Abstract
Background
Impaired cognitive flexibility in anorexia nervosa (AN) causes clinical problems and makes the disease hard to treat, but its neural basis has yet to be fully elucidated. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the brain activity of individuals with AN while performing a task requiring cognitive flexibility on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), which is one of the most frequently used neurocognitive measures of cognitive flexibility and problem-solving ability.
Methods
Participants were 15 female AN patients and 15 age- and intelligence quotient-matched healthy control women. Participants completed the WCST while their brain activity was measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging during the task. Brain activation in response to set shifting error feedback and the correlation between such brain activity and set shifting performance were analyzed.
Results
The correct rate on the WCST was significantly poorer for AN patients than for controls. Patients showed poorer activity in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and bilateral parahippocampal cortex on set shifting than controls. Controls showed a positive correlation between correct rate and ventrolateral prefrontal activity in response to set shifting whereas patients did not.
Conclusion
These findings suggest dysfunction of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and parahippocampal cortex as a cause of impaired cognitive flexibility in AN patients.
Introduction
Patients with anorexia nervosa (AN) have a cognitive deficit relating to own body weight and shape [1]. Their perception of body shape is seriously distorted and they refuse to maintain a minimally normal weight because of an intense fear of gaining weight. AN patients strictly limit their food intake (restrictive type: ANR) and/or binge eat and purge (binge-purge type: ANBP), and in many cases, their thoughts are occupied with food. Accordingly, a number of neuroimaging studies on AN have focused on the role of body image [2]–[6] or food stimuli [7]–[9], but they have produced inconsistent results and other pathogenic factors need to be examined in greater detail in AN research.
Cognitive impairment in AN extends beyond symptoms concerning body image and food to involve visuospatial ability [10]–[13], attention [12]–[14], memory [11], [13]–[16], and cognitive flexibility [17]–[23]. Cognitive flexibility is the ability to alter a behavior in response to changes in the situation, and impaired cognitive flexibility is considered to be a risk factor of AN [20], [24]. The deficit causes behavioral rigidity which leads to maintenance of symptoms [20], [25] and resistance to treatment. Impaired cognitive flexibility in AN patients has been found to have no correlation with body weight [17], [23], [26]. Recovered AN patients have also shown cognitive flexibility impairment [23], [27], and interestingly unaffected sisters of AN patients have shown poorer cognitive flexibility than healthy controls [23], [26]. These findings suggest that impaired cognitive flexibility in AN patients is not a temporary state due to starvation but is a trait characteristic. One study has reported that AN patients without comorbid depression showed intact cognitive flexibility during several cognitive tasks [28]. As many as 86% of AN patients are reported to have lifetime comorbid depressive disorder [29], 64% to have anxiety disorders [30], and 21.7% to have at least one personality disorder [31]. However, the nature of the relationship between cognitive flexibility and comorbidities in AN patients remains to be elucidated.
In recent years, cognitive functions not associated with food or body image have started to be evaluated in individuals with AN, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) [32]–[36]. Recovered AN patients were found to have higher caudate activity during a monetary reward task than healthy controls, and while healthy controls responded differently to reward and penalty feedback in the anterior ventral striatum, recovered AN patients had almost the same response to both conditions [32]. Adolescents with AN showed significantly higher activation than healthy controls in the temporal and parietal areas during a working memory task, a difference that disappeared after weight recovery [35]. Moreover, patients with ANBP showed greater activation than controls in the bilateral precentral gyri, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and superior and middle temporal gyri in a response inhibition task, while patients with ANR showed poorer activity than those with ANBP in the hypothalamus and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) [36]. Recovered AN patients showed poorer medial prefrontal activity than controls during a more difficult response inhibition task [34]. Zastrow et al. reported that AN patients had a significantly higher error rate in behavioral response shifting during a target detection task focused on cognitive and behavioral flexibility [33]. During the behavioral response shifting, the patients showed less activation than controls in the left and right thalamus, ventral striatum, ACC, and sensorimotor brain regions but higher activation in the frontal and parietal regions. Hypoactivity of anterior cingulate-striato-thalamic loop appeared to be associated with impaired behavioral response shifting, but no deficit in cognitive set shifting was seen during the task. Any definitive evidence of the pathogenesis of AN remains elusive, however.
The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) [37] is one of the most widely used neurocognitive measures to evaluate cognitive flexibility. A lesion in the prefrontal cortex can cause difficulties on the WCST [38]–[40], and neuroimaging studies have demonstrated activation of the frontostriatal circuit during the WCST [41]–[45]. The lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) is also a key area that is activated during cognitive flexibility tasks [46]. Healthy participants have shown significant LPFC activation in the condition requiring set shifting during the WCST [42], [44], [45]. AN patients have been reported by several authors to show poor performance on the WCST [17], [20]–[22], [47], but there have been no brain imaging studies conducted on individuals with AN while completing the WCST.
We hypothesized that AN patients would show poor performance and hypoactivity in the LPFC during the WCST. To test this hypothesis and try to elucidate the neural basis of impaired cognitive flexibility in AN patients, we administered the WCST to AN patients and controls while measuring blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signals of the brain with fMRI.
Participants and Methods
1. Participants
Forty-eight right-handed women participated in this study. Twenty-one individuals who fulfilled the DSM-IV-TR criteria for AN [1] —11 with ANR and 10 with ANBP—were recruited from outpatients and inpatients at Tohoku University Hospital. Handedness was determined by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [48]. We excluded individuals with claustrophobia, visual impairment including defect in color perception, metallic implant, lifetime presence of head trauma and/or neurological disease, or life-threatening physical condition. Those with past/present Axis I or II psychiatric disorders were also excluded, but AN patients with depressive disorder, anxiety disorder, or personality disorder, which are highly prevalent comorbidities in AN patients, were included in this study to reflect the typical clinical situation. None of the healthy controls took medication. Given that cognitive flexibility is impaired by major tranquilizers [49], that some cognitive functions are impaired by both acute [50] and long-term [51] benzodiazepine administration, but that serotonin selective reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) minimally affects cognitive task performance [52], we also excluded the patients who took major tranquilizer/benzodiazepine anxiolytics but included those who took SSRIs. On this basis, the following AN patients were excluded: one who had mental retardation, and 2 with ANR and 3 with ANBP who took a major tranquilizer and/or benzodiazepine anxiolytic and were later excluded. Participant screening and diagnosis were performed by board certified specialists of the Japanese Society of Psychosomatic Medicine at Tohoku University Hospital based on medical interview according to the DSM-IV-TR. This left 15 AN patients as participants in this study—9 ANR patients and 6 ANBP patients. Four of the 15 patients were taking a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) and took it even on the day of fMRI session (3 diagnosed with depression and the remaining patient diagnosed with obsessive-compulsive disorder). One patient was diagnosed as having borderline personality disorder. Twenty-seven healthy controls were registered for this study and we selected 15 who were intelligent quotient (IQ)- and age-matched to the patients. Although all 15 had served female controls in our previous fMRI study on the brain activity of patients with irritable bowel syndrome [53], the hypothesis and target disease of the two studies are different, and our control data used in the previous study was composed of 15 men combined with 15 women, and therefore the women's control data have not been reported previously. All controls were within the normal weight range (body mass index (BMI) 18–23 kg/m2) and all had been recruited by advertisement from among university students, were free from medication, and reported no history (lifetime diagnosis) of psychiatric disease.
2. Ethics statement
This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Tohoku University School of Medicine and all participants provided written informed consent to participate. All participants were judged to have the ability to give consent through a medical interview by board certified specialists of the Japanese Society of Psychosomatic Medicine at Tohoku University Hospital. Next of kin, caretakers or guardians consented on the behalf of those participants under the age of 20.
3. Psychological assessment
The 26-item Eating Attitudes Test (EAT-26) [54] was administered to all participants to evaluate their eating behavior and severity of eating disorder. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised was administered to evaluate intelligence and exclude the possible influence of hidden impairment of intellectual functioning. Full-scale intelligence quotient (IQ), Verbal IQ, and performance IQ scores were calculated. The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) Japanese version [55] was administered to all participants. Depressive and anxiety disorders are prevalent comorbidities of AN patients, as described above. On the MMPI, an elevated score on scale 2, depression (D), indicates feelings of depression and sadness, and an elevated score on scale 7, psychasthenia (Pt), indicates generalized feelings of anxiety and discomfort [56]. Therefore, we report the T scores for MMPI scale 2 and 7.
4. Task
The WCST was administered in the same manner as in our previous fMRI study on patients with irritable bowel syndrome [53]. In brief, the WCST task presentation was computerized with the pictures rear-projected onto the screen of the MRI scanner. Four fixed reference cards were displayed at the corners of the screen. Participants looked at the screen through a periscope mirror mounted on the head coil. The graphic forms on the four cards all had the elements of color, shape, and number: “one red star”, “two green squares”, “three yellow crosses”, and “four blue circles”. A test card was presented in the center of the screen. The participant held a switch box in the right hand and clicked a button to select one of the four reference cards that she judged to have the same kind of element of the test card. Three different matching rules were used in the judgment: color matching, shape matching, and number matching. Participants were not informed about the matching rules applied during the test. An “O” symbol was used as feedback for a correct response ( Fig. 1 ) and an “X” symbol for an error. When five consecutive responses were correctly judged, the rule was changed without notice. The participant would realize a rule change when receiving the error symbol and would need to change the response strategy; that is, shift the cognitive set, a function strongly correlated with WCST performance [57]. Problems in set shifting may manifest as impaired cognitive flexibility [58]. The participants were fully trained in the task on a personal computer prior to scanning. One trial consisted of a period of rest, task presentation, card selection, and feedback. A total of 128 trials were conducted in a single session lasting 17 minutes and 6 seconds. We programmed a rule change after five consecutive correct responses were made and a jitter during the resting period and when waiting for feedback ( Fig. 1 ). To determine task performance, we calculated from the responses the correct rate of selection, total error rate, perseverative error rate, and non-perseverative error rate. Perseverative error, an index of persistency, was defined as a response following a rule that had been correct earlier but was incorrect later [38].
5. Image acquisition
We acquired images on a 1.5 T Siemens Magnetom Symphony® MRI scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using a standard two-channel head coil at Sendai Nakae Hospital. A time-course series of 342 volumes was acquired with T2*-weighted gradient-echo-planer imaging sequences depicting BOLD contrasts during the task. Each volume consisted of 30 slices parallel to the anterior commissure-posterior commissure line in ascending order. Repetition time was 100 ms per slice (total repetition time 3000 ms) with an echo time of 62 ms and a flip angle of 90°. The field of view was 192 mm and the matrix size was 64×64, giving a voxel dimension of 3.0×3.0×5.0 mm with no gaps. Structural scans were acquired using a T1-weighted gradient echo pulse sequence, which facilitated localization.
6. fMRI data analysis
Image processing and statistical analysis was performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM 5; (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) implemented in Matlab, version 7 (Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA). The first two volumes of the fMRI scans were discarded because of unsteady magnetization. Each set of functional volumes was realigned to the first scan with allowed motion limited to ±1 mm translation and ±1 degree rotation. The images were corrected for differences in slice acquisition timing and were spatially normalized to a standard template based on the Montreal Neurological Institute reference brain. Volumes were smoothed using an 8-mm full width, half maximum Gaussian filter. Low-frequency signal drifts were removed using a 128-s high-pass filter. As prefrontal cortex activity is reported to be increased in response to negative or positive feedback and to be greater than the response to matching [45], [59], we analyzed the brain activity in response to feedback. In the first level analysis, a General Linear Model (GLM) was fitted to data for a single participant using six regressors of interest, namely set shifting error feedback and first to fifth feedback in a series of five consecutive correct responses (referred to hereafter as “first correct feedback”), which were modeled as a stick function and convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function. Responses to set shifting error feedback and first correct feedback were considered for analysis. Both set shifting error feedback and first correct feedback signaled to the participant that the situation had changed. The first correct feedback signaled to the participant to keep the strategy (cognitive set keeping), whereas the set shifting error feedback signaled to shift the cognitive set. Thus, with this contrast we could extract brain activity specific to set shifting. For the group analysis, contrast images from each participant were entered into a hierarchical model equivalent to a random-effects model. Group activation maps used a height threshold of p<0.001 (uncorrected) and clusters were considered statistically significant at cluster-level p<0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons across the whole brain. We used a one-way ANOVA to evaluate brain activity in response to set shifting error feedback vs. first correct feedback among the ANR, ANBP and control participants. Voxel-wise significance was set at p<0.05 (family wise error (FWE) corrected). Post-hoc t-tests between all combinations of two out of the three groups were done using Bonferroni correction. Voxel-wise significance was set at p<0.00033 and cluster-wise significance at p<0.017. Anatomical labeling of peak coordinates was done using Talairach Client ver. 2.42 (http://www.talairach.org/). Correlational analysis was performed for task performance, demographic data, and brain activity in the regions that showed a significant difference between the AN patients and control groups. We calculated the mean contrast value of a spherical region of interest centered at the peak voxel of the cluster and with a diameter of 6 mm. MarsBar [60], a toolbox for SPM, was used for this purpose. Values of p<0.05 were considered significant.
7. Statistical analysis
We used JMP Pro® ver.9 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) for statistical analysis of demographic and WCST task performance data. The AN and HC groups were compared with Student's two sample t-test. A one-side t-test was performed for task performance as previous studies on the WCST reported that AN patients showed significantly poor performance [17], [20]–[22]. We performed correlation analysis between demographic data and WCST performance for each group (HC and all AN patients). To compare demographic/clinical characteristics and WCST performance of the two subgroups of AN (ANR and ANBP) and HC, we used a Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Significance was set at p<0.05. Multiple comparisons between all combinations of two out of the three groups were done using the Steel-Dwass method, with significance set at p<0.05.
Results
1. Demographic data and behavior results
There were no differences in age or IQ between the groups. BMI was significantly lower in patients than in controls (p<0.0001, Table 1 ). Duration of AN was 3.6±3.7 (mean ± SD) years, with no significant difference between the two AN subgroups (ANR 3.6±3.6 years, ANBP 3.5±4.1 years) (Table 1). EAT-26 score was significantly higher in AN patients than in controls (p = 0.0002, Table 1 ). AN patients had significantly higher T scores on MMPI scale 2 (depression, p = 0.0465) and 7 (anxiety, p = 0.0276) than the controls ( Table 1 ). AN patients showed a significantly lower correct rate on the WCST than controls (p = 0.0420, Table 2 ). None of the other performance data differed between the controls and AN patients. Neither BMI nor MMPI scale 2 and 7 scores were correlated with WCST performance in the controls or AN patients. One-way ANOVA of the demographic/clinical characteristics and WCST performance for the ANR, ANBP, and control participants showed no significant results. Multiple comparison revealed the ANR and ANBP patients had significantly lower BMI and EAT-26 score than the controls ( Table 1 ). Multiple comparison showed no other significant results.
Table 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Patients with Anorexia Nervosa and Healthy Controls.
Characteristic | AN (n = 15) | ANR (n = 9) | ANBP (n = 6) | HC (n = 15) | p |
Age (years) | 23±7 | 21±5 | 26±9 | 22±3 | 0.4997 |
Body mass index (kg/m2) | 14.6±1.5 | 14.7±1.9a | 14.5±0.7a | 20.6±1.2 | <0.0001** |
duration (years) | 3.6±3.7 | 3.6±3.6 | 3.5±4.1 | - | - |
Full-scale IQ | 97.8±13.7 | 99.6±15.4 | 95.2±11.4 | 104.9±11.3 | 0.1344 |
Verbal IQ | 97.5±12.7 | 100.4±13.7 | 93.2±10.6 | 104.5±12.8 | 0.1440 |
Performance IQ | 98.0±13.9 | 98.6±15.9 | 97.2±11.8 | 104.5±12.6 | 0.1942 |
MMPI Scale 2 | 65.8±17.3 | 68.9±17.6 | 61.2±17.3 | 55.2±8.8 | 0.0465* |
MMPI Scale 7 | 65.2±14.2 | 66.1±15.3 | 63.8±13.7 | 55.6±6.5 | 0.0276* |
EAT-26 | 25.3±15.9 | 21.3±12.1a | 31.2+20.2a | 4.3±4.6 | 0.0002** |
AN: all anorexia nervosa patients, ANR: restrictive anorexia nervosa patients, ANBP: binge-purge anorexia nervosa patients, HC: healthy controls, IQ: intelligent quatient, MMPI: Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory, EAT-26: 26-item Eating Attitudes Test. Mean ± SD,
p<0.05,
p<0.01, two sample t-test between all AN patients and HC.
: p<0.01, multiple comparison vs. HC using the Steel-Dwass method.
Table 2. Task Performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.
Performance (%) | AN (n = 15) | ANR (n = 9) | ANBP (n = 6) | HC (n = 15) | p |
Correct rate | 70.0±8.8 | 72.1±9.0 | 66.8±8.2 | 74.7±4.8 | 0.0420* |
Total error rate | 23.5±4.0 | 21.7±2.6 | 26.2±4.4 | 23.6±4.7 | 0.4743 |
Perseverative error rate | 6.8±5.6 | 6.1±7.1 | 7.9±1.9 | 6.4±4.3 | 0.3995 |
Non-perseverative error rate | 16.7±4.3 | 15.6±4.8 | 18.2±3.0 | 17.2±1.1 | 0.3105 |
AN: all anorexia nervosa patients, ANR: restrictive anorexia nervosa patients, ANBP: binge-purge anorexia nervosa patients,HC: healthy controls. Mean ± SD,
p<0.05, two sample t-test between all AN patients and HC.
2. Imaging results
Clusters of significant brain activation are shown in Figure 2 and Table 3 – 8 . In the tables, cerebellar activations are not shown because activity in the cerebral cortical and subcortical regions is the focus of this study. Healthy controls showed significantly more activity in the DLPFC and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), cingulate cortex, insula, occipital cortex, parahippocampal cortex (PHC), and basal ganglia of both hemispheres at set shifting than at first correct feedback ( Table 3 , Fig. 2A ). The all-AN patient group showed significantly more activity in response to set shifting in bilateral occipital cortices, bilateral insula, bilateral basal ganglia, and bilateral cerebellum than to first correct feedback ( Table 4 , Fig. 2B ). ANR patients showed significantly more activity in response to set shifting in the putamen, insula, and caudate head (Table 5). ANBP patients showed no significant activation. In the group comparison between the all-AN patients and controls, AN patients showed poorer activity than the controls in the right VLPFC (BA47) and bilateral PHC ( Table 6 , Fig. 2C ). AN patients did not show higher brain activity than the controls in any brain region. Whole brain one-way ANOVA among the two subgroups of AN patients and controls showed significant clusters in the cingulate cortex, putamen, and insula (Table 7). ANBP patients showed significantly lower activity in right VLPFC than controls (Table 8). Multiple comparison showed no other significant results.
Table 3. Brain Activity of Healthy Controls (n = 15) on Set Shifting Error Feedback vs. Activity on First Correct Feedback of Five Consecutive Corrects.
L/R | Area | BA | x | y | z | T value | voxel | p |
L | Insula | 13 | −35 | 14 | −5 | 10.91 | 4379 | 0.000 |
L | Parahippocampal gyrus | 19 | −17 | −48 | −4 | 10.29 | 3793 | 0.000 |
L | Cingulate gyrus | 32, 24 | −9 | 22 | 33 | 10.18 | 2034 | 0.000 |
R | Insula | 13 | 34 | 18 | 0 | 7.27 | 1944 | 0.000 |
R | Anterior cingulate | 10 | 21 | 45 | −3 | 5.56 | 228 | 0.002 |
L/R: Left/Right, BA: Brodmann area, x, y, z: Talairach coordinates of the peak voxel. 1 voxel = 2 mm×2 mm×2 mm. p: cluster-wise p value (corrected). Significance levels were voxel-wise p<0.001 (uncorrected) and cluster-wise p<0.05 (corrected), one sample t-test.
Table 8. Post-hoc Group Comparison between Binge-purge Anorexia Nervosa Patients (n = 6) and Healthy Controls (n = 15) for Brain Activity on Set Shifting Error Feedback vs. Activity on First Correct Feedback of Five Consecutive Corrects.
L/R | Area | BA | x | y | z | T score | voxel | p |
R | Inferior frontal gyrus | 47 | 48 | 38 | −15 | 5.48 | 116 | 0.006 |
L/R: Left/Right, BA: Brodmann area, x, y, z: Talairach coordinates of the peak voxel. 1 voxel = 2 mm×2 mm×2 mm. p: cluster-wise p value (corrected). Significance levels were voxel-wise p<0.00033 (Bonferroni corrected) and cluster-wise p<0.017 (Bonferroni corrected), post-hoc two sample t-test.
Table 4. Brain Activity of Anorexia Nervosa Patients (n = 15) on Set Shifting Error Feedback vs. Activity on First Correct Feedback of Five Consecutive Corrects.
L/R | Area | BA | x | y | z | T value | voxel | p |
L | Putamen | - | −17 | 3 | 4 | 8.86 | 838 | 0.000 |
R | Insula | 13 | 34 | 17 | 7 | 7.94 | 265 | 0.000 |
R | Globus pallidus | - | 11 | 0 | 3 | 6.55 | 435 | 0.000 |
L | Middle occipital gyrus | 19, 18 | −34 | −92 | 8 | 6.28 | 229 | 0.000 |
L | Middle frontal gyrus | 8 | −7 | 18 | 44 | 6.10 | 95 | 0.020 |
R | Middle occipital gyrus | 18, 19 | 29 | −89 | 3 | 5.22 | 205 | 0.000 |
L/R: Left/Right, BA: Brodmann area, x, y, z: Talairach coordinates of the peak voxel. 1 voxel = 2 mm×2 mm×2 mm. p: cluster-wise corrected p value (corrected). Significance levels were voxel-wise p<0.001 (uncorrected) and cluster-wise p<0.05 (corrected), one sample t-test.
Table 5. Brain Activity of Restrictive Anorexia Nervosa Patients (n = 9) on Set Shifting Error Feedback vs. Activity on First Correct Feedback of Five Consecutive Corrects.
L/R | Area | BA | x | y | z | T score | Voxel | p |
L | Claustrum | - | −30 | 21 | −1 | 9.05 | 101 | 0.000 |
L | Putamen | - | −16 | 8 | 0 | 8.02 | 118 | 0.000 |
R | Insula | 13 | 36 | 20 | 3 | 6.81 | 104 | 0.000 |
R | Caudate head | - | 8 | 4 | 3 | 6.58 | 96 | 0.001 |
L/R: Left/Right, BA: Brodmann area, x, y, z: Talairach coordinates of the peak voxel. 1 voxel = 2 mm×2 mm×2 mm. p: cluster-wise corrected p value. Significance levels were voxel-wise p<0.001 (uncorrected) and cluster-wise p<0.05 (corrected), one sample t-test.
Table 6. Group Comparison between Anorexia Nervosa Patients (n = 15) and Healthy Controls (n = 15) for Brain Activity on Set Shifting Error Feedback vs. Activity on First Correct Feedback of Five Consecutive Corrects.
L/R | Area | BA | x | y | z | T value | voxel | p |
R | Parahippocampal gyrus | 19, 27 | 27 | −50 | 0 | 5.44 | 161 | 0.022 |
L | Parahippocampal gyrus | 30 | −9 | −40 | 0 | 5.16 | 152 | 0.028 |
R | Inrerior Frontal gyrus | 47 | 48 | 38 | −13 | 5.15 | 199 | 0.008 |
L/R: Left/Right, BA: Brodmann area, x, y, z: Talairach coordinates of the peak voxel. 1 voxel = 2 mm×2 mm×2 mm. p: cluster-wise p value (corrected). Significance levels were voxel-wise p<0.001 (uncorrected) and cluster-wise p<0.05 (corrected), two sample t-test.
Table 7. One-way ANOVA of Brain Activity among Healthy Control Women (n = 15), Restrictive Anorexia Nervosa Patients (n = 9), and Binge-Purge Anorexia Nervosa Patients (n = 6) on Set Shifting Error Feedback vs. Activity on First Correct Feedback of Five Consecutive Corrects.
L/R | Area | BA | x | y | z | F score | voxel | p |
L | Cingulate gyrus | 32 | −8 | 23 | 34 | 30.6 | 30 | 0.002 |
L | Putamen | - | −16 | 8 | 0 | 29.32 | 66 | 0.002 |
R | Insula | 13 | 38 | 19 | −1 | 25.08 | 17 | 0.012 |
L/R: Left/Right, BA: Brodmann area, x, y, z: Talairach coordinates of the peak voxel. 1 voxel = 2 mm×2 mm×2 mm. p: voxel-wise p value (Family Wise Error corrected). Significance level was p<0.05, one-way ANOVA. Degree of freedom = [3.0, 27, 0].
Individual mean contrast values for right VLPFC activation on set shifting error feedback greater than activity on first correct feedback showed a positive correlation with correct rate in controls (r = 0.51, p = 0.0499) but not in AN patients (r = −0.18, p = 0.5176) ( Fig. 3A ). In controls, the higher the right VLPFC activity was in response to set shifting feedback, the higher the correct rate achieved. In contrast, in AN patients, right VLPFC activity was not related to the correct rate. Neither controls nor AN patients showed a significant correlation between BMI and activity in any brain region of interest ( Fig. 3B shows a correlation between BMI and right VLPFC activity). A significant negative correlation was found between age and left PHC activity in AN patients (r = −0.60, p = 0.0176) but not in controls (r = 0.23, p = 0.4208) ( Fig. 3C ). Left PHC activity in AN lessened with advancing age.
Discussion
This study showed that the correct rate on the WCST was poorer for AN patients than healthy controls, but there was no difference in perseverative error rate between the two groups. This result is consistent with an earlier study that found adolescents with ANR showed more total errors on the WCST than IQ-matched controls, but the same perseverative errors as the controls [61]. Other earlier WCST studies comparing AN patients with controls at the same educational level showed that AN patients had poorer performance in regard to total error and perseverative error [17], [20]–[22], [47], but the IQ of participants in these studies were not matched. One large study reported that AN patients were more perseverant than healthy controls on the WCST [62], but neither educational level nor IQ was mentioned in the study. Given our findings, if the IQ of healthy controls had been matched with that of AN patients in these previous studies, the patients would likely have had fewer perseverative errors on the WCST. BMI showed no correlation with WCST performance in either the controls or AN patients in the present study, which is consistent with the findings of earlier studies on cognitive flexibility in AN patients [17], [23], [26]. These results suggest that impaired cognitive flexibility in AN patients is not a state due to starvation but is a trait of the disease.
Our results for the controls showed set shifting-related activity in the LPFC, cingulate cortex, PHC, and basal ganglia in both hemispheres. These findings are consistent with earlier neuroimaging studies on WCST performance in normal healthy participants [63]. By contrast, our control participants showed no activity in the parietal region, an area shown to be activated in many WCST neuroimaging studies [45], [59], [64]–[66]. This conflicting finding was probably because we subtracted the response to first correct feedback from the response to set shifting error feedback. Parietal cortex activation has been reported for both positive and negative feedback [45], and the magnitude of response to set shifting error feedback in the parietal cortex may be similar to that for first correct feedback.
AN patients showed higher activation in the cingulate cortex and striatum, insula, and occipital cortex during set shifting in the present study. Several earlier studies of AN patients reported higher activity in these regions in a variety of conditions [5], [32], [35], [67], [68]. Adolescent AN patients before treatment showed higher activity in the occipital and cingulate cortices during a working memory task than they did after treatment [35]. Recovered AN women showed greater hemodynamic activation in the caudate than control women [32], and AN patients showed greater activation in the insula during a body image task [5], [67], [68]. However, in the lateral frontal regions, which are thought to be crucial for set shifting [46], our AN patients showed no activation.
The AN patients in our study showed a poorer response in the right VLPFC (BA47) and bilateral PHC (BA19, 30). ANBP patients also showed significantly lower activity than controls in the right VLPFC, a region which was also highlighted in the group comparison results between controls and all AN patients. This result is different from that of an earlier study on set shifting in AN patients [33] which followed a paradigm by Shafritz et al. focusing on the difference between behavioral response shifting and cognitive set shifting [46]. Shafritz et al. observed activation in the DLPFC, ACC, and inferior parietal lobe during behavioral response shifting, but in the VLPFC, ACC, and striatum during cognitive set shifting. In a study by Zastrow et al., AN patients showed a poorer response than controls in the ACC, ventral striatum, and thalamus on behavioral response shifting, but the patients demonstrated no deficit in cognitive set shifting [33]. Our AN patients showed hypoactivity in the VLPFC in response to set shifting feedback, but full activation of the ACC and striatum, other areas involved in cognitive set shifting. The difference between the two studies probably derives from the difference in paradigms. The WCST requires participants to create a new cognitive set on the rule change, whereas the task used in the earlier study was designed to present a new behavioral strategy overtly, which required a passive behavioral change (i.e. behavioral response shifting). The covert rule change on the WCST in our study meant that participants made cognitive set shifts voluntarily, which was why we decided to use the WCST.
The VLPFC is thought to be one of the regions critical for set shifting ability. This region, where a group difference was observed in our study, did not show significant activation in healthy participants on the single-group statistical test, but correlational analysis showed that controls with better performance on the WCST showed better brain activity in this region. Some previous studies have reported activation in the VLPFC on the WCST [45], [59]. VLPFC activity was shown to be increased specifically in response to negative feedback, prompting a change in the task [45]. Shifts in cognitive set were found to be mediated by the VLPFC, ACC, and striatum [46], and to occur specifically in these areas with the inhibition of previously acquired stimulus–response rules and the acquisition of new stimulus–response associations [59]. Activity change in the right VLPFC was demonstrated on stopping the response to the previously relevant stimulus and shifting it in response to the newly relevant stimulus [69]. In our study, the VLPFC in AN patients was less activated in response to set shifting error feedback, which may explain the impaired cognitive flexibility reported in AN patients.
Bilateral activation of the PHC was observed in our healthy controls. In the group comparison, the controls showed greater activation of the PHC than AN patients. Poor parahippocampal activity seen in our AN patients is consistent with some earlier studies. Lower parahippocampal activity was observed in AN patients viewing their own body [4], and healthy women showed more activity in the left PHC in response to negative words about body image, whereas AN patients show no activity in the PHC [6]. In an electroencephalography study on healthy participants, delta wave activity was increased in the PHC, PFC, ACC, and other cortical-subcortical regions during the WCST [70]. Regional cerebral blood flow of healthy participants, evaluated with positron emission tomography, was also shown to be increased in the left PHC during the modified card sorting test [71]. The PHC is thought to be part of a widespread neural network involved in efficient WCST performance [63] and appears to be activated during future event simulation [72]. Many patients with eating disorder have difficulty in talking about recovery (i.e., a better future) or to have less ability to imagine for themselves [73]. Our result may explain AN patients' deficit in future imagination, a function that is important for problem solving.
We found no correlation between BMI and brain activity in any of three regions (right VLPFC, right PHC, and left PHC) in which AN patients showed hypoactivity in our study.Furthermore, all three such regions showed no correlation with other characteristics of AN symptomatology, namely duration and EAT-26 score. Our results support the speculation that impaired cognitive flexibility in AN patients is a trait, not a state due to starvation. Left parahippocampal activation on set shifting showed negative correlation with age in AN patients. This may reflect chronicity of the disease. Further investigation is needed to clarify the effect of chronicity.
Our study has several limitations. First, the small sample size limits statistical power. It did not allow us to investigate the difference between the two subtypes of AN (ANR and ANBP) Whole brain one-way ANOVA among the ANR, ANBP and control participants showed significant clusters in the cingulate cortex, putamen, and insula. However, post-hoc analysis showed no difference in these regions, perhaps because of the small sample size. Investigation with a larger sample size is needed. Second, we could not completely exclude the influence of psychiatric comorbidity (e.g., depression, anxiety, obsessive-compulsive disorder, or borderline personality disorder) on our results. In regard to the possible effect of depression, our AN patients showed significantly higher MMPI depression score than controls. One study has reported that set shifting ability during several cognitive tasks including the WCST was intact in AN patients withoutcomorbid depression [28]. In our study, none of the WCST performance scores were correlated with the depression scale score of AN patients or controls. Moreover, AN patients' hypoactive regions in response to set shifting feedback showed no correlation with depression score. We also found AN patients had a higher MMPI psychasthenia score than controls. There was no correlation between MMPI psychasthenia scale score and WCST performance, and psychasthenia score had no correlation with brain activity where AN patients showed hypoactivity in response to set shifting feedback.. Thus, depression or anxiety at the least unlikely affected our results. Third, a possible confounding factor in our study is that 4 patients took an SSRI. However, blockade of the serotonin transporter by SSRIs has been documented for the midbrain, striatum, amygdala and other subcortical areas [74] but not for the LPFC or PHC. Further studies are needed to clarify the effects of regional brain dysfunction in AN patients.
In conclusion, women AN patients showed poorer WCST performance than healthy control women. They also showed set shifting-specific hypoactivity in the VLPFC and PHC. Such hypoactivity in the brain of AN patients may be responsible for their impaired cognitive flexibility.
Funding Statement
This study was funded by Research Grant No. 17B-1 for Nervous and Mental Disorders from the Ministry of Health, Labor and Welfare of Japan. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The authors disclose no financial arrangement (e.g., consultancies, stock ownership, equity interests, parent-licensing arrangements, research support, major honoraria) they may have with a company whose product figures prominently in the submitted manuscript.
References
- 1.American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition, text revision ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
- 2. Wagner A, Ruf M, Braus D, Schmidt M (2003) Neuronal activity changes and body image distortion in anorexia nervosa. Neuroreport 14: 2193–2197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Uher R, Murphy T, Friederich H, Dalgleish T, Brammer M, et al. (2005) Functional neuroanatomy of body shape perception in healthy and eating-disordered women. Biol Psychiatry 58: 990–997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Vocks S, Busch M, Grönemeyer D, Schulte D, Herpertz S, et al. (2010) Neural correlates of viewing photographs of one's own body and another woman's body in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: an fMRI study. J Psychiatry Neurosci 35: 163–176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Mohr H, Zimmermann J, Röder C, Lenz C, Overbeck G, et al. (2010) Separating two components of body image in anorexia nervosa using fMRI. Psychol Med 40: 1519–1529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Miyake Y, Okamoto Y, Onoda K, Kurosaki M, Shirao N, et al. (2010) Brain activation during the perception of distorted body images in eating disorders. Psychiatry Res 181: 183–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Uher R, Brammer M, Murphy T, Campbell I, Ng V, et al. (2003) Recovery and chronicity in anorexia nervosa: brain activity associated with differential outcomes. Biol Psychiatry 54: 934–942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Wagner A, Aizenstein H, Mazurkewicz L, Fudge J, Frank G, et al. (2008) Altered insula response to taste stimuli in individuals recovered from restricting-type anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 513–523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Brooks SJ, O'Daly OG, Uher R, Friederich HC, Giampietro V, et al. (2011) Differential neural responses to food images in women with bulimia versus anorexia nervosa. PLoS One 6: e22259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Palazidou E, Robinson P, Lishman WA (1990) Neuroradiological and neuropsychological assessment in anorexia nervosa. Psychol Med 20: 521–527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Jones BP, Duncan CC, Brouwers P, Mirsky AF (1991) Cognition in eating disorders. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 13: 711–728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Szmukler GI, Andrewes D, Kingston K, Chen L, Stargatt R, et al. (1992) Neuropsychological impairment in anorexia nervosa: before and after refeeding. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 14: 347–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Kingston K, Szmukler G, Andrewes D, Tress B, Desmond P (1996) Neuropsychological and structural brain changes in anorexia nervosa before and after refeeding. Psychol Med 26: 15–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Seed JA, Dixon RA, McCluskey SE, Young AH (2000) Basal activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and cognitive function in anorexia nervosa. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 250: 11–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Green MW, Elliman NA, Wakeling A, Rogers PJ (1996) Cognitive functioning, weight change and therapy in anorexia nervosa. J Psychiatr Res 30: 401–410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Nikendei C, Funiok C, Pfüller U, Zastrow A, Aschenbrenner S, et al. (2011) Memory performance in acute and weight-restored anorexia nervosa patients. Psychol Med 41: 829–838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Fassino S, Pieró A, Daga G, Leombruni P, Mortara P, et al. (2002) Attentional biases and frontal functioning in anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 31: 274–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Tchanturia K, Serpell L, Troop N, Treasure J (2001) Perceptual illusions in eating disorders: rigid and fluctuating styles. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 32: 107–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Tchanturia K, Anderluh M, Morris R, Rabe-Hesketh S, Collier D, et al. (2004) Cognitive flexibility in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 10: 513–520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Steinglass J, Walsh B, Stern Y (2006) Set shifting deficit in anorexia nervosa. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 12: 431–435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Nakazato M, Tchanturia K, Schmidt U, Campbell I, Treasure J, et al. (2009) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and set-shifting in currently ill and recovered anorexia nervosa (AN) patients. Psychol Med 39: 1029–1035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Nakazato M, Hashimoto K, Schmidt U, Tchanturia K, Campbell I, et al. (2010) Serum glutamine, set-shifting ability and anorexia nervosa. Ann Gen Psychiatry 9: 29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Roberts M, Tchanturia K, Treasure J (2010) Exploring the neurocognitive signature of poor set-shifting in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. J Psychiatr Res [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Tchanturia K, Campbell I, Morris R, Treasure J (2005) Neuropsychological studies in anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 37 Suppl: S72–76 discussion S87–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Schmidt U, Treasure J (2006) Anorexia nervosa: valued and visible. A cognitive-interpersonal maintenance model and its implications for research and practice. Br J Clin Psychol 45: 343–366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Holliday J, Tchanturia K, Landau S, Collier D, Treasure J (2005) Is impaired set-shifting an endophenotype of anorexia nervosa? Am J Psychiatry 162: 2269–2275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Tchanturia K, Morris R, Anderluh M, Collier D, Nikolaou V, et al. (2004) Set shifting in anorexia nervosa: an examination before and after weight gain, in full recovery and relationship to childhood and adult OCPD traits. J Psychiatr Res 38: 545–552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Giel KE, Wittorf A, Wolkenstein L, Klingberg S, Drimmer E, et al. (2012) Is impaired set-shifting a feature of “pure” anorexia nervosa? Investigating the role of depression in set-shifting ability in anorexia nervosa and unipolar depression. Psychiatry Res 200: 538–543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. O'Brien KM, Vincent NK (2003) Psychiatric comorbidity in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: nature, prevalence, and causal relationships. Clin Psychol Rev 23: 57–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Kaye WH, Bulik CM, Thornton L, Barbarich N, Masters K (2004) Comorbidity of anxiety disorders with anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry 161: 2215–2221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Godt K (2008) Personality disorders in 545 patients with eating disorders. Eur Eat Disord Rev 16: 94–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Wagner A, Aizenstein H, Venkatraman V, Fudge J, May J, et al. (2007) Altered reward processing in women recovered from anorexia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry 164: 1842–1849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Zastrow A, Kaiser S, Stippich C, Walther S, Herzog W, et al. (2009) Neural correlates of impaired cognitive-behavioral flexibility in anorexia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry 166: 608–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Oberndorfer TA, Kaye WH, Simmons AN, Strigo IA, Matthews SC (2011) Demand-specific alteration of medial prefrontal cortex response during an inhibition task in recovered anorexic women. Int J Eat Disord 44: 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Castro-Fornieles J, Caldú X, Andrés-Perpiñá S, Lázaro L, Bargalló N, et al. (2010) A cross-sectional and follow-up functional MRI study with a working memory task in adolescent anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychologia 48: 4111–4116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Lock J, Garrett A, Beenhakker J, Reiss AL (2011) Aberrant brain activation during a response inhibition task in adolescent eating disorder subtypes. Am J Psychiatry 168: 55–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Grant DA, Berg EA (1948) A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. J Exp Psychol 38: 404–411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Milner B (1963) Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting – role of frontal lobes. Archives of Neurology 9: 90-&. [Google Scholar]
- 39. Stuss DT, Levine B, Alexander MP, Hong J, Palumbo C, et al. (2000) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with focal frontal and posterior brain damage: effects of lesion location and test structure on separable cognitive processes. Neuropsychologia 38: 388–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Nelson HE (1976) Modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex 12: 313–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Berman K, Ostrem J, Randolph C, Gold J, Goldberg T, et al. (1995) Physiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: a positron emission tomography study. Neuropsychologia 33: 1027–1046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I, Kameyama M, Nakahara K, et al. (1998) Transient activation of inferior prefrontal cortex during cognitive set shifting. Nat Neurosci 1: 80–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I, Kikyo H, Kameyama M, et al. (1999) Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI. Brain 122 (Pt 5) 981–991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Konishi S, Kawazu M, Uchida I, Kikyo H, Asakura I, et al. (1999) Contribution of working memory to transient activation in human inferior prefrontal cortex during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Cereb Cortex 9: 745–753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Monchi O, Petrides M, Petre V, Worsley K, Dagher A (2001) Wisconsin Card Sorting revisited: distinct neural circuits participating in different stages of the task identified by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 21: 7733–7741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Shafritz K, Kartheiser P, Belger A (2005) Dissociation of neural systems mediating shifts in behavioral response and cognitive set. Neuroimage 25: 600–606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Abbate-Daga G, Buzzichelli S, Amianto F, Rocca G, Marzola E, et al. (2011) Cognitive flexibility in verbal and nonverbal domains and decision making in anorexia nervosa patients: a pilot study. BMC Psychiatry 11: 162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9: 97–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. van Holstein M, Aarts E, van der Schaaf ME, Geurts DE, Verkes RJ, et al. (2011) Human cognitive flexibility depends on dopamine D2 receptor signaling. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 218: 567–578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Stewart SA (2005) The effects of benzodiazepines on cognition. J Clin Psychiatry 66 Suppl 2: 9–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Barker MJ, Greenwood KM, Jackson M, Crowe SF (2004) Cognitive effects of long-term benzodiazepine use: a meta-analysis. CNS Drugs 18: 37–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Robbe HW, O'Hanlon JF (1995) Acute and subchronic effects of paroxetine 20 and 40 mg on actual driving, psychomotor performance and subjective assessments in healthy volunteers. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 5: 35–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Aizawa E, Sato Y, Kochiyama T, Saito N, Izumiyama M, et al. (2012) Altered cognitive function of prefrontal cortex during error feedback in patients with irritable bowel syndrome, based on FMRI and dynamic causal modeling. Gastroenterology 143: 1188–1198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Garner D (1981) Body image in anorexia nervosa. Can J Psychiatry 26: 224–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.New Japan Committee of MMPI (1997) Study of Standardization for the New Japanese MMPI. Kyoto: San-Kyo-Bo.
- 56. Rocca WA, Grossardt BR, Peterson BJ, Bower JH, Trenerry MR, et al. (2006) The Mayo Clinic cohort study of personality and aging: design and sampling, reliability and validity of instruments, and baseline description. Neuroepidemiology 26: 119–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, et al. (2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol 41: 49–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Roberts M, Tchanturia K, Stahl D, Southgate L, Treasure J (2007) A systematic review and meta-analysis of set-shifting ability in eating disorders. Psychol Med 37: 1075–1084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Lie C, Specht K, Marshall J, Fink G (2006) Using fMRI to decompose the neural processes underlying the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuroimage 30: 1038–1049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Brett M, Anton JL, Valabregue R, Poline JP (2002) Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox [abstract] Presented at the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, June 2–6, 2002, Sendai, Japan. Available on CD-ROM in NeuroImage 16. [Google Scholar]
- 61. McAnarney ER, Zarcone J, Singh P, Michels J, Welsh S, et al. (2011) Restrictive anorexia nervosa and set-shifting in adolescents: a biobehavioral interface. J Adolesc Health 49: 99–101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Tchanturia K, Davies H, Roberts M, Harrison A, Nakazato M, et al. (2012) Poor cognitive flexibility in eating disorders: examining the evidence using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task. PLoS One 7: e28331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Nyhus E, Barceló F (2009) The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and the cognitive assessment of prefrontal executive functions: a critical update. Brain Cogn 71: 437–451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Asari T, Konishi S, Jimura K, Miyashita Y (2005) Multiple components of lateral posterior parietal activation associated with cognitive set shifting. Neuroimage 26: 694–702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Konishi S, Morimoto H, Jimura K, Asari T, Chikazoe J, et al. (2008) Differential superior prefrontal activity on initial versus subsequent shifts in naive subjects. Neuroimage 41: 575–580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Nose I, Murai J, Taira M (2009) Disclosing concealed information on the basis of cortical activations. Neuroimage 44: 1380–1386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Fladung AK, Grön G, Grammer K, Herrnberger B, Schilly E, et al. (2010) A neural signature of anorexia nervosa in the ventral striatal reward system. Am J Psychiatry 167: 206–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Friederich H, Brooks S, Uher R, Campbell I, Giampietro V, et al. (2010) Neural correlates of body dissatisfaction in anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychologia 48: 2878–2885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Cools R, Clark L, Owen AM, Robbins TW (2002) Defining the neural mechanisms of probabilistic reversal learning using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 22: 4563–4567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. González-Hernández JA, Pita-Alcorta C, Cedeño I, Bosch-Bayard J, Galán-Garcia L, et al. (2002) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test synchronizes the prefrontal, temporal and posterior association cortex in different frequency ranges and extensions. Hum Brain Mapp 17: 37–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Nagahama Y, Fukuyama H, Yamauchi H, Matsuzaki S, Konishi J, et al. (1996) Cerebral activation during performance of a card sorting test. Brain 119 (Pt 5) 1667–1675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Schacter DL, Addis DR (2009) On the nature of medial temporal lobe contributions to the constructive simulation of future events. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364: 1245–1253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Malson H, Bailey L, Clarke S, Treasure J, Anderson G, et al. (2011) Un/imaginable future selves: a discourse analysis of in-patients' talk about recovery from an ‘eating disorder’. Eur Eat Disord Rev 19: 25–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Linden DE (2006) How psychotherapy changes the brain–the contribution of functional neuroimaging. Mol Psychiatry 11: 528–538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]