Abstract
Purpose
Variation in sleep duration has been linked with mortality risk. The purpose of this review is to provide an updated evaluation of the literature on sleep duration and mortality, including a critical examination of sleep duration measurement and an examination of correlates of self-reported sleep duration.
Methods
We did a systematic search of studies reporting associations between sleep duration and all-cause mortality and extracted the sleep duration measure and the measure(s) of association.
Results
We identified 42 prospective studies of sleep duration and mortality drawing on 35 distinct study populations across the globe. Unlike previous reviews, we find that the published literature does not support a consistent finding of an association between self-reported sleep duration and mortality. Most studies have employed survey measures of sleep duration, which are not highly correlated with estimates based on physiologic measures.
Conclusions
Despite a large body of literature, it is premature to conclude, as previous reviews have, that a robust, U-shaped association between sleep duration and mortality risk exists across populations. Careful attention must be paid to measurement, response bias, confounding, and reverse causation in the interpretation of associations between sleep duration and mortality.
Keywords: Sleep, Mortality, Review, Epidemiology, Adult, Polysomnography, Actigraphy
Introduction
Over the past decades, a growing body of literature has examined how habitual sleep duration is related to mortality [1-36]. Several recent reviews have summarized this literature and have concluded that a robust association exists between sleep duration and mortality risk; specifically, that mortality risk is higher among both short sleepers and long sleepers compared to normal length sleepers [37-40]. Upon re-examination, however, the literature is not as consistent as might be expected given these conclusions. A careful reconsideration of the sleep duration/mortality literature is warranted, given the increasing interest of epidemiologists in studying sleep [41] and the number of studies citing this literature to motivate research focused on physiologic mechanisms linking sleep and health outcomes.
Past reviews have not described the specific ways in which sleep duration was queried in each of the original studies or the possible implications of these measurement differences for study findings. Although all of the reviews acknowledge methodologic issues, insufficient attention has been paid to the several plausible alternative explanations to a causal link from sleep to mortality, including systematic measurement bias, unmeasured confounding, and reverse causation.
Therefore, in this review we: (1) review how sleep duration has been measured in epidemiologic studies and discuss the challenges of using these different measurement approaches; (2) identify the correlates of short and/or long self-reported sleep duration in the interest of highlighting the threat of confounding by these factors; and (3) summarize the findings of prospective studies of sleep duration and mortality among adults, discussing possible differences in these relationships by sex and measurement type.
Methods
To find studies of sleep duration and all-cause mortality among adults, we searched all studies in PubMed up to October 5, 2012 using the search terms “sleep duration” and “mortality,” which yielded 281 articles. Eligible studies were those published in English as full-length articles that included sleep duration as the exposure and all-cause mortality as the outcome among adult populations (≥18 years of age). We additionally searched citation lists of earlier review articles on the topic.
Several research groups have published multiple papers from the same study population [2, 5, 15-21, 26, 42]. In these cases, all of the studies are described in Table 1, but only the report with the longest follow-up time and most detailed mortality analysis is included in our primary results table (Table 2). Effect estimates from the most fully-adjusted models from each study were extracted from data tables. We considered results to be significant at the P < 0.05 level on the basis of reported P-values or 95% confidence intervals. When available, we reported sex-stratified results in Table 2. Sex-specific findings from the same study were considered to be independent.
Table 1.
Cohort name | Country | Age Range (Years) |
Study Population | Year(s) Sleep Data Collected |
Follow-Up (Years)b |
Cohort Size (N) |
Reference |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Survey Sleep Measure: Nighttime Sleep a | |||||||
American Cancer Society (ACS) |
USA | 30-90+ | Individuals enrolled from 25 states in the U.S. via ACS volunteers |
1959-60 | 2 | 1,064,004 | Hammond 1964b [2] |
6 | 823,065 | Kripke et al. 1979c [5] |
|||||
Cancer Prevention Study II | USA | 30-102 | Friends and relatives of American Cancer Society volunteers |
1982 | 6 | 1,116,936 | Kripke et al. 2002 [3] |
Dalarna County postal survey |
Sweden | 45-65 | Random sample of adults living in the County of Dalarna |
1983 | 12 | 1,870 | Mallon et al. 2002 [8] |
Elderly cohort | USA | 65-98 | Representative sample of elderly residents in an urban community |
1984-5 | 3.5 | 1,855 | Pollak et al. 1990 [10] |
FINRISK Surveys | Finland | 25-64 | Representative population-based sample of North Karelia and Kuopio residents |
1972, 1977 | 35 | 23,290 | Kronholm et al. 2011 [11] |
Japan Collaborative Cohort Study for Evaluation of Cancer |
Japan | 40-79 | Attendees of screening programs in 45 areas | 1988-90 | Mean=9.9 | 104,010 | Tamakoshi & Ohno 2004b [19] |
NR | NR | Suzuki et al. 2007b [20] |
|||||
Median=14.3 | 98,634 | Ikehara et al. 2009c [21] |
|||||
Massachusetts Health Care Panel Study |
USA | 66-98 | Probability sample of the non-institutionalized elders in Massachusetts |
1976 | 5 | 1,235 | Branch and Jette 1984 [24] |
Miyagi Prefecture survey | Japan | ≥40 | Residents of a rural town in Miyagi prefecture | 1988 | 4 | 4,318 | Tsubono et al. 1993 [25] |
National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey I |
USA | 32-86 | Probability sample of the civilian adult noninstitutionalized population |
1982-4 | 10 | 7,844 | Qureshi et al. 1997b [42] |
8-10 | 9,789 | Gangwisch et al. 2008c [26] |
|||||
Rancho Bernardo Study | USA | 60-96 | Community-dwelling residents from Rancho Bernardo, CA |
1984-7 | 19 | 2,001 | Jung et al. 2012 [98] |
Whitehall II Cohort | UK | 35-55 | White-collar British civil servants | 1985-8 | Mean=17.1 | 10,308 | Ferrie et al. 2007 [35] |
Survey Sleep Measure: 24-HOur Sleep a | |||||||
Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey (CLHLS) |
China | 65-100+ | Representative sample of elderly population in Mainland China |
2005 | 3 | 12,671 | Qiu et al. 2011 [97] |
Finnish Twin Cohort |
Finland | 24-101 | All Finnish twin pairs of the same sex born before 1958 with both co-twins alive in 1975 |
1975, 1981 | 21 | 21,268 | Hublin et al. 2007 [1] |
Health Interview Study of Barcelona (HISB) |
Spain | ≥65 | Random sample of non-institutionalized individuals in Barcelona |
1986 | 5 | 1,219 | Ruigomez et al. 1995 [14] |
National Health Interview Survey (NHIS) |
USA | ≥18 | Nationally representative sample of adults in the USA |
1990 | 16 | 38,891 | Krueger et al. 2011 [115] |
Nurses’ Health Study | USA | 40-65 | Female health professionals | 1986 | 14 | 82,969 | Patel et al. 2004 [29] |
Ohsaki Cohort Study | Japan | 40-79 | National Health Insurance beneficiaries | 1994 | Mean=10.8 | 49,236 | Kakizaki et al. 2012 [116] |
Scottish working cohort | Scotland | <60 females; <65 males |
Employees of a variety of workplaces in the west of Scotland |
1970-3; 1977 | 25 | 6,797 | Heslop et al. 2002 [30] |
Shizuoka Study | Japan | 65-85 | Population-based cohort study in the Shizuoka prefecture |
1999 | 6 | 11,395 | Suzuki et al. 2009 [31] |
Spanish cohort | Spain | ≥60 | Probability sample of non-institutionalized persons representative of Spanish population |
2000-1 | Mean=6.8 | 3,820 | Mesas et al. 2010 [32] |
Study of Osteoporotic Fractures |
USA | ≥69 | Population-based cohort of ambulatory, community-dwelling women |
1993-4 | Mean=6.9 | 8,101 | Stone et al. 2009 [33] |
Survey Sleep Measure: Bedtime and Waking Time a | |||||||
Bambui Health and Ageing Study |
Brazil | ≥60 | Residents of Bambui city | 1997 | 9 | 1,512 | Castro-Costa et al. 2011 [6] |
Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Aging Study |
Israel | 75-94 | Random sample of older Jewish population in Israel |
1989-92 | 20 | 1,166 | Cohen-Mansfield et al. 2012 [99] |
Department of Health and Social Security Survey |
UK | ≥65 | Random sample of older adults living in eight areas of Great Britain |
1973-4 | 23 | 862 | Gale & Martyn 1998 [9] |
Gifu Prefecture cohort study |
Japan | 20-67 | Residents of Shirakawa town who attended health examinations |
1982-6 | Mean=11.9 | 5,322 | Kojima et al. 2000 [12] |
Jichi Medical School Cohort Study |
Japan | 19-93 | Population-based study in 12 rural areas | 1992-5 | Mean=8.2 | 11,325 | Amagai et al. 2004 [22] |
Kiryat Yovel Community Health Study |
Israel | ≥50 | Population-based study of residents of a West Jerusalem community |
1985-7 | 9-11 | 1,842 | Burazeri et al. 2003 [23] |
Survey of Health and Living Status of the Elderly in Taiwan |
Taiwan | ≥64 | Probability sample of the elderly population of Taiwan |
1993 | 10 | 3,079 | Lan et al. 2007 [34] |
Survey Sleep Measure: Not Clearly Specified a | |||||||
Chin-Shan Community Cardiovascular Cohort Study |
Taiwan | ≥35 | Ethnic Chinese residents of Chin-Shan township | 1990-1 | Median=15.9 | 3,430 | Chien et al. 2010 [7] |
Health and Lifestyle Survey | UK | ≥18 | Representative sample of the British non- institutionalized population |
1984-5 | 7 | 6,096 | Huppert & Whittington 1995 [13] |
Human Population Laboratory (Alameda County study) |
USA | <45-75+ | Probability sample of non-institutionalized adult residents of Alameda County, California |
1965 | 5.5 | 6,928 | Belloc 1973b [15] |
30-69 | 1965 | 9 | 4,713 | Wingard & Berkman 1983b [17] |
|||
<45-75+ | 1965 | 9.5 | 6,928 | Breslow & Enstrom 1980b [16] |
|||
38-94 | 1965 | 17 | 4,174 | Kaplan et al. 1987c [18] |
|||
Nottingham Longitudinal Study of Activity and Ageing |
UK | >65 | Randomly selected older community residents | 1985 | 5 | 1,042 | Rumble and Morgan 1992 [28] |
Polysomnography [27, 36] and Wrist Actigraphy [4] a | |||||||
NIH-funded protocols | USA | ≥50 | Healthy elders who underwent polysomnography at University of Pittsburgh’s sleep center |
1981-1997 | 4.1-19.5 | 184 | Dew et al. 2003 [27] |
Penn State Cohort | USA | ≥20 | Probability sample of adults from central Pennsylvania |
NR | 14 (males); 10(females) |
1,741 | Vgontzas et al. 2010 [36] |
Women’s Health Initiative Observational Study |
USA | 50-81 | Subsample from WHI San Diego site structured to over-represent women reporting sleep durations of ≤6 hrs or ≥8 hrs |
1995-9 | Mean=10.5 | 444 | Kripke et al. 2011 [4] |
Sleep measure types are described in detail in the “Sleep measurement” component of the Results section.
Study included for completeness, but not included in the results table (Table 2) given multiple publications on the same study population.
Study relied upon in results table (Table 2) given multiple publications on same study population.
NR indicates “not reported.”
Table 2.
Significant Elevation in Mortality Risk Reporteda | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||||
Cohort Name (Country) | Age Range (Years) |
Follow-Up (Years) |
Cohort Size (N) |
Percent Male |
Findings for Specific Sleep Categoriesb |
Short Sleep Effect Only |
Long Sleep Effect Only |
Short and Long Sleep Effect (U- Shaped) |
No Sleep Effect |
Survey Sleep Measure: Nighttime Sleep | |||||||||
American Cancer Society (USA) [5] |
30-95+ | 6 | 823,065 | 44% | <4, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, ≥10 |
Malesc Femalesc |
|||
Cancer Prevention Study II (USA) [3] |
30-102 | 6 | 1,116,936 | 43% | 3f, 4mf, 5mf, 6mf8mf, 9mf, ≥10mf | Malesc Femalesc |
|||
Dalarna County postal survey (Sweden) [8] |
45-65 | 12 | 1,870 | 48% | <6, 6-8, >8m | Males | Females | ||
Elderly cohort (USA) [10] | 65-98 | 3.5 | 1,855 | 34% | <5, 5, 6, 7, 8, ≥9 | Males Females |
|||
FINRISK Surveys (Finland) [11] |
25-64 | 35 | 23,290 | 49% | ≤5mf, 6f, 7-8, 9f, ≥10mf | Males Females |
|||
Japan Collaborative Cohort Study for Evaluation of Cancer (Japan) [21] |
40-79 | Median=14.3 | 98,634 | 42% | ≤4mf, 5, 6, 7, 8f, 9mf, ≥10mf |
Males Females |
|||
Massachusetts Health Care Panel Study (USA) [24] |
66-98 | 5 | 1,235 | 38% | 1-6 or ≥9h vs. 7-8 | Males Females |
|||
Miyagi Prefecture survey (Japan) [25] |
≥40 | 4 | 4,318 | 40% | 7-8 vs. ≤6 or ≥9 | Combined sexes |
|||
National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey I (USA) [26] |
32-86 | 8-10 | 9,789 | 37% | ≤5, 6, 7, 8*, ≥9* | Combined sexes |
|||
Rancho Bernardo Study (USA) [98] |
60-96 | 19 | 2,001 | 44% | <6, 6, 7, 8, ≥9f | Females | Males | ||
Whitehall II Cohort (UK) [35] | 35-55 | Mean=17.1 | 10,308 | 67% | ≤5, 6, 7, 8, ≥9 | Combined sexes |
|||
Survey Sleep Measure: 24-Hour Sleep | |||||||||
Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey (CLHLS) (China) [97] |
65-100+ | 3 | 12,671 | 43% | ≤5m, 6, 7, 8, 9, ≥10m |
Males | Females | ||
Finnish Twin Cohort (Finland) [1] |
24-101 | 21 | 21,268 | 48% | <7mf, 7-8, >8mf | Males Females |
|||
Health Interview Study of Barcelona (HISB) (Spain) [14] |
≥65 | 5 | 1,219 | 38% | <7, 7-9, >9 | Males Females |
|||
National Health Interview Survey (NHIS) (USA) [115] |
≥18 | 16 | 38,891 | ≤6, 7-8, ≥9 | Combined sexes | ||||
Nurses’ Health Study (USA) [29] |
40-65 | 14 | 82,969 | 0% | ≤5, 6, 7, 8f, ≥9f | Females | |||
Ohsaki Cohort Study (Japan) [116] |
40-79 | Mean=10.8 | 49,256 | 48% | ≤6, 7, 8*, 9*, ≥10* |
Combined sexes |
|||
Scottish working cohort (Scotland) [30] |
<60 females; <65 for males |
25 | 6,797 | 86% | <7, 7-8, >8 | Males Females |
|||
Shizuoka Study (Japan) [31] | 65-85 | 6 | 11,395 | 51% | ≤5, 6, 7, 8m, 9m, ≥10mf |
Males Females |
|||
Spanish cohort (Spain) [32] | ≥60 | Mean=6.8 | 3,820 | 44% | ≤6f, 7, 8m, 9m, ≥10mf |
Males | Females | ||
Study of Osteoporotic Fractures (USA) [33] |
≥69 | Mean=6.9 | 8,101 | 0% | <6, 6-7, 8, 9f, ≥10f | Females | |||
Survey Sleep Measure: Bedtime and Waking Time | |||||||||
Bambui Health and Ageing Study (Brazil) [6] |
≥60 | 23 | 1,512 | 39% | <6, 6, 7, 8, ≥9* | Combined sexes |
|||
Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Aging Study (Israel) [99] |
75-94 | 20 | 1,166 | 55% | <7, 7-9, >9* | Combined sexes |
|||
Department of Health and Social Security Survey (UK) [9] |
≥65 | 10 | 862 | 51%d | <7, 8, 9, 10, 11, ≥12* |
Combined sexes |
|||
Gifu Prefecture cohort study (Japan) [12] |
20-67 | Mean=11.9 | 5,322 | 46% | <7m, 7-8, 9, ≥10 | Males | Females | ||
Jichi Medical School Cohort Study (Japan) [22] |
19-93 | Mean=8.2 | 11,325 | 39% | <6m; 6; 7; 8; ≥9 | Males | Females | ||
Kiryat Yovel Community Health Study (Israel) [23] |
≥50 | 9-11 | 1,842 | 46% | <6, 6-8, >8 | Malese Femalese |
|||
Survey of Health and Living Status of the Elderly in Taiwan (Taiwan) [34] |
≥64 | 10 | 3,079 | 57% | <7, 7, 8f, 9f, ≥10mf | Males Females |
|||
Survey Sleep Measure: Not Clearly Specified | |||||||||
Chin-Shan Community Cardiovascular Cohort Study (Taiwan) [7] |
≥35 | Median=15.9 | 3,430 | 56% | ≤5; 6, 7, 8, ≥9mf | Combined sexes |
|||
Health and Lifestyle Survey (UK) [13] |
≥18 | 7 | 6,096 | 44% | <6, 6-8, ≥9 | Malesc | Females | ||
Human Population Laboratory (Alameda County study) (USA) [18] |
38-94 | 17 | 1,454 | NR | <7 or >8 vs. 7-8 | Combined sexesg |
|||
Nottingham Longitudinal Study of Activity and Ageing (UK) [28] |
≥65 | 5 | 1,042 | NR | <4, 4-9, ≥10 | Combined sexes |
|||
Polysomnography [36] and Wrist Actigraphy [4] | |||||||||
NIH-funded protocols (USA) [27] |
58.7-91.4 | Mean=12.8 | 184 | 46% | <6, ≥6 | Combined sexes |
|||
Penn State Cohort Study (USA) [36] |
≥20 | 14 (men); 10 (women) |
1,741 | 42% | <6, ≥6 | Males Females |
|||
Women’s Health Initiative Observational Study (USA) [4] | 50-81 | Mean=10.5 | 444 | 0% | <5f, 5-6.5, >6.5f | Females |
Results were considered statistically significant on the basis of either stated P-values (P < 0.05) or 95% confidence intervals. “Combined sexes” denotes studies adjusting for sex. “Males” and “Females” indicate sex-specific results.
Number in bold indicates the reference category;
indicate a statistically significant elevation in mortality risk associated with a particular sleep category among: males and females in a model adjusted for sex; males in sex-specific analyses; and females in sex-specific analyses, respectively.
Significance tests not reported for specific sleep categories; overall sleep effect inferred from text.
C Gale, University of Southampton, personal communication, 2012.
Burazeri et al. used the short sleeping category as the reference category and reported a significant difference for males between the longest sleepers (>8 hrs) and the shortest sleepers (<6 hrs). However, the confidence intervals for the effect estimates for the long and short sleepers relative to the average sleepers (6-8 hrs) all overlapped substantially, strongly suggesting that if the middle group had been the reference, no significant effect of either long or short sleep would have been found.
Kaplan et al. did observe a significant elevation in mortality risk for 50-59 year olds, but no significant increases in risk for those 38-49, 60-69, or 70+ years of age.
NR denotes not reported.
Results
Sleep duration measurement
There is no perfect way to measure sleep duration. Survey-based methods include both retrospective questions and prospective sleep logs. However, almost all epidemiological studies examining sleep duration and mortality have used retrospective habitual sleep questions, and the actual questions have varied. Broadly, questions fall into three categories. A key question, not previously explored, is whether the type of question asked matters for associations with mortality.
Nighttime sleep
The most frequently used questions query sleep duration during a typical night. Wording varies, such as “About how many hours of sleep did you get on a typical night during the past 4 weeks” [4] versus “In the last two weeks, how many hours a night did you usually sleep?” [10]. Other variations include asking separately about weekdays and weekends, or asking about “average” rather than “typical” sleep.
24-hour sleep
Other surveys ask about sleep over 24 hours: e.g.,“How many hours do you usually sleep per day (including both nighttime and daytime sleep)” [32], or simply specify sleep “per 24 hours” [1]. Naps may be explicitly mentioned, and a few surveys separately ask about nap frequency and duration [33].
Bedtimes and waking times
Some studies ask about usual bedtime and waking time, and then calculate duration (e.g., [9, 12, 22, 34]). In theory, there may be advantages to this approach over asking about total duration. Even though bedtime and waking time may also be difficult to report accurately, they do not require mental arithmetic. Also, while eight hours may be considered a normative sleep duration for adults, it is not clear there is an equivalent normative answer for bedtime and waking time, and, therefore, responses may be less subject to biases such as social desirability bias.
Non-survey sleep measurement
There are two “objective” ways to estimate sleep duration based on physiological monitoring: polysomnography and actigraphy. Polysomnography estimates sleep based on electrical brain activity and is considered by many to be the “gold standard” for defining sleep [43, 44]. However, polysomnography is not well suited to estimating usual sleep duration in a population-based study; the unfamiliarity of the procedure and its invasiveness often alter sleep. While generally carried out in a clinical laboratory, polysomnography can be adapted for home monitoring [45]. Typically polysomnography includes about 20 electrodes and monitors measuring brain activity, muscle activity, eye movements, heart activity, airflow, breathing, and blood oxygen levels, but fewer monitors could be used were the focus just on sleep duration. Two studies reviewed here estimate duration from polysomnography [27, 36]; one included an acclimation night [27].
Actigraphy estimates sleep based on arm movement using multi-directional accelerometers that measure activity counts per epoch (e.g., 30 seconds); it is non-invasive, can be worn for many days and is simple to use. Activity counts are used to categorize epochs as sleep or wake. Actigraphy is thought not to alter sleep because there is no “first-night effect” [46]. Hip actigraphs have been used to measure physical activity [47], but wrist models are used to estimate sleep. They can store many days’ data and yield estimates of total duration and some other sleep parameters, such as fragmentation, but not sleep stages or apnea.
Actigraphy has recently been incorporated into several epidemiological studies [48-50]. Total sleep duration generally correlates highly between polysomnography and actigraphy – a comprehensive review summarized the correlation at around 90 percent [46], with higher estimated total sleep for actigraphy than polysomnography and worse agreement for those with worse quality sleep [51, 52]. Only one of the mortality studies has used actigraphy [4].
Comparisons of survey sleep duration and actigraphy or polysomnography
Many small clinical studies have compared survey-measured sleep duration to an “objective” measure among individuals with sleep or health problems, but we have identified only four large studies of community-based adults with such comparisons. In these studies average physiologically estimated sleep is between 6 and 6.5 hours, with a standard deviation of around one hour. Very few individuals have estimated sleep durations longer than nine hours, often the definition of “long sleep” in surveys.
In the Sleep Heart Health Study, a population-based study of older adults, one night of home polysomnography was compared to the next-morning survey estimate of the same night and to previously reported habitual sleep [53]. The average polysomnography sleep time was 6 hours, and the average survey estimate for the same night was 18 minutes longer, but the correlation was only 0.16. Habitual sleep reports averaged an hour longer than polysomnography and the correlation was 0.18. Greater divergence was found for those with less education.
The Rotterdam Study of older adults included several nights of actigraphy (average duration 6.5 hours), and subjects also reported estimated sleep each morning [54]. On average the morning estimates were 23 minutes longer than actigraph sleep, but about one-third of the sample had more than an hour difference. On average, women reported a quarter hour less sleep than men while their actigraph sleep was a quarter hour longer. Several covariates, including age and cognitive function, had opposite effects on sleep diary and actigraph sleep.
An ancillary study to the Women’s Health Initiative [4] oversampled women reporting short or long sleep in response to “About how many hours of sleep did you get on a typical night during the past 4 weeks?” The question was asked twice, a few months apart, with a correlation of 0.60. Participants wore an actigraph for seven nights and wrote down nightly estimates. Self-reported typical sleep and averaged self-reported nightly estimates were very similar (correlation 0.84). Actigraph sleep averaged six hours, about 48 minutes shorter than either self-report measure. The correlation between actigraph sleep and self-reported sleep was 0.48. Examining time in bed from the actigraphy – rather than total minutes coded as actual sleep – did not increase the correlation.
An ancillary study to CARDIA compared 3 nights of actigraph sleep duration (average six hours) with self-reported measures in a middle-aged population [55]. The correlation between the average actigraph and self-reported sleep was 0.47, with self-reported sleep averaging 48 minutes longer, similar to the Women’s Health Initiative [4]. Persons with mortality risk factors (fair/poor self-rated health, obese or more depressive symptoms) systematically reported shorter sleep at the same level of measured sleep, implying that reporting bias could generate associations between shorter reported sleep and mortality even were there no association for actigraph-estimated sleep.
These studies consistently find that sleep estimated from polysomnography or actigraphy is shorter than self-reported sleep. This does not just represent different calibration since the correlation is only low to moderate. As with any imperfect measure, a correlation depressed by random noise is less concerning than systematic bias. Studies examining whether demographic or health factors alter associations between measured and self-reported sleep find that they do, suggesting there may be systemic biases.
Correlates of self-reported sleep duration
Systematic and strong associations between sociodemographic and/or health-related factors and reported sleep duration could theoretically explain some of the reported associations between sleep duration and mortality. Therefore, in the following section we identify correlates of self-reported sleep and consider the implications of these correlations for our understanding of the relationship between sleep and mortality.
Demographic correlates
Men typically report longer sleep than do women [56]. Older individuals are more likely to report either short or long sleep compared to younger individuals [57]. Compared to Whites, Hispanics are more likely to report short sleep [58] and Blacks are more likely to report both short and long sleep [57-59]. Reporting short or long sleep is also associated with low socioeconomic status [57, 60, 61], and with working long hours [57, 62].
Social correlates
Sleep is often shared between partners and is likely shaped by household and partnership characteristics. Unmarried individuals are more likely to report shorter sleep [63] or shorter and longer sleep [57, 64]. Co-sleeping is associated with shorter self-reported sleep duration [65]. Differences in objective sleep duration based on marital status have not been reported although there are indications of differences in sleep quality and continuity [66]. Finally, living in the inner city and perceived neighborhood disorder also influence individuals’ self-reported sleep properties [58, 67, 68]. These household- and neighborhood-level characteristics are also linked to health outcomes, including mortality risk (e.g., [69-73]).
Health correlates
Longer self-reported sleep has been associated with: increased total cholesterol and a higher total/HDL cholesterol ratio among the elderly [74]; greater carotid intimal medial thickness [75]; recent treatment for cancer [62]; having had a heart attack or angina [62]; poorer cognitive function [76], having a history of depression [60]; and using antidepressants [60].
Shorter self-reported sleep has been reported as associated with: obesity [77-81]; metabolic syndrome [82, 83]; diabetes [81]; hypertension [84]; cortisol secretion [85]; poor general health [63]; and having more medical conditions [86, 87]. Shorter self-reported sleep has also been associated with reduced cognitive function [56], unhappiness, symptoms of depression [56, 88], and more suicidal ideation and attempts [89].
Other studies have documented an increased odds of reporting either shorter sleep or longer sleep in association with obesity [57], diabetes [57, 90], hypertension [57], cardiovascular disease [57, 90, 91], metabolic syndrome [92], physical illness [61], activity limitations [57], poorer self-rated health [93], poorer objective and subjective cognitive functioning [94], depression [57], other psychiatric conditions [61, 86], and with deleterious health behaviors including smoking, alcohol consumption, and low levels of physical activity [57].
Implications of correlates with self-reported sleep duration
Nearly all of the demographic, social, and health-related factors identified above as correlated with self-reported sleep are also known to be associated with mortality risk (e.g., [69, 70, 95, 96]), suggesting that associations observed between self-reported sleep duration and mortality risk need to be interpreted with care. Demographic and social factors logically lie upstream of sleep duration while health factors may lie upstream or downstream from self-reported sleep duration. Ambiguities about where in the causal pathway the health conditions lie make it impossible, given the present state of knowledge, to distinguish confounders and mediators.
Differences in sleep length by sociodemographic factors or by health conditions could reflect true differences, differential reporting or a combination of the two. Regardless, failure to fully adjust for these factors when testing for effects of short or long sleep on mortality (unless they are mediators) would result in residual confounding and an incorrect inference with regard to the independent effect of sleep on mortality risk.
Review of studies of sleep duration and mortality
We identified 42 prospective studies of sleep duration and mortality drawing on 35 distinct study populations across the globe (Table 1). Sample sizes ranged from 184 [27] to over 1 million individuals [2, 3]; the interquartile range was from 1,512 to 12,671. Follow-up times ranged from 3 years in the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey [97] to 35 years in a Finnish population-based study [11]. Three studies employed polysomnography or actigraphy [4, 27, 36]; the remainder relied on self-reported sleep duration. A number of studies reported sex-specific results and thus the number of findings in Table 2 (N=55) exceeds the number of distinct study populations (N=35).
With the exception of individual reports of decreased mortality risk in association with long sleep in Scottish men [30] and with short sleep in older Chinese women [97], there are four types of findings that have been reported in these studies: (1) that only short sleep is associated with increased mortality; (2) that only long sleep is associated with increased mortality; (3) that both long and short sleep duration are associated with increased mortality (the “U-shaped effect”); and (4) that sleep duration is not associated with increased mortality.
Findings have not been consistent (Table 2). Among 55 findings, two support an association of increased mortality risk with short sleep only (both in populations of young to middle-aged Japanese men) [12, 22], 16 support an association with long sleep only, 14 support a U-shaped effect, and 23 support no significant association.
Even among studies reporting significant associations, effect measures have been modest. Higher values, such as the hazard ratio (HR) of 2.4 reported by Amagai et al. 2004 [22] for males sleeping less than 6 hours, were rare. The largest study – the Cancer Prevention Study II [3] – reported significant effect sizes ranging from HR=1.07 for women who reported sleeping 5 hours per night to a maximum HR=1.41 for women who reported sleeping 10 or more hours per night. This study controlled for many demographic, behavioral, and medical risk factors and the authors suggest that most of the mortality risk observed in association with short sleep could be explained by comorbidities [3].
The variability among study findings is striking given that recent reviews have concluded that both short and long sleep are consistently associated with increased mortality risk [37-40]. Below we explore some possible explanations for heterogeneity among study findings.
Sex effects
Interestingly, a number of studies report divergent effects for males and females (Table 2) [8, 12, 13, 22, 32, 97, 98], however the distribution across study finding types is very similar for the two sexes. There appears to be no consistent difference between males and females with regard to the pattern of sleep duration effects on mortality.
Measurement effects
Given the potential problems with survey sleep measures, it is relevant to first examine the results of the studies employing physiologically estimated sleep duration measures. A study employing actigraphy among women 50-81 years of age (N=444) concluded that the relationship between sleep duration and mortality was U-shaped [4]. Although the study population was specifically sampled to over-represent short and long sleepers, the longest average actigraphic sleep time was 8.37 hours. While mortality was elevated for both the short sleepers and long sleepers when sleep categories were collapsed into three groups (<5; 5-6.5 (referent); >6.5 hours), the death rates by more detailed sleep categories do not show a dose response for either side of the duration distribution. The top and bottom categories (<4.5 or >7.5 hours) have relatively low mortality, albeit with small numbers, and the highest mortality risk was observed in women sleeping either 4.5-5 hours or – interestingly – between 7 and 7.5 hours.
Neither of the two studies employing polysomnography reported significant associations between sleep duration and mortality [27, 36]. In both studies, sleep duration was dichotomized at < 6 and 6+ hours, precluding the possibility of finding a U-shaped effect. As in the study employing actigraphy, Dew et al [27] observed that no subjects had recorded sleep times greater than 8.3 hours.
One interesting pattern among studies using survey sleep measures is that all of the studies reporting U-shaped associations measured sleep duration with questions about typical nighttime sleep or 24-hour sleep (Table 2). None of the studies that asked about usual bedtimes and waketimes reported a U-shaped association; rather, they reported either no association [12, 22, 23] or only a long sleep association [6, 9, 34, 99], or, in the case of two studies of young to middle-aged Japanese men, only a short sleep association [12, 22]. That the U-shaped associations are exclusively found in studies asking about usual sleep duration may be informative and suggests the possibility of systematic response biases, with people in generally good health more likely to give a “normative” response (i.e., 7 or 8 hours) and those in worse health more likely to give a “non-normative” (shorter or longer duration) response.
In three studies [1, 30, 35], sleep duration data were collected twice, several years apart. In one of these studies there was a significant U-shaped sleep duration effect when using the more recent sleep data, but no significant sleep duration effect with longer follow-up [35]. In two studies, moving to either shorter or longer sleep (from average sleep) was associated with increased mortality [1, 35] however no such effect was observed in the third [30]. Change in reported sleep may also reflect an underlying health process.
Table 2 shows how sleep duration was categorized, with the numbers in bold indicating the reference category (where specified). The number of analytic sleep categories varied from two to eight. The rationale for any particular categorization scheme was rarely stated, and almost no studies report sensitivity analyses with alternate cut-points. Short sleep was typically defined as <6 or <7 hours per night, though lower values were also represented. Long sleep was variously defined as >8, 9, or 10 hours. The reference category was most often 7 hours, but was sometimes 7-8 and occasionally 6-8 or 7-9 hours. In two instances, short and long sleepers were combined and compared to the reference group with a single statistical test [18, 24]. As noted earlier, in the two polysomnography studies, short sleepers (<6h) were compared to all others [27, 36]. In the remaining studies, however, separate statistical tests were carried out for each sleep category relative to the reference category. Plausibly, these coding and analytic differences could help to explain the diverse results. For example, two studies published two years apart from the same cohort yielded different findings: increased mortality for long sleep only when using three categories [20] and increased mortality for both long and short sleep when using seven categories [21].
One possibility is that those studies with null findings were underpowered, given the modest effect sizes expected. However, the study populations of studies reporting null effects were not anomalously small, the duration of follow-up was similar to those studies reporting significant effects, and the effect sizes that they reported were not consistently above one. Thus it is unlikely that the null findings were uniformly due to lack of power.
Adjustment effects
Studies varied in the number of sociodemographic and health-related factors that they adjusted for. Some study investigators suggest (e.g., [29]) that including health conditions like diabetes and hypertension – which themselves may be on the causal pathway between sleep duration and mortality risk – would result in overadjustment. The judgment about whether some of these health conditions lie upstream or downstream from sleep duration (or are simply confounders) will influence the interpretation of these adjusted models. It is certainly the case, however, that divergent results among studies could be due in part to the inclusion of different sets of control variables.
Discussion
Sleep is complex. Different ways of measuring it yield different results and elucidating the role it plays in our health is similarly challenging. One of the classic criteria for inferring causality from a body of observational studies is consistency [100], and, as shown here, findings are not consistent for the association between sleep duration and mortality.
While a substantial fraction of the findings support the possibility that at least long self-reported sleep duration may be associated with increased mortality, we do not know whether long sleep duration is itself a causal factor for mortality, a marker of illness [26, 101], or a product of response bias driven by other mortality risk factors. Studies with objective sleep measurement have found that there are very few individuals with measured sleep longer than eight and one half hours [4, 27, 48], emphasizing an essential difference between sleep as estimated by physiologic measures and sleep duration as reported with survey questions. It is possible that the two types of measures capture different aspects of the sleep experience.
To date, the literature has focused on perceived sleep but perceived sleep is likely influenced by a complex combination of true sleep duration, some of the psychosocial and health-related factors identified earlier, and, very possibly, other, yet-to-be identified factors. Although actigraphy may be the most suitable measurement modality for sleep duration at present, it may be prohibitively expensive for use on a large scale. If survey measures must be used, we suspect that less biased data might result from querying usual bedtime and waking time before asking about duration, to help walk respondents through the steps of calculating an answer, or using prospective sleep log data collection, which was not employed in any of these studies.
Additional sleep phenotypes of importance include sleep quality and timing and the presence or absence of sleep apnea; these are often not considered in studies focused on sleep duration. Indeed, the diverse results across studies of sleep duration and mortality may point to a very complicated relationship between the two, one that may be modified by demographic, social, and health-related factors, as well, possibly, as by these other key sleep phenotypes. It is also essential to consider that using a single measurement over the life course, as is typically done, is clearly sub-optimal for describing this complex, and likely dynamic, exposure.
Prior reviews of this topic have outlined possible mechanisms linking sleep duration to increased mortality risk. Typically, short sleep duration is thought to increase mortality risk through adverse endocrinologic, immunologic, or metabolic effects [102-105], through the induction of chronic, low-grade inflammation [106-109] (but see [110]), or via increases in cortisol secretion or altered growth hormone metabolism [85, 111]. However, although the pathway outlined from the metabolic changes or pro-inflammatory changes observed in the experimental setting to mortality risk is via increased cardiovascular disease risk, a recent meta-analysis reported no significant increase in cardiovascular-related mortality [40].
Biologic mechanisms proposed for the association between long sleep and mortality risk include increased sleep fragmentation, fatigue, changes in immune function, photoperiodic abnormalities, lack of physiologic challenge, depression, or underlying disease processes [37]. As suggested by others [101], it is critical to distinguish between those mechanisms that may mediate the relationship between sleep duration and mortality and those that would confound the relationship as the two alternative explanations have very different implications.
Reverse causation – that is, that individuals with health conditions putting them at increased risk of mortality have either lengthened or shortened sleep – is an additional compelling alternative explanation for observed associations between sleep duration and mortality [101]. Past reviews have discounted this possibility given that a number of studies controlled for participants’ health conditions. However, a complete accounting of health status – particularly the determination of underlying but as yet undetected disease – would be virtually impossible.
Our conclusions differ from those of two recent meta-analyses that report consistent increases in mortality risk among short and long sleepers, with an approximately 10% increase in mortality risk among short sleepers and a 20-30% increase in mortality risk among long sleepers [39, 40]. Our review includes a substantially larger number of studies than either of those meta-analyses and includes the studies employing objective sleep duration measures, two of which were published in the intervening years. Although a summary estimate of the relationship between sleep duration and mortality risk is compelling, meta-analysis of these studies is challenging because of the substantial variability across the original studies with regard to the actual sleep duration question(s) used, which reference sleep category was employed, how long and short sleep were categorized, the level of confounder adjustment, the age range of study subjects, and the length of follow-up.
Integrating across studies employing different definitions of long and short sleep seems particularly problematic with regard to interpreting the final results. In one meta-analysis [39], short sleep definitions from the studies included were ≤4, ≤5, ≤6, and <7 hours per night and long sleep definitions were >8, ≥9, ≥10, and ≥12 hours. The reference category was variously defined as 7 hours, 7-8 hours, 7-9 hours, 6-8 hours and 9 hours. Although in some of the original studies there was only one short or long sleep category, in others results for multiple categories above and/or below the reference category were presented and no decision rule was stated for which category would be chosen for the meta-analysis. In these cases, it appears that most of the time, the shortest of the “short sleep” categories and the longest of the “long sleep” categories were used in the meta-analysis [9, 21, 22, 26, 29, 33-35]. However, in some cases a different category was employed (i.e., the middle of the short/long sleep categories or the shortest of the long sleep categories) [3, 12, 22]. In the other meta-analysis [40], results from multiple short or long sleep categories were pooled to comprise a single short or long sleep group, although how that pooling was undertaken was not specified. Reference groups were also somewhat diverse (i.e., 7-7.9; 7-8; 6-8; 6-7.9; 7-8.9; 9-9.9) in this meta-analysis [40]. In neither meta-analysis was the type of sleep question used explored as a source of heterogeneity.
An additional issue to be considered with meta-analysis is the statistical method used to integrate the original estimates. One of the meta-analyses detected significant heterogeneity between the studies [39] while the other did not [40]. Both meta-analyses employed the DerSimonian and Laird random-effect model [112, 113]; this method, however, has been shown to underestimate heterogeneity across studies, which in turn can lead to overall effect estimates biased away from the null [114].
In summary, the substantial number of negative findings in the literature and our observation that only one quarter of the findings reported a significant U-shaped association between sleep duration and mortality risk would make us question the conclusions of recent reviews that sleep duration is consistently associated with mortality in a U-shaped fashion [37-40]. We would instead suggest that there is not a clear consensus on this association and that careful attention must be paid to measurement bias, confounding, and reverse causation in the interpretation of associations between sleep duration and mortality. Because sleep research is of great interest to the public, researchers should be careful not to overstate the support for these associations as they are so simply – and often – translated into advice.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by grants from the Office of Behavioral and Social Research, National Institute on Aging, and National Institutes of Health (R01 AG042164; R01 AG033903-01; R37 AG030481).
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Abbreviations: none
Conflict of interest: none declared.
References
- [1].Hublin C, Partinen M, Koskenvuo M, Kaprio J. Sleep and mortality: a population-based 22-year follow-up study. Sleep. 2007;30:1245–53. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.10.1245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Hammond EC. Some Preliminary Findings on Physical Complaints From a Prospective Study of 1,064,004 Men and Women. Am J Public Health N. 1964;54:11–23. doi: 10.2105/ajph.54.1.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Kripke DF, Garfinkel L, Wingard DL, Klauber MR, Marler MR. Mortality associated with sleep duration and insomnia. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2002;59:131–6. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.2.131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Kripke DF, Langer RD, Elliott Ja, Klauber MR, Rex KM. Mortality related to actigraphic long and short sleep. Sleep Med. 2011;12:28–33. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.04.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Kripke DF, Simons RN, Garfinkel L, Hammond EC. Short and long sleep and sleeping pills. Is increased mortality associated? Arch Gen Psych. 1979;36:103–16. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1979.01780010109014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Castro-Costa E, Dewey ME, Ferri CP, Uchôa E, Firmo JOA, Rocha FL, et al. Association between sleep duration and all-cause mortality in old age: 9-year follow-up of the Bambuí Cohort Study, Brazil. Journal Sleep Res. 2011;20:303–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00884.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Chien K-L, Chen P-C, Hsu H-C, Su T-C, Sung F-C, Chen M-F, et al. Habitual sleep duration and insomnia and the risk of cardiovascular events and all-cause death: report from a community-based cohort. Sleep. 2010;33:177–84. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.2.177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Mallon L, Broman JE, Hetta J. Sleep complaints predict coronary artery disease mortality in males: a 12-year follow-up study of a middle-aged Swedish population. J Intern Med. 2002;251:207–16. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2796.2002.00941.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Gale C, Martyn C. Larks and owls and health, wealth, and wisdom. BMJ. 1998;317:1675–7. doi: 10.1136/bmj.317.7174.1675. Clinical research ed. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Pollak CP, Perlick D, Linsner JP, Wenston J, Hsieh F. Sleep problems in the community elderly as predictors of death and nursing home placement. J Commun Health. 1990;15:123–35. doi: 10.1007/BF01321316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Kronholm E, Laatikainen T, Peltonen M, Sippola R, Partonen T. Self-reported sleep duration, all-cause mortality, cardiovascular mortality and morbidity in Finland. Sleep Med. 2011;12:215–21. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Kojima M, Wakai K, Kawamura T, Tamakoshi A, Aoki R, Lin Y, et al. Sleep patterns and total mortality: a 12-year follow-up study in Japan. J Epidemiol. 2000;10:87–93. doi: 10.2188/jea.10.87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Huppert FA, Whittington JE. Symptoms of psychological distress predict 7-year mortality. Psychol Med. 1995;25:1073–86. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700037569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Ruigómez A, Alonso J, Antó JM. Relationship of health behaviours to five-year mortality in an elderly cohort. Age Ageing. 1995;24:113–9. doi: 10.1093/ageing/24.2.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Belloc NB. Relationship of health practices and mortality. Prev Med. 1973;2:67–81. doi: 10.1016/0091-7435(73)90009-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Breslow L, Enstrom JE. Persistence of health habits and their relationship to mortality. Prev Med. 1980;9:469–83. doi: 10.1016/0091-7435(80)90042-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Wingard DL, Berkman LF. Mortality risk associated with sleeping patterns among adults. Sleep. 1983;6:102–7. doi: 10.1093/sleep/6.2.102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Kaplan GA, Seeman TE, Cohen RD, Knudsen LP, Guralnik J. Mortality among the elderly in the Alameda County Study: behavioral and demographic risk factors. Am J Public Health. 1987;77:307–12. doi: 10.2105/ajph.77.3.307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Tamakoshi A, Ohno Y. Self-reported sleep duration as a predictor of all-cause mortality: results from the JACC study, Japan. Sleep. 2004;27:51–4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Suzuki K. Health conditions and mortality in the Japan Collaborative Cohort Study for Evaluation of Cancer (JACC) APJCP. 2007;825(Suppl) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Ikehara S, Iso H, Date C, Kikuchi S, Watanabe Y, Wada Y, et al. Association of sleep duration with mortality from cardiovascular disease and other causes for Japanese men and women: the JACC study. Sleep. 2009;32:295–301. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.3.295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Amagai Y, Ishikawa S, Gotoh T, Doi Y, Kayaba K, Nakamura Y, et al. Sleep duration and mortality in Japan: the Jichi Medical School Cohort Study. J Epidemiol. 2004;14:124–8. doi: 10.2188/jea.14.124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Burazeri G, Gofin J, Kark JD. Over 8 hours of sleep--marker of increased mortality in Mediterranean population: follow-up population study. Croat Med J. 2003;44:193–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Branch LG, Jette AM. Personal health practices and mortality among the elderly. Am J Public Health. 1984;74:1126–9. doi: 10.2105/ajph.74.10.1126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Tsubono Y, Fukao A, Hisamichi S. Health practices and mortality in a rural Japanese population. Tohuku J Exp Med. 1993;171:339–48. doi: 10.1620/tjem.171.339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].JE Gangwisch, Heymsfield SB, Boden-Albala B, Buijs RM, Kreier F, Opler MG, et al. Sleep duration associated with mortality in elderly, but not middle-aged, adults in a large US sample. Sleep. 2008;31:1087–96. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [27].Dew MA, Hoch CC, Buysse DJ, Monk TH, Begley AE, Houck PR, et al. Healthy older adults’ sleep predicts all-cause mortality at 4 to 19 years of follow-up. Psychosom Med. 2003;65(1):63–73. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000039756.23250.7c. Epub 2003/01/30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].Rumble R, Morgan K. Hypnotics, sleep, and mortality in elderly people. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1992;40:787–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1992.tb01850.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].Patel SR, Ayas NT, Malhotra MR, White DP, Schernhammer ES, Speizer FE, et al. A prospective study of sleep duration and mortality risk in women. Sleep. 2004;27:440–4. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.3.440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].Heslop P, Smith GD, Metcalfe C, Macleod J, Hart C. Sleep duration and mortality: The effect of short or long sleep duration on cardiovascular and all-cause mortality in working men and women. Sleep Med. 2002;3:305–14. doi: 10.1016/s1389-9457(02)00016-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [31].Suzuki E, Yorifuji T, Ueshima K, Takao S, Sugiyama M, Ohta T, et al. Sleep duration, sleep quality and cardiovascular disease mortality among the elderly: a population-based cohort study. Prev Med. 2009;49:135–41. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Mesas AE, López-García E, León-Muñoz LM, Guallar-Castillón P, Rodríguez-Artalejo F. Sleep duration and mortality according to health status in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010;58:1870–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.03071.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].Stone KL, Ewing SK, Ancoli-Israel S, Ensrud KE, Redline S, Bauer DC, et al. Self-reported sleep and nap habits and risk of mortality in a large cohort of older women. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009;57:604–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.02171.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [34].Lan T-Y, Lan T-H, Wen C-P, Lin Y-H, Chuang Y-L. Nighttime sleep, Chinese afternoon nap, and mortality in the elderly. Sleep. 2007;30:1105–10. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.9.1105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [35].Ferrie JE, Shipley MJ, Cappuccio FP, Brunner E, Miller MA, Kumari M, et al. A prospective study of change in sleep duration: associations with mortality in the Whitehall II cohort. Sleep. 2007;30:1659–66. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.12.1659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [36].Vgontzas AN, Liao D, Pejovic S, Calhoun S, Karataraki M, Basta M, et al. Insomnia with short sleep duration and mortality: the Penn State cohort. Sleep. 2010;33(9):1159–64. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.9.1159. Epub 2010/09/23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [37].Grandner Ma, Drummond SPa. Who are the long sleepers? Towards an understanding of the mortality relationship. Sleep medicine reviews. 2007;11:341–60. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [38].Grandner Ma, Hale L, Moore M, Patel NP. Mortality associated with short sleep duration: The evidence, the possible mechanisms, and the future. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14:191–203. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [39].Cappuccio FP, D’Elia L, Strazzullo P, Miller Ma. Sleep duration and all-cause mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Sleep. 2010;33:585–92. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.5.585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [40].Gallicchio L, Kalesan B. Sleep duration and mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Sleep Research. 2009;18:148–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00732.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [41].Ferrie JE, Kumari M, Salo P, Singh-Manoux A, Kivimaki M. Sleep epidemiology--a rapidly growing field. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;40(6):1431–7. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyr203. Epub 2011/12/14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [42].Qureshi AI, Giles WH, Croft JB, Bliwise DL. Habitual sleep patterns and risk for stroke and coronary heart disease: a 10-year follow-up from NHANES I. Neurology. 1997;48:904–11. doi: 10.1212/wnl.48.4.904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [43].Dement W, Kleitman N. Cyclic variations in EEG during sleep and their relation to eye movements, body motility, and dreaming. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1957;9(4):673–90. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(57)90088-3. Epub 1957/11/01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [44].Rechtschaffen A, Kales A, editors. A Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques, and Scoring System for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects. Brain Information Service/Brain Research Institute, University of California; Los Angeles: 1968. [Google Scholar]
- [45].Redline S, Sanders MH, Lind BK, Quan SF, Iber C, Gottlieb DJ, et al. Methods for obtaining and analyzing unattended polysomnography data for a multicenter study. Sleep Heart Health Research Group. Sleep. 1998;21:759–67. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [46].Ancoli-Israel S, Cole R, Alessi C, Chambers M, Moorcroft W, Pollak CP. The role of actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep. 2003;26(3):342–92. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.3.342. Epub 2003/05/17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [47].Luke A, Dugas LR, Durazo-Arvizu RA, Cao G, Cooper RS. Assessing physical activity and its relationship to cardiovascular risk factors: NHANES 2003-2006. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:387. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-387. Epub 2011/05/27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [48].Lauderdale DS, Knutson KL, Yan LL, Rathouz PJ, Hulley SB, Sidney S, et al. Objectively measured sleep characteristics among early-middle-aged adults: the CARDIA study. Am J Epidemiol. 2006;164:1–5. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [49].Song Y, Ancoli-Israel S, Lewis CE, Redline S, Harrison SL, Stone KL. The association of race/ethnicity with objectively measured sleep characteristics in older men. Behav Sleep Med. 2011;10(1):54–69. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2012.636276. Epub 2012/01/19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [50].Stone KL, Ancoli-Israel S, Blackwell T, Ensrud KE, Cauley JA, Redline S, et al. Actigraphy-measured sleep characteristics and risk of falls in older women. Arch Intern Med. 2008;168(16):1768–75. doi: 10.1001/archinte.168.16.1768. Epub 2008/09/10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [51].Blackwell T, Ancoli-Israel S, Redline S, Stone KL. Factors that may influence the classification of sleep-wake by wrist actigraphy: the MrOS Sleep Study. J Clin Sleep Med. 2011;7(4):357–67. doi: 10.5664/JCSM.1190. Epub 2011/09/08. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [52].Blackwell T, Redline S, Ancoli-Israel S, Schneider JL, Surovec S, Johnson NL, et al. Comparison of sleep parameters from actigraphy and polysomnography in older women: the SOF study. Sleep. 2008;31:283–91. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.2.283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [53].Silva GE, Goodwin JL, Sherrill DL, Arnold JL, Bootzin RR, Smith T, et al. Relationship Between Reported and Measured Sleep Times: The Sleep Heart Health Study (SHHS) J Clin Sleep Med. 2007;3:622–30. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [54].Van Den Berg JF, Van Rooij FJ, Vos H, Tulen JH, Hofman A, Miedema HM, et al. Disagreement between subjective and actigraphic measures of sleep duration in a population-based study of elderly persons. J Sleep Res. 2008;17(3):295–302. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00638.x. Epub 2008/03/07. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [55].Lauderdale DS, Knutson KL, Yan LL, Liu K, Rathouz PJ. Self-reported and measured sleep duration: how similar are they? Epidemiology. 2008;19:838–45. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e318187a7b0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [56].van den Berg JF, Miedema HM, Tulen JH, Hofman A, Neven AK, Tiemeier H. Sex differences in subjective and actigraphic sleep measures: a population-based study of elderly persons. Sleep. 2009;32(10):1367–75. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.10.1367. Epub 2009/10/24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [57].Krueger PM, Friedman EM. Sleep duration in the United States: a cross-sectional population-based study. Am J Epidemiol. 2009;169:1052–63. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [58].Hale L, Do DP. Racial differences in self-reports of sleep duration in a population-based study. Sleep. 2007;30:1096–103. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.9.1096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [59].Nunes J, Jean-Louis G, Zizi F, Casimir GJ, von Gizycki H, Brown CD, et al. Sleep duration among black and white Americans: results of the National Health Interview Survey. J Natl Med Assoc. 2008;100:317–22. doi: 10.1016/s0027-9684(15)31244-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [60].Patel SR, Malhotra A, Gottlieb DJ, White DP, Hu FB. Correlates of long sleep duration. Sleep. 2006;29:881–9. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.7.881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [61].Park S, Cho MJ, Chang SM, Bae JN, Jeon HJ, Cho S-J, et al. Relationships of sleep duration with sociodemographic and health-related factors, psychiatric disorders and sleep disturbances in a community sample of Korean adults. J Sleep Res. 2010;19:567–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00841.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [62].Magee CA, Iverson DC, Caputi P. Factors associated with short and long sleep. Prev Med. 2009;49:461–7. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [63].Stranges S, Dorn JM, Shipley MJ, Kandala NB, Trevisan M, Miller MA, et al. Correlates of short and long sleep duration: a cross-cultural comparison between the United Kingdom and the United States: the Whitehall II Study and the Western New York Health Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;168(12):1353–64. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn337. Epub 2008/10/24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [64].Hale L. Who has time to sleep? J Public Health (Oxf) 2005;27:205–11. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdi004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [65].Worthman CM, Brown Ra. Companionable sleep: social regulation of sleep and cosleeping in Egyptian families. J Fam Psychol. 2007;21:124–35. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [66].Troxel WM, Buysse DJ, Matthews KA, Kravitz HM, Bromberger JT, Sowers M, et al. Marital/cohabitation status and history in relation to sleep in midlife women. Sleep. 2010;33(7):973–81. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.7.973. Epub 2010/07/10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [67].Hill TD, Burdette AM, Hale L. Neighborhood disorder, sleep quality, and psychological distress: testing a model of structural amplification. Health Place. 2009;15:1006–13. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2009.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [68].Hale L, Hill TD, Burdette AM. Does sleep quality mediate the association between neighborhood disorder and self-rated physical health? Prev Med. 2010;51:275–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [69].Waite LJ. Does marriage matter? Demography. 1995;32(4):483–507. Epub 1995/11/01. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [70].Waite LJ, Lehrer EL. The Benefits from Marriage and Religion in the United States: A Comparative Analysis. Popul Dev Rev. 2003;29(2):255–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1728-4457.2003.00255.x. Epub 2003/06/01. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [71].Bosma H, van de Mheen HD, Borsboom GJ, Mackenbach JP. Neighborhood socioeconomic status and all-cause mortality. Am J Epidemiol. 2001;153:363–71. doi: 10.1093/aje/153.4.363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [72].Leclere FB, Rogers RG, Peters K. Neighborhood Social Context and Racial Differences in Women ’ s Heart Disease Mortality *. Health (San Francisco) 2012;39:91–107. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [73].Sundquist K, Theobald H, Yang M, Li X, Johansson S-E, Sundquist J. Neighborhood violent crime and unemployment increase the risk of coronary heart disease: a multilevel study in an urban setting. Soc Sci Med. 2006;62:2061–71. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.08.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [74].van den Berg JF, Miedema HM, Tulen JH, Neven AK, Hofman A, Witteman JC, et al. Long sleep duration is associated with serum cholesterol in the elderly: the Rotterdam Study. Psychosom Med. 2008;70(9):1005–11. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318186e656. Epub 2008/10/10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [75].Abe T, Aoki T, Yata S, Okada M. Sleep duration is significantly associated with carotid artery atherosclerosis incidence in a Japanese population. Atherosclerosis. 2011;217:509–13. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2011.02.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [76].Faubel R, López-García E, Guallar-Castillón P, Graciani A, Banegas JR, Rodríguez-Artalejo F. Usual sleep duration and cognitive function in older adults in Spain. J Sleep Res. 2009;18:427–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00759.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [77].Park SE, Kim HM, Kim DH, Kim J, Cha BS, Kim DJ. The association between sleep duration and general and abdominal obesity in Koreans: data from the Korean National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 2001 and 2005. Obesity. 2009;17:767–71. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [78].Stranges S, Cappuccio FP, Kandala N-B, Miller MA, Taggart FM, Kumari M, et al. Cross-sectional versus prospective associations of sleep duration with changes in relative weight and body fat distribution: the Whitehall II Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;167:321–9. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [79].Anic GM, Titus-Ernstoff L, Newcomb PA, Trentham-Dietz A, Egan KM. Sleep duration and obesity in a population-based study. Sleep Med. 2010;11:447–51. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.11.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [80].Magee CA, Iverson DC, Caputi P. Sleep duration and obesity in middle-aged Australian adults. Obesity. 2010;18:420–1. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [81].Singh M, Drake CL, Roehrs T, Hudgel DW, Roth T. The association between obesity and short sleep duration: a population-based study. JCSM. 2005;1:357–63. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [82].Hall MH, Muldoon MF, Jennings JR, Buysse DJ, Flory JD, Manuck SB. Self-reported sleep duration is associated with the metabolic syndrome in midlife adults. Sleep. 2008;31:635–43. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.5.635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [83].Kobayashi D, Takahashi O, Deshpande GA, Shimbo T, Fukui T. Relation between metabolic syndrome and sleep duration in Japan: a large scale cross-sectional study. Internal Med. 2011;50:103–7. doi: 10.2169/internalmedicine.50.4317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [84].Kim J, Jo I. Age-dependent association between sleep duration and hypertension in the adult Korean population. Am J Hypertens. 2010;23:1286–91. doi: 10.1038/ajh.2010.166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [85].Kumari M, Badrick E, Ferrie J, Perski A, Marmot M, Chandola T. Self-reported sleep duration and sleep disturbance are independently associated with cortisol secretion in the Whitehall II study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2009;94:4801–9. doi: 10.1210/jc.2009-0555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [86].Xiang Y-T, Ma X, Lu J-Y, Cai Z-J, Li S-R, Xiang Y-Q, et al. Relationships of sleep duration with sleep disturbances, basic socio-demographic factors, and BMI in Chinese people. Sleep Med. 2009;10:1085–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [87].Foley D, Ancoli-Israel S, Britz P, Walsh J. Sleep disturbances and chronic disease in older adults: results of the 2003 National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Survey. J Psychosom Res. 2004;56:497–502. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2004.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [88].Kawada T, Kuratomi Y, Kanai T. Lifestyle determinants of depressive feeling and a feeling of unhappiness among workers: a study in Japan. Work. 2009;33(3):255–60. doi: 10.3233/WOR-2009-0873. Epub 2009/09/18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [89].Goodwin RD, Marusic A. Association between short sleep and suicidal ideation and suicide attempt among adults in the general population. Sleep. 2008;31:1097–101. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [90].Buxton OM, Marcelli E. Short and long sleep are positively associated with obesity, diabetes, hypertension, and cardiovascular disease among adults in the United States. Soc Sci Med. 2010;71:1027–36. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.05.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [91].Sabanayagam C, Shankar A. Sleep duration and cardiovascular disease: results from the National Health Interview Survey. Sleep. 2010;33:1037–42. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.8.1037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [92].Choi KM, Lee JS, Park HS, Baik SH, Choi DS, Kim SM. Relationship between sleep duration and the metabolic syndrome: Korean National Health and Nutrition Survey 2001. Int J Obes (Lond) 2008;32:1091–7. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [93].Magee CA, Caputi P, Iverson DC. Relationships between self-rated health, quality of life and sleep duration in middle aged and elderly Australians. Sleep Med. 2011;12:346–50. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [94].Kronholm E, Sallinen M, Suutama T, Sulkava R, Era P, Partonen T. Self-reported sleep duration and cognitive functioning in the general population. J Sleep Res. 2009;18:436–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00765.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [95].Schulz R, Beach SR, Ives DG, Martire LM, Ariyo AA, Kop WJ. Association between depression and mortality in older adults: the Cardiovascular Health Study. Arch Intern Med. 2000;160:1761–8. doi: 10.1001/archinte.160.12.1761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [96].Saz P, Dewey ME. Depression, depressive symptoms and mortality in persons aged 65 and over living in the community: a systematic review of the literature. Int J Geriatr Psychiat. 2001;16:622–30. doi: 10.1002/gps.396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [97].Qiu L, Sautter J, Liu Y, Gu D. Age and gender differences in linkages of sleep with subsequent mortality and health among very old Chinese. Sleep Med. 2011;12(10):1008–17. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2011.04.014. Epub 2011/11/01. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [98].Jung KI, Song CH, Ancoli-Israel S, Barrett-Connor E. Gender differences in nighttime sleep and daytime napping as predictors of mortality in older adults: The Rancho Bernardo Study. Sleep Med. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2012.06.004. Epub 2012/09/07. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [99].Cohen-Mansfield J, Perach R. Sleep duration, nap habits, and mortality in older persons. Sleep. 2012;35(7):1003–9. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1970. Epub 2012/07/04. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [100].Bradford Hill A. The environment and disease: association or causation? Proc Royal Soc Med. 1965;58:295–300. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [101].Stamatakis KA, Punjabi NM. Long sleep duration: a risk to health or a marker of risk? Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11(5):337–9. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.006. Epub 2007/09/15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [102].Knutson KL, Spiegel K, Penev P, Van Cauter E. The metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11(3):163–78. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.01.002. Epub 2007/04/20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [103].Spiegel K, Leproult R, L’Hermite-Baleriaux M, Copinschi G, Penev PD, Van Cauter E. Leptin levels are dependent on sleep duration: relationships with sympathovagal balance, carbohydrate regulation, cortisol, and thyrotropin. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2004;89(11):5762–71. doi: 10.1210/jc.2004-1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [104].Spiegel K, Tasali E, Penev P, Van Cauter E. Brief communication: Sleep curtailment in healthy young men is associated with decreased leptin levels, elevated ghrelin levels, and increased hunger and appetite. Ann Intern Med. 2004;141(11):846–50. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-141-11-200412070-00008. Epub 2004/12/08. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [105].Taheri S, Lin L, Austin D, Young T, Mignot E. Short sleep duration is associated with reduced leptin, elevated ghrelin, and increased body mass index. PLoS Med. 2004;1(3):e62. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0010062. Epub 2004/12/17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [106].Miller MA, Cappuccio FP. Inflammation, sleep, obesity and cardiovascular disease. Current vascular pharmacology. 2007;5(2):93–102. doi: 10.2174/157016107780368280. Epub 2007/04/14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [107].Meier-Ewert HK, Ridker PM, Rifai N, Regan MM, Price NJ, Dinges DF, et al. Effect of sleep loss on C-reactive protein, an inflammatory marker of cardiovascular risk. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2004;43(4):678–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2003.07.050. Epub 2004/02/21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [108].Miller MA, Kandala NB, Kivimaki M, Kumari M, Brunner EJ, Lowe GD, et al. Gender differences in the cross-sectional relationships between sleep duration and markers of inflammation: Whitehall II study. Sleep. 2009;32(7):857–64. Epub 2009/07/31. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [109].Patel SR, Zhu X, Storfer-Isser A, Mehra R, Jenny NS, Tracy R, et al. Sleep duration and biomarkers of inflammation. Sleep. 2009;32(2):200–4. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.2.200. Epub 2009/02/26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [110].Taheri S, Austin D, Lin L, Nieto FJ, Young T, Mignot E. Correlates of serum C-reactive protein (CRP)--no association with sleep duration or sleep disordered breathing. Sleep. 2007;30(8):991–6. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.8.991. Epub 2007/08/19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [111].Copinschi G. Metabolic and endocrine effects of sleep deprivation. Essential psychopharmacology. 2005;6(6):341–7. Epub 2006/02/08. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [112].Bax L, Yu LM, Ikeda N, Tsuruta H, Moons KG. Development and validation of MIX: comprehensive free software for meta-analysis of causal research data. BMC medical research methodology. 2006;6:50. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-6-50. Epub 2006/10/14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [113].DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials. 1986;7(3):177–88. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(86)90046-2. Epub 1986/09/01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [114].Bhaumik DK, Amatya A, Normand S-LT, Greenhouse J, Kaizar E, Neelon B, et al. Meta-analysis of rare binary adverse event data. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 2012;107(498):555–67. doi: 10.1080/01621459.2012.664484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [115].Krueger PM, Saint Onge JM, Chang VW. Race/ethnic differences in adult mortality: the role of perceived stress and health behaviors. Soc Sci Med. 2011;73(9):1312–22. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.08.007. Epub 2011/09/17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [116].Kakizaki M, Kuriyama S, Nakaya N, Sone T, Nagai M, Sugawara Y, et al. Long sleep duration and cause-specific mortality according to physical function and self-rated health: the Ohsaki Cohort Study. J Sleep Res. 2012 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01053.x. Epub 2012/09/26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]