Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2013 Jun;7(6):675–679. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2013.02.006

Opinion: Sex, Gender and the Diagnosis of Autism — A Biosocial View of the Male Preponderance

Sylvie Goldman a,b,c
PMCID: PMC3655776  NIHMSID: NIHMS451550  PMID: 23687516

Barbara, Virginia, and Elaine were the three girls among 11 children whom Leo Kanner described in his seminal paper published in 1943. Hans Asperger did not report any girls in his original 1944 paper. Today in the United States, 1 in 88 individuals will be diagnosed with an Autism Spectrum Disorder and 1 in 4 will be a boy. Seventy years later, this opinion paper outlines different attempts to explain the lower ratio of girls diagnosed with autism – from the role of sex hormones and genetic susceptibility to the biases inherent to clinical diagnosis.

Autism Spectrum Disorders – autism for short – are behaviorally-defined neurodevelopmental disorders of mostly polygenic etiologies influenced by environmental factors. The best known yet less understood characteristic of autism is prominent, unexplained prevalence in males. From a biosocial perspective that encompasses early gender-based socialization practices, I propose that this striking sex difference may be enhanced by clinical expectations and by the gender-biased standardized instruments used to support the diagnosis.

The hypothesis presented in this article rests on the assumption that both sex and gender contribute, but in distinct ways, to the male preponderance in autism (Cheslack-Postava & Jordan-Young, 2012). With rarest of exceptions, sex is dichotomous and genetically determined at conception. Biological sex is expressed in the individual’s sexual dimorphisms, whereas gender emerges from early socialization processes. Whereas sex is biologically defined, gender is a psycho-social construct expressed through specific behaviors consistent with socio-cultural expectations derived from individuals’ genetic sex. Unlike the sex ratio in clinically diagnosed autism which is established merely by determining the number of males and females, scores on diagnostic instrument like the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) (Lord et al., 1989) or the parent report on the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) (Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994), may be influenced by the child’s expected behaviors in accordance with the prominent socialization gender attribute. So far the only widely used screening instrument using a differential gender cutoff is the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) (Constantino et al., 2003).

Let me raise the following two questions (1) what can be said about a child’s biological sex and its effect on autism, and (2) how do universal, culture-specific gendered socialization practices affect the sex ratio in diagnosing autism? The first question ought to be addressed in the larger context of autism being but one among many other developmental disorders, such as dyslexia, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), or conduct disorders which affect more boys than girls. The reasons for the male preponderance in these conditions remain unclear and, thus far, it is being explained by tantalizing, but highly speculative mechanisms. Some studies suggest that boys are more vulnerable to a variety of prenatal insults (e.g., infection, malnutrition, stress) (Gardener, 2011) and, for uncertain reasons, might be more susceptible to genetic mutations relevant to autism (Goin-Kochel, Abbacchi, & Constantino, 2007; Szatmari et al., 2012). Other studies suggest that girls are more resilient to these prenatal potentially deleterious factors. A favored explanation for such a protective feature is higher exposure to female sex hormones and to oxytocin at critical periods of fetal development (Carter, 2007; Yamasue, Kuwabara, Kawakubo, & Kasai, 2009). Oxytocin (OT) is a sexually dimorphic neuropeptide associated with attachment, affiliative, and maternal behaviors. OT is known to foster pro-social behaviors, including social recognition, social learning, and reproductive behavior in both animals and humans (Feldman, Gordon, Influs, Gutbir, & Ebstein, 2013; Insel, 1997). Furthermore, in rodents estrogens acting in the amygdala together with OT reduce social anxiety — frequently elevated in autism — and foster higher levels of positive social interactions (Lee, Macbeth, Pagani, & Young, III, 2009; Ross & Young, 2009). In doing so these hormones — in animals — apparently oppose the influence of testosterone-dependent social arousal and anxiety (Lukas & Neumann, 2012). Several clinical trials are on their way testing the effect of daily nasal spray of OT on prosocial behaviors in ASD, however so far no results from longitudinal studies are available to assess the long term effects of these treatments in children (Miller, 2013).

According to one hypothesis, the male preponderance in autism could be merely due to the fact that girls need a larger genetic load to become symptomatic. Yet a recent large epidemiological study reports lack of evidence for genetic loading among families with affected girls (Goin-Kochel et al., 2007). Clearly, there is no shortage of complex explanations to envision how sex, primarily through its endocrine influences on the developing brain, may modulate the propensity to develop a neuro-developmental disorder like autism (Bale et al., 2010; Connors et al., 2008; Marco, Macri, & Laviola, 2011). Fetal testosterone (fT) plays a central role in human neurobehavioral sexual differentiation and in later social behavior (Bergman, Glover, Sarkar, Abbott, & O’Connor, 2010; Constantinescu & Hines, 2012). Thus, elevated testosterone exposure during critical periods of early development might be responsible for permanent behavioral changes. These influences have been demonstrated primarily in individuals who experienced marked prenatal hormone abnormalities associated with ambiguous genital development (e.g. congenital adrenal hyperplasia). Recently, Auyeung and colleagues examined the link between autistic traits and fT levels measured in amniotic fluid during routine amniocentesis and suggested that the brain basis of autistic traits may reflect individual differences in prenatal androgens and androgen-related genes (Auyeung, Taylor, Hackett, & Baron-Cohen, 2010; Knickmeyer, Baron-Cohen, Auyeung, & Ashwin, 2008).

In regard to epidemiological studies, it is worth remembering that the male prevalence of 4:1 in autism (Fombonne, 2009), is highly modulated by cognitive function. For instance, as intellectual functioning, namely IQ, decreases, the skewness of the sex ratio decreases to the extent that in severely cognitively impaired children with autism the sex ratio approaches 1:1. In contrast, when IQ increases, skewness of the sex ratio increases as well, which is illustrated in the 8:1 sex ratio in Asperger Syndrome, a form of autism with near-average to high intellectual function (Scott, Baron-Cohen, Bolton, & Brayne, 2002).

Thus the relationship between IQ and sex ratio seems to be biologically based, but might a diagnosis of autism perhaps also be related to the qualitative aspects of the clinical behaviorally-based diagnosis? (Banach et al., 2009; McLennan, Lord, & Schopler, 1993; Pilowsky, Yirmiya, Shulman, & Dover, 1998). Severe cognitive deficits may override and attenuate other more subtle gendered behaviors and characteristics of communication that contribute to making a diagnosis of autism (Kopp & Gillberg, 2011). Autism is not a disease like diabetes or Parkinson disease which is diagnosed on the basis of well-recognized and validated biological criteria. Instead, autism is a developmentally defined syndrome characterized by a set of behavioral symptoms, not on validated biomarkers (Walsh, Elsabbagh, Bolton, & Singh, 2011). Therefore to arrive at the diagnosis of autism, clinicians rely on a series of observations, and scores on descriptive behavioral tests based on the magnitude of departures from the expected norms at specific ages, often supplemented by parent questionnaires.

What is the role of gender during this diagnosis? The process of socialization begins at birth and differs by sex. A mother attends to her baby boy or girl’s emotions differently and her responses are moulded by to the baby’s sex. Similar gender-based differences are observed toward the baby’s motor behaviors. The same scenario will take place in the case of a child who only later will be diagnosed with autism, and the mother will naturally be influenced by the same psychological and cultural beliefs when interacting with that child. Children with autism do not come into the world with an “autism mark” on their forehead or with defining stigmata. Moreover, some children will not manifest clear symptoms until age 18 months or so when language deficits or highly routinized play become apparent. Thus, those children diagnosed in the second year of life are raised like any other children from the day they were born, according to their sex. They are perceived as girls or boys and are taught to play, talk, and interact in accordance with the particular gender-based rules of their families (Condry & Condry, 1976). Therefore, despite their symptomatic social impairment, most verbal children with autism presumably have sufficiently preserved sociability, especially in the language and play domains, to acquire many of the gender markers to conform to the main sex-based behavioral expectations of their culture. On the other hand, in non-verbal, severely cognitively impaired children with limited communication skills, the typical gender traits may be less noticeable and thus boys and girls may be identified at a more comparable rate. At the other extreme, usual female higher verbal communication skills and social empathy may help girls with high functioning autism to more easily camouflage their social deficits. In turn, in boys the same social communication issues may readily be more salient and thus reinforce male identification (Attwood, 2006; Lai et al., 2011). Another significant issue related to the effect of gender in autism is the later identification of high functioning girls with autism possibly due to specific presentation of social and verbal abilities as well as variations in the frequency of specific types of co-morbid disorders (Begeer et al., 2012). These factors may play a role in the longer delay between parental concern and diagnosis in girls.

Biologic sex almost certainly bears the major responsibility for the male preponderance in autism, yet the issue of the sensitivity of the clinical diagnostic instruments needs to be raised. Germane to this discussion is the fact that the majority of the behavioral instruments used to diagnose autism have been standardized using male cohorts and very few studies have tested their sensitivity for identifying girls with autism (Kopp & Gillberg, 2011; Rinehart, Cornish, & Tonge, 2011). Also, the number of studies that examine common and sex-specific core symptoms of autism across age remain limited (Kozlowski, Matson, & Rieske, 2012; Rivet & Matson, 2011; Sipes, Matson, Worley.J.A, & Kozlowski, 2011). Lastly, although externalizing (e.g., aggressiveness, destructiveness) and internalizing behaviors (e.g., depression, anxiety) are known to be differentially distributed across the sexes, diagnostic instruments remain mostly sensitive to the presentation of the syndrome in males.

Conclusion

Clinical diagnosis is in the eyes of the beholder. Overall, girls are expected to be social, caring and empathic, therefore they must show greater positive symptoms like externalizing behaviors (e.g., aggressiveness, hyperactivity, severe lack of reciprocity) to be referred and diagnosed with autism (Bauminger, Solomon, & Rogers, 2010; Constantino & Charman, 2012; Dworzynski, Ronald, Bolton, & Happe, 2012). Stronger examiner bias toward male expectations may inflate the prevalence of autism in males, along with the fact that so far, the science of autism has been dominated by a male view of the disorder (e.g., test standardization, MRI samples, Baron-Cohen’s “extreme male theory” (Baron-Cohen, 2002)).

It is now undisputable that both male and female brain development requires interactions with the environment and that these interactions shape the expression of behaviors, in particular language and communication (Hines, 2011). In turn, continuous cross-talk between sex and gender contributes to differences in the autism phenotype and its variable outcome. The potential effect of gendered social environment on the expression of symptoms may be quite different for a boy and a girl with autism, to the point where, despite the same social deficit, a girl may be perceived as shy and a boy as unresponsive. These gendered perceptions of symptoms may contribute to the diagnostic bias toward increased identification of social psychopathology in boys (Giarelli et al., 2010; Lai et al., 2011; Worley.J.A & Matson.J.L., 2011). Furthermore, socialization being an ongoing developmental process, the outcome of autism in terms of symptom severity and success of remediation is no doubt influenced in part by hormonal changes during puberty, but also by reinforcement of behaviors shaped by perceived gender.

As long as we have such imperfect understanding of the pathophysiology and etiologies of autism explanations for the striking male preponderance, both the biological and psycho-social contributions to the imbalance need to be explored. More studies should examine the expression of symptoms as a function of IQ and of both sex and gendered social factors in autism and other developmental disorders (Rinehart et al., 2011). Cognitive functioning being negatively associated with severity of autistic features, sex discrepancy in IQ in some studies may have obscured true differences in autistic symptoms between boys and girls (Lai et al., 2012). As an example, it would be relevant to identify verbal and non-verbal gender markers from ADOS videos and in unidentified gender written protocols or standardized testing reports, and to develop a coding system to rate gender magnitude across IQ levels and autism severity. Together, basic scientists and clinicians ought to design longitudinal studies using instruments standardized for each sex to test the hypothesis that girls are less likely to be diagnosed with autism but when they are, girls are often more severely affected or may display a distinctive presentation.

Highlights.

  1. Biosocial perspective of the male preponderance in ASD

  2. Effects of biological sex and gendered socialization practices on ASD diagnosis

  3. Gender normed standardized clinical instruments

  4. Differential expression of autism symptoms in girls and boys e

Acknowledgements

This article is based on an invited talk to the Research Forum on Sex and Gender Differences in Cognition and Neurobiology on behalf of the Institute for Women’s Health and Leadership at Drexel University, October 27th 2011, Philadelphia.

The author wishes to thank Isabelle Rapin, from Albert Einstein College of Medicine for her support and stimulating conversation around this topic. The author is supported by the Einstein/Montefiore Autism Center and a grant from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) under Award Number P30HD071593.

Footnotes

Potential conflicts of interest None declared.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Reference List

  1. Attwood T. Asperger’s and girls. Future Horizons, Inc; Arlington, TX: 2006. The pattern of abilities and development of girls with Asperger’s syndrome; pp. 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  2. Auyeung B, Taylor K, Hackett G, Baron-Cohen S. Foetal testosterone and autistic traits in 18 to 24-month-old children. Molecular Autism. 2010;1(1):11. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-1-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bale TL, Baram TZ, Brown AS, Goldstein JM, Insel TR, McCarthy MM, et al. Early life programming and neurodevelopmental disorders. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;68(4):314–319. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.05.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Banach R, Thompson A, Szatmari P, Goldberg J, Tuff L, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. Brief Report: Relationship between non-verbal IQ and gender in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2009;39(1):188–193. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0612-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baron-Cohen S. The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2002;6(6):248–254. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)01904-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bauminger N, Solomon M, Rogers SJ. Externalizing and internalizing behaviors in ASD. Autism Research. 2010;3(3):101–112. doi: 10.1002/aur.131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Begeer S, Mandell D, Wijnker-Holmes B, Venderbosch S, Rem D, Stekelenburg F, et al. Sex Differences in the Timing of Identification Among Children and Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2012 doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1656-z. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bergman K, Glover V, Sarkar P, Abbott DH, O’Connor TG. In utero cortisol and testosterone exposure and fear reactivity in infancy. Hormones and Behavior. 2010;57(3):306–312. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.12.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Carter CS. Sex differences in oxytocin and vasopressin: implications for autism spectrum disorders? Behavioural Brain Research. 2007;176(1):170–186. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.08.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cheslack-Postava K, Jordan-Young RM. Autism spectrum disorders: Toward a gendered embodiment model. Social Science and Medicine. 2012;74(11):1667–1674. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.06.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Condry J, Condry S. Sex Differences: A Study of the Eye of the Beholder. Child Development. 1976;47(3):812–819. [Google Scholar]
  12. Connors SL, Levitt P, Matthews SG, Slotkin TA, Johnston MV, Kinney HC, et al. Fetal mechanisms in neurodevelopmental disorders. Pediatric Neurology. 2008;38(3):163–176. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2007.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Constantinescu M, Hines M. Relating prenatal testosterone exposure to postnatal behavior in typically developing children: methods and findings. Child Development Perpsectives. 2012;6(4):407–413. [Google Scholar]
  14. Constantino JN, Charman T. Gender bias, female resilience, and the sex ratio in autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2012;51(8):756–758. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Constantino JN, Davis SA, Todd RD, Schindler MK, Gross MM, Brophy SL, et al. Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the autism diagnostic interview-revised. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2003;33(4):427–433. doi: 10.1023/a:1025014929212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dworzynski K, Ronald A, Bolton P, Happe F. How different are girls and boys above and below the diagnostic threshold for autism spectrum disorders? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2012;51(8):788–797. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Feldman R, Gordon I, Influs M, Gutbir T, Ebstein RP. Parental Oxytocin and Early Caregiving Jointly Shape Children’s Oxytocin Response and Social Reciprocity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013 doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.22. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fombonne E. Epidemiology of pervasive developmental disorders. Pediatric Research. 2009;65(6):591–598. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31819e7203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gardener H, Spiegelman D, Buka SL. Perinatal and neonatal risk factors for autism: a comprehensive meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2011 Aug;128(2):344–55. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1036. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1036. Epub 2011 Jul 11. Review. PubMed PMID: 21746727; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3387855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Giarelli E, Wiggins LD, Rice CE, Levy SE, Kirby RS, Pinto-Martin J, et al. Sex differences in the evaluation and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders among children. Disability and Health Journal. 2010;3(2):107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2009.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Goin-Kochel RP, Abbacchi A, Constantino JN. Lack of evidence for increased genetic loading for autism among families of affected females: a replication from family history data in two large samples. Autism. 2007;11(3):279–286. doi: 10.1177/1362361307076857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hines M. Gender development and the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2011;34:69–88. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Insel TR. A neurobiological basis of social attachment. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;154(6):726–735. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.6.726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Knickmeyer RC, Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Ashwin E. How to test the extreme male brain theory of autism in terms of foetal androgens? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2008;38(5):995–996. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0553-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kopp S, Gillberg C. The Autism Spectrum Screening Questionnaire (ASSQ)-Revised Extended Version (ASSQ-REV): An instrument for better capturing the autism phenotype in girls? A preliminary study involving 191 clinical cases and community controls. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2011;32(6):2875–2888. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kozlowski AM, Matson JL, Rieske RD. Gender effect on challenging behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2012;6(2):958–964. [Google Scholar]
  27. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Pasco G, Ruigrok AN, Wheelwright SJ, Sadek SA, et al. A behavioral comparison of male and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum conditions. PLoS One. 2011;6(6):e20835. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Ruigrok AN, Chakrabarti B, Wheelwright SJ, Auyeung B, et al. Cognition in males and females with autism: similarities and differences. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e47198. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lee HJ, Macbeth AH, Pagani JH, Young WS., III Oxytocin: the great facilitator of life. Progress in Neurobiology. 2009;88(2):127–151. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lord C, Rutter M, Goode S, Heemsbergen J, Jordan H, Mawhood L, et al. Autism diagnostic observation schedule: a standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1989;19(2):185–212. doi: 10.1007/BF02211841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1994;24(5):659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lukas M, Neumann ID. Oxytocin and vasopressin in rodent behaviors related to social dysfunctions in autism spectrum disorders. Behavioural Brain Research. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.08.011. [Epub ahead of print]( http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.08.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Marco EM, Macri S, Laviola G. Critical age windows for neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders: evidence from animal models. Neurotoxicity Research. 2011;19(2):286–307. doi: 10.1007/s12640-010-9205-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. McLennan JD, Lord C, Schopler E. Sex differences in higher functioning people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1993;23(2):217–227. doi: 10.1007/BF01046216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Miller G. Neuroscience. The promise and perils of oxytocin. Science. 2013;339(6117):267–269. doi: 10.1126/science.339.6117.267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Pilowsky T, Yirmiya N, Shulman C, Dover R. The Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised and the Childhood Autism Rating Scale: differences between diagnostic systems and comparison between genders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1998;28(2):143–151. doi: 10.1023/a:1026092632466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rinehart NJ, Cornish KM, Tonge BJ. Gender differences in neurodevelopmental disorders: autism and fragile × syndrome. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences. 2011;8:209–229. doi: 10.1007/7854_2010_96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Rivet TT, Matson JL. Review of gender differences in core symptomatology in autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2011;5(3):957–976. [Google Scholar]
  39. Ross HE, Young LJ. Oxytocin and the neural mechanisms regulating social cognition and affiliative behavior. Frontieres in Neuroendocrinonolgy. 2009;30(4):534–547. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Scott FJ, Baron-Cohen S, Bolton P, Brayne C. Brief report: prevalence of autism spectrum conditions in children aged 5-11 years in Cambridgeshire, UK. Autism. 2002;6(3):231–237. doi: 10.1177/1362361302006003002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Sipes M, Matson JL, Worley JA, Kozlowski AM. Gender differences in symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorders in toddlers. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2011;5(4):1465–1470. [Google Scholar]
  42. Szatmari P, Liu XQ, Goldberg J, Zwaigenbaum L, Paterson AD, Woodbury-Smith M, et al. Sex differences in repetitive stereotyped behaviors in autism: Implications for genetic liability. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2012;159B(1):5–12. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Walsh P, Elsabbagh M, Bolton P, Singh I. In search of biomarkers for autism: scientific, social and ethical challenges. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2011;12(10):603–612. doi: 10.1038/nrn3113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Worley JA, Matson JL. Psychiatric symptoms in children diagnosed with an Autism Spectrum Disorder: An examination of gender differences. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2011;5(3):1086–1091. [Google Scholar]
  45. Yamasue H, Kuwabara H, Kawakubo Y, Kasai K. Oxytocin, sexually dimorphic features of the social brain, and autism. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2009;63(2):129–140. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.01944.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES