Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Learn Individ Differ. 2013 Feb 13;25:12–20. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.01.015

How Many Pathways Underlie Socioeconomic Differences in the Development of Cognition and Achievement?

Elliot M Tucker-Drob 1
PMCID: PMC3660050  NIHMSID: NIHMS441763  PMID: 23710118

Abstract

Children whose parents have higher education enjoy greater age-linked gains in cognitive abilities and academic achievement. Different researchers have typically focused on different outcomes, and the extent to which parental education relates to multiple child outcomes via a single developmental pathway has received little empirical attention. This issue was examined by applying common factor structural equation models to a large (N = 4,810) nationally representative sample of kindergarten through 12th grade children, who were measured on 6 distinct cognitive abilities and 5 distinct forms of knowledge and academic achievement. Results indicated that a single pathway accounted for the relations between parental education and age differences in children’s cognitive abilities. However, additional unique pathways were necessary to account for the relations between parental education and age differences in academic knowledge and mathematics. These results suggest that while socioeconomic differences are largely manifest in global aspects of cognitive development, they have incremental relations with some forms of academic achievement.

Keywords: Socioeconomic Status, Cognitive Development, Academic Achievement


Indices of socioeconomic status (SES), such as parental educational attainment are routinely linked with higher levels of cognitive performance and academic achievement throughout development. SES is associated with higher performance on general indices of cognitive ability (Ceci, 1996) and academic achievement (Sirin, 2005; White, 1982), and on specific indices of mathematics (Carneiro, Meghir, & Parey, 2007), reading (Carniero et al., 2007), language and vocabulary (Noble, McCandliss, & Farah, 2007; Noble, Norman, & Farah, 2005; Petrill, Pike, Price, & Plomin, 2004), visuospatial processing (Noble et al., 2007), episodic memory (Noble et al., 2007), and working memory (Evans & Schamberg, 2009). With these associations now well-established, researchers are refocusing their efforts on clarifying the mechanisms through which socioeconomic differences in cognition and achievement emerge over the course of development. For instance, a number of researchers have focused on distinguishing between social selection mechanisms, in which the same traits that influence parental SES (e.g. intelligence, work ethic, motivation) are inherited by children and influence their cognitive development, and social causal mechanisms, in which the environmental conditions under which children born to low SES parents live (e.g. poor nutrition, lower quality social stimulation) impede their cognitive development (Huston & Bentley, 2010; Lubinkski, 2009; Scarr, 1992; Strenze, 2007). However, one basic, descriptive question that has until now been largely overlooked is the extent to which the SES-related differences that have been identified for many different forms of cognition and achievement represent differences in a single global developmental process with far reaching effects versus differences in many different developmental processes. Answering this question is crucial for determining the extents to which the same set of mechanisms is sufficient for explaining SES-related differences in many different cognition and achievement outcomes, or different sets mechanisms are necessary for explaining SES-related differences in each individual cognition and achievement outcome.

There are a number of possible explanations for the observation that family socioeconomic status is associated with the development of very many cognition and achievement outcomes. One possibility is that the associations are reflections of associations with the development of a broader dimension of general cognitive ability. Under this scenario, socioeconomic status has a singular relation with general cognitive development, and performance in more specific domains are related to SES solely by virtue of their partial reliance on general functions. For example, deficits in the development of general cognitive ability would result in deficits in memory, mathematics skills, and spatial reasoning, simply because general cognitive ability plays a role in each of these domains. A second possibility is that socioeconomic differences in many outcomes reflect socioeconomic differences in the development of many separable, domain-specific, processes. Under this scenario, SES is independently and directly related to the development of different cognitive functions for different reasons. For example, when parents help children with their homework, reading skills might be more likely to benefit than would visuospatial processing ability. Finally, it is possible that SES relates to both domain-general and domain-specific processes. This hybrid scenario is particularly plausible given that the results of factor analyses of multivariate cognition and achievement data consistently indicate both that general a dimension that underlies variation in many different domains of functioning, and specific dimensions that underlie variation in particular domains and not others (Carroll, 1993; Tucker-Drob, 2009, Rhemtullah & Tucker-Drob, 2011). Examining SES in the context of such a multivariate approach is necessary, however, to test the extents to which SES relates to development via domain-general and domain-specific pathways. Previous investigations of socioeconomic differences in cognitive development have rarely analyzed data from multiple outcome simultaneously. Even when multiple outcomes are measured, they are often analyzed separately.

The goal of the current project was to test the extents to which socioeconomic differences in age trends in eleven different forms of cognition and achievement can be attributed to a domain-general developmental pathway and domain-specific pathways. Identifying the extents to which socioeconomic differences in cognition and achievement are manifest via global cognitive development and the development of multiple domains specific cognitive functions will help to delineate what type of mechanistic accounts will be necessary to ultimately explain how socioeconomic differences in cognition and achievement emerge over development. In order to address these questions I fit three structural equation models to multivariate age-heterogeneous data from a nationally representative sample of children in the United States. The first model allows parental education to individually relate to age-related differences in each specific ability, but does not provide for mediation by a common factor (general cognitive ability). The second model allows parental education to only relate to age-related differences in the common factor, but does not allow parental education to directly relate to age-related gains in the specific abilities. In other words, this second model requires parental education to relate to age-related differences in the specific abilities entirely through differences in the common factor. The third, hybrid, model allows parental education to relate to age-related differences in the common factor and, where needed, to incrementally relate to age-related differences in specific abilities. Using this series of models, one can ask the theoretical question of whether SES relates to age-related gains in specific abilities exclusively by way of its relations to gains in general cognitive ability or whether SES relates to age-related gains in specific abilities even after adjusting for age-related gains in general cognitive ability.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Data for this study come from the standardization sample of the Woodcock-Johnson III (WJ-III) Tests of Cognitive Abilities and the WJ-III Tests of Cognitive Achievement (Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001). Participants were recruited using a three-stage stratified sampling procedure to be nationally representative of the United States population, as indexed by the 2000 census projections (McGrew & Woodcock, 2001). Moreover, individual subject weights were used in all analyses to correct for any insufficiencies in achieving this goal. Because this article focuses on child development, analyses were limited to the grade school (K-12) participants from the WJ-III sample up to 21 years of age (N=4,810). The vast majority of participants were between 5 and 18 years of age. Age was centered at 5 (the typical age at kindergarten entry) for all analyses. The age distribution, prior to centering, is presented in Table A1 of the Appendix.

Measures

Descriptive statistics for the study variables are presented in Table 2. Participants were measured on eleven theoretically and empirically distinguishable domains of cognition and achievement (Visual-Spatial Thinking, Abstract Reasoning, Speed of Processing, Short-Term Memory, Long-Term Retrieval, Auditory Processing) and achievement (Reading, Writing, Mathematics, Academic Knowledge, and General Knowledge). Scores for each of these domains were derived from up to three individual tests from the WJ-III Tests of Cognitive Abilities and the WJ-III Tests of Cognitive Achievement (see Table 1). Scores are on a W scale, which is derived using 1 parameter logistic item response theory scoring. The W scale has interval measurement properties such that at any level of performance (a) a difference between person ability and item difficulty (ability minus difficulty) of 0 corresponds to 50% probability of success, (b) a difference between person ability and item difficulty of 10 corresponds to a 75% probability of success, and (c) a difference between person ability and item difficulty of -10 corresponds to a 25% chance of success.

Table 2.

Variable Intercorrelations, Means, and Standard Deviations.

Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. Mean Standard Deviation
1. General Knowledge 1.000 502.035 26.093
2. Spatial-Visualization 0.727 1.000 499.831 12.592
3. Abstract Reasoning 0.854 0.729 1.000 498.694 23.624
4. Processing Speed 0.832 0.738 0.811 1.000 501.223 39.164
5. Short-Term Memory 0.799 0.679 0.789 0.782 1.000 500.765 28.588
6. Long-Term Retrieval 0.837 0.743 0.832 0.827 0.786 1.000 499.733 9.054
7. Auditory Processing 0.799 0.669 0.752 0.761 0.751 0.766 1.000 500.574 13.760
8. Reading 0.908 0.74 0.854 0.913 0.831 0.859 0.803 1.000 496.961 52.284
9. Mathematics 0.885 0.746 0.863 0.906 0.817 0.844 0.767 0.938 1.000 500.154 32.363
10. Writing 0.885 0.734 0.842 0.908 0.822 0.85 0.791 0.959 0.933 1.000 498.41 27.923
11. Academic Knowledge 0.944 0.721 0.832 0.822 0.785 0.827 0.778 0.891 0.882 0.872 1.000 502.341 29.907
12. Parental Education 0.265 0.145 0.212 0.07 0.175 0.162 0.187 0.163 0.136 0.146 0.239 1.000 3.611 0.964
13. Age 0.813 0.661 0.713 0.842 0.692 0.71 0.672 0.824 0.842 0.822 0.804 0.032 1.000 10.865 3.861

Note: The means for Parental Education and Age were caculated prior to centering these variables.

Table 1.

Descriptions of Measures of Broad Cognitive Functions and Academic Achievement Areas.

Cognition/Achievement Domain Measure Description
Visual-Spatial Thinking (r=.80) Spatial Relations Identify the pieces needed to construct a specified shape.
Picture Recognition Identify previously presented pictures within a field of distracting pictures.
Fluid Reasoning (r=.95) Concept Formation Identify, categorize, and determine rules from a complete stimulus set.
Analysis-Synthesis Learn and apply novel symbolic formulations to determine the missing components of puzzles.
Processing Speed (r=.92) Visual Matching Quickly locate and circle two identical numbers in a row of numbers.
Decision Speed Quickly locate and circle two conceptually related pictures in a row of pictures.
Short-Term Memory (r=.87) Numbers Reversed Recall a series of numbers from immediate awareness in reverse sequence.
Memory for Words Repeat a list of unrelated words in the sequence presented.
Long-Term Retrieval (r=.87) Visual-Auditory Learning Learn and recall pictorial representations of words.
Retrieval Fluency Name as many examples as possible from a specified category.
Auditory Processing (r=.89) Sound Blending Synthesize phonetic units.
Auditory Attention Identify auditorily presented words in the presence of increasing intensities of background noise.
Comprehension Knowledge (r=.94) Verbal Comprehension Name pictured objects, select synonyms and antonyms, and complete verbal analogies.
General Information Identify where specified objects can be found, and what specified objects are typically used for.
Academic Knowledge (r=.88) Science, Social Studies, and Humanities subtests. Pointing responses on early items. Oral responses on later items.
Reading (r=.93) Letter-Word Identification Reading Fluency Identify printed letters and words orally. Read printed statements rapidly and reply true or false.
Passage Comprehension Identify a missing key word to make a written passage make sense.
Writing (r=.94) Spelling Spell orally presented words.
Writing Fluency Formulate and write simple sentences rapidly.
Writing Samples Write meaningful sentences for a given purpose.
Mathematics (r=.95) Calculation Perform mathematical calculations of various types.
Math Fluency Add, subtract, or multiply rapidly.
Applied Problems Perform mathematical calculations to solve orally presented problems.

Note: Adapted from Table 4–2 of WJ-III Technical Manual (McGrew et al., 2001). Composite score reliability estimates (r) are from Mather & Woodcock (2001a) and Mather & Woodcock (2001b).

For the current project, the average of maternal and paternal education was used as an index of socioeconomic status. As Huston and Bentley (2010, p. 420) have discussed, parental education is a central component of SES, is highly correlated with other aspects of socioeconomic advantage, and, of all the possible indices of SES commonly used, is “one of the best predictors of children’s intellectual functioning.” For the current sample, maternal and paternal education were reported by parents on a five point scale. 1=less than fifth grade, 2=less than high school diploma, 3=high school graduate, 4=1 to 3 years college, 5=bachelors degree or higher. Maternal and paternal education correlated at approximately r=.5. For the analyses reported below, the parental education index was centered at its mean. The distribution of this index, prior to centering, is presented in Table A2 of the Appendix.

Analytical Methods

Analyses made use of Mplus software (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2007) for full information maximum likelihood estimation of Structural Equation Models (SEMs). SEM’s are used to simultaneously estimate the parameters of systems of regression equations that relate multiple variables. Regressions can involve variables that have been measured directly, and those that are implied by the data (e.g. latent factors, or random effects). The basic SEM used for the current investigation simultaneously estimates regressions of the eleven different outcomes on age (and-age squared to account for nonlinear age trends), SES, the interaction between age and SES, and a common factor. The age by SES interaction is used to test for socioeconomic differences in the age trends in the eleven outcomes. The common factor represents a dimension of individual differences that is shared across the eleven outcomes (i.e. general cognitive ability or g). The variation in performance in each test that cannot be accounted for by the common factor can be attributed to a combination of domain-specific aspects of test performance and measurement error. Of interest here is the extent to which the common factor mediates the influence of the age by SES interaction on the eleven outcomes, or whether direct influences of the age by SES interaction on the domain-specific aspects of test performance exist. This is examined by fitting three classes of models (Muthén, 1989).

Independent Pathways Model

In this model each of the eleven outcomes, Yk, is directly regressed onto the interaction term and the other terms of the model. This is written as:

Yk=τk+α1,k·age+α2,k·age2+β1,k·PEd+β2,k·PEd·age+λk·F+uk [1]

where the subscript k indicates that a term is specific to each outcome, such that τ is an outcome-specific regression intercept, α1 and α2 represent the linear and nonlinear influences of age on each outcome, β1 represents the main effect of parental education (PEd) on performance on each outcome, β2 represents socioeconomic differences in the age trends in each outcome, λ represents the relation of each outcome to the common factor F, and the u’s represent variation in each outcome that is not accounted for by the other terms in the model. Age, age2, PEd, and PEd age intercorrelate, but F and uk do not. In this model, parental education is assumed to relate to each outcome through a different developmental pathway. Given that eleven independent pathways are estimated for the effects of parental education on age-related differences in each outcome, this model lacks parsimony. Nevertheless, it is important both as a baseline model, and for producing unrestricted estimates of the parental education-specific age trajectories that a more restrictive parsimonious model will have to reproduce.

Common Pathway Model

In this model the eleven outcomes are not directly regressed onto the interaction term. Rather, the single common factor is regressed onto this term. Any effects of the interaction term on the eleven outcomes are therefore indirect. That is, the PEd age interaction only relates to the eleven outcomes by way of a single relation with the common factor1. Because this model imposes strong restrictions on the route through which parental education can relate to age differences in the cognitive outcomes, this model is the most restrictive (and hence most parsimonious) account of the effects of parental education on cognitive development, and is sensitive to misfit. This model is written in multilevel notation as

Yk=τk+α1,k·age+α2,k·age2+β1,k·PEd+λk·F+uk [2]

and

F=β2·PEd·age+s, [3]

where s is a residual.

Common plus Independent Pathways Model

In this hybrid model, the common factor is regressed onto the PEd age interaction term (a domain-general pathway), and direct effects from the interaction term to the individual outcomes (domain-speciifc pathways) are added where statistically significant. This is achieved by adding the direct effects one by one beginning with the effect that produces the largest increment in model fit, until addition of further direct effect do not result in statistically significant fit improvements at p<.05. This model produces the most parsimonious representation able to account for the socioeconomic differences in age trends in all 11 outcomes. A schematic of this model is represented in Figure 1, with a predictor variable (e.g. parental education, age, or the parental education age interaction) labeled x, the common factor (general cognitive ability) labeled F, and the individual cognition and achievement outcomes labeled y. Domain-general and domain-specific pathways are represented with dotted lines and factor loadings represented with solid lines. Note that when the domain-general pathway is removed and all domain-specific pathways are estimated, this model represents the independent pathways model. When the domain-specific pathways are removed, this model represents the common pathways model.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

A generalized representation of the modeling approach employed. Domain-general and domain-specific pathways are represented with dotted lines and factor loadings are represtned with solid lines. For ease of presentation, only one domain-specific pathway is included. Note that when the domain-general pathway is removed and all domain-specific pathways are estimated, this model represents the independent pathways model. When the domain-specific pathways are removed, this model represents the common pathways model.

Results

Age trajectories for low (less than high school diploma), middle (high school diploma or some college), and high (bachelor’s degree or higher) parental education groups are displayed in Figure 2 for each of the eleven cognition and achievement outcomes. The age means in the figure were derived from the raw data, whereas the age curves were derived from parameter estimates from the least restrictive independent pathways model. The Y-axis has been scaled to an IQ metric (standard deviation = 15) based on the age-independent sample standard deviation, and centered at 0 for 18-year olds. For all outcomes, except for processing speed, parental education was associated with steeper age-related differences. Alternatively put, socioeconomic differences in cognition and achievement increased with age. In terms of the parameter estimates from the independent pathways model, the interaction term, β 2, was not statistically significant for processing speed, was statistically significant at p<.05 for short-term memory and auditory processing, and was significant at p<.01 for the eight remaining outcomes. A complete report of parameter estimates and fit statistics for the independent pathways model is presented in the top portion of Table 3.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Developmental trajectories in each of the eleven cognition and achievement outcomes for low (less than high school diploma), middle (high school diploma or some college), and high (bachelor’s degree or higher) levels of parental education. For interpretability, the vertical axis has been converted to a z metric (standard deviation = 1) based on the age-independent sample standard deviation and centered at 0 for mid-SES 18 year olds. The points represent the means from the raw data. The curves are based on estimates from the independent pathways model. Data come from 4,810 participants. Note that, while the figure presents results according to low, middle, and high levels of parental education, all results reported (including the age curves plotted in this figure) are based models applied to the more continuous index of parental education described in the Measures section.

Table 3.

Parameter Estimates from Independent Pathways Model, Common Pathway Model, and Common plus Independent Pathways Model.

Parameter Estimates
Outcome τ α1 α2 β1 β2 λ (λstd) σu
Independent Pathways Model: χ2 = 823.373, df = 42, RMSEA =.062 [90% CI = .059-.066], CFI=.985; TLI=.966
General Knowledge 462.647 9.707 −0.343 5.491 0.261 2.864 (.721) 75.961
Spatial-Visualization 482.617 4.664 −0.202 0.918* 0.174 1.215 (.444) 60.161
Abstract Reasoning 463.054 9.988 −0.455 3.961 0.246 3.074 (.695) 101.361
Processing Speed 433.782 17.709 −0.735 2.874 −0.021 3.326 (.615) 181.491
Short-Term Memory 459.818 11.167 −0.489 4.000 0.219* 3.674 (.631) 204.457
Long-Term Retrieval 485.672 4.027 −0.191 0.797 0.134 1.181 (.697) 14.774
Auditory Processing 481.484 5.207 −0.227 2.053 0.115* 1.648 (.569) 56.677
Reading 405.188 25.094 −1.113 6.947 0.353 5.902 (.830) 157.798
Mathematics 442.991 15.389 −0.666 1.772 0.486 3.171 (.761) 72.994
Writing 449.290 13.548 −0.610 2.997 0.235 2.869 (.762) 59.532
Academic Knowledge 456.936 11.335 −0.416 4.846 0.413 3.103 (.652) 130.205
Common Pathway Model: χ2 = 837.236, df = 52, RMSEA = .056 [90% CI = .059-.066], CFI = .985, TLI = .972
General Knowledge 462.661 9.708 −0.343 5.550 2.866 (.721) 75.846
Spatial-Visualization 482.629 4.651 −0.201 1.311 1.224 (.446) 60.183
Abstract Reasoning 463.063 9.985 −0.455 3.827 3.074 (.695) 101.207
Processing Speed 433.615 17.777 −0.741 1.044 3.281 (.608) 183.315
Short-Term Memory 459.752 11.192 −0.491 3.399 3.662 (.629) 204.638
Long-Term Retrieval 485.663 4.025 −0.190 0.976 1.186 (.698) 14.784
Auditory Processing 481.485 5.207 −0.228 1.883 1.644 (.568) 56.707
Reading 405.142 25.119 −1.116 5.878 5.878 (.828) 158.800
Mathematics 443.183 15.344 −0.664 3.064 3.184 (.761) 73.450
Writing 449.280 13.557 −0.610 2.892 2.864 (.761) 59.638
Academic Knowledge 456.997 11.329 −0.416 5.664 3.117 (.653) 130.361
Common Factor (F) 0.088
Common plus Independent Pathways Model : χ2 = 797.731, df = 49, RMSEA = .056 [90% CI = .053-.060], CFI = .986, TLI = .972
General Knowledge 462.638 9.711 −0.343 5.667 2.866 (.721) 76.011
Spatial-Visualization 482.627 4.651 −0.201 1.365 1.224 (.446) 60.198
Abstract Reasoning 463.054 9.987 −0.455 3.960 3.074 (.695) 101.333
Processing Speed 433.773 17.712 −0.735 2.861 −0.286 3.325 (.615) 181.531
Short-Term Memory 459.755 11.191 −0.491 3.557 3.665 (.629) 204.647
Long-Term Retrieval 485.660 4.026 −0.190 1.028 1.187 (.699) 14.775
Auditory Processing 481.487 5.207 −0.227 1.956 1.645 (.568) 56.696
Reading 405.065 25.138 −1.117 6.144 5.891 (.829) 158.235
Mathematics 442.961 15.399 −0.667 1.730 0.238 3.172 (.761) 72.966
Writing 449.267 13.559 −0.610 3.023 2.869 (.762) 59.547
Academic Knowledge 456.932 11.338 −0.416 4.949 0.147 3.105 (.652) 130.182
Common Factor (F) 0.080

Note: The fit of the individual model is provided following the name of the each model. Age was centered at 5 prior to analyses. Education was centered at its mean prior to analyses. In order to avoid estimation difficulties associated with large unstandardized factor loadings, the common factor was scaled to have a variance of 10. τ = regression intercept. α1 = coefficient on age. α2 = coefficient on age2. β1 = coefficient on parental education. β2 = coefficient on interaction between age and parental education. λ = unstandardized factor loading. λstd = standardized factor loading. σ2u = residual variance. RMSEA = Room Mean Square Error of Approximation. CFI = Comparative Fit Index. TLI = Tucker Lewis Index. All parameters significant at p<.01, except where otherwise noted.

*

p<.05.

p>.05.

The individual model χ2 values reported in this cannot be directly compared to one another to produce an accurate χ2 difference test. The χ2 difference tests reported in the text use model-specific scaling coefficients in order to make correct comparisons.

The first question addressed was whether the information captured by these ten statistically significant interaction terms in the independent pathways model could be just as adequately captured with the common pathway model, in which a single interaction term influenced the common factor. In other words, could the relations between parental education and age trends in the ten cognition and academic outcomes be attributed to differences in a single developmental process? The fit of the common pathway model suggested that they could not, as this model resulted in a statistically significant loss of information relative to the independent pathways model2[10]=49, p<.01).2 A complete report of parameter estimates and fit statistics for the common pathway model is presented in the middle portion of Table 3.

Next, a common plus independent pathways model was fit. Compared to the least parsimonious independent pathways model, this more parsimonious model did not result in a statistically significant loss of information (χ2[7]=8.6, p=.72), and was therefore accepted as the best representation of the data. In addition to allowing for the interaction term to act on a common pathway (p<.01), this model allowed for direct age parental education interaction effects on individual outcomes where statistically significant. There were three such direct pathways: (a) a negative interaction effect on processing speed; (b) a positive interaction effect on mathematics; and (c) a positive interaction effect on academic knowledge (all interaction effects were significant at p < .01). The negative speed pathway suggests that age differences in speed are less related to parental education than might be expected based on its relation with the common factor, whereas the positive mathematics and academic knowledge pathways suggest that the associations between parental education and age-related differences in these achievement outcomes are attributable to supplemental mechanisms that are specific to these outcomes. For academic knowledge, the indirect interaction effect of parental education that was mediated through the common factor (b1·λ) was .25, and the direct interaction effect of parental education (b2) was .15. For mathematics, the indirect interaction effect of parental education that was mediated through the common factor was .25, and the direct interaction effect of parental education (b2) was .24. A complete report of parameter estimates and fit statistics for the common plus independent pathways model is presented in the bottom portion of Table 3.

Discussion

Consistent with past research, the current project identified socioeconomic differences in many different cognitive abilities and forms of academic achievement that widened with development. To illustrate, at 5 years of age, children whose parents did not complete high school differed in abstract reasoning from children whose parents had completed college by approximately .5 points on a z-scale (age independent standard deviation=1), but at 17 years of age this difference had grown to over 1.1 points.3 On an IQ scale (age independent standard deviation=15), this amounts to an increase in the difference between low and high SES children from 7.5 points to 16.5 points over the course of gradeschool. The novel finding reported in this article is that these SES-related differences in age-related trends could be fully accounted for by a single common pathway – with two important exceptions. Socioeconomic differences in age-related trends in mathematics achievement and academic knowledge were larger in magnitude than would have been predicted by a common developmental pathway. Additional pathways were needed to account for these differences, suggesting the incremental operation of added mechanisms that are specific to these outcomes. Interestingly, age trends in processing speed did not differ by level of parental education, and hence, a third additional pathway was needed to disparities in general cognitive ability.

Interpreting the Pathways

The current findings indicate both domain-general and domain-specific pathways by which parental education relates to age trends in a diverse set of cognitive abilities and academic achievement domains. How should these findings be interpreted? One key point to emphasize is that the factors extracted and pathways identified represented statistical dimensions of variation and covariation. Importantly, a single statistical dimension need not represent a single social process, biological substrate, or cognitive mechanism. For example, it has been discussed at length in the literature that general cognitive ability may not represent a single biological or psychological entity (Carroll, 1993; Gould, 1996; Tucker-Drob, 2011; van der Maas et al., 2006). Rather, general cognitive ability can be affected by many thousands of genes and environments (Davies et al., 2011) and environments (Turkheimer, 2000), each with small, albeit generalized effects, and can be undergirded by multiple neurophysiological structures and cognitive processes (Colom et al., 2006; Kovas & Plomin, 2006) Moreover, the domain-general and domain-specific pathways indentified need not represent a single process; each pathway may represent a constellation of processes that covary with parental education (and hence with one another) and have similar effects on the domain(s) in question. For instance, if availability of books in the home and parental help with homework both covary with parental education and have incremental relations with the development of academic knowledge above and beyond relations with the development of general cognitive ability, then they may both underlie the domain-specific pathway between parental education and academic knowledge. Identifying the intermediary mechanisms between parental educational attainment and both domain-general and domain-specific aspects of cognitive development will necessitate detailed multivariate measurement of the social, biological, and experiential factors that covary with both parental education and cognitive development, along with equally detailed multivariate measurement of cognitive abilities.

The three individual domain-specific pathways identified are themselves noteworthy. SES was related to age trends in mathematics achievement and academic knowledge incremental to its association with age-related gains in general cognitive ability. This indicates that children from low SES backgrounds come to be at an even greater disadvantage in achievement domains than they would be expected to be at based on their disadvantage in general cognitive ability. One possible explanation for this finding is that SES-related inequalities in the educational system have incremental effects on academic skills above and beyond more generalized cognitive disparities associated with family resources (cf. Ceci, 1991). Moreover, that age trends in processing speed did not differ by level of parental education may indicate that processing speed is an ability that is particularly robust to environmental influence, as a number of researchers have previously suggested (e.g. Nettlebeck & Wilson, 2004).

Implications

The current results have both theoretical and practical implications for cognitive development research. On the theoretical side, these results make clear that socioeconomic status does not simply relate to the development of a few specialized types of knowledge or skill, but rather, relates to the development of manifold domains of cognitive functioning and academic achievement, ranging from simple recall of information to complex reasoning skills and mathematical proficiency. Moreover, these relations appear to be largely- although, in the case of mathematics and academic knowledge, not entirely- mediated by a more proximal relation between socioeconomic status and the development of a very general dimension of cognitive functioning that is common to many specific cognition and achievement domains. On the practical side, these findings present a challenge to standard interpretations of the simple bivariate relations that are often observed between socioeconomic status and a single cognition or achievement outcome. That is, while it may be tempting to interpret such bivariate relations as indication that a circumscribed domain-specific mechanism is at work, it is likely to be the case that the observed relation is, at least in part, a manifestation of differences in a very broad dimension of functioning that requires a domain-general explanation. Multivariate approaches can help to overcome this challenge by allowing researchers to take into account the codependence that multiple outcomes have with one another so that they can meaningfully examine the incremental effects of socioeconomic measures on individual outcomes.

The implications of the current results for applied interventions are less clear. This was a descriptive study that did not involve experimental manipulation. The findings do, however, raise interesting questions about ways to structure interventions. For example, the finding that the majority of SES-related differences in age trends in multiple abilities occurred via a domain-general pathway indicates that it might be fruitful to investigate whether interventions for impoverished children that target very generalized processes can have far-reaching effects on cognitive development. If this were the case, it would obviate the need to directly target every specific ability on which a deficit was observed. However, based on recent dynamical systems theories cognitive development (van der Maas et al. 2006; Dickens, 2007) , it is also possible that transfer of domain-specific training could occur, in which tutoring specific skills could have far reaching effects beyond the specific skills tutored. Randomized intervention research taking these different approaches is of necessary to test these possibilities. Of course, ameliorative interventions for social class differences in cognitive development need not target the mechanisms that are the original sources of those differences. Interventions can also be compensatory, by training different mechanisms than those affected and/or training children to rely more heavily on different skills and abilities (cf. Baltes, 1997).

Limitations

The current findings are particularly informative about the broad patterns by which socioeconomic differences in cognition and achievement emerge over the school years. Nevertheless, because data on very early childhood were not available, the current results only apply to development that occurs during the school years, and not to development that occurs prior to school entry. There is substantial evidence that socioeconomic differences in cognitive development and school readiness begin to arise during the first five years of life (Heckman, 2006; Tucker-Drob, 2012; Tucker-Drob, Rhemtulla, Harden, Turkheimer, & Fask, 2011). During infancy, children are primarily confined to their home environments, whereas during the school years, substantial portions of children’s experiences occur outside of the home. As such, the patterns by which socioeconomic differences are manifest during infancy have the potential to be quite different from those by which they are manifest during the school years.

A second limitation is that the current study was based on cross-sectional, rather than longitudinal, data. Unfortunately, the nationally representative longitudinal datasets that do exist on child cognitive development and academic achievement (e.g. the Early Childhood Longitudinal Studies conducted by the US Department of Education) only include measures on a small set of cognition and achievement domains, the domains are only measured using brief tests, and not all domains are represented in each wave of assessment. In contrast, the nationally representative WJ-III dataset analyzed here includes measures of 11 domains of cognition and achievement, each domain is measured using up to three highly sensitive individual tests, and all 11 domains are measured for participants between ages 5 and 18. Therefore, while longitudinal data would have been ideal for testing the hypotheses in question, there do not appear to be longitudinal data available of comparable quality to the cross-sectional data analyzed here.

Future Directions

An overarching goal of research on socioeconomic differences in cognitive development will continue to be to identify the developmental mechanisms that give rise these differences. This is a particularly important area of ongoing research in light of recent evidence implicating cognitive abilities and academic achievement as “fundamental” mediators of socioeconomic differences in multiple social, economic, and health outcomes, including better wages and employment, lower teenage pregnancy, less smoking, lower law-breaking, and greater overall health, longevity, and subjective well-being (Cunha & Heckman, 2009; Deary, 2008; Goldman & Smith, 2002; Gottfredson, 2004; Sackett, Kuncel, Arneson, Cooper, & Waters, 2009). In other words, children raised in low SES homes are at elevated risks for multiple adverse life outcomes in part because of lower cognitive ability and lower school achievement. The goal of the current study was not to identify specific mechanisms underlying the SES-cognition relation, but rather, to clarify the developmental patterns that will eventually need to be explained. Results suggest that two sets of mechanisms will prove important: those that underlie socioeconomic differences in general cognitive development, and those that incrementally underlie socioeconomic differences in specific academic domains– academic knowledge and mathematics in particular. What forms might such mechanisms might take? As discussed earlier, mechanisms underlying the SES-cognition relation can be generally classified as either social selection mechanisms or social causation mechanisms (Huston & Bentley, 2010). Both mechanisms are likely to contribute to the differences observed to some extent. For instance, there is strong evidence from adoption studies that, genes for general cognitive ability and academic achievement are genetically transmitted from parents to their biological offspring, and that being raised in a more privileged home has a substantial causal effect on cognitive development (Capron & Duyme, 1989; Nelson et al., 2007; Plug & Vijverberg, 2003). However, as many have previously argued (e.g. Ceci, 1996; Harden, Turkheimer, & Loehlin, 2007; Scarr-Salapatek, 1971; Turkheimer, Haley, Waldron, D’Onofrio, & Gottesman, 2003; Rhemtulla & Tucker-Drob, 2012; Tucker-Drob et al., 2011), even the realization of genetic potentials is likely to rely on the adequacy of experiential inputs. Aspects of the environment that are routinely linked with SES and have been suggested to play key roles in cognitive development and academic achievement include availability of educational materials (Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Coll, 2001), parental allocation of time spent with their children (Guryan, Hurst, & Kearney, 2008; Kalil, Ryan, & Corey, 2010), and school and teacher quality (Kazdin, Kraemer, Kessler, Kupfer, & Offord, 1997; Nisbett, 2009; Taylor et al., 2010), to name a few.

Conclusions

In summary, parental education was found to relate to age trends in multiple diverse forms of cognition and achievement throughout the school years. The differences were entirely mediated by a general dimension of cognition function for all but two domains: mathematics and academic knowledge. For these domains, additional direct pathways were needed to account for widening socioeconomic differences with age. These results suggest that while socioeconomic differences are largely manifest in global aspects of cognitive development, they have incremental relations with some forms of academic achievement. These results help to delineate the specific developmental patterns that will eventually need to be accounted for by explanatory theories of social inequality.

Highlights.

  • Higher Parental Education is associated with larger developmental gains in multiple cognitive domains.

  • It is not yet clear whether these differences represent many domain-specific relations or a more general relation.

  • Multivariate ability data were obtained from 4,800 children in kindergarten through 12th grades.

  • A single pathway accounted for the relation between Parental Education and age differences in all domains except academic knowledge and mathematics

  • These results suggest that while socioeconomic status is largely associated with the development of general cognitive ability, it has incremental associations with some forms of academic achievement

Acknowledgments

The Woodcock-Munõz Foundation provided access to the standardization data from the Woodcock –Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities and the WJ-III Tests of Cognitive Achievement. John Loehlin and Paige Harden provided valuable comments on previous versions of this article. Parts of this article were written while the author was supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) research grant R21HD069772. The Population Research Center at the University of Texas at Austin is supported by NIH center grant R24HD042849.

Appendix: Distributions of Age and Parental Education

Table A1.

Distribution of Age.

Age (Years) Proportion of Sample
4 and Below 1.4%
5.00 4.6%
6.00 6.3%
7.00 7.0%
8.00 8.5%
9.00 10.3%
10.00 11.3%
11.00 8.5%
12.00 7.2%
13.00 6.7%
14.00 6.0%
15.00 6.3%
16.00 6.4%
17.00 4.8%
18 and Above 3.3%

Table A2.

Distribution of Parental Educational Attainment (Mean of Maternal and Paternal Educational Attainment).

Education Category Proportion of Sample
1.00 (Less Than 5th Grade) .7%
1.50 .4%
2.00 (Less Than High School Diploma) 4.7%
2.50 6.5%
3.00 (High School Graduate) 22.2%
3.50 15.8%
4.00 (Some College) 18.8%
4.50 11.4%
5.00 (Bachelors Degree and Above) 19.5%

Note. Educational attainment was measured for each parent on a 1–5 integer scale. However, because educational attainment for mothers and fathers was averaged for the purposes for the current paper, educational attainment took on half-point values as well.

Footnotes

1

For theoretical reasons, two exceptions were made a priori. First, ureading and uwriting were allowed to correlate. Second, ugeneral knowledge and uacademic knowledge were allowed to correlate. These substantially benefited model fit, but did not affect the major findings reported here.

2

Note that the χ2 difference tests reported here make use of model-specific scaling coefficients. The individual model χ2 values reported in the tables cannot be directly compared to one another to produce an accurate χ2 difference test.

3

z-scale (SD=1) and IQ scale (SD=15) is based on the age-independent sample standard deviation and centered at 0 for mid-SES 18 year olds. correct for the disparities in processing speed that would have otherwise been expected based on

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Baltes PB. On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny: Selection, optimization, and compensation as foundation of developmental theory. American Psychologist. 1997;52:366–380. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.52.4.366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53:371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Capron C, Duyme M. Assessment of the effects of socio-economic status on IQ in a full cross-fostering study. Nature. 1989;340:552–554. [Google Scholar]
  4. Carneiro P, Meghir C, Parey M. Maternal education, home environments, and the development of children and adolescents. Institute for the Study of Labor Discussion Paper 3072 2007 [Google Scholar]
  5. Carroll JB. Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic Studies. Cambridge University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  6. Ceci SJ. How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence. Developmental psychology. 1991;27:703. [Google Scholar]
  7. Ceci SJ. On Intelligence: A bio-ecological treatise on intellectual development. 2. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  8. Colom R, Jung RE, Haier RJ. Distributed brain sites for the g-factor of intelligence. Neuroimage. 2006;31:1359–1365. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cunha F, Heckman JJ. The economics and psychology of inequality and human development. Journal of European Economic Association. 2009;7:320–364. doi: 10.1162/jeea.2009.7.2-3.320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Davies G, Tenesa A, Payton A, Yang J, Harris SE, Liewald D, Deary IJ. Genome-wide association studies establish that human intelligence is highly heritable and polygenic. Molecular Psychiatry. 2011;16:996–1005. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Deary I. Why do intelligent people live longer? Nature. 2008;456:175–176. doi: 10.1038/456175a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dickens WT. What is g? Downloaded on 12/10/12. 2007 from http://www.brookings.edu/~/media/Research/Files/Papers/2007/5/03education%20dickens/20070503.PDF.
  13. Evans GW, Schamberg MA. Childhood poverty, chronic stress, and adult working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106:6545–6549. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811910106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Goldman D, Smith J. Can patient self-management help explain the SES health gradient? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2002;99:10929–10934. doi: 10.1073/pnas.162086599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gottfredson LS. Intelligence: Is it the epidemiologists' elusive "fundamental cause" of social class inequalities in health? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2004;86:174–199. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gould SJ. The Mismeasure Of Man. WW Norton & Company; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  17. Guryan J, Hurst E, Kearney M. Parental education and parental time with children. Journal of Economic Perspectives. 2008;22:23–46. [Google Scholar]
  18. Harden KP, Turkheimer E, Loehlin JC. Genotype by environment interaction in adolescents' cognitive aptitude. Behavior Genetics. 2007;37:273–283. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9113-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Heckman JJ. Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science. 2006;312:1900–1902. doi: 10.1126/science.1128898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Huston AC, Bentley AC. Human development in society context. Annual Review of Psychology. 2010;61:411–437. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kalil A, Ryan R, Corey M. Diverging destinies: Maternal education and investments in children. 2010 doi: 10.1007/s13524-012-0129-5. Manuscript submitted for publicationRetrieved on 5/15/2010 from http://www.popcenter.umd.edu/research/sponsored-events/atus-conf-workshop-2009/papers-atus-2009/01-kalil-etal.pdf. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  22. Kazdin AE, Kraemer HC, Kessler RC, Kupfer DJ, Offord DR. Contributions of risk-factor research to developmental psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review. 1997;17:375– 406. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(97)00012-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kovas Y, Plomin R. Generalist genes: implications for the cognitive sciences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006;10:198–203. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Lubinski D. Cognitive epidemiology: With emphasis on untangling cognitive ability and socioeconomic status. Intelligence. 2009;37:625–633. [Google Scholar]
  25. McGrew KS, Werder JK, Woodcock RW. WJ-R Technical Manual. Allen, TX: DLM; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  26. McGrew KS, Woodcock RW. Technical manual. Woodcock Johnson III. Itasca, IL: Riverside; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  27. Muthén BO. Latent variable modeling in heterogeneous populations. Psychometrika. 1989;54:557–585. [Google Scholar]
  28. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. 5. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2007. [Google Scholar]
  29. Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Marshall PJ, Smyke AT, Guthrie D. Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: The Bucharest early intervention project. Science. 2007;318:1937–1940. doi: 10.1126/science.1143921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Nettelbeck T, Wilson C. The Flynn effect: Smarter not faster. Intelligence. 2004;32:85–93. [Google Scholar]
  31. Nisbett RE. Intelligence and How to Get It. Norton; New York: 2009. [Google Scholar]
  32. Noble KG, McCandliss BD, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science. 2007;10:464–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Noble KG, Norman MF, Farah MJ. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science. 2005;8:74–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Petrill SA, Pike A, Price T, Plomin R. Chaos in the home and socioeconomic status are associated with cognitive development in early childhood: Environmental mediators identified in a genetic design. Intelligence. 2004;32:445–460. [Google Scholar]
  35. Plug E, Vijverberg W. Schooling, family background, and adoption: Is it nature or is it nurture? The Journal of Political Economy. 2003;111:611–641. [Google Scholar]
  36. Rhemtulla M, Tucker-Drob EM. Correlated longitudinal changes across linguistic, achievement, and psychomotor domains in early childhood: Evidence for a global dimension of development. Developmental Science. 2011;14:1245–1254. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01071.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rhemtulla M, Tucker-Drob EM. Gene-by-socioeconomic status interaction on school readiness. Behavior Genetics. 2012;42:549–558. doi: 10.1007/s10519-012-9527-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Sackett PR, Kuncel NR, Arneson JJ, Cooper SR, Waters SD. Does socioeconomic status explain the relationship between admissions tests and post-secondary academic performance? Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135:1–22. doi: 10.1037/a0013978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Scarr-Salapatek S. Race, social class, and IQ. Science. 1971;174:1285–1295. doi: 10.1126/science.174.4016.1285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Scarr S. Developmental theories for the 1990’s: Development and individual differences. Child Development. 1992;63:1–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Sirin SR. Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Review of Educational Research. 2005;75:417–453. [Google Scholar]
  42. Strenze T. Intelligence and socioeconomic status: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal research. Intelligence. 2007;35:401–426. [Google Scholar]
  43. Taylor J, Roehrig AD, Soden Hensler B, Connor CM, Schatschneider C. Teacher quality moderates the genetic effects on early reading. Science. 2010;328:512–514. doi: 10.1126/science.1186149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Tucker-Drob EM. Differentiation of cognitive abilities across the life span. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45:1097–1118. doi: 10.1037/a0015864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Tucker-Drob EM. Global and domain-specific changes in cognition throughout adulthood. Developmental Psychology. 2011;47:331. doi: 10.1037/a0021361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Tucker-Drob EM. Preschools reduce early academic achievement gaps: A longitudinal twin approach. Psychological Science. 2012;23:310–319. doi: 10.1177/0956797611426728. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Tucker-Drob EM, Rhemtulla M, Harden KP, Turkheimer E, Fask D. Emergence of a gene-by-socioeconomic status interaction on infant mental ability between 10 months and 2 years. Psychological Science. 2011;22:125–133. doi: 10.1177/0956797610392926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Turkheimer E. Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2000;9:160–164. [Google Scholar]
  49. Turkheimer E, Haley A, Waldron M, D’Onofrio BM, Gottesman II. Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science. 2003;14:623–628. doi: 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Van Der Maas HL, Dolan CV, Grasman RP, Wicherts JM, Huizenga HM, Raijmakers ME. A dynamical model of general intelligence: the positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review. 2006;113:842. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.4.842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. White KR. The relation between socioeconomic status and academic achievement. Psychological Bulletin. 1982;91:461–481. [Google Scholar]
  52. Woodcock RW, McGrew KS, Mather N. Woodcock–Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities. Itasca, IL: Riverside; 2001. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES