Abstract
Altered functional characteristics have been reported in amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD); nonetheless, comprehensive analyses of the resting-state networks (RSNs) are rare. This study combined multiple imaging modalities to investigate the functional and structural changes within each RSN and between RSNs in aMCI/AD patients. Eight RSNs were identified from functional MRI data from 35 AD, 18 aMCI and 21 normal control subjects using independent component analysis. We compared functional connectivity (FC) within each RSN and found decreased FC in the several cognitive-related RSNs in AD, including the bilateral precuneus of the precuneus network, the posterior cingulate cortex and left precuneus of the posterior default mode network (DMN), and the left superior parietal lobule of the left frontoparietal network (LFP). We further compared the grey matter volumes and amplitudes of low-frequency fluctuations of these regions and found decreases in these measures in AD. Importantly, we found decreased inter-network connectivity between the visual network and the LFP and between the anterior and posterior DMNs in AD. All indices in aMCI patients were numerically between those of controls and AD patients. These results suggest that the brain networks supporting complex cognitive processes are specifically and progressively impaired over the course of AD, and the FC impairments are present not only within networks but also between networks.
Introduction
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common type of dementia in the elderly population and is clinically characterized by an early impairment of memory function, followed by a slow progression of additional cognitive deficits that ultimately develop into overt dementia. AD is a genetically complex and irreversible neurodegenerative disease of the central nervous system with an insidious onset, but its pathogenesis is poorly understood, and effective therapies remain elusive. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI), especially amnestic MCI (aMCI), is considered an intermediary state between normal cognition and AD [1], [2]. Much evidence has shown that AD is associated with cortical atrophy and a disruption of metabolism and function [3], [4]. Neuropsychological studies have shown that patients with AD exhibit impairments in multiple cognitive domains, such as episodic memory, execution, attention, visuospatial orientation, and verbal ability [5], which suggests that AD is a disorder that causes deficits in multiple neural networks [6]. A better understanding of the neurobiology of AD requires investigations at the brain network level.
Independent component analysis (ICA) of resting-state functional MRI (fMRI) data is intrinsically a multivariate, data-driven method that extracts from the BOLD time series a number of independent resting-state networks (RSNs) (spatial components), each with its own specific time course [7], [8]. The common RSNs include the default mode network (DMN), the frontoparietal network (FPN), the central-executive network (CEN), the visual network (VN), the auditory network (AN), and the sensorimotor network (SMN) [9]–[14]. Among these RSNs, the DMN has been extensively investigated and found to be impaired in MCI/AD patients [15]–[21]. Recently, several studies have revealed that MCI or AD patients also show functional changes in other RSNs, such as the attention-related networks [22],[23], the frontal cognitive networks [24], [25], the self-referential network [26], and the motor and visual processing networks [27], [28]. Currently, we still have a limited understanding of the changes in the functional architecture of the non-DMN networks in AD/MCI patients. More importantly, whether the FCs between different RSNs are altered in MCI/AD patients remains largely unknown, although a recent region of interest (ROI)-based FC study revealed decreased network connectivity in AD [29], and a resting-state fMRI study discovered altered directional connectivity among RSNs in AD [30]. FC between different brain functional networks that can be considered a larger scale of FC is termed functional network connectivity (FNC) [31]. FNC can also be investigated with ICA because different ICA components are maximally spatially independent, but their corresponding time courses can show considerable amounts of temporal dependency. Thus, FNC analysis can be performed by analyzing the dependencies among ICA time courses.
In the present study, we performed a comprehensive analysis on both structural and functional MRI data from normal controls (NC) and patients with aMCI/AD to answer the following questions. Which RSNs are selectively impaired in AD, and are these RSNs also impaired in aMCI [25]? Do brain areas with impaired FC in a RSN in AD also exhibit alterations in grey matter volume (GMV) or regional brain activity as assessed by the amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations (ALFF)? Which functional changes are independent of the structural changes [32], [33]? And are FNCs also changed in MCI/AD patients [29], [30]?
Materials and Methods
Subjects
This study was approved by the Medical Research Ethics Committee of Xuanwu Hospital of Capital Medical University, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Eighty-seven older subjects underwent a standard dementia screening that included acquisition of a medical history, physical and neurological examinations, screening laboratory tests, extensive neuropsychological testing and brain MRI. Cognitive function was evaluated with the mini-mental state examination (MMSE), and the degree of dementia was determined by the clinical dementia rating scale (CDR). The diagnosis of AD met the NINCDS-ADRDA (National Institute of Neurological Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association) criteria for “probable AD” [34]. The diagnosis of aMCI met the Petersen criteria, which is based on cognitive impairments that predominantly affect memory in the absence of dementia or significant functional loss [35], and a clinical dementia rating (CDR) score of 0.5 [36]. All subjects satisfied the following criteria: (1) age of 50–90 years; (2) ability to cooperatively finish all tests; (3) free of definite stroke history; and (4) free of any serious medical, neurological (except for AD) or psychiatric disorders, or a history of brain injury. We excluded mixed dementia and other brain disorders based on conventional MR images. Two experienced radiologists assessed conventional MR images of each subject and excluded subjects who satisfied any of the following criteria: (1) with any brain lesions except for lacunar infarction and white matter hyperintensity; (2) with more than one lacunar infarction which is defined as a maximal lesion diameter of <1 cm; and (3) with moderate to severe white matter hyperintensity as assessed by a Fazekas scale of >2. The Fazekas scale is a widely used measure (grades from 0 to 6) to assess the severity of white matter hyperintensity [37]. These older subjects were categorized into 28 NC, 20 aMCI patients, and 39 AD patients. Thirteen additional subjects were excluded due to excessive head motion during MR scanning or poor image quality. In total, 74 subjects, including 21 NC, 18 aMCI, and 35 AD, were included in the further analyses. The demographic and neuropsychological data for the 74 subjects are shown in Table 1.
Table 1. Demographic information and clinical measures of the NC, aMCI, and AD groups.
NC | aMCI | AD | P values | |
Number of subjects | 21 | 18 | 35 | |
Gender(males/females) | 7/14 | 10/8 | 17/18 | 0.348 |
Age (years) | 65.0±8.1 | 70.2±7.9 | 65.8±8.3 | <0.001 |
Years of education | 11.0±4.4 | 9.4±4.8 | 10.6±4.2 | 0.030 |
MMSE | 28.5±1.4 | 21.9±5.0 | 10.1±6.7 | <0.001 |
CDR | 0 | 0.5 | 1.63±0.69 |
Data are shown as the means ± the standard deviations. The P values refer to analysis of variance or chi square tests. Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer’s disease; aMCI, amnestic mild cognitive impairment; CDR, Clinical Dementia Rating; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; NC, normal control.
MRI Acquisition
MR images were acquired on a 3.0 Tesla MR scanner (Magnetom Trio, Siemens, Germany). Resting-state fMRI scans were acquired with an echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence with the following scan parameters: repetition time (TR) = 2000 ms, echo time (TE) = 30 ms, flip angle (FA) = 90°, matrix = 64×64, field of view (FOV) = 220×220 mm2, slice thickness = 3 mm, and slice gap = 1 mm. Each brain volume comprised 32 axial slices, and 180 volumes were acquired. During fMRI scans, all subjects were instructed to keep their eyes closed, to stay as motionless as possible, to think of nothing in particular, and not to fall asleep. Sagittal T1-weighted MR images were acquired by a magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MP-RAGE) sequence (TR/TE = 2000/2.6 ms; FA = 9°; matrix = 256×224; FOV = 256×224 mm2; inversion time = 900 ms; slice thickness = 1 mm, no gap; 176 slices; a voxel size of 1 mm×1 mm×1 mm).
Data Preprocessing
fMRI preprocessing
The fMRI data were analyzed using the Data Processing Assistant for Resting-State fMRI (DPARSFA) [38]. The first 10 volumes from each subject were discarded to allow the signal reach equilibrium and the participants adapt to the scanning noise. The remaining 170 volumes were corrected for acquisition time delay between different slices. Then, head motion parameters were estimated, and each volume was realigned to the mean map of the whole volume to correct for geometrical displacements using a six-parameter rigid-body transformation. Five subjects were excluded from further analysis because they had maximum displacements in one or more of the orthogonal directions (x, y, z) of >3 mm or a maximum rotation (x, y, z) >3.0°. The data were spatially normalized to the standard EPI template and re-sampled to 2-mm3 voxels. The normalized data were smoothed with a 4 mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel.
Identification of RSNs
We performed ICA using the group ICA (GICA) of the fMRI toolbox (Stable and Consistent Group ICA of the fMRI Toolbox, version 1.2; http://www.nitrc.org/projects/cogicat/) that was established for the analysis of fMRI data. Recently, Zhang et al. found that in multi-stage principal component analysis (PCA) reduction, which is adopted and implemented in GIFT [39] and MELODIC [40], different subject concatenation orders (SCOs) produce variation in the GICA results. To achieve robust and accurate results, an improved algorithm, the Subject Order Independent Group ICA (SOI-GICA) [41], was implemented multiple times with randomized initial values and different subject orders. Then, the multiple results were integrated to form the final output. The toolbox supports a GICA approach that first concatenates the individual data across time and subsequently computes the subject specific components and time courses. The toolbox performed the analysis in three stages: (i) data reduction, (ii) application of the ICA algorithm, and (iii) back-reconstruction for each individual subject. In the present study, we adopted the SOI-GICA, performed GICA 100 times, and obtained 20 independent components (ICs). Eight meaningful components were identified as RSNs via visual inspection. The individual-level components were obtained from back-reconstruction and converted into z-scores, which reflect the degree to which the time series of a given voxel correlates with the mean time series of the component to which it belongs.
ALFF calculation
The ALFF was computed using the DPARSFA [38]. Because the ALFF represents the low-frequency band, linear-trend removing and temporal band-pass filtering (0.01–0.08 Hz) were performed on the time series of each voxel to reduce the effects of very-low-frequency drift and high-frequency noise [42], [43]. Then, the time series of each voxel was transformed to the frequency domain using the fast Fourier transform (parameters: taper percent = 0, length = shortest), and the power spectrum was obtained. The square root of the power spectrum was calculated at each frequency and averaged across 0.01–0.08 Hz for each voxel. This averaged square root was taken as the ALFF [44]. For standardization purposes, the ALFF of each voxel was divided by the global mean ALFF within the brain tissue mask. The standardized ALFF of each voxel should have a value of approximately 1, and this standardization procedure is analogous to that used in PET studies [45]. Finally, spatial smoothing was conducted on the standardized ALFF map of each subject with an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 4 mm full-width at half-maximum.
Structural MRI preprocessing
Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis was performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8). The structural MR images were segmented into grey matter (GM), white matter and cerebrospinal fluid [46]. Following segmentation, GM population templates were generated from the entire image dataset using diffeomorphic anatomical registration through the exponentiated Lie algebra (DARTEL) technique [47]. After an initial affine registration of the GM DARTEL template to the tissue probability map in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space (http://www.mni.mcgill.ca/), non-linear warping of GM images was performed to the DARTEL GM template in MNI space with a resolution of 1.5-mm3 (as recommended for the DARTEL procedure). The GMV of each voxel was obtained by multiplying the GM concentration map by the non-linear determinants derived from the spatial normalization step. The GMVs represent the probability that each voxel is grey matter with a correction for individual brain sizes. Finally, to compensate for residual between-subject anatomical differences, the GMV images were smoothed with a FWHM kernel of 4 mm. In effect, the analysis of modulated data tests for regional differences in the absolute volume of the brain and removes the confounding effect of variance in individual brain sizes. After spatial pre-processing, the smoothed, modulated, normalized GMV maps were used for statistical analysis.
Statistical Analysis
All statistical analysis in this study, other than those included in the MRI analysis tools, were performed using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences version 16.0 (SPSS, Chicago, Ill). Post hoc contrasts were tested with the Bonferroni correction (P<0.05) for multiple comparisons. ICA components representing RSNs were entered into a one-sample random-effect analyses in SPM8 using a family-wise error (FWE) correction (P<0.05 and T = 8) and a cluster size of >100 voxels, to create a sample-specific component map (Fig. 1). All of the following statistical analyses were performed with age, sex, and years of education as covariates of no interest.
The FNC was computed for each pair of the 8 RSNs. We first extracted the mean time series of each RSN of each subject. Then FNC between each pair of the RSNs was calculated using pair-wise correlation between the mean time series of the two RSNs [48]. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to test which pairs of inter-network FC (FNC) showed significant differences across the three groups. Multiple comparisons were corrected using the Bonferroni method (P<0.05, corrected). The uncorrected P<(corrected P = 0.05)/(number of comparisons = 28). When a significant group difference was detected, a post hoc comparison was performed to test the differences in FNCs between every two groups (P<0.05, Bonferroni correction; uncorrected P value = 0.05/3 = 0.017).
For the intra-network analyses, we first compared the intra-network FCs among the three groups in a voxel-wise manner using ANOVA with a false discovery rate (FDR) corrected threshold of P<0.05 and a cluster size of >20 voxels. To further determine whether brain areas with impaired intra-network FC in AD also exhibited alterations in GMV or regional brain activity as assessed by ALFF, brain regions whose intra-network FCs showed significant differences among the three groups were extracted as regions of interest (ROIs). After registration of the FC, ALFF, and GMV maps and ROI resampling, the ALFF and GMV of each ROI were extracted and compared among the three groups using ANOVA and post hoc analysis. To exclude the atrophy effect on the FC and ALFF comparisons, we also repeated the ROI-based analyses of FC and ALFF by controlling for the GMV of each ROI.
Finally, partial correlation analysis was performed to investigate the association between the GMV of each ROI and the MMSE scores in AD and aMCI groups with age, sex, and years of education as covariates of no interest. Similar analyses were also performed for the intra-network FC and ALFF. Moreover, the same analyses for the functional measures were repeated after treating the GMV of each ROI as an additional covariate of no interest. Partial correlation analysis was also performed for the inter-network FC, with and without treating the mean GMV of the whole brain as an additional covariate of no interest. The statistical threshold for these correlation analyses was P<0.05.
Results
Components of the RSNs
Eight RSNs were identified by the SOI-GICA technique. The RSNs were the SMN, VN, AN, precuneus (Pcu), anterior (aDMN) and posterior (pDMN) DMN, and left (LFP) and right (RFP) FPN (Fig. 1). The components and locations of each RSN were consistent with previous studies [27], [49].
Aberrant FC between RSNs
Compared with the NC group, AD patients showed significantly (P<0.05, Bonferroni correction) decreased FNCs between the aDMN and pDMN and between the VN and LFP. The values of the aMCI group were numerically between the NC and AD groups (Fig. 2). Although there were no significant (P<0.05, Bonferroni correction) differences in the FNCs of the NC and aMCI patients or the MCI and AD patients, AD patients showed a trend toward decreased (P<0.05, uncorrected) FNC between the aDMN and pDMN compared to the aMCI patients.
Aberrant FC within RSNs
We compared the intra-network FCs among the three groups in a voxel-wise manner and extracted these significant brain regions as ROIs (Fig. 3 and Table 2). AD patients showed significantly decreased FCs in the bilateral (B-Pcu) and right (R-Pcu) Pcu of the Pcu network, the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and left Pcu (L-Pcu) of the pDMN, and the left superior parietal lobule (L-SPL) of the left FPN compared with the NCs (Fig. 4A). All of the comparisons between aMCI and AD patients were significant (P<0.05, Bonferroni correction). The FC values of these ROIs of aMCI patients were numerically between those of the NCs and AD patients. Compared with NCs, aMCI patients showed significantly (P<0.05, Bonferroni correction) decreased FC in the PCC.
Table 2. Brain areas with significant differences in FC across groups.
RSN | Regions | BA | Cluster size (voxels) | Peak z-score | MNI Coordination (x, y, z) |
Pcu | R-Pcu | 7 | 67 | 14.26 | 6, −70, 54 |
Pcu | B-Pcu | 7 | 86 | 12.73 | 0, −54, 62 |
pDMN | PCC | 23 | 155 | 15.76 | 6, −52, 30 |
pDMN | L-Pcu | 7 | 64 | 11.44 | −2, −58, 40 |
LFP | L-SPL | 7 | 22 | 13.39 | −28, −68, 56 |
Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer’s disease; B, bilateral; BA, Brodmann’s area; FC, functional connectivity; L, left; LFP, left frontoparietal network; MNI, Montreal Neurological Institute; NC, normal controls; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; Pcu, precuneus; pDMN, posterior default mode network; R, right; RSN, resting-state network; SPL, superior parietal lobule.
AD patients had significant decreases in GMV in several ROIs, such as the PCC and Pcu, which could explain the intra-network FC differences between groups [50]. Thus, we repeated the ROI-based intra-network FC comparisons while controlling for the GMV of each ROI. After GMV correction, compared with aMCI patients, AD patients showed only a trend toward decreased (P<0.05, uncorrected) FCs in the ROIs of the pDMN, and the other results did not change (Fig. 4B).
Aberrant ALFF within RSNs
We also performed ROI-based ALFF analysis to reveal differences in regional brain activity across the three groups. Compared with the NC, we found that the ALFF was significantly (P<0.05, Bonferroni correction) decreased in all five ROIs of the AD patients. Compared with aMCI patients, AD patients showed significantly (P<0.05, Bonferroni correction) decreased ALFF in the R-Pcu, B-Pcu, PCC and L-SPL and exhibited a trend toward decreased (P<0.05, uncorrected) ALFF in the L-Pcu. The aMCI group showed a trend toward decreased (P<0.05, uncorrected) ALFF in the PCC relative to the NC group. Notably, the ALFFs of the aMCI group were numerically between the NC and AD groups (Fig. 5A). After GMV correction, most of the between-group differences in ALFF remained (P<0.05, Bonferroni correction) except for the ROIs of the pDMN between the aMCI and AD groups. That is, the AD group showed a trend toward decreases (P<0.05, uncorrected) ALFF in the PCC compared to the aMCI group, and no significant difference (P<0.05, uncorrected) in ALFF between the aMCI and AD groups was found in the L-Pcu (Fig. 5B).
Aberrant GMV within RSNs
The GMV comparisons across the three groups are shown in Fig. 6. Compared with NCs, patients with AD showed significant (P<0.05, Bonferroni correction) GMV decreases in the PCC and L-Pcu, and a trend toward decreased (P<0.05, uncorrected) GMV in the R-Pcu. Compared with aMCI patients, AD patients showed significantly decreased GMV in the PCC and L-Pcu. The aMCI group showed a trend toward decreased (P<0.05, uncorrected) GMV in the PCC and L-Pcu compared with the NC group. The B-Pcu and L-SPL did not show any significant (P<0.05, uncorrected) changes in GMV across the three groups.
Differences in ALFF and GMV across the Whole Brain
Considering that the ROI-based analyses of the ALFF and GMV cannot provide a full picture of the whole brain changes, we also compared the ALFF and GMV among AD, aMCI and NC groups in a voxel-wise manner of the whole brain using ANOVA. Significant ALFF differences (P<0.005, uncorrected) among the three groups are found in the multiple brain regions (Table S1 and Figure S1). To test whether the ROIs with significant differences in the intra-network FCs also show significant differences in ALFF, we overlaid these ROIs onto the maps that show significant differences in the ALFF (Figure S2). We found that most of these ROIs were overlapped with brain regions with significant differences in the ALFF (Figure S2). In the voxel-based GMV analysis (P<0.05, FDR corrected), we found widespread brain regions showed significant GMV differences among the three groups (Figure S3), including several ROIs with significant differences in intra-network FC, such as the PCC/Pcu.
Correlations between MRI Indices and MMSE
To determine whether MRI indices with significant group differences contributed to the decline in cognitive function in patients, we performed partial correlation analyses between these MRI indices and MMSE scores. We found that most of the FCs, ALFFs, and GMVs of the ROIs were correlated with MMSE scores (P<0.05), except for the FNC between the LFP and VN (P = 0.329), the FCs of the PCC (P = 0.052), the GMV of the B-Pcu (P = 0.360) (Fig. 7, Fig. 8, Table 3, and Table 4). After correction for GMV, most of the correlations of functional measures were significant (P<0.05), except for the FNC between the aDMN and pDMN (P = 0.955), the FCs of the L-Pcu (P = 0.142), the ALFF of the PCC (P = 0.233) and L-Pcu (P = 0.189) (Table 3 and Table 4).
Table 3. Correlations between MMSEs and FNCs.
FNC | Without GMV correction | With GMV correction | ||
PCC | P | PCC | P | |
aDMN-pDMN | 0.323 | 0.022 | −0.008 | 0.955 |
LFP-VN | 0.141 | 0.329 | 0.277 | 0.054 |
Note: P values were adjusted for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni correction.
Abbreviations: aDMN, anterior default mode network; FNC, functional network connectivity; GMV, grey matter volume; LFP, left frontoparietal network; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; PCC, partial correlation coefficient; pDMN, posterior default mode network; VN, visual network.
Table 4. Correlations between MMSE scores and functional/structural indices.
Regions | FC | ALFF | GMV | |||||||
Without GMV correction | With GMV correction | Without GMV correction | With GMV correction | |||||||
PCC | P | PCC | P | PCC | P | PCC | P | PCC | P | |
R-Pcu | 0.426 | 0.002 | 0.384 | 0.006 | 0.367 | 0.009 | 0.370 | 0.009 | 0.326 | 0.021 |
B-Pcu | 0.458 | 0.001 | 0.454 | 0.001 | 0.402 | 0.004 | 0.403 | 0.004 | 0.132 | 0.360 |
PCCa | 0.276 | 0.052 | 0.163 | 0.263 | 0.289 | 0.042 | 0.174 | 0.233 | 0.556 | <0.001 |
L-Pcu | 0.301 | 0.033 | 0.213 | 0.142 | 0.299 | 0.035 | 0.191 | 0.189 | 0.553 | <0.001 |
L-SPL | 0.328 | 0.020 | 0.337 | 0.018 | 0.377 | 0.007 | 0.321 | 0.025 | 0.285 | 0.045 |
Note: P values were adjusted for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni correction.
Abbreviations: ALFF, amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation; B, bilateral; FC, functional connectivity; GMV, grey matter volume; L, left; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; PCC, partial correlation coefficient; PCCa, posterior cingulate cortex; Pcu, precuneus; R, right; SPL, superior parietal lobule.
Discussion
In the present study, we answered the four questions proposed in the introduction. We found the following: (1) the Pcu, pDMN and LFP networks were selectively impaired in AD, and only the pDMN was impaired in MCI; (2) brain regions with impaired FC in AD also exhibited alterations in ALFF and GMV; (3) most of the functional changes were independent of the structural changes; (4) FNCs between the aDMN and pDMN as well as between the VN and LFP were also changed in AD; and (5) most of these MRI measures were associated with cognitive decline in older people. The results suggest that the brain networks supporting complex cognitive processes are specifically and progressively impaired over the course of AD.
Multiple RSN Impairments in AD
The main function of the DMN is episodic memory processing [51], which is impaired in AD [4]. The structural and functional impairments within the DMN, as found here, have been frequently reported in AD [19], [25]. The novelty of our study is the finding that the functional deficits of the DMN regions are independent of their structural impairments in AD patients. It is generally assumed that the functional connectivity within the intrinsic networks reflects the direct or indirect structural connectivity. Nonetheless, it’s not a simple one-to-one mapping. Functional connectivity can exist in the absence of structural connectivity. Functional connectivity abnormalities are found at an early stage of some diseases, even before the detectable structural impairments [52]. We found that most of the functional differences remained significant after controlling for GMV, suggesting that the functional deficits in AD cannot be completely explained by the decreased GMV. That is to say, AD is associated with relatively more serious functional damage, which is not only secondary to the structure impairments. These findings suggest that both structural and functional deficits in the PCC and L-Pcu of the DMN contribute to AD pathology.
Beyond the DMN, several other RSNs have been shown to be impaired in AD [22], [25], [27]. In the present study, AD patients showed decreased FC, ALFF, and GMV within the Pcu and the left FPN compared with the NCs. The Pcu network has been identified as an independent RSN in a previous ICA study [49]. This network responds to a wide range of cognitive processes, such as reflective, self-related processing [53], [54], awareness and conscious information processing [55], [56], episodic memory [57]–[59], and visuospatial processing [60], [61]. Our finding of structural and functional impairments in the Pcu network in AD is consistent with the established concept that the precuneus is particularly vulnerable in AD [62]–[68].
As a lateralized RSN, the FPN has been identified by most previous ICA studies of resting-state fMRI data [69]–[71]. The FPN is activated during a wide array of goal-directed cognitive tasks [72]–[74] and is associated with memory [14], language [75], attention [73], [76], and visual [77] processes. As a core node of the FPN, the SPL plays an important role in visual attention [78]–[81]. The decreased FC, ALFF, and GMV in the SPL may underlie deficits in visual attention in AD [82]. Moreover, structural and functional abnormalities in the SPL in AD have been demonstrated in several previous studies and include decreases in GMV and cortical thickness [23], [83], [84], activation [82], resting-state FC [22], and effective connectivity [23]. In direct support of our finding, decreased LFP connectivity has been described in AD [25]. We did not find connectivity changes in the RFP, suggesting the laterality (left) of impaired FPN in AD. The left lateralized FPN impairment in AD is supported by a previous study of resting-state FC changes in the attention network in AD [85]. However, the laterality pattern needs to be further validated.
Inter-network Connectivity Impairments in AD
As mentioned in the introduction, little is known about the FNC alterations in AD. A recent ROI-based FC study revealed decreased anticorrelations between three pairs of anticorrelated RSNs, the DMN-dorsal attention network, the DMN-SMN, and the control network-SMN [29]. Moreover, a resting-state fMRI study detected three decreased connections (the SMN→self-referential network (SRN), the SRN→ventral attention network (VAN), and the VAN→dorsal attention network (DAN) and one increased connection (DMN→DAN) in AD [30]. However, we used the data-driven method of ICA to identify RSNs and computed FNCs between different pairs of RSNs [30], [31], [86]. We found decreased positive correlations between the aDMN and pDMN and between the LFP and VN. The interaction of the anterior and posterior regions of the DMN may serve to organize neuronal activity [87] or to support mind wandering during self-referential mental processing [88], [89]. Decreases in resting-state FCs between the anterior and posterior portions of the DMN have been well established in AD [90]–[95]. It has been suggested that decreased connectivity between the anterior and posterior components of the DMN may underlie deficits in self-referential processing, attention control and working memory [96].
It has been suggested that the FPN and VN cooperate to support visual attention [97]. A pioneering study revealed that visual cortical areas that selectively process relevant information are functionally connected with the FPN [98], which is associated with top-down enhancement of task-relevant stimuli [72]. The impairment of selective attention, especially visual attention, has been documented in patients with AD [82], [99]–[104]. The decreased FNC between the LFP and VN may be a possible mechanism of impaired visual attention in AD.
Network Markers of aMCI
Although the values of MRI measures of the aMCI patients were numerically between those of NCs and AD patients, only the PCC of the posterior DMN showed decreased FC, ALFF and GMV in aMCI patients compared with NCs. These findings suggest that the impairment of the PCC occurs as early as the MCI stage, which is consistent with the observations of previous studies [15], [16], [18], [19]. It seems that the PCC is preferentially affected in aMCI patients and could be used as a biological marker to distinguish MCI patients from NCs.
Biomarkers for Cognitive Decline in Older People
In older people, most of the FCs, ALFFs, and GMVs of the ROIs were correlated with MMSE scores except for the FCs of the PCC and GMV of the B-Pcu. Most of these correlations of functional measures remained significant after GMV correction. These findings suggest that both structural and functional impairments in the cognitive-related RSNs independently contribute to cognitive decline in older people.
Limitations
It should be noted that the significance of our results are not as high as some previous reports [105], the differences in sample size, demographics, analyzing methods may partly account for the differences in significance across studies. More importantly, our current study is limited by the relatively small number of available samples, especially in the aMCI group, and the large difference in age between the 3 groups. Further studies with a larger sample size and more matched groups should be done to validate our findings.
Supporting Information
Funding Statement
This work was supported by the Natural Science Foundation of Tianjin (No. 11JCZDJC19300), the National Basic Research Program of China (973 program, No. 2011CB707801), the Natural Science Foundation of China (Nos. 81271551, 60831004, 30900476), and the International Cooperation and Exchanges NSFC (No. 30370434). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Kokmen E, et al.. (1997) Aging, memory, and mild cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr (Suppl 1): 65–69. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 2. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, et al. (1999) Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 56: 303–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Fita IG, Enciu AM, Stanoiu BP (2011) New insights on Alzheimer’s disease diagnostic. Rom J Morphol Embryol 52: 975–979. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Weintraub S, Wicklund AH, Salmon DP (2012) The neuropsychological profile of Alzheimer disease. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med 2: a006171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. He Y, Chen Z, Gong G, Evans A (2009) Neuronal networks in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroscientist 15: 333–350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Seeley WW, Crawford RK, Zhou J, Miller BL, Greicius MD (2009) Neurodegenerative diseases target large-scale human brain networks. Neuron 62: 42–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Calhoun VD, Adali T, Pearlson GD, Pekar JJ (2001) Spatial and temporal independent component analysis of functional MRI data containing a pair of task-related waveforms. Hum Brain Mapp 13: 43–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. McKeown MJ, Makeig S, Brown GG, Jung TP, Kindermann SS, et al. (1998) Analysis of fMRI data by blind separation into independent spatial components. Hum Brain Mapp 6: 160–188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Liao W, Mantini D, Zhang Z, Pan Z, Ding J, et al. (2010) Evaluating the effective connectivity of resting state networks using conditional Granger causality. Biol Cybern 102: 57–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Liao W, Chen H, Feng Y, Mantini D, Gentili C, et al. (2010) Selective aberrant functional connectivity of resting state networks in social anxiety disorder. Neuroimage 52: 1549–1558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. van de Ven V, Bledowski C, Prvulovic D, Goebel R, Formisano E, et al. (2008) Visual target modulation of functional connectivity networks revealed by self-organizing group ICA. Hum Brain Mapp 29: 1450–1461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Mantini D, Perrucci MG, Del Gratta C, Romani GL, Corbetta M (2007) Electrophysiological signatures of resting state networks in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104: 13170–13175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME (2006) Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103: 10046–10051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SA, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, et al. (2006) Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103: 13848–13853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Sorg C, Riedl V, Muhlau M, Calhoun VD, Eichele T, et al. (2007) Selective changes of resting-state networks in individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104: 18760–18765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Bai F, Zhang Z, Yu H, Shi Y, Yuan Y, et al. (2008) Default-mode network activity distinguishes amnestic type mild cognitive impairment from healthy aging: a combined structural and resting-state functional MRI study. Neurosci Lett 438: 111–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Bai F, Watson DR, Yu H, Shi Y, Yuan Y, et al. (2009) Abnormal resting-state functional connectivity of posterior cingulate cortex in amnestic type mild cognitive impairment. Brain Res 1302: 167–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Qi Z, Wu X, Wang Z, Zhang N, Dong H, et al. (2010) Impairment and compensation coexist in amnestic MCI default mode network. Neuroimage 50: 48–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Jin M, Pelak VS, Cordes D (2012) Aberrant default mode network in subjects with amnestic mild cognitive impairment using resting-state functional MRI. Magn Reson Imaging 30: 48–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Bai F, Watson DR, Shi Y, Wang Y, Yue C, et al. (2011) Specifically progressive deficits of brain functional marker in amnestic type mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One 6: e24271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Petrella JR, Sheldon FC, Prince SE, Calhoun VD, Doraiswamy PM (2011) Default mode network connectivity in stable vs progressive mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 76: 511–517. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Li R, Wu X, Fleisher AS, Reiman EM, Chen K, et al. (2011) Attention-related networks in Alzheimer’s disease: A resting functional MRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 33: 1076–1088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Neufang S, Akhrif A, Riedl V, Forstl H, Kurz A, et al. (2011) Disconnection of frontal and parietal areas contributes to impaired attention in very early Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 25: 309–321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Zhou J, Greicius MD, Gennatas ED, Growdon ME, Jang JY, et al. (2010) Divergent network connectivity changes in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 133: 1352–1367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Agosta F, Pievani M, Geroldi C, Copetti M, Frisoni GB, et al. (2011) Resting state fMRI in Alzheimer’s disease: beyond the default mode network. Neurobiol Aging 33: 1564–1578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Bai F, Shi Y, Yuan Y, Wang Y, Yue C, et al. (2011) Altered self-referential network in resting-state amnestic type mild cognitive impairment. Cortex 48: 604–613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Rombouts SA, Damoiseaux JS, Goekoop R, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, et al. (2009) Model-free group analysis shows altered BOLD FMRI networks in dementia. Hum Brain Mapp 30: 256–266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Bokde AL, Lopez-Bayo P, Born C, Dong W, Meindl T, et al. (2008) Functional abnormalities of the visual processing system in subjects with mild cognitive impairment: an fMRI study. Psychiatry Res 163: 248–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Brier MR, Thomas JB, Snyder AZ, Benzinger TL, Zhang D, et al. (2012) Loss of intranetwork and internetwork resting state functional connections with Alzheimer’s disease progression. J Neurosci 32: 8890–8899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Li R, Wu X, Chen K, Fleisher AS, Reiman EM, et al.. (2012) Alterations of Directional Connectivity among Resting-State Networks in Alzheimer Disease. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol, in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 31. Jafri MJ, Pearlson GD, Stevens M, Calhoun VD (2008) A method for functional network connectivity among spatially independent resting-state components in schizophrenia. Neuroimage 39: 1666–1681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Binnewijzend MA, Schoonheim MM, Sanz-Arigita E, Wink AM, van der Flier WM, et al. (2011) Resting-state fMRI changes in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging 33: 2018–2028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Wang Z, Yan C, Zhao C, Qi Z, Zhou W, et al. (2011) Spatial patterns of intrinsic brain activity in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: a resting-state functional MRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 32: 1720–1740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, et al. (1984) Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 34: 939–944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, Mohs RC, Morris JC, et al. (2001) Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 58: 1985–1992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Morris JC (1993) The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 43: 2412–2414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Fazekas F, Chawluk JB, Alavi A, Hurtig HI, Zimmerman RA (1987) MR signal abnormalities at 1.5 T in Alzheimer’s dementia and normal aging. AJR Am J Roentgenol 149: 351–356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Yan C, Zang Y (2010) DPARSF: A MATLAB Toolbox for “Pipeline” Data Analysis of Resting-State fMRI. Front Syst Neurosci 4: 13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Calhoun VD, Adali T, Pearlson GD, Pekar JJ (2001) A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 14: 140–151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, et al.. (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage (Suppl 1): S208–219. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 41. Zhang H, Zuo XN, Ma SY, Zang YF, Milham MP, et al. (2010) Subject order-independent group ICA (SOI-GICA) for functional MRI data analysis. Neuroimage 51: 1414–1424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Biswal BB, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS (1995) Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med 34: 537–541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Lowe MJ, Mock BJ, Sorenson JA (1998) Functional connectivity in single and multislice echoplanar imaging using resting-state fluctuations. Neuroimage 7: 119–132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Zang YF, He Y, Zhu CZ, Cao QJ, Sui MQ, et al. (2007) Altered baseline brain activity in children with ADHD revealed by resting-state functional MRI. Brain Dev 29: 83–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, et al. (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98: 676–682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Rajapakse JC, Giedd JN, Rapoport JL (1997) Statistical approach to segmentation of single-channel cerebral MR images. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 16: 176–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Ashburner J (2007) A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38: 95–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Steffener J, Habeck CG, Stern Y (2012) Age-related changes in task related functional network connectivity. PLoS One 7: e44421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Veer IM, Beckmann CF, van Tol MJ, Ferrarini L, Milles J, et al. (2010) Whole brain resting-state analysis reveals decreased functional connectivity in major depression. Front Syst Neurosci 4: 41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Damoiseaux JS, Beckmann CF, Arigita EJ, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, et al. (2008) Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging. Cereb Cortex 18: 1856–1864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Greicius MD, Menon V (2004) Default-mode activity during a passive sensory task: uncoupled from deactivation but impacting activation. J Cogn Neurosci 16: 1484–1492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Roosendaal SD, Schoonheim MM, Hulst HE, Sanz-Arigita EJ, Smith SM, et al. (2010) Resting state networks change in clinically isolated syndrome. Brain 133: 1612–1621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Kjaer TW, Nowak M, Lou HC (2002) Reflective self-awareness and conscious states: PET evidence for a common midline parietofrontal core. Neuroimage 17: 1080–1086. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Lou HC, Luber B, Crupain M, Keenan JP, Nowak M, et al. (2004) Parietal cortex and representation of the mental Self. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101: 6827–6832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Kjaer TW, Nowak M, Kjaer KW, Lou AR, Lou HC (2001) Precuneus-prefrontal activity during awareness of visual verbal stimuli. Conscious Cogn 10: 356–365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Vogt BA, Laureys S (2005) Posterior cingulate, precuneal and retrosplenial cortices: cytology and components of the neural network correlates of consciousness. Prog Brain Res 150: 205–217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Dorfel D, Werner A, Schaefer M, von Kummer R, Karl A (2009) Distinct brain networks in recognition memory share a defined region in the precuneus. Eur J Neurosci 30: 1947–1959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Lundstrom BN, Petersson KM, Andersson J, Johansson M, Fransson P, et al. (2003) Isolating the retrieval of imagined pictures during episodic memory: activation of the left precuneus and left prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 20: 1934–1943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Lundstrom BN, Ingvar M, Petersson KM (2005) The role of precuneus and left inferior frontal cortex during source memory episodic retrieval. Neuroimage 27: 824–834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Kawashima R, Roland PE, O’Sullivan BT (1995) Functional anatomy of reaching and visuomotor learning: a positron emission tomography study. Cereb Cortex 5: 111–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Wenderoth N, Debaere F, Sunaert S, Swinnen SP (2005) The role of anterior cingulate cortex and precuneus in the coordination of motor behaviour. Eur J Neurosci 22: 235–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Karas G, Scheltens P, Rombouts S, van Schijndel R, Klein M, et al. (2007) Precuneus atrophy in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease: a morphometric structural MRI study. Neuroradiology 49: 967–976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Petrella JR, Wang L, Krishnan S, Slavin MJ, Prince SE, et al. (2007) Cortical deactivation in mild cognitive impairment: high-field-strength functional MR imaging. Radiology 245: 224–235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Vannini P, Almkvist O, Dierks T, Lehmann C, Wahlund LO (2007) Reduced neuronal efficacy in progressive mild cognitive impairment: a prospective fMRI study on visuospatial processing. Psychiatry Res 156: 43–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Edison P, Archer HA, Hinz R, Hammers A, Pavese N, et al. (2007) Amyloid, hypometabolism, and cognition in Alzheimer disease: an [11C]PIB and [18F]FDG PET study. Neurology 68: 501–508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Damoiseaux JS, Prater KE, Miller BL, Greicius MD (2011) Functional connectivity tracks clinical deterioration in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 33: 828.e19–828.e30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Buckner RL (2004) Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron 44: 195–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, Shannon BJ, LaRossa G, Sachs R, et al. (2005) Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer’s disease: evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J Neurosci 25: 7709–7717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Koechlin E, Summerfield C (2007) An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function. Trends Cogn Sci 11: 229–235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, et al. (2007) Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 27: 2349–2356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Vincent JL, Kahn I, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME, Buckner RL (2008) Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 100: 3328–3342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL (2008) The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58: 306–324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, et al. (2005) The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102: 9673–9678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Corbetta M (1998) Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95: 831–838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, et al. (2009) Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: 13040–13045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Dosenbach NU, Fair DA, Miezin FM, Cohen AL, Wenger KK, et al. (2007) Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104: 11073–11078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. De Luca M, Beckmann CF, De Stefano N, Matthews PM, Smith SM (2006) fMRI resting state networks define distinct modes of long-distance interactions in the human brain. Neuroimage 29: 1359–1367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Han S, Jiang Y, Gu H (2004) Neural substrates differentiating global/local processing of bilateral visual inputs. Hum Brain Mapp 22: 321–328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3: 201–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Kastner S, Ungerleider LG (2001) The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex. Neuropsychologia 39: 1263–1276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Hedden T, Gabrieli JD (2006) The ebb and flow of attention in the human brain. Nat Neurosci 9: 863–865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Hao J, Li K, Zhang D, Wang W, Yang Y, et al. (2005) Visual attention deficits in Alzheimer’s disease: an fMRI study. Neurosci Lett 385: 18–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Hanggi J, Streffer J, Jancke L, Hock C (2011) Volumes of lateral temporal and parietal structures distinguish between healthy aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 26: 719–734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Putcha D, Brickhouse M, O’Keefe K, Sullivan C, Rentz D, et al. (2011) Hippocampal hyperactivation associated with cortical thinning in Alzheimer's disease signature regions in non-demented elderly adults. J Neurosci 31: 17680–17688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. li R, Wu X, Fleisher AS, Reiman EM, Chen K, et al. (2011) Attention-related networks in Alzheimer’s disease: A resting functional MRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 33: 1076–1088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Onoda K, Ishihara M, Yamaguchi S (2012) Decreased Functional Connectivity by Aging Is Associated with Cognitive Decline. J Cogn Neurosci, in press. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 87. Buzsaki G, Draguhn A (2004) Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304: 1926–1929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88. Fransson P (2005) Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: an fMRI investigation of the resting-state default mode of brain function hypothesis. Hum Brain Mapp 26: 15–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89. Mason MF, Norton MI, Van Horn JD, Wegner DM, Grafton ST, et al. (2007) Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315: 393–395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90. Greicius MD, Srivastava G, Reiss AL, Menon V (2004) Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101: 4637–4642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Supekar K, Menon V, Rubin D, Musen M, Greicius MD (2008) Network analysis of intrinsic functional brain connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS Comput Biol 4: e1000100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Zhang HY, Wang SJ, Xing J, Liu B, Ma ZL, et al. (2009) Detection of PCC functional connectivity characteristics in resting-state fMRI in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Behav Brain Res 197: 103–108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. Lustig C, Snyder AZ, Bhakta M, O’Brien KC, McAvoy M, et al. (2003) Functional deactivations: change with age and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100: 14504–14509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94. Sheline YI, Raichle ME, Snyder AZ, Morris JC, Head D, et al. (2010) Amyloid plaques disrupt resting state default mode network connectivity in cognitively normal elderly. Biol Psychiatry 67: 584–587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. Galvin JE, Price JL, Yan Z, Morris JC, Sheline YI (2011) Resting bold fMRI differentiates dementia with Lewy bodies vs Alzheimer disease. Neurology 76: 1797–1803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Broyd SJ, Demanuele C, Debener S, Helps SK, James CJ, et al. (2009) Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33: 279–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97. Shomstein S, Kravitz DJ, Behrmann M (2012) Attentional control: temporal relationships within the fronto-parietal network. Neuropsychologia 50: 1202–1210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98. Chadick JZ, Gazzaley A (2011) Differential coupling of visual cortex with default or frontal-parietal network based on goals. Nat Neurosci 14: 830–832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99. Rosler A, Mapstone M, Hays-Wicklund A, Gitelman DR, Weintraub S (2005) The “zoom lens” of focal attention in visual search: changes in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 41: 512–519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100. Tales A, Muir JL, Bayer A, Snowden RJ (2002) Spatial shifts in visual attention in normal ageing and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia 40: 2000–2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101. Pignatti R, Rabuffetti M, Imbornone E, Mantovani F, Alberoni M, et al. (2005) Specific impairments of selective attention in mild Alzheimer’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 27: 436–448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102. Reid W, Broe G, Creasey H, Grayson D, McCusker E, et al. (1996) Age at onset and pattern of neuropsychological impairment in mild early-stage Alzheimer disease. A study of a community-based population. Arch Neurol 53: 1056–1061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103. Perry RJ, Hodges JR (1999) Attention and executive deficits in Alzheimer’s disease. A critical review. Brain 122(Pt 3): 383–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104. Perry RJ, Watson P, Hodges JR (2000) The nature and staging of attention dysfunction in early (minimal and mild) Alzheimer’s disease: relationship to episodic and semantic memory impairment. Neuropsychologia 38: 252–271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Schwindt GC, Chaudhary S, Crane D, Ganda A, Masellis M, et al.. (2012) Modulation of the Default-Mode Network Between Rest and Task in Alzheimer’s Disease. Cereb Cortex, in press. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 106. Van Essen DC (2005) A Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage 28: 635–662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.