Whereas much has been published about the effect of sleep loss on relatively simple tasks highly sensitive to sleepiness, especially under monotonous situations,1,2 less is known about more complex aspects of behavior, especially decision making and “risky” behaviors concerning “executive thinking” that largely involve the prefrontal cortex (PFC).3,4 Such skills are likely to be required by managers, physicians, control room operators, the military and emergency services, often early morning during the daily circadian trough, after having being awake for around 20 h during the first night shift.4 Especially relevant to those working in international financial markets, often necessitating working through the night, are the latest findings by Libedinsky and colleagues,5 described in the current issue of SLEEP, following a series of earlier studies by this group into executive functioning during sleep loss.6–9 Previously they had noted that, “even a single night of total sleep deprivation can have dramatic effects on economic decision making.”9
This latest report by Libedinsky et al. involved a series of rather complex but unique experiments concerning decisions to be made either at 06:00 following 22 h of wakefulness, or at around 08:00 after normal sleep. The unusual feature about this study was that these decisions concerned a future event. Under one scenario, participants could opt for a relatively small financial reward obtainable soon, or choose between larger rewards increasing over a duration of months, but not available until then (i.e., “delay discounting”). The second scenario (i.e., “effort discounting”) involved participants choosing between levels requiring increasing effort (typing increasing numbers of words backwards) but having greater financial rewards. Thus, delay discounting assessed the willingness to have a smaller sum sooner, rather than a larger sum later, whereas effort discounting was the willingness to accept smaller rewards for less effort. A real world analogy, offered by the authors, was the sleep deprived surgeon having to decide on one of two surgical techniques to be adopted for a future operation (when the surgeon would be in a fit and alert state); these being a difficult procedure having a good outcome versus a simper procedure with reasonable outcome.
Although Libedinsky et al.5 hypothesized that sleep loss would affect delay discounting, with participants more likely to opt for a “quick buck,” this was not the outcome of their study. Nonetheless, it should be noted that the delays incurred could be very long (months), rather than just a few hours. On the other hand, effort discounting was significantly impaired, which might seem unsurprising at first glance but, again, it must be remembered that it applied to a future event when the participants would be refreshed. The authors reasoned that sleep loss reduced their participants' capability to decide, because they had projected this impairment into the future. However, I suggest that this outcome also involves changes to risk perception, as other evidence indicates that if sleep deprived people believe that they are likely to succeed, then they take greater risks,6,10,11 or, if they perceive a likely failure, then they become risk averse.3,10,12,13 Clearly, the perceived uncertainty and the surrounding physical and social circumstances do have a major influence during sleep loss, with decisions becoming more influenced by emotions rather than by rational thought.14 Also, apart from a loss of insight into one's deteriorating executive skills,15 there are individual differences in coping with extended wakefulness, especially in terms of personality and cognitive styles.16,17 Interestingly, people with clock gene polymorphism (PER3 -5/5) associated with extreme morningness, display poorer executive functioning during early morning extended wakefulness.18,19
Whereas there is some contention over whether executive functions are impaired by sleep loss,20 many such tests only assess limited aspects of the complex range of executive function, often excluding behaviors classed operationally as “higher” or “supervisory” executive functions.21 These involve dealing with: novelty, unexpected change, uncertainty, ignoring of conflicting and irrelevant information, tracking of and remembering very recent developments, maintaining focus on key issues, foreseeing and weighing up potential outcomes, and the need to be innovative in planning appropriate responses.
Ideally, to be applicable to the real world, executive tests need to be “ecological” as reflected by Libedinsky's team; albeit an approach adopted by only a few other sleep loss studies.e.g.,22 More commonly used “non-supervisory” executive tests (e.g., Sternberg working memory, Stroop tests, digit-symbol substitution) incorporate little inherent novelty,23 especially when presented repeatedly, when the test becomes “routine”23–25 and “executiveness” is lost. Here, participants often adopt new strategies resulting in faster, more correct responses, thus nullifying ostensible declines from seep loss.25 Another confound is that tests are often of fairly short duration, albeit lasting 10 minutes or so. However, in reality, overnight critical situations can be protracted over longer periods, to the extent that shorter experimental periods may underrate a potential worsening of executive function. This is even more likely to occur in the presence of additional stressors, as studies outside sleep deprivation certainly point to adverse stress effects on executive decisions.26
Even in situations involving frustrating, protracted and delicate discussions during the early morning hours, typically timed for sleep, when, for example sleepless politicians attempt to resolve deadlocks and negotiate terms—other aspects of executive function can be affected,4,13 with people becoming irritable, obdurate, losing track of when and what was recently said, having difficulty in finding the appropriate (and diplomatic) words,27,28 being more wary of possible exploitation,15 and failing to detect subtle facial expressions and otherwise useful nonverbal cues.29
Whereas a short nap,30 caffeine,31 and even monetary incentives32 are effective countermeasures to “sleepiness,” such methods as a means for rectifying impaired executive function are less clear,3,33 especially when there are emotional changes.4 Finally, an often overlooked aspect to sleep-related impaired (supervisory) executive thinking, concerns being suddenly and unexpectedly awoken early morning (especially from deep sleep) and confronted with a crisis. At least 20 min seems to be required to overcome inertial effects on the PFC34 and to enable effective supervisory executive decision making.35
CITATION
Horne J. Overnight sleep loss and “executive” decision making—subtle findings. SLEEP 2013;36(6):823-824.
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
Dr. Horne has indicated no financial conflicts of interest.
REFERENCES
- 1.Lim J, Dinges DF. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129:305–22. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Horne JA. Sleepiness as a need for sleep: when is enough, enough. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:108–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Killgore WD. Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Prog Brain Res. 2010;185:105–29. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00007-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Horne JA. Working through the night- subtle impairments to critical decision making. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:2226–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Libedinsky C, Massar SAA, Ling A, Chee W, Huettal SA, Chee MWL. Sleep deprivation alters effort discounting but not delay discounting of monetary rewards. Sleep. 2013;36:899–904. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Venkatraman V, Chuah YM, Huettel SA, Chee MW. Sleep deprivation elevates expectation of gains and attenuates response to losses following risky decisions. Sleep. 2007;30:603–9. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.5.603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Venkatraman V, Huettel SA. Strategic control in decision-making under uncertainty. Eur J Neurosci. 2012;35:1075–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08009.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Venkatraman V, Huettel SA, Chuah LY, Payne JW, Chee MW. Sleep deprivation biases the neural mechanisms underlying economic preferences. J Neurosci. 2011;31:3712–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4407-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Libedinsky C, Smith DV, Teng CS, et al. Sleep deprivation alters valuation signals in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Front Behav Neurosci. 2011 doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00070. 5,70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.McKenna BS, Dickinson DL, Orff HJ, Drummond SP. The effects of one night of sleep deprivation on known-risk and ambiguous-risk decisions. J Sleep Res. 2007;16:245–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2007.00591.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Anderson C, Platten CR. Sleep deprivation lowers inhibition and enhances impulsivity to negative stimuli. Behav Brain Res. 2011;217:463–6. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Chaumet G, Taillard J, Sagaspe P, et al. Confinement and sleep deprivation effects on propensity to take risks. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2009;80:73–80. doi: 10.3357/asem.2366.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep loss on decision making - a review. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2000;6:236–49. doi: 10.1037//1076-898x.6.3.236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Anderson C, Dickinson DL. Bargaining and trust: the effects of 36-h total sleep deprivation on socially interactive decisions. J Sleep Res. 2010;19:54–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00767.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Baranski JV. Fatigue, sleep loss, and confidence in judgment. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2007;13:182–96. doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.13.4.182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Maddox WT, Glass BD, Wolosin SM, et al. The effects of sleep deprivation on information-integration categorization performance. Sleep. 2009;31:1439–48. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.11.1439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Saksvik IB, Bjorvatn B, Hetland H, Sandal GM, Pallesen S. Individual differences in tolerance to shift work--a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2011;15:221–35. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2010.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Groeger JA, Viola AU, Lo JC, von Schantz M, Archer SN, Dijk DJ. Early morning executive functioning during sleep deprivation is compromised by a PERIOD3 polymorphism. Sleep. 2008;31:159–67. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Goel N, Banks S, Mignot E, Dinges DF. PER3 polymorphism predicts cumulative sleep homeostatic but not neurobehavioral changes to chronic partial sleep deprivation. PLoS One. 2009;4:e5874. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Tucker AM, Whitney P, Belenky G, Hinson JM, Van Dongen HPA. Effects of sleep deprivation on dissociated components of executive functioning. Sleep. 2010;33:47–57. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.1.47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006;16:17–42. doi: 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Nilsson JP, Söderström M, Karlsson AU, et al. Less effective executive functioning after one night's sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res. 2005;14:1–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2005.00442.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Dulaney CL, Rogers WA. Mechanisms underlying reduction in Stroop interference with practice for young and old adults. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1994;20:470–84. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.20.2.470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Jansma JM, Ramsey NF, Slagter HA, Kahn RS. Functional anatomical correlates of controlled and automatic processing. J Cogn Neurosci. 2001;13:730–43. doi: 10.1162/08989290152541403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Horne JA. Testing of executive function. Chronobiol Int. 2012;29:1284. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2012.719970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Starcke C, Brand M. Decision making under stress. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:1228–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Harrison Y, Horne JA. Sleep deprivation affects speech. Sleep. 1997;20:871–7. doi: 10.1093/sleep/20.10.871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Pilcher JJ, McClelland LE, Moore DD, et al. Language performance under sustained work and sleep deprivation conditions. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2007;78(Suppl 5):B25–38. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.van der Helm E, Gujar N, Walker MP. Sleep deprivation impairs the accurate recognition of human emotions. Sleep. 2010;33:335–42. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.3.335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Asaoka S, Fukuda K, Murphy TI, Abe T, Inoue Y. The effects of a nighttime nap on the error monitoring functions during extended wakefulness. Sleep. 2012;35:871–8. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bonnet MH, Balkin TJ, Dinges DF, Roehrs T, Rogers NL, Wesensten NJ. The use of stimulants to modify performance during sleep loss: a review by the Sleep Deprivation and Stimulant Task Force of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Sleep. 2005;28:1163–87. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.9.1163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hsieh S, Li TH, Tsai LL. Impact of monetary incentives on cognitive performance and error monitoring following sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2010;33:499–507. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.4.499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Killgore WD, Grugle NL, Balkin TJ. Gambling when sleep deprived: don't bet on stimulants. Chronobiol Int. 2012;29:43–54. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2011.635230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Balkin TJ, Braun AR, Wesensten NJ, et al. The process of awakening: a PET study of regional brain activity patterns mediating the re-establishment of alertness and consciousness. Brain. 2002;125:2308–19. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Horne JA, Moseley R. Sudden early-morning awakening impairs immediate tactical planning in a changing ‘emergency’ scenario. J Sleep Res. 2011;20:275–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00904.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]