major depressive disorders. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2008b;62(2):197–202. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2008.01755.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Hobson RF. Prognostic factors in electric convulsive therapy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1953;16(4):275–81. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.16.4.275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Hoeppner J, Padberg F, Domes G, Zinke A, Herpertz SC, Grossheinrich N, et al. Influence of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on psychomotor symptoms in major depression. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2010;260(3):197–202. doi: 10.1007/s00406-009-0039-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Hoffmann GM, Gonze JC, Mendlewicz J. Speech pause time as a method for the evaluation of psychomotor retardation in depressive illness. Br J Psychiatry. 1985;146:535–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.146.5.535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Hoppner J, Schulz M, Irmisch G, Mau R, Schlafke D, Richter J. Antidepressant efficacy of two different rTMS procedures. High frequency over left versus low frequency over right prefrontal cortex compared with sham stimulation. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2003;253(2):103–9. doi: 10.1007/s00406-003-0416-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Hordern A, Holt NF, Burt CG, Gordon WF. Amitriptyline in depressive states: phenomenology and prognostic considerations. Br J Psychiatry. 1963;109:815–25. doi: 10.1192/bjp.109.463.815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Hordern A, Burt CG, Gordon WF, Holt NF. Amitriptyline in depressive states: six-month treatment results. Br J Psychiatry. 1964;110:641–7. doi: 10.1192/bjp.110.468.641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Iverson GL. Objective assessment of psychomotor retardation in primary care patients with depression. J Behav Med. 2004;27(1):31–7. doi: 10.1023/b:jobm.0000013642.43978.f9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Joffe RT, Uhde TW, Post RM, Minichiello MD. Motor activity in depressed patients treated with carbamazepine. Biol Psychiatry. 1987;22(8):941–6. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(87)90003-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Jones IH, Pansa M. Some nonverbal aspects of depression and schizophrenia occurring during the interview. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1979;167(7):402–9. doi: 10.1097/00005053-197907000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Joyce PR, Mulder RT, Luty SE, McKenzie JM, Sullivan PF, Abbott RM, et al. Melancholia: definitions, risk factors, personality, neuroendocrine markers and differential antidepressant response. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2002;36(3):376–83. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2001.01025.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Kapur S, Mann JJ. Role of the dopaminergic system in depression. Biol Psychiatry. 1992;32(1):1–17. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(92)90137-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Kemp AH, Gordon E, Rush AJ, Williams LM. Improving the prediction of treatment response in depression: integration of clinical, cognitive, psychophysiological, neuroimaging, and genetic measures. CNS Spectr. 2008;13(12):1066–86. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900017120. quiz 87–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Klein HE, Bender W, Mayr H, Niederschweiberer A, Schmauss M. The DST and its relationship to psychiatric diagnosis, symptoms and treatment outcome. Br J Psychiatry. 1984;145:591–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.145.6.591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Knott VJ, Lapierre YD. Electrophysiological and behavioral correlates of psychomotor responsivity in depression. Biol Psychiatry. 1987;22(3):313–24. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(87)90149-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Kornstein SG, Harvey AT, Rush AJ, Wisniewski SR, Trivedi MH, Svikis DS, et al. Self-reported premenstrual exacerbation of depressive symptoms in patients seeking treatment for major depression. Psychol Med. 2005;35(5):683–92. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704004106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Laakmann G, Blaschke D, Engel R, Schwarz A. Fluoxetine vs amitriptyline in the treatment of depressed out-patients. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 1988;3:64–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Lemke MR, Puhl P, Koethe N, Winkler T. Psychomotor retardation and anhedonia in depression. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1999;99(4):252–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb07221.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Litvan I, Dickson DW, Buttner-Ennever JA, Delacourte A, Hutton M, Dubois B, et al. Research goals in progressive supranuclear palsy. First International Brainstorming Conference on PSP. Mov Disord. 2000;15(3):446–58. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Loo CK, Sachdev P, Mitchell PB, Gandevia SC, Malhi GS, Todd G, et al. A study using transcranial magnetic stimulation to investigate motor mechanisms in psychomotor retardation in depression. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;11(7):935–46. doi: 10.1017/S1461145708008821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Lorr M, McNair DM, Klett CJ, Lasky JJ. Inpatient Multidimensionul Psychiuiric Scale. Consulting Psychologists Press; 1966. pp. 549–58. [Google Scholar]
  80. Mallinckrodt CH, Watkin JG, Liu C, Wohlreich MM, Raskin J. Duloxetine in the treatment of major depressive disorder: a comparison of efficacy in patients with and without melancholic features. BMC Psychiatry. 2005;5:1. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-5-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Mallinckrodt CH, Prakash A, Houston JP, Swindle R, Detke MJ, Fava M. Differential antidepressant symptom efficacy: placebo-controlled comparisons of duloxetine and SSRIs (fluoxetine, paroxetine, escitalopram) Neuropsychobiology. 2007;56(2–3):73–85. doi: 10.1159/000111537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Martinot M, Bragulat V, Artiges E, Dolle F, Hinnen F, Jouvent R, et al. Decreased presynaptic dopamine function in the left caudate of depressed patients with affective flattening and psychomotor retardation. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(2):314–6. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.2.314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Mayberg HS, Starkstein SE, Peyser CE, Brandt J, Dannals RF, Folstein SE. Paralimbic frontal lobe hypometabolism in depression associated with Huntington’s disease. Neurology. 1992;42(9):1791–7. doi: 10.1212/wnl.42.9.1791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Mayberg HS, Lewis PJ, Regenold W, Wagner HN., Jr Paralimbic hypoperfusion in unipolar depression. J Nucl Med. 1994;35(6):929–34. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. McGrath PJ, Khan AY, Trivedi MH, Stewart JW, Morris DW, Wisniewski SR, et al. Response to a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (citalopram) in major depressive disorder with melancholic features: a STAR*D report. J Clin Psychiatry. 2008;69(12):1847–55. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v69n1201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Mendels J. Electroconvulsive therapy and depression. 3. A method for prognosis. Br J Psychiatry. 1965a;111:687–90. doi: 10.1192/bjp.111.477.687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Mendels J. Electroconvulsive therapy and depression. II. Significance of endogenous and reactive syndromes. Br J Psychiatry. 1965b;111:682–6. doi: 10.1192/bjp.111.477.682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Mendels J. Electroconvulsive therapy and depression. I. The prognostic significance of clinical factors. Br J Psychiatry. 1965c;111:675–81. doi: 10.1192/bjp.111.477.675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Minassian A, Henry BL, Geyer MA, Paulus MP, Young JW, Perry W. The quantitative assessment of motor activity in mania and schizophrenia. J Affect Disord. 2010;120 (1–3):200–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.04.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Misdraji EL, Gass CS. The trail making test and its neurobehavioral components. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2009:1–6. [Google Scholar]
  91. Mitchell PB. The new antidepressants — are they worth the cost? Aust Prescriber. 1995;18:82–4. [Google Scholar]
  92. Mitchell PB. Managing depression in a community setting. Med J Aust. 1997;167(7):383–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Mitchell PB, Malhi GS. Bipolar depression: phenomenological overview and clinical characteristics. Bipolar Disord. 2004;6(6):530–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2004.00137.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Mitchell P, Hadzi-Pavlovic D, Parker G, Hickie I, Wilhelm K, Brodaty H, et al. Depressive psychomotor disturbance, cortisol, and dexamethasone. Biol Psychiatry. 1996;40 (10):941–50. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00635-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Moffoot AP, O’Carroll RE, Bennie J, Carroll S, Dick H, Ebmeier KP, et al. Diurnal variation of mood and neuropsychological function in major depression with melancholia. J Affect Disord. 1994;32(4):257–69. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(94)90090-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Morgan M, Phillips JG, Bradshaw JL, Mattingley JB, Iansek R, Bradshaw JA. Age-related motor slowness: simply strategic? J Gerontol. 1994;49(3):M133–9. doi: 10.1093/geronj/49.3.m133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Mulder RT, Joyce PR, Frampton CM, Luty SE, Sullivan PF. Six months of treatment for depression: outcome and predictors of the course of illness. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(1):95–100. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.1.95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Naismith S, Hickie I, Ward PB, Turner K, Scott E, Little C, et al. Caudate nucleus volumes and genetic determinants of homocysteine metabolism in the prediction of psychomotor speed in older persons with depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159 (12):2096–8. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Narita H, Odawara T, Iseki E, Kosaka K, Hirayasu Y. Psychomotor retardation correlates with frontal hypoperfusion and the Modified Stroop Test in patients under 60-years-old with major depression. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2004;58(4):389–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2004.01273.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Neu P, Kiesslinger U, Schlattmann P, Reischies FM. Time-related cognitive deficiency in four different types of depression. Psychiatry Res. 2001;103(2–3):237–47. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(01)00286-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Niculescu AB, III, Akiskal HS. Proposed endophenotypes of dysthymia: evolutionary, clinical and pharmacogenomic considerations. Mol Psychiatry. 2001;6(4):363–6. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Parker G. Melancholia. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(6):1066. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Parker G, Hadzi-Pavlovic D. Melancholia: a disorder of movement and mood: a phenomenological and neurobiological review. Cambridge; New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  104. Parker G, Fink M, Shorter E, Taylor MA, Akiskal H, Berrios G, et al. Issues for DSM-5: whither melancholia? The case for its classification as a distinct mood disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(7):745–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09101525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Petrides G, Fink M, Husain MM, Knapp RG, Rush AJ, Mueller M, et al. ECT remission rates in psychotic versus nonpsychotic depressed patients: a report from CORE. J ECT. 2001;17(4):244–53. doi: 10.1097/00124509-200112000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Pier MP, Hulstijn W, Sabbe BG. No psychomotor slowing in fine motor tasks in dysthymia. J Affect Disord. 2004;83(2–3):109–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2004.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Pope B, Blass T, Siegman AW, Raher J. Anxiety and depression in speech. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1970;35(1):128–33. doi: 10.1037/h0029659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Ranelli CJ, Miller RE. Behavioral predictors of amitriptyline response in depression. Am J Psychiatry. 1981;138(1):30–4. doi: 10.1176/ajp.138.1.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Raoux N, Benoit O, Dantchev N, Denise P, Franc B, Allilaire JF, et al. Circadian pattern of motor activity in major depressed patients undergoing antidepressant therapy: relationship between actigraphic measures and clinical course. Psychiatry Res. 1994;52(1):85–98. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(94)90122-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Rasmussen KG. Clinical applications of recent research on electroconvulsive therapy. Bull Menninger Clin. 2003;67(1):18–31. doi: 10.1521/bumc.67.1.18.23449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Rasmussen-Torvik LJ, McAlpine DD. Genetic screening for SSRI drug response among those with major depression: great promise and unseen perils. Depress Anxiety. 2007;24(5):350–7. doi: 10.1002/da.20251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Roose SP, Glassman AH, Attia E, Woodring S. Comparative efficacy of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and tricyclics in the treatment of melancholia. Am J Psychiatry. 1994;151(12):1735–9. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.12.1735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Ruohonen J, Karhu J. Navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurophysiol Clin. 2010;40(1):7–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2010.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Rush AJ, Weissenburger JE. Melancholic symptom features and DSM-IV. Am J Psychiatry. 1994;151(4):489–98. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.4.489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Sabbe B, van Hoof J, Hulstijn W, Zitman F. Depressive retardation and treatment with fluoxetine: assessment of the motor component. J Affect Disord. 1997;43(1):53–61. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(96)01409-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Sabbe B, Hulstijn W, van Hoof J, Tuynman-Qua HG, Zitman F. Retardation in depression: assessment by means of simple motor tasks. J Affect Disord. 1999;55(1):39–44. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(98)00087-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Sabbe B, Hulstijn W, Maes M, Pier M, Scharpe S, Zitman F. Psychomotor slowing, neuroendocrine responses, and behavioral changes after oral administration of meta-chlorophenylpiperazine in normal volunteers. Psychiatry Res. 2001;105(3):151–63. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(01)00326-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Sahakian BJ, Owen AM. Computerized assessment in neuropsychiatry using CANTAB: discussion paper. J R Soc Med. 1992;85(7):399–402. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Samson JA, Mirin SM, Griffin M, Borrelli D, Schildkraut JJ. Urinary MHPG and clinical symptoms in patients with unipolar depression. Psychiatry Res. 1994;51(2):157–65. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(94)90035-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Saxton JA, Ratcliff G, Dodge H, Pandav R, Baddeley A, Ganguli M. Speed and capacity of language processing test: normative data from an older American community-dwelling sample. Appl Neuropsychol. 2001;8(4):193–203. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN0804_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Schmid-Priscoveanu A, Allum JH. Infrared and video oculography — alternatives to electrooculography? HNO. 1999;47(5):472–8. doi: 10.1007/s001060050407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Schonfeldt-Lecuona C, Cardenas-Morales L, Freudenmann RW, Kammer T, Herwig U. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in depression — lessons from the multicentre trials. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2010;28(4):569–76. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2010-0561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Schrijvers D, de Bruijn ER, Maas Y, De Grave C, Sabbe BG, Hulstijn W. Action monitoring in major depressive disorder with psychomotor retardation. Cortex. 2008a;44(5):569–79. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Schrijvers D, Hulstijn W, Sabbe BG. Psychomotor symptoms in depression: a diagnostic, pathophysiological and therapeutic tool. J Affect Disord. 2008b;109(1–2):1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Schrijvers D, Maas YJ, Pier MP, Madani Y, Hulstijn W, Sabbe BG. Psychomotor changes in major depressive disorder during sertraline treatment. Neuropsychobiology. 2009;59(1):34–42. doi: 10.1159/000205516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Schwartz GE, Fair PL, Salt P, Mandel MR, Klerman GL. Facial expression and imagery in depression: an electromyographic study. Psychosom Med. 1976;38(5):337–47. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197609000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Shah PJ, Ogilvie AD, Goodwin GM, Ebmeier KP. Clinical and psychometric correlates of dopamine D2 binding in depression. Psychol Med. 1997;27(6):1247–56. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797005382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Simon GE, Von Korff M. Medical co-morbidity and validity of DSM-IV depression criteria. Psychol Med. 2006;36(1):27–36. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Smith J, Carr V, Morris H, Gilliland J. The dexamethasone suppression test in relation to symptomatology: preliminary findings controlling for serum dexamethasone concentrations. Psychiatry Res. 1988;25(2):123–33. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(88)90043-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Smith MJ, Brebion G, Banquet JP, Cohen L. Retardation of mentation in depressives: Posner’s covert orientation of visual attention test. J Affect Disord. 1995;35(3):107–15. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(95)00044-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Snaith RP. Hamilton rating scale for depression. Br J Psychiatry. 1977;131:431–2. doi: 10.1192/bjp.131.4.431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Sobel RM, Lotkowski S, Mandel S. Update on depression in neurologic illness: stroke, epilepsy, and multiple sclerosis. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2005;7(5):396–403. doi: 10.1007/s11920-005-0043-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Sobin C, Sackeim HA. Psychomotor symptoms of depression. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154 (1):4–17. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.1.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Sobin C, Mayer L, Endicott J. The motor agitation and retardation scale: a scale for the assessment of motor abnormalities in depressed patients. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998;10(1):85–92. doi: 10.1176/jnp.10.1.85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Starkstein SE, Merello M, Jorge R, Brockman S, Bruce D, Petracca G, et al. A validation study of depressive syndromes in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2008;23(4):538–46. doi: 10.1002/mds.21866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Steele JD, Glabus MF, Shajahan PM, Ebmeier KP. Increased cortical inhibition in depression: a prolonged silent period with transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) Psychol Med. 2000;30(3):565–70. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799002032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Steffens DC, Krishnan KR. Structural neuroimaging and mood disorders: recent findings, implications for classification, and future directions. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43(10):705–12. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00084-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Strian F, Albert W, Klicpera C. Course of depressive mood and psychomotor activation in endogenous depression. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr. 1979;227(3):193–206. doi: 10.1007/BF00367390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Szabadi E, Bradshaw CM, Besson JA. Elongation of pause-time in speech: a simple, objective measure of motor retardation in depression. Br J Psychiatry. 1976;129:592–7. doi: 10.1192/bjp.129.6.592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Taylor BP, Bruder GE, Stewart JW, McGrath PJ, Halperin J, Ehrlichman H, et al. Psychomotor slowing as a predictor of fluoxetine nonresponse in depressed outpatients. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(1):73–8. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.1.73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Thase ME, Trivedi MH, Rush AJ. MAOIs in the contemporary treatment of depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1995;12(3):185–219. doi: 10.1016/0893-133X(94)00058-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. The Northwick Park ECT trial. Predictors of response to real and simulated ECT, Clinical Research Centre, Division of Psychiatry. Br J Psychiatry. 1984;144:227–37. doi: 10.1192/bjp.144.3.227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Trivedi MH, Rush AJ, Wisniewski SR, Nierenberg AA, Warden D, Ritz L, et al. Evaluation of outcomes with citalopram for depression using measurement-based care in STAR*D: implications for clinical practice. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(1):28–40. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.1.28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. van Hoof JJ, Hulstijn W, van Mier H, Pagen M. Figure drawing and psychomotor retardation: preliminary report. J Affect Disord. 1993;29(4):263–6. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(93)90016-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. van Londen L, Goekoop JG, van Kempen GM, Frankhuijzen-Sierevogel AC, Wiegant VM, van der Velde EA, et al. Plasma levels of arginine vasopressin elevated in patients with major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997;17(4):284–92. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00054-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. van Londen L, Kerkhof GA, van den Berg F, Goekoop JG, Zwinderman KH, Frankhuijzen-Sierevogel AC, et al. Plasma arginine vasopressin and motor activity in major depression. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43(3):196–204. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(97)80433-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Vidailhet M, Bonnet AM, Marconi R, Gouider-Khouja N, Agid Y. Do parkinsonian symptoms and levodopa-induced dyskinesias start in the foot? Neurology. 1994;44 (9):1613–6. doi: 10.1212/wnl.44.9.1613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Videbech P, Ravnkilde B, Pedersen TH, Hartvig H, Egander A, Clemmensen K, et al. The Danish PET/depression project: clinical symptoms and cerebral blood flow. A regions-of-interest analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2002;106(1):35–44. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2002.02245.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Weller EB, Weller RA. Treatment options in the management of adolescent depression. J Affect Disord. 2000;61(Suppl 1):23–8. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00286-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. White K, White J. Tranylcypromine: patterns and predictors of response. J Clin Psychiatry. 1986;47(7):380–2. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Whitwell J. Historical notes on psychiatry. London: Lewis; 1936. [Google Scholar]
  152. Widlocher DJ. Psychomotor retardation: clinical, theoretical, and psychometric aspects. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1983;6(1):27–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Winograd-Gurvich C, Fitzgerald PB, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Bradshaw JL, White OB. Negative symptoms: a review of schizophrenia, melancholic depression and Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res Bull. 2006;70(4–6):312–21. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Yoshimura R, Nakamura J, Shinkai K, Ueda N. Clinical response to antidepressant treatment and 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol levels: mini review. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2004;28(4):611–6. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2004.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Zarate CA, Kando JC, Tohen M, Weiss MK, Cole JO. Does intolerance or lack of response with fluoxetine predict the same will happen with sertraline? J Clin Psychiatry. 1996;57(2):67–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Zilboorg G. A history of medical psychology. New York: Norton; 1944. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES