Abstract
Background
Gamma-synuclein is a member of the synuclein family of cytoplasmic, predominantly neuron-specific proteins. Despite numerous evidences for the importance of gamma-synuclein in the control of monoamine homeostasis, cytoskeleton reorganization and chaperone activity, its role in the regulation of cognitive behavior still remain unknown. Our previous study revealed that gamma-synuclein knockout mice are characterized by high habituation scores. Since a number of processes including spatial memory of the environment may affect habituation, in the present study we have carried out behavioral evaluation of spatial and working memory in gamma-synuclein knockout mice.
Results
Inactivation of gamma-synuclein gene led to the improvement of working memory in mice as revealed by passive and active avoidance tests. At the same time behavioral tests, designed to assess spatial learning and memory (Morris water maze and Object location tests), showed no differences between gamma-synuclein knockouts and wild type mice.
Conclusions
These findings indicate that young mice with targeted inactivation of gamma-synuclein gene have improved working memory, but not spatial learning and memory. Our results suggest that gamma-synuclein is directly involved in the regulation of cognitive functions.
Keywords: Gamma-synuclein, Knockout mice, Learning, Memory
Background
Gamma(γ)-synuclein – is a member of the synuclein family of small cytoplasmic acidic, predominantly neuron-specific proteins [1]. It has been speculated that in the nervous system γ-synuclein is involved in modulation of monoamine transporters [2,3], cytoprotection [4,5], chaperone activity [6], microtubule regulation and microtubule mediated organelle trafficking [7]. However, the exact mechanisms and consequences of this involvement are to be resolved. In cooperation with other members of the family γ-synuclein plays role in regulation of dopaminergic neurotransmission [8,9]. Changes of γ-synuclein expression in peripheral tissues has been linked with metabolic and oncological diseases [10,11].
Our previous study showed that γ-synuclein knockout mice display low levels of anxiety-like behavior, high exploratory activity and enhanced habituation [12]. It is well documented that cognitive abilities correlate with the level of exploratory activity and habituation scores in rodents [13,14]. These data allow to suggest that γ-synuclein may be one of the factors affecting cognitive function. Cognitive dysfunction is one of the most typical characteristics in various neurodegenerative pathologies such as Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease [15,16]. Since γ-synuclein has been implicated in hippocampal axon pathology in Parkinson’s disease [17]. Specific changes of γ-synuclein expression in retina and optical nerve have been reported in Alzheimer’s disease patients as well as in patients with glaucoma [18,19]. Overexpression of γ-synuclein induces neurodegeneration in animal models [20,21]. The first evidence of a possible link between the expression of γ-synuclein and choline acetyltransferase – an important component of cholinergic neurotransmission have been received [22]. At the same time, cholinergic neurotransmission is the key component of cognitive process [23,24]. However, there are no data available on the influence of γ-synuclein on learning and memory.
We hypothesized that γ-synuclein might be involved in some aspects of learning and memory processes in experimental animals. To test this, in the present work, we evaluated learning abilities of mice with target inactivation of γ-synuclein gene in the behavioral tasks that require intact working and spatial memory.
Results
Rod suspension test
On the first stage of the work we examined locomotor abilities of the experimental animals. We assessed the grip strength, which is a critical parameter for swimming task as well as for the other tests which require motor activity. In this test the γ-КО group did not show (p>0.05; Mann–Whitney U-test) significant differences in performance from the group of WT mice (Figure 1).
Passive avoidance test
Baseline step down latencies have shown no significant differences between groups at the pre-training stage. In γ-КО mice we observed 7.8-fold (T=0, p=0.008; Wilcoxon T-test, hereinafter) increase in step-down latency after training versus 3.9-fold (T=0, p=0.027) increase in WT animals (Figure 2). Thus, γ-KO showed an increase in step-down latency by 168% (U=4.5, p=0.006; Mann–Whitney U-test) compared to the WT mice.
Active avoidance test
Mice were trained for 8 days. The results of these experiments expressed as the number of successful avoidance responses over the number of the trials per day are shown in Figure 3. Starting from the 4th day γ-KO displayed better dynamics of learning. γ-KO mice made a significantly greater number of avoidance responses: on Day 4–3 fold (U=8, p=0.037; Mann–Whitney U-test, hereinafter), Day 5–4.3-fold (U=7, p=0.015), Day 6–2.4-fold (U=2, p=0.002), Day 7–3.9-fold (U=1.5, p=0.002) and Day 8–2.3-fold (U=6, p=0.01) compared to WT mice.
Morris water maze
To assess spatial learning, mice were tested in the Morris water maze. The results of the test are given in Figure 4. γ-KO and WT mice were not significantly different in all phases of water maze training, except for the training Day 4 when γ-KO mice showed 50% (U=9.5, p=0.03; Mann–Whitney U-test) lower escape latencies compared to WT animals.
Object location test
On day 1 (the acquisition trial) WT- and γ-КО mice time spent equal time investigating both objects (data not shown). On day 2 (test) both WT and γ-КО mice explored the object that was located in a new position for a significantly longer time than the other object (p<0.05; Mann–Whitney U-test).
γ-КО mice explored the displaced object 1.67 times longer (T=0; p=0.03; Wilcoxon T-test, hereinafter), than the object in familiar location. WT mice explored the displaced object 1.76 times longer (T=0; p=0.023), than object in familiar location (Figure 5A). During the test the γ-КО and WT groups showed the same (p>0.05) discrimination ratio: 0.277 and 0.252 for γ-КО and WT groups respectively (Figure 5B).
Discussion
To analyze locomotor abilities of experimental animals, we estimated the grip strength. Our study found no significant differences of the motor functions in the knockout mice, which is consistent with previously published data obtained in different motor tasks [12]. Intact grip strength by the knockouts suggests that muscle tone is not impaired in these mice.
We have shown that young mice with the target inactivation of γ-synuclein gene are characterized high working-memory capacity, but have no alterations in spatial learning and memory. Our previous study had revealed that behavioral phenotype of γ-KO mice can be characterized by low level of anxiety and enhanced habituation. These data can explain the improvement of learning in the passive and active avoidance models, used in the current investigation.
At the moment molecular and biochemical basis of the memory improvement in passive and active avoidance tests in γ-KO mice is not well understood. Several hypotheses can be assumed. Interestingly, inactivation of the expression of highly homologous to γ-synuclein, α-synuclein protein produces partially opposing phenotype – working and spatial memory impairment in adult mice [25]. However in spite of high homology in the amino acid primary sequence α-synuclein and γ-synuclein proteins differ in their secondary structure: γ-synuclein has an increased α-helical propensity in the amyloid-forming region (NAC-region) [26] which is involved in trafficking of monoamine transporters [3]. We can suggest that variations in the secondary structure are responsible for the opposite effects of α- and γ-synuclein gene knockout on expression of dopamine transporter (DAT) in the brain [27], that can in turn affect their cognitive ability [28]. Moreover, inactivation of the gene expression of α- and γ-synuclein has opposite effects on the emotional status of animals [12,29,30], which may also be reflected in the observed changes in cognitive abilities. It has been shown previously using the same line of knockout mice that inactivation of γ-synuclein gene alone does not affect expression of α-synuclein mRNA or protein in neural tissue [31], therefore our results present a clear effect of γ-synuclein’s inactivation.
It is also possible that in the absence of γ-synuclein a general mechanism of synaptic vesicle turnover and neurotransmitter release are perturbed to the degree that does not noticeably alter animal physiology but affects certain types of behaviour. This idea is consistent with more pronounced changes in neurotransmission observed in α/γ-synuclein double knockout comparing to α-synuclein knockout mice [8] and in triple synuclein knockout comparing to α/β-synuclein double knockout mice [32-34].
γ-Synuclein inactivation did not affect spatial learning in the Morris water maze although γ-synuclein is highly expressed in the brain areas involved in spatial learning [35] and its inactivation was shown to cause developmental deficit in the number of dopamine neurons in the midbrain [27,31] – an essential component for a water maze cued task learning [36]. We also did not reveal alterations in spatial memory of γ-KO mice. The most obvious explanation for this fact can be based on different strategies used for the platform search. “Route navigation” strategy probably allows γ-KO mice to use their high performing working memory and thus compensate the deficient spatial memory which is critical in «locale navigation» strategy [37,38]. This phenomenon as well as enhancement of working memory in γ-KO compared to WT mice needs further investigation.
Thus, our data provide the first evidence that γ-synuclein may be the important component of learning process which primarily based on the functioning of working memory.
Conclusions
Inactivation of γ-synuclein gene leads to improvement of working memory capacity, but not to change spatial memory and learning. Our data provide the first evidence that γ-synuclein plays an important role in learning process that is primarily based on the functioning of working memory.
Methods
Ethical note
The study was approved by the ethical committee of the Institute of Physiologically Active Compounds of RAS, the protocol number 12/12. All methods used were in compliance with the European Communities Council Directive of 24 November 1986 (86/609/EEC).
Animals
γ-Synuclein homozygous knockout mice on С57BL/6J genetic background were obtained from Cardiff University Transgenic Animal Unit and characterized previously [39]. Briefly, the targeted inactivation of gamma-synuclein transcription was induced through deletion of exons I, II, and III and promoter region of the gene. The control animals with no genome modifications (WT), and the mice with targeted null mutation of the gene encoding γ-synuclein (γ-КО), descended from the common heterozygous ancestors С57BL/6J (Charles River). Genotyping was carried out as described previously [31]. WT and γ-KO cohorts were housed individually in the same conditions on a 12 h light/dark cycle. The mice had free access to food and water and were maintained at the temperature of 19–21°C and 50% humidity. In all the experiments 3-month male mice (20–23 g) were used.
Behavioral tests
All behavioral tests were performed during the light phase of the cycle. The interval between the behavioral tests was 2 days. Tests were performed in the following order: rod suspension, object location test, passive avoidance test, Morris water maze and after that – active avoidance test.
Rod suspension
Animals are hung by their front paws from a rod 0.7 cm diameter, suspended 20 cm above the ground. The rod is 3 cm in length and terminated on the plastic walls. Latency to drop from the rod (as a criterion of grip strength) is recorded [40]. The experiment was performed during two consecutive days. On the first day the animals were given an opportunity to get acquainted with the test: a mouse was helped to climb up the rod, and in case of the fall in less than two minutes it was put on the rod again (not more than 3 attempts were given). When the mouse managed to stay on the rod for a longer time, it was not taken from there until it falls.. The test was repeated after 24 hours with one attempt given and the latency to drop from the rod was recorded.
Passive avoidance test
We used a step-down passive avoidance test, which consisted of a Plexiglas cage (25 × 25 × 45 cm) with a grid floor and a platform (6 × 6 × 1.2 cm) in the center. During the training session, mice were placed on the platform and covered with a beaker. After 35 seconds the glass was removed and the latency to step down with all four paws was measured. Immediately after stepping down on the grid, the animals received an electric shock (80V sinusoidal voltage). Retention test sessions were carried out 24 h after training. Each mouse was placed on the platform and the step-down latency was measured in the absence of electric foot shocks. Step-down latency was used as a measure of memory retention [41]. A cut-off time of 120 sec was set.
Active avoidance test
The test was performed as described before [25]. Two-way shuttle avoidance begins when a mouse is placed into one of the two equal compartments of the shuttle-box. The compartments have independently controlled electrified grid floors and illumination. Each trial is separated by 10 sec and consists of a 6-sec light-on that is followed by unipolar square electric impulses (30 V, 100 Hz) through the grid in the compartment where the mouse is located. Both the light and foot shock are terminated when the animal crosses to the alternate chamber. We used 5 trials a day during the Days 1–2 and 15 trials – during the Days 3–5. Successful avoidances consist of the trials where the mouse crosses to the adjacent chamber following the onset of the light, but before the foot shock.
Morris water maze test
A standard Morris water maze test was carried out in a pool (70 cm in diameter) of water at 22–23°C. An escape platform 6 cm in diameter was placed 0.5 cm below the surface of the water in the middle of one of the four quadrants of the pool. The platform and pool was black with anti-glare coating, which together with shadowless lamp lighting creates the effect of an invisible platform which is hidden under water. As visual cues we used 3 illuminated paper-figures fixed on the walls. Mice were placed into the tank, facing the wall of the pool, and were allowed to navigate the pool in search of the escape platform for a maximum of 60 s. If an animal failed to locate the platform within 60 s during the first training day, it was guided to the platform by the experimenter. The start points used for each trial varied. The time to reach the escape platform was recorded, and the animals were permitted 30 s to rest on the platform before removal from the tank [42]. Mice were tested during 5 days on 4 daily trials which were then averaged.
Object location test
The test was performed as described before [43] with some modifications. Mice were placed into a gray Plexiglas open-field box (40 cm wide×35 cm deep×16 cm high). Brown glass identical vials (5 cm in height×3 cm in diameter) were used as objects. The illumination was 60 lux in the center of box. The acquisition trial: mice were introduced to two identical objects located within the open field arena for 5 min. After a delay of 24 hours mice were introduced to the same arena but one of the objects was moved to a new location. The time the mouse spent in exploring each object was recorded for 5 min. The object-location discrimination ratio was used as a criterion of spatial memory [43,44] and calculated as (T1-T2)/(T1+T2), where T1 - the time spent by the animal exploring the new-located object, T2 – the old-located object.
Statistical analysis
Standard data processing was performed with Statistica 8 software (StatSoft Inc., USA). Depending on normality verified using Shapiro-Wilk’s W-test, either t-test for dependent or independent samples, or non-parametric Mann–Whitney U-test for independent groups and Wilcoxon T-test for dependent samples were used.
Abbreviations
γ-КО: γ-synuclein knockout mice; WT: Wild type mice with no genome modifications.
Authors’ contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: VSK, SOB, IYuS. Performed the experiments: VSK, GIV. Analyzed the data: VSK. Contributed materials/analysis tools: GIV, IYuS. Wrote the paper: VSK, IYuS. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Contributor Information
Viktor S Kokhan, Email: viktor_kohan@hotmail.com.
Gennadiy I Van’kin, Email: g_vankin@mail.ru.
Sergey O Bachurin, Email: bachurin@ipac.ac.ru.
Inna Yu Shamakina, Email: shamakina@yahoo.com.
Acknowledgement
The study was supported by State Contract №8829 (Ministry of Education and Science of the Russian Federation), the Program of Russian Academy of Sciences and “Fundamental Sciences for Medicine”.
References
- Surguchov A. Molecular and cellular biology of synucleins. Int Rev Cell Mol Biol. 2008;270:225–317. doi: 10.1016/S1937-6448(08)01406-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wersinger C, Sidhu A. Partial regulation of serotonin transporter function by gamma-synuclein. Neurosci Lett. 2009;453:157–161. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.02.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oaks AW, Sidhu A. Synuclein modulation of monoamine transporters. FEBS Lett. 2011;585:1001–1006. doi: 10.1016/j.febslet.2011.03.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quilty MC, Gai WP, Pountney DL, West AK, Vickers JC. Localization of alpha-, beta-, and gamma-synuclein during neuronal development and alterations associated with the neuronal response to axonal trauma. Exp Neurol. 2003;182:195–207. doi: 10.1016/S0014-4886(03)00108-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ninkina N, Peters OM, Connor-Robson N, Lytkina O, Sharfeddin E, Buchman VL. Contrasting effects of alpha-synuclein and gamma-synuclein on the phenotype of cysteine string protein alpha (CSPalpha) null mutant mice suggest distinct function of these proteins in neuronal synapses. J Biol Chem. 2012;287:44471–44477. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M112.422402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiang Y, Liu YE, Goldberg ID, Shi YE. Gamma synuclein, a novel heat-shock protein-associated chaperone, stimulates ligand-dependent estrogen receptor alpha signaling and mammary tumorigenesis. Cancer Res. 2004;64:4539–4546. doi: 10.1158/0008-5472.CAN-03-3650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang H, Kouadio A, Cartledge D, Godwin AK. Role of gamma-synuclein in microtubule regulation. Exp Cell Res. 2011;317:1330–1339. doi: 10.1016/j.yexcr.2010.10.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Senior SL, Ninkina N, Deacon R, Bannerman D, Buchman VL, Cragg SJ, Wade-Martins R. Increased striatal dopamine release and hyperdopaminergic-like behaviour in mice lacking both alpha-synuclein and gamma-synuclein. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;27:947–957. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06055.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anwar S, Peters O, Millership S, Ninkina N, Doig N, Connor-Robson N, Threlfell S, Kooner G, Deacon RM, Bannerman DM. Functional alterations to the nigrostriatal system in mice lacking all three members of the synuclein family. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2011;31:7264–7274. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6194-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oort PJ, Knotts TA, Grino M, Naour N, Bastard JP, Clement K, Ninkina N, Buchman VL, Permana PA, Luo X. Gamma-synuclein is an adipocyte-neuron gene coordinately expressed with leptin and increased in human obesity. J Nutr. 2008;138:841–848. doi: 10.1093/jn/138.5.841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Millership S, Ninkina N, Guschina IA, Norton J, Brambilla R, Oort PJ, Adams SH, Dennis RJ, Voshol PJ, Rochford JJ, Buchman VL. Increased lipolysis and altered lipid homeostasis protect gamma-synuclein-null mutant mice from diet-induced obesity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:20943–20948. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1210022110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kokhan VS, Bolkunov AV, Ustiugov AA, Van’kin GI, Shelkovnikova TA, Redkozubova OM, Strekalova TV, Bukhman VL, Ninkina NN, Bachurin SO. Targeted inactivation of gamma-synuclein gene affects anxiety and exploratory behaviour of mice. Zhurnal vysshei nervnoi deiatelnosti imeni I P Pavlova. 2011;61:85–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Light KR, Grossman H, Kolata S, Wass C, Matzel LD. General learning ability regulates exploration through its influence on rate of habituation. Behav Brain Res. 2011;223:297–309. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.04.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francia N, Cirulli F, Chiarotti F, Antonelli A, Aloe L, Alleva E. Spatial memory deficits in middle-aged mice correlate with lower exploratory activity and a subordinate status: role of hippocampal neurotrophins. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;23:711–728. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04585.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varanese S, Perfetti B, Ghilardi MF, Di Rocco A. Apathy, but not depression, reflects inefficient cognitive strategies in Parkinson’s disease. PLoS One. 2011;6:e17846. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown RG, Lacomblez L, Landwehrmeyer BG, Bak T, Uttner I, Dubois B, Agid Y, Ludolph A, Bensimon G, Payan C, Leigh NP. Cognitive impairment in patients with multiple system atrophy and progressive supranuclear palsy. Brain. 2010;133:2382–2393. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galvin JE, Uryu K, Lee VM, Trojanowski JQ. Axon pathology in Parkinson’s disease and lewy body dementia hippocampus contains alpha-, beta-, and gamma-synuclein. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1999;96:13450–13455. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.23.13450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Surguchov A, McMahan B, Masliah E, Surgucheva I. Synucleins in ocular tissues. J Neurosci Res. 2001;65:68–77. doi: 10.1002/jnr.1129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Surgucheva I, Weisman AD, Goldberg JL, Shnyra A, Surguchov A. Gamma-synuclein as a marker of retinal ganglion cells. Mol Vis. 2008;14:1540–1548. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ninkina N, Peters O, Millership S, Salem H, van der Putten H, Buchman VL. Gamma-synucleinopathy: neurodegeneration associated with overexpression of the mouse protein. Hum Mol Genet. 2009;18:1779–1794. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddp090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters OM, Millership S, Shelkovnikova TA, Soto I, Keeling L, Hann A, Marsh-Armstrong N, Buchman VL, Ninkina N. Selective pattern of motor system damage in gamma-synuclein transgenic mice mirrors the respective pathology in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Neurobiol Dis. 2012;48:124–131. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.06.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mukaetova-Ladinska EB, Andras A, Milne J, Abdel-All Z, Borr I, Jaros E, Perry RH, Honer WG, Cleghorn A, Doherty J. Synaptic proteins and choline acetyltransferase loss in visual cortex in dementia with lewy bodies. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2013;72:53–60. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e31827c5710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry E, Walker M, Grace J, Perry R. Acetylcholine in mind: a neurotransmitter correlate of consciousness? Trends Neurosci. 1999;22:273–280. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01361-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeKosky ST, Ikonomovic MD, Styren SD, Beckett L, Wisniewski S, Bennett DA, Cochran EJ, Kordower JH, Mufson EJ. Upregulation of choline acetyltransferase activity in hippocampus and frontal cortex of elderly subjects with mild cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol. 2002;51:145–155. doi: 10.1002/ana.10069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kokhan VS, Afanasyeva MA, Van’kin GI. Alpha-synuclein knockout mice have cognitive impairments. Behav Brain Res. 2012;231:226–230. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.03.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsh JA, Singh VK, Jia Z, Forman-Kay JD. Sensitivity of secondary structure propensities to sequence differences between alpha- and gamma-synuclein: implications for fibrillation. Protein science : a publication of the Protein Society. 2006;15:2795–2804. doi: 10.1110/ps.062465306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Al-Wandi A, Ninkina N, Millership S, Williamson SJ, Jones PA, Buchman VL. Absence of alpha-synuclein affects dopamine metabolism and synaptic markers in the striatum of aging mice. Neurobiol Aging. 2010;31:796–804. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erixon-Lindroth N, Farde L, Wahlin TB, Sovago J, Halldin C, Backman L. The role of the striatal dopamine transporter in cognitive aging. Psychiatry Res. 2005;138:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2004.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pena-Oliver Y, Buchman VL, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Schumann G, Ripley TL, King SL, Stephens DN. Deletion of alpha-synuclein decreases impulsivity in mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2012;11:137–146. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2011.00758.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pena-Oliver Y, Buchman VL, Stephens DN. Lack of involvement of alpha-synuclein in unconditioned anxiety in mice. Behav Brain Res. 2010;209:234–240. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.01.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robertson DC, Schmidt O, Ninkina N, Jones PA, Sharkey J, Buchman VL. Developmental loss and resistance to MPTP toxicity of dopaminergic neurones in substantia nigra pars compacta of gamma-synuclein, alpha-synuclein and double alpha/gamma-synuclein null mutant mice. J Neurochem. 2004;89:1126–1136. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2004.02378.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chandra S, Fornai F, Kwon HB, Yazdani U, Atasoy D, Liu X, Hammer RE, Battaglia G, German DC, Castillo PE, Sudhof TC. Double-knockout mice for alpha- and beta-synucleins: effect on synaptic functions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:14966–14971. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0406283101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burre J, Sharma M, Tsetsenis T, Buchman V, Etherton MR, Sudhof TC. Alpha-synuclein promotes SNARE-complex assembly in vivo and in vitro. Science. 2010;329:1663–1667. doi: 10.1126/science.1195227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greten-Harrison B, Polydoro M, Morimoto-Tomita M, Diao L, Williams AM, Nie EH, Makani S, Tian N, Castillo PE, Buchman VL, Chandra SS. Alphabetagamma-synuclein triple knockout mice reveal age-dependent neuronal dysfunction. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:19573–19578. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1005005107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavedan C, Leroy E, Dehejia A, Buchholtz S, Dutra A, Nussbaum RL, Polymeropoulos MH. Identification, localization and characterization of the human gamma-synuclein gene. Hum Genet. 1998;103:106–112. doi: 10.1007/s004390050792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Da Cunha C, Wietzikoski S, Wietzikoski EC, Miyoshi E, Ferro MM, Anselmo-Franci JA, Canteras NS. Evidence for the substantia nigra pars compacta as an essential component of a memory system independent of the hippocampal memory system. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2003;79:236–242. doi: 10.1016/S1074-7427(03)00008-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redish AD. Beyond the cognitive map : from place cells to episodic memory. Cambridge, Mass,London: MIT Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton DA, Rosenfelt CS, Whishaw IQ. Sequential control of navigation by locale and taxon cues in the morris water task. Behav Brain Res. 2004;154:385–397. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ninkina N, Papachroni K, Robertson DC, Schmidt O, Delaney L, O’Neill F, Court F, Rosenthal A, Fleetwood-Walker SM, Davies AM, Buchman VL. Neurons expressing the highest levels of gamma-synuclein are unaffected by targeted inactivation of the gene. Mol Cell Biol. 2003;23:8233–8245. doi: 10.1128/MCB.23.22.8233-8245.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matzel LD, Babiarz J, Townsend DA, Grossman HC, Grumet M. Neuronal cell adhesion molecule deletion induces a cognitive and behavioral phenotype reflective of impulsivity. Genes Brain Behav. 2008;7:470–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00382.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vignisse J, Steinbusch HW, Bolkunov A, Nunes J, Santos AI, Grandfils C, Bachurin S, Strekalova T. Dimebon enhances hippocampus-dependent learning in both appetitive and inhibitory memory tasks in mice. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35:510–522. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buccafusco JJ. Methods of behavioral analysis in neuroscience. 2. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2009. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murai T, Okuda S, Tanaka T, Ohta H. Characteristics of object location memory in mice: behavioral and pharmacological studies. Physiol Behav. 2007;90:116–124. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang W, Li S, Dong HP, Lv S, Tang YY. Differential impairment of spatial and nonspatial cognition in a mouse model of brain aging. Life Sci. 2009;85:127–135. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2009.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]