Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013 Mar 2;54(6):603–618. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12061

Research Review: Social motivation and oxytocin in autism – implications for joint attention development and intervention

Katherine K M Stavropoulos 1, Leslie J Carver 1
PMCID: PMC3663901  NIHMSID: NIHMS439938  PMID: 23451765

Abstract

Background and Scope

The social motivation hypothesis (SMH) suggests that individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are less intrinsically rewarded by social stimuli than their neurotypical peers. This difference in social motivation has been posited as a factor contributing to social deficits in ASD. Social motivation is thought to involve the neuropeptide oxytocin. Here, we review the evidence for oxytocin effects in ASD, and discuss its potential role in one important social cognitive behavior.

Methods

Systematic searches were conducted using the PsychINFO and MEDLINE databases and the search terms “oxytocin”, and “autism”; the same databases were used for separate searches for “joint attention”, “intervention”, and “autism”, using the same inclusion criteria as an earlier 2011 review but updating it for the period 2010 to October 2012.

Findings

Several studies suggest that giving oxytocin to both individuals with ASD and typically developing individuals can enhance performance on social cognitive tasks. Studies that have attempted to intervene in joint attention in ASD suggest that social motivation may be a particular obstacle to lasting effects.

Conclusions

The review of the evidence for the SMH suggests a potential role for oxytocin in social motivation deficits in ASD. Because of its importance for later communicative and social development, the focus here is on implications of oxytocin and social motivation in the development of and interventions in joint attention. Joint attention is a central impairment in ASD, and as a result is the focus of several behavioral interventions. In describing this previous research on joint attention interventions in ASD, we pay particular attention to problems encountered in such studies, and propose ways that oxytocin may facilitate behavioral intervention in this area. For future research, integrating behavioral and pharmacological interventions (oxytocin administration) would be a worthwhile experimental direction to improve understanding of the role of oxytocin in ASD and help optimize outcomes for children with ASD.

Keywords: Autism spectrum disorders, behavioural interventions, social motivation hypothesis

Introduction

Atypicalities in social behavior and social cognition are a central characteristic of Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). Although it is clear that individuals with autism are impaired in multiple aspects of social behavior, the basis for these concerns has been the subject of debate. Effective strategies for intervening in social deficits in ASD can be improved by an understanding of the mechanisms behind the deficits, as well as effective behavioral treatments. Ideally, treatments would integrate knowledge that has proven effective in the lab, and our growing understanding of the neural basis of ASD.

The social motivation hypothesis (SMH) (Dawson, 2008; Dawson & Bernier, 2007; Dawson, et al., 2002; Dawson et al., 2005; Grelotti, Gauthier, & Schultz, 2002) has been proposed as an explanation for social deficits in ASD. According to the SMH, children with ASD lack motivation to engage in social activities (joint attention, eye gaze) because they find these activities less rewarding than others. Brain reward circuits in neurotypical individuals are activated by social rewards such as faces (Kampe, Frith, Dolan, & Frith, 2001; Vrtička, Andersson, Grandjean, Sander, & Vuilleumier, 2008). In contrast, in ASD, reward centers are less activated for social stimuli than in controls (Scott-Van Zeeland, Dapretto, Ghahremani, Poldrack, & Brookheimer, 2010; Kohls, et al., 2012). It is not yet clear whether reward deficits in ASD are specific to social rewards, or reflect a general reward processing deficit (Dichter, et al., 2012; Dichter, Richey, Rittenberg, Sabatino, & Bodfish, 2012). The SMH is the first theory to suggest that the lack of social motivation itself leads to later autism symptomology including abnormal brain responses to faces (McPartland, Dawson, Webb, Panagiotides, & Carver, 2004), language and communication problems (Charman, 2003), and impaired joint attention ability (Charman, 2003; Mundy 1995; Mundy, Sigman, Ungerer, & Sherman, 1986), rather than that abnormal social brain function precedes and causes the symptoms of autism.

If there is a deficit in social motivation in ASD, a likely candidate mechanism is abnormality in the function of the neuropeptide oxytocin. Oxytocin has been implicated in several aspects of social behavior in animals (e.g. Liu & Wang, 2003) and humans (e.g. Guastella, Mitchell, & Dadds, 2008), and likely plays a role in social reward systems (e.g. Baskerville & Douglas, 2010). Here we examine the literature on the SMH in ASD, and the possible role of oxytocin in it. As a “test case”, we examine the putative role of oxytocin and social motivation in a specific social cognitive behavior, joint attention, impairments in which are one of the earliest and clearest signs of ASD. It is important to note that while this review will focus specifically on joint attention, there are many other problems in ASD that are relevant to social motivation. In a recent review, Dawson, Berneir, and Ring (2012) discussed the role of oxytocin in a different facet of social behavior: social orienting.

First, we will describe the role of oxytocin in social motivation. Next, we will review the recent research on oxytocin levels and genetics in individuals with ASD. We next describe the effect of oxytocin administration in people with ASD and typical development. We will discuss how social motivation may contribute to the development of joint attention in both typically developing individuals and those with ASD. Next, we describe behavioral interventions that have attempted to improve joint attention in individuals with ASD. This review will discuss behavioral interventions insofar as they relate to the SMH and the potential role of oxytocin. It is not written to evaluate clinical practice, but rather to review interventions that have attempted to improve joint attention, relate them to the SMH and oxytocin, and discuss implications of oxytocin findings for interventions.

Method

Systematic searches were conducted using the PsychINFO and MEDLINE databases and the search terms “oxytocin”, and “autism”. The search was limited to empirical peer reviewed articles on human populations that were written in English. Forty studies fit these criteria. We examined the reference sections of the remaining studies to check for papers missed by the original search. Of these, we included ten papers that administered oxytocin to humans and measured social behaviors as a direct outcome. Papers that administered oxytocin but did not measure social behaviors (e.g. Kirsch, et al., 2005) were not included.

For our discussion of joint attention and interventions targeting joint attention, searches were conducted using PsychINFO and MEDLINE with the search terms “joint attention”, “intervention”, and “autism”. The search was limited to empirical peer-reviewed articles written in English. 76 peer reviewed articles remained for consideration. White and colleagues (2011) published an excellent and comprehensive review summarizing behavioral interventions that focused on joint attention in ASD up through 2010. The authors searched in multiple online databases, and used the following criteria for inclusion: papers must utilize an intervention for joint attention, use joint attention as a dependent variable, utilize experimental control, and have at least one child with ASD in the study. The authors found 27 articles that met the above criteria for review. The current review utilized the same inclusion criteria as White et al. (2011). In order to avoid redundancy with White et al. (2011), we included studies from 2010 to October 2012 (the time of manuscript submission), as well as studies from White et al., (2011) that were particularly relevant to the SMH and joint attention.

Oxytocin’s Role in Social Motivation

Although this review will focus on oxytocin and its role in social motivation, it is important to note that oxytocin does not work alone to modulate social behaviors. Gordon, Martin, Feldman, & Leckman (2011) reviewed oxytocin’s relation with the related neuropeptide arginine vasopressin, and how both interact with sex hormones and the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis to affect sexual, maternal, and adult bonds. For the purposes of this review, we will focus on the neurochemical oxytocin and its interactions with dopamine in the putative social motivation system.

Dopamine is the primary neurotransmitter involved in the reward system (Schultz, 1998). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) research has shown that areas involved in the dopamine reward pathway (e.g., the ventral striatum) are activated in neurotypical adults when pictures of faces are shown (Cacioppo, Norris, Decety, Monteleone, & Nusbaum, 2009). This suggests that the dopamine reward circuit responds to social stimuli. This study also measured brain activity of lonely compared to non-lonely participants, and found that lonely individuals show less activity of the ventral striatum reward pathway in response to faces compared to non-lonely individuals. Thus, social stimuli might be more rewarding for some people than for others. For example, if people who find social stimuli less rewarding become lonely (Cacioppo, et al., 2009). Alternatively, of course, it may be that spending a great deal of time alone leads to a reduction in oxytocin levels. However, if low oxytocin levels lead to social isolation, this may explain one aspect of social function in ASD. Perhaps individuals with ASD find social stimuli less rewarding (because of differences in the reward pathways in the brain), and therefore are not motivated to seek out those interactions. This failure to find social stimuli rewarding could in turn contribute to symptoms of autism.

Bell, Nicholson, Mulder, Luty, and Joyce (2006), measured plasma oxytocin levels of individuals with different personality traits. People with low levels of reward dependence, as assessed using the Temperament and Character Inventory, also had low levels of oxytocin. Another study found similar results for reward dependence, and found that women who were more likely to express and share emotions with friends showed higher oxytocin levels (Tops, Van Peer, Korf, Wijers, & Tucker, 2007). These studies support the hypothesis that oxytocin plays an important role in social behaviors, and that oxytocin is related to variations in personal characteristics related to social rewards.

The dopamine reward system is activated in response to eye contact (Kampe, et al., 2001) and smiling supportive faces (Vrtička, et al., 2008). However, research involving nonhuman mammals has shown that dopamine alone might not account for social motivation. Oxytocin might be involved in social behavior and rewards via the mesocorticolimbic dopamine circuit (Dawson, 2008; Insel & Frenald, 2004; Neuhaus, Beauchaine, & Bernier, 2010). Oxytocin is a peptide synthesized in the hypothalamus and released into the blood stream via the posterior pituitary (Insel, O’Brien, & Leckman, 1999). Neuropeptides such as oxytocin might modulate the dopamine reward pathway when social interactions occur (Baskerville & Douglas, 2010; Young, Liu, & Wang, 2008). In female prairie voles, blocking oxytocin receptors in the nucleus accumbens prevented partner preference induced by dopamine agonists. Conversely, blocking dopamine receptors in the nucleus accumbens prevented partner preference induced by oxytocin agonists (Liu & Wang, 2003). The animal literature suggests that an association between dopamine and oxytocin could also exist in humans.

There is an extensive literature on oxytocin in animals (Liu & Wang, 2003; Ferguson, Young, Hearn, Insel, & Winslow, 2000; for a review see Modi & Young, 2012). As these studies examine oxytocin function in animals, they do not directly inform the question of how oxytocin dysfunction may be related to ASD. Thus, we will not discuss them further here. In addition, although this review will discuss oxytocin as it is relevant to social dysfunction in ASD, others have written excellent reviews on oxytocin and other aspects of social behavior, including parenting and romantic bonds (e.g. Feldman, 2012). This review will focus on oxytocin and its relationship to the reward system, but it is important to note that other neuropeptides, including vasopressin, have also gained attention as potentially important for social behavior in humans. For an in-depth review of studies that have administered either oxytocin or vasopressin and measured various aspects of social behavior, see Zink and Meyer-Lindenberg (2012).

We will next consider evidence that supports the hypothesis that oxytocin is deficient or different in individuals with ASD compared to neurotypical individuals.

Evidence for deficient/different oxytocin levels in ASD

Initial papers looking at oxytocin levels in individuals with ASD measured blood plasma levels of oxytocin. In a study of plasma oxytocin levels in children with autism and neurotypical peers, Modahl, et al. (1998), found that children with autism had lower oxytocin levels than controls. The relationship between oxytocin levels and social functioning was different for typically developing children than those with ASD. Typically developing children who had higher oxytocin levels scored higher on social interaction scales, while children with ASD with higher oxytocin levels were more impaired in social and linguistic development (Modahl et al., 1998). However, plasma levels of oxytocin were measured, which are thought to provide a less direct measure of neuropeptide levels than cerebral spinal fluid (CSF). Further, the ASD and control groups were not matched for verbal or nonverbal measures of IQ, or on measures of daily living, communication, or socialization. The control group scored significantly higher on these measures than the ASD group (Modhal et al., 1998). There was also large variability in oxytocin levels in the sample (e.g. there were children with ASD with high levels of oxytocin, and typically developing children with low levels of oxytocin).

Individuals with ASD not only may have lower levels of oxytocin, they also show differences in an alternative peptide form of oxytocin (the extended form of oxytocin, oxytocin-X, with a three amino acid extension, Gainer, Lively, & Morris 1995). Oxytocin-X becomes oxytocin through enzymatic activity (Green et al., 2001). Individuals with ASD showed higher levels of oxytocin-X compared to neurotypical individuals, but lower levels of oxytocin, resulting in a large oxytocin-X/oxytocin ratio difference between the two groups (Green et al., 2001). In typically developing children, oxytocin, but not oxytocin-X was positively associated with age. In contrast, there was a positive association between oxytocin-X and age in the sample of children with ASD. Although these studies are suggestive of a relation between the synthesis of oxytocin from oxytocin-X and ASD, they are very preliminary. Participants from Modhal et al., (1998) were used in this study, and, as described above, ASD participants were not matched with the comparison group on IQ, vocabulary, communication, socialization, and daily living abilities. Further, as these studies used the same participants, one must consider their results as one piece of evidence rather than a replication of findings.

These results suggest that individuals with ASD have differences in the enzymatic activity that converts oxytocin-X to oxytocin in typical individuals, which could be the result of defects in the genes controlling oxytocin synthesis, the oxytocin gene itself, or genes that regulate developmental changes in activity. Although these studies must be interpreted with caution, they provided important preliminary evidence for differences in oxytocin between typical individuals and those with ASD, and have served to motivate later research on this topic.

Genetic studies

In considering research that focuses on genetic studies exploring differences in oxytocin between typical individuals and those with ASD, there are multiple studies that shed light on potential genetic mechanisms that may be responsible for the differences in oxytocin in individuals with ASD. Studies have largely focused on two genes, the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR), and a gene hypothesized to be involved in oxytocin release (CD38) (Ebstein et al., 2009; Lerer et al., 2010; Munesue, et al., 2010; Riebold et al., 2011).

Several studies investigating the relation between ASD and OXTR have found correlations between single nucleotide polymorphisms and ASD symptoms (Campbell et al., 2011; Ebstein et al., 2009; Jacob et al., 2007; Lerer, et al., 2008; Liu et al., 2010; Walum et al., 2012; Wu et al., 2005; Ylisaukko-oja et al., 2006; Yrigollen et al., 2008; For a review of the relation between single nucleotide polymorphisms of the oxytocin receptor gene and various psychiatric disorders including ASD, depression, and anxiety, see Brüne, 2012). However, several studies that have found relations between ASD and OXTR alleles have not corrected for multiple comparisons, and one that did (e.g. Campbell, et al., 2011) found that effects were not maintained after corrections were applied. As has been described by Sullivan (2007), there are risks of false positive significant results in genetic association studies where correction of the significance threshold to account for the number of tests conducted is not applied. Of the papers mentioned above, only Ebstein et al., (2009), Liu et al., (2010) and Yrigollen et al., (2008) adjusted p-values for multiple comparisons. Lerer et al., (2008), and Lerer et al., (2010), used a similar, albeit slightly less conservative technique of correcting for multiple comparisons but accounting for correlations between markers. As Lerer et al. (2010) point out, this technique increases power, but results should nevertheless be interpreted somewhat cautiously. Jacob et al. (2007) does not discuss correction for multiple comparisons, but because the authors only tested two a priori identified single nucleotide polymorphisms, this potential confound is likely not applicable to this study. Similarly, Riebold et al. (2011), examined CD38 expression, and a priori identified single nucleotide polymorphism, so the concerns about multiple comparisons likely do not apply. Campbell et al. (2011) undertook the largest-scale study of single nucleotide polymorphisms, observed phenotypes, and ASD. Although the authors found multiple nominally significant results, they point out that none of the results would survive corrections for multiple comparisons. The results of this study can be interpreted as preliminary evidence for association between alterations in the oxytocin receptor gene and ASD, and provide useful future directions for research associating genetic results with observable phenotypes of ASD. Future studies should be careful to employ rigorous statistical controls to confirm this finding.

Not all studies find relations between single nucleotide polymorphisms and ASD (e.g. Tansey et al., 2010; Wermter et al., 2009). In independent samples from Portugal and the UK, Tansey et al., (2010) did not find a relation between any significant single nucleotide polymorphism on the oxytocin receptor gene and ASD that survived statistical correction for multiple comparisons. The authors suggest heterogeneity of participants as one reason for lack of replication from previous studies. The two samples did not utilize the same inclusion criteria, however, which might also have affected the outcome (Tansey et al., 2010).

Other studies have suggested that oxytocin genes are associated with reward dependence, and fMRI activation in response to social stimuli (Tost et al., 2010). Finally, a genetic study of ASD suggested that over methylation of OXTR could be responsible for gene silencing in some patients (Gregory et al., 2009). Although these genetic studies are by no means conclusive evidence that the OXTR or CD38 genes are causally related to ASD, they do suggest that it is important to study oxytocin in ASD. Given the diversity of genetic results, epigenetic approaches may be a beneficial avenue for future research in pharmacological treatment for ASD.

This section has briefly reviewed genetic studies concerning oxytocin and ASD. Other studies have investigated single nucleotide polymorphisms on oxytocin related genes in typically developing individuals (e.g. Chen & Johnson, 2011; Sauer et al., 2012; Walum et al., 2012), as well as genetic studies of arginine vasopressin and ASD. Those studies are outside the scope of the current review, but are discussed in other reviews (e.g. Ebstein, Knafo, Mankuta, Hong Chew, & San Lai, 2012; Skuse & Gallagher, 2011)

Effects of oxytocin administration on social behavior

Studies of oxytocin levels and genetic studies suggest that deficits in oxytocin function should be considered as a possible contributor to social dysfunction in ASD. Table 1 summarizes the human oxytocin administration studies that measured social behaviors. In neurotypical individuals, several studies show that social behavior is enhanced under oxytocin administration. These include recognition of emotions (Domes, Heinrichs, Michel, Berger, & Herpertz, 2007), gaze to the eye region of the face (Gamer, Zurowski, & Büchel, 2010; Guastella, Mitchell, & Dadds, 2008), the salience of positive social memories (Guastella, Mitchell, & Mathews, 2008), and the effects of social reinforcement on learning (Hurlemann et al., 2010). Neurotypical individuals given oxytocin show increased trust during a social computer game (Kosfeld, Heinrichs, Zak, Fischbacher & Fehr, 2005). Oxytocin also decreases amygdala response to fearful scenes and faces (Gamer, et al., 2010; Kirsch et al., 2005). These studies provide important information about the role of oxytocin in social behavior in neurotypical individuals. However, in neurotypical individuals, effects of oxytocin administration are seen only on difficult versions of tasks (Domes et al., 2007). For example, in the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste, & Plumb, 2001; Domes et al., 2007), intranasal doses of oxytocin improved neurotypical adults’ performance, but only on items that had been identified in previous research as “difficult” (Domes et al., 2007). There was no improvement on items identified as “easy”. This is likely because the Reading the Mind through the Eyes Test was designed to measure severe impairments in mind reading (e.g. with individuals with ASD), and thus typically developing adults already score highly on items rated as “easy” (Domes et al., 2007).

Table 1.

Studies that have examined effects of oxytocin on social behavior.

Study authors Population, sample size (N), age (mean, M) Administration Dependent variable(s) Results of oxytocin administration Effect size
Andari, Duhamel, Zalla, Herbrecht, Leboyer, & Sirigu, 2010 HFAD, AS (N = 13)
TD (N = 13)
M = 26 yrs
Intranasal 1. Cooperation with computer partners in a computer game 1. Demonstrated preference for “good” vs. “bad” computer partner a
2. Gaze to regions of the face 2. Increased gaze to eye region of the face a
Bartz, Zaki, Bolger, Hollander, Ludwig, Kolevzon et al., 2010 TD
N = 27
M = 26.8 yrs
Intranasal Accuracy of emotional/empathetic recognition as a function of autism quotient (AQ) scores Individuals with low AQ scores did well on the empathy task regardless of oxytocin; whereas individuals with high AQ scores improved after oxytocin --a
Domes, Heinrichs, Michel, Berger, and Herpertz, 2007 TD
N = 30
M = 25 yrs
Intranasal Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (REMET) Improvement on items rated as “difficult” --a
Gamer, Zurowski, & Büchel, 2010 TD
N = 46
M = 28 yrs
Intranasal 1. Fixation to regions of emotional faces 1. More gaze changes towards eye region regardless of facial emotion --a
2. Amygdala activity in response to emotional faces 2. Dampened amygdala response to fearful faces; enhanced response to happy/neutral faces --a
Guastella, Einfeld, Gray, Rinehart, Tonge, Lambert, et al., 2010 HFAD, AS
N = 16
M = 14.8 yrs
Intranasal REMET Improvement on items rated as “easy” --a
Guastella, Mitchell, & Dadds, 2008 TD
N = 52
M = 19.80 yrs
Intranasal Looking time and fixation to regions of neutral faces Longer gaze + more fixations to the eyes Fixation count to eyes placebo vs. oxytocin: ES = .88
Gaze time to eyes placebo vs. oxytocin: ES = 1.20
Guastella, Mitchell, & Mathews, 2008 TD
N = 69
M = 19.98 yrs
Intranasal Making “remember”, “know” or “new” judgment for new and previously seen happy, neutral, and angry faces More likely to make “remember” and “know” judgments for previously seen happy faces, but not for neutral or angry faces “know” = .608b
“remember” = .421b
Hollendar, Bartz, Chaplin, Phillips, Sumner, Soorya et al., 2007 ASD, AS
N = 15
M = 32 yrs
Intravenous Ability to identify and comprehend affective speech 1. Improved ability to identify and comprehend affective speech --a
2. Improvements lasted to session 2 if session 1 was oxytocin vs. placebo d = .66
Hurlemann, Patin, Onur, Cohen, Baumgartener, & Metzler et al., 2010 TD
N = 48
M = 25.9 yrs
Intranasal 1. Learning task with either social or nonsocial feedback 1. Improved learning when feedback is social .848 main effect of oxytocinb
Kosfeld, Heinrichs, Zak, Fischbacher, & Fehr, 2005 TD
N = 58
M = 22 yrs
Intranasal Trust in a partner during an investment game (willingness to invest) Increased willingness to invest with partner .249c
a

Authors did not report effect sizes, and we are unable to calculate effect sizes accurately from the published information

b

Authors did not report effect sizes. We calculated them with the information provided in the results section. Whenever there were multiple interactions, we calculated the effect size for the main effect.

c

Authors did not report effect sizes. We calculated them with the information provided in the results section. Note that these effect sizes were calculated from a Mann-Whitney U test, and will not be directly comparable to the other effect sizes reported above (e.g., Cohen’s d). An effect size calculated this way considers .3 as a moderate effect size.

ASD: autism spectrum disorder; HFAD: high functioning autistic disorder; AS, Asperger’s syndrome; TD: typical development; RTMET: Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test.

Domes et al.’s (2007) results may be explained in part by changes in looking behavior to the face under oxytocin administration. Guastella, Mitchell, & Dadds, (2008) administered intranasal oxytocin to neurotypical adults and measured both looking time and fixations to various regions of the face. Participants who received oxytocin had significantly longer gaze and more fixations to the eye region of the face compared to those who received placebo. Increased gaze to the eyes may have helped participants better identify ambiguous emotions in the Reading the Mind through the Eyes Test. Following the direction of eye gaze is an important preliminary component of joint attention (Scaife & Bruner, 1975). Because there is significant evidence that people with ASD tend to look less at eyes than neurotypical controls (Jones, Carr, & Klin, 2008; Klin, Jones, Schultz, Volkmar, & Cohen, 2002), results from the Guastella, Mitchell, & Dadds, (2008) study have provocative implications for ASD, and particularly the joint attention deficits reported in children with ASD.

Administering oxytocin to individuals with ASD

Studies that have administered oxytocin to individuals with ASDs are relatively limited. Of those that exist, there is a range of procedures that have been utilized. In general, results of these studies suggest that oxytocin improves performance on social tasks relative to placebo. These tasks include: recognition of affective speech (Hollander, Bartz, Chaplin, & Phillips, 2007), recognition of emotion in eyes (REMET) (Guastella, et al., 2010), and social cooperation in an online computer game (Andari, et al., 2010). Andari et al. (2010) had adult participants play a cooperative ball tossing game with fictitious partners who were programmed to have “neutral”, “bad”, or “good” traits depending on the amount they cooperated (i.e. passing the ball back). In a baseline measure, participants with ASD or Asperger’s syndrome did not distinguish between the characteristics of the partners, throwing the ball equally often to each of the players regardless of their programmed behavior. In contrast, control participants tended to throw the ball exclusively to cooperative partners by the end of the game. When participants with ASD or Asperger’s syndrome were given intranasal doses of oxytocin, their performance became more comparable to neurotypical participants in that they preferred to interact with the good compared to bad partner (Andari et al., 2010). Participants with ASD or Asperger’s syndrome also showed increased gaze fixation on socially relevant areas of the face (e.g. the eyes) after oxytocin administration. The results of this study suggest that, like controls, individuals with ASD or Asperger’s syndrome improve on social tasks after oxytocin administration. However, even after oxytocin administration, individuals with ASD or Asperger’s syndrome spent less time gazing at the face and eye region compared to typically developing participants. Although oxytocin increased the time spent gazing at the face and eyes in individuals with ASD or Asperger’s, performance was still different from controls. Further, because the neurotypical participants were not tested with oxytocin, it is difficult to contextualize the improvements seen in individuals with ASD or Asperger’s syndrome. For example, the results of the study can not tell us how typically developing individuals might perform on these tasks with oxytocin, and whether the groups would differ on eye gaze measures before and after oxytocin administration.

Guastella et al., (2010) had adolescents with ASD or Asperger’s syndrome complete the Reading the Mind through the Eyes Test after taking either placebo or oxytocin. Participants showed significant improvement after oxytocin. Notably, this study was the first to administer oxytocin to young adolescents, and when analyses were restricted to individuals under 16 years of age, performance on the Reading the Mind through the Eyes Test still improved after oxytocin. However, when the items were separated into “easy” and “hard”, improvements were significant only for “easy” items. These results contrast with those of Domes et al. (2007), who found that while typically developing adults showed significant improvement on the Reading the Mind through the Eyes Task after oxytocin, that improvement was significant only for items rated as “hard”. As Domes et al. (2007) speculate, their result could be due to the fact that typically developing adults are unlikely to improve further on items that they already perform extremely well on. Guastella et al. (2010), suggested that oxytocin might improve performance on tasks that are of medium difficulty (e.g. neither too hard nor too easy). One could speculate that because individuals with ASD or Asperger’s syndrome find items rated as “easy” to be somewhat difficult, and items rated as “hard” to be extremely difficult, selective improvement could occur. Unfortunately, because Domes et al. (2007) only studied typically developing adults, and Guastella et al. (2010) studied only adolescents with ASD or Asperger’s syndrome, a direct comparison between the two studies is not possible.

In one within-subjects study, order effects were seen in an affective speech recognition task when adults were given oxytocin during one session (either session one or two), and placebo during the other session (Hollander et al. 2007). Individuals who received oxytocin first maintained high levels of performance even during the later placebo session. In contrast, participants who were administered placebo in the first session showed decreased performance during their second, oxytocin session. This finding suggests two noteworthy things: administration of placebo or oxytocin during the first session improved performance relative to baseline; and performance was improved as a result of the expectation of receiving treatment in the placebo condition. However, when the first session injection was placebo, the improvements did not last until the second session, whereas those seen from oxytocin maintained even after the delay. Thus, there appear to be effects of oxytocin on the ability to recognize and remember affective speech even above the placebo effect. These order effects suggest that oxytocin administration has the potential to facilitate improvements in the ability to recognize affective speech that last beyond a single testing session.

These results have important implications for ASD interventions. If oxytocin is given before a behavioral intervention, improvement might continue throughout the intervention – and thus oxytocin should be administered prior to behavioral interventions. The Hollander et al. (2007) results also suggest that oxytocin affects the ability of individuals with ASD to correctly identify affective speech – which is an important aspect of social cognitive functioning.

However, because the Hollander et al., (2007) study was done with adults, it is difficult to make assumptions about whether oxytocin administration would have equally long-lasting results in adolescents or children with ASD. In addition, therapeutic use of oxytocin would require repeated administration over time and early in development. Currently, little is known about the effects of repeated doses of oxytocin in development.

One recent animal study looked at the effects of repeated oxytocin administration. Bales et al. (2012), repeatedly administered intranasal oxytocin to prairie voles from weaning to sexual maturity. As expected, initial oxytocin administrations led to increases in social behaviors. However, long-term administration led to deficits in partner preference behavior (Bales et al., 2012). These results suggest that oxytocin administration might have unexpected negative results over time. Relatedly Bales and Perkeybile (2012) reviewed the animal literature, and suggested that oxytocin and vasopressin administration may change how social experiences affect development. Although these papers are relevant to the animal literature, they point out the need for further research into repeated exposures of oxytocin, as well as research that will shed light on effects of early or repeated oxytocin exposure in humans.

The aforementioned studies suggest that oxytocin might mediate some social cognitive deficits seen in ASD. However, it should be noted that there have only been three completed studies that investigate the effects of oxytocin on social deficits in individuals with ASDs, and of those three, two of them were with adults. It is likely that many other studies will be completed soon. The website clinicaltrials.gov lists eight active and one completed study investigating the effects of oxytocin on individuals with ASD. Of these, four involve recruiting adolescents or children, and the other four are recruiting adults. From the information on the website, it appears as though none of the listed studies are administering oxytocin concurrently with a behavioral intervention with ASD, but are either measuring brain or behavior after a single administration of oxytocin, or giving oxytocin over time and measuring behavior afterwards (clinical.trials.gov., 2012).

We have reviewed results of oxytocin administration on social behaviors in both typical individuals and those with ASD. We have limited the discussed studies to those that explicitly measured social behaviors rather than including those that discuss how social information is processed. For a review of all studies concerning oxytocin administration and social information processing, see Graustella and MacLeod, (2012). For a review of studies administering oxytocin or vasopressin and using imaging methods, please see Zink and Meyer-Lindenberg (2012).

We have not discussed studies that have administered oxytocin to individuals with disorders other than ASD. Oxytocin administration has been preliminarily helpful in treating symptoms of schizophrenia (e.g. Feifel et al., 2010; Pedersen et al., 2011). For a review of the literature on oxytocin administration and schizophrenia, see MacDonald & Feifel (2012). Of particular note, Pedersen et al. (2011) found that intranasal oxytocin improved measures of social cognition in patients with schizophrenia, including theory of mind and recognition of suspicious faces. These results suggest that intranasal oxytocin might be helpful for social deficits seen in a variety of disorders, not only ASD.

Potential issues in intranasal oxytocin administration

Some questions about the efficacy of intranasal oxytocin administration exist. It is not clear whether oxytocin and other neuropeptides cross the blood–brain barrier when administered intranasally, although there is recent evidence that intranasal oxytocin appears in saliva (Weisman, Zagoory-Sharon, & Feldman, 2012) and blood (Andari et al., 2010) for a short time after intranasal administration. Weisman et al. (2012) demonstrated that in typically developing adults, intranasal oxytocin administration increases the amount of oxytocin in saliva after 15 minutes, reaches a peak 45 minutes after administration, and still not does return to baseline after 4 hours. This has important implications for the appropriate timeline for conducting experiments concerning intranasal oxytocin and social behaviors. Nine of the 10 studies reported in Table 1 measured social behaviors 45–50 minutes after intranasal oxytocin administration. The only exception (Hollander et al., 2007), administered oxytocin intravenously, and measured comprehension of affective speech at five separate time points as the amount of oxytocin administered was increased. Thus, the majority of studies have measured social behavior at the time of peak levels of oxytocin.

Churchland and Winkielman (2012) adeptly point out the error in assuming oxytocin has passed through the blood brain barrier based solely on reports of increased peripheral concentration of the peptide (e.g. in blood plasma). However, other studies report that peripheral and CSF levels of neuropeptides closely related to oxytocin (e.g. arginine vasopressin) are correlated (Born et al., 2002; Riekkinen et al., 1987). Studies have suggested that intranasal administration of neuropeptides, such as vasopressin, caused CSF levels to increase within 10 minutes of administration, and continued to rise for 80 minutes after administration (Born et al., 2002).

A more general issue with measuring plasma or CSF levels of oxytocin is that the amount of oxytocin in the blood or brain does not necessarily reflect whether oxytocin has reached the appropriate binding sites in the brain. Even if oxytocin reaches the brain via intranasal administration, there have not been any studies investigating where it goes, and whether it reaches the appropriate receptors (see Churchland & Winkielman, 2012, for a more in-depth discussion of this issue). Plasma and CSF measures of oxytocin may not reflect efficient oxytocin binding. If oxytocin receptors are damaged or the genes that control them are mutated, and oxytocin is available in the brain but not binding correctly, plasma and CSF measurements would not accurately represent the success or failure of oxytocin to bind, but only represent the net amount available in the brain. Further research on the location, timing, and binding of oxytocin after intranasal administration is necessary (Churchland & Winkielman, 2012). Such research would likely be possible with non-human primates, where concentrations of oxytocin in the brain could be measured after intranasal administration. While all of these limitations concerning intranasal oxytocin administration must be considered carefully, evidence in sum suggests that intranasal oxytocin administration succeeds in increasing oxytocin levels.

Social motivation and joint attention in ASD

We turn now to a case of social behavior that is impaired in ASD, and for which oxytocin may have important treatment implications. Joint attention is a crucial developmental milestone for typical social-cognitive development, and is among the most commonly identified deficits in ASD. Joint attention occurs when two people share attention to an object, location, or event in space. Joint attention allows information to be effectively conveyed from one person to another about an object without using language. In typical development, joint attention is thought to emerge during the second half of the first year of life, and is important for both successful language learning and later social cognition (Bates, 1979; Baldwin, 1991; Brooks & Meltzoff, 2008; Brooks & Meltzoff, 2005; Bruner, 1983; Corkum & Moore, 1998; Mundy & Gomes, 1998; Sigman & Kasari, 2005; Tomasello & Farrar, 1986). For reasons we will expand upon below, joint attention is an especially interesting test case for the social motivation hypothesis. A typical example of joint attention is the following scenario: An infant looks to his mother, up at a passing airplane, and back to her as if to indicate, “look at that unusual thing in the sky!” This triadic interaction involving self (infant), other (mother), and their shared object of attention towards an object (airplane) is the defining feature of joint attention (Bakeman & Adamson, 1984; Scaife & Bruner, 1975; Tomasello, 1995). According to the SMH, individuals with ASD have impairments in joint attention because they find social activities less rewarding than neurotypical individuals. Given the evidence reviewed above that oxytocin may play a significant role in social behavior and social motivation, interventions focusing on joint attention that are combined with administration of oxytocin in may increase intrinsic social motivation and improve outcomes from joint attention interventions.

Although joint attention is often mentioned as a unified concept, it can be separated into two distinct subtypes that likely develop separately (Mundy & Gomes, 1998; Mundy, Sullivan, & Mastergeorge, 2009). This differentiation is important because being able to respond to joint attention (i.e. following another person’s gaze) likely develops earlier than initiating joint attention (i.e. seeking to share attention with another person through gaze that you initiate, Dunham & Moore, 1995). Joint attention can also serve multiple communicative functions (Gomez, Sarria, & Tamarit, 1993; Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari 1993). Imperative joint attention occurs if a child points or gazes towards an object with the intention of requesting that object. Declarative joint attention occurs if a child points or gazes at an object with the intention of sharing his interest in that object with an adult) (Gomez et al., 1993; Mundy et al., 1993). Using these distinctions, one could respond to a joint attention bid from another person that is either imperative or declarative, and also initiate joint attention for either of these two functions. As we will see in a subsequent section, these distinctions between two types of joint attention and the two functions they serve are especially important when discussing joint attention difficulties in ASD.

Joint attention in children with ASD

Individuals with ASD are profoundly impaired in joint attention (Mundy 1995; Mundy et al., 1986). This inability to share attention with another person is central to ASD and has been incorporated into the DSM-IV criteria for the disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Deficits in joint attention differentiate children with ASD from both typically developing and other developmentally delayed children (Bacon, Fein, Morris, Waterhouse, & Allen, 1998; Charman, et al., 1998; Dawson, Meltzoff, Osterling, & Rinaldi, 1998; Mundy et al., 1986; Sigman, Kasari, Kwon, & Yirmiya, 1992).

However, individuals with ASD are not equally impaired in all aspects of joint attention. Initiating joint attention is more impaired, and predicts symptoms better, in ASD than responding to joint attention (Leekam, Lopez, & Moore, 2000; Mundy et al., 1986, 1993, 2009; Sigman, Mundy, Sherman, & Ungerer 1986). Perhaps this is not surprising because responding to joint attention chiefly involves following another’s gaze. One could argue that an object of another’s attention is often rewarding on its own and that responding to joint attention therefore does not necessitate motivation that is purely social (Corkum & Moore, 1998). Initiating joint attention, on the other hand, requires one to be motivated to share something interesting with another individual – meaning that the motivation behind initiating joint attention is likely purely social in nature (Mundy & Gomes, 1998; Tomasello, 1995; Mundy, 1995).

Based on this reasoning, the SMH (Dawson & Bernier, 2007; Dawson, et al., 2005; Dawson, et al., 2002; Grelotti, et al., 2002; Dawson, 2008) emphasizes initiating joint attention more than responding to joint attention skills. Similarly, individuals with ASD are impaired in both declarative and imperative joint attention, but show more profound problems with the declarative type, which can also be characterized as involving social motivation (Baron-Cohen, 1989; 1993; Mundy et al., 1986; 1993).

Improving joint attention skills in individuals with ASD

Because of its importance for later social and linguistic development, joint attention has been a focus of a great deal of intervention research. Research suggests that children with ASD who engage in joint attention gain language skills more rapidly than their peers who do not engage in joint attention over equivalent time periods (Bono, Daley, & Sigman, 2004; Siller & Sigman, 2002). Further, several studies suggest that relatively good joint attention skills early in development in children with ASD predicts better language and social outcomes up to several years later (Charman, 2003; Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari 1990; Sigman & Ruskin, 1999). Interventions for children with ASD that focus on nonverbal social communication skills lead to improvements in language and social skills in these children (see White, et al., 2011 for review).

Interventions designed to improve joint attention in individuals with ASD

Interventions have sought to train individuals with ASD to engage in joint attention behaviors through various behavior modification procedures (e.g. discrete trial training, pivotal response training). Such interventions use principles of positive and negative reinforcement in order to increase desired behavior. As mentioned above, White et al., (2011) provides a thorough review of these interventions conducted prior to 2010. Table 2 summarizes studies of interventions to improve joint attention in individuals with ASD since 2010, and this section will review papers from table 2, as well as those from White et al., (2011) that are particularly relevant to the scope of the current review.

Table 2.

Studies that have examined the effects of joint attention interventions on individuals with autism spectrum disorders since 2010 (updating the studies reviewed in White et al. (2011)).

Study authors Sample size, Age (mean, M) Focus of intervention Length (weeks) Sessions per week Significant improvement (Effect Size) Follow up? Follow up duration (months) Effects maintained at follow up
Ferraioli & Harris, 2011 4
M = 4.2 yrs
Sibling JA 7–8 -- RJA
IJA (2 participants only)a
Y 3 RJA, IJA (2 participants only
Ingersoll, 2012 14 intervention
13 control
M = 37.95 months
Imitation intervention 10 3 IJA (η2p = .16) Y 2–3 IJA
Kaale, Smith, & Sponheim, 2012 61
M = 48.4 months
Preschool intervention 8 10 JE with mothers (d = .67)
IJA with teachers (d = .44)
N --
Kasari, Gulsrud, Wong, Kwon, & Locke, 2010 19 intervention
19 control
M = 30.83 months
Caregiver JA 8 3 JE (d = .87)
RJA (d = .74)
Y 12 JE, RJA
Landa, Holman, O’Neill, & Stuart, 2011 24 intervention
24 control
M = 2 yrs
IJA, SPA 24 4 IJA (trend) (d = .89) Y 6 IJA (trend) (d = 1.56)
Lawton & Kasari, 2012 9 intervention
7 control
M = 44.69 months
IJA 6 2 IJA for classroom observation only (d = 1.85) N --
Lawson & Kasari, 2012a 20 JA intervention
16 symbolic play intervention
16 control
M = 42.01 months
JAPA, JAPAU 5–6 7 No significant improvement at exit Y 6, 12 JAPA (6 months: ES = 1.36; 12 months: ES = 1.52), JAPAU (6 months: ES = 1.16; 12 moths: ES = 1.75)
For JA vs. controlb
Schertz, Odom, Baggett, Sideris, 2012 11 intervention
12 control
M = 26.11 months
IJA, RJA 16–56 (M= 28) 1 RJA (d = 1.39) Y 1, 2 RJA (d= 1.18)
a

Authors did not report effect size, and N was too small to calculate effect size

b

Authors did not report effect size. Effect size was calculated with information given by the authors in the results section of published work.

JA: joint attention; RJA: responding to joint attention; IJA: initiating joint attention; SPA: shared positive affect; JAPA: joint attention with positive affect; JAPAU: joint attention with positive affect and utterances; JE: joint engagement

Kasari, Freeman, & Paparella, (2006), and Kasari, Gulstrud, Wong, Kwon, & Locke, (2010), had children engage in 5 to 8 minutes of discrete trial training designed to improve initiating and responding to joint attention. This structured intervention was immediately followed by child-directed floor time designed to improve targeted skills in a less structured setting. In interventions like this, children are extrinsically reinforced for responding to and initiating joint attention bids, and generally show marked improvements in joint attention skills. However, many intervention studies fail to conduct follow-up sessions in order to assess whether skills gained during intervention are maintained, and those that did varied in duration (e.g. Kaale, Smith, & Spoonheim, 2012; Ferraioli & Harris, 2011). Of studies that did assess improvement at follow-up, often initiating joint attention skills did not last over time (e.g. Whalen & Schreibman, 2003), or were not as successfully improved as other primary outcome measures (e.g. Schertz, Odom, Baggett, & Sideris, 2012; Kasari et al., 2010; Landa, Holman, O’Neill, & Stuart, 2011). Dysfunction in social motivation may explain the finding that joint attention intervention effects often do not last to follow-up. Because of the lack of intrinsic social motivation, extrinsic rewards are used, and when these rewards are no longer available, joint attention regresses. Studies that have attempted to use more naturalistic reinforcers, or that have attempted to generate intrinsic social motivation have shown better success on follow up (e.g. Ingersoll, 2012; Isaksen & Holth, 2009; Naoi et al., 2008). However, even some studies that follow this model still report mixed levels of success (e.g. Taylor & Hoch, 2008). Only a few studies have attempted to teach responding to or initiation joint attention without tangible extrinsic rewards, although Jones and Carr (2004), suggested it as a useful direction.

Isaksen and Holth (2009) attempted to deal with the problem of joint attention regression after interventions that utilize extrinsic motivation. They reinforced the smiling, nodding, and verbalization that typically occur after initiating joint attention behaviors. For example, an adult and a child with ASD were seated across from each other and various desirable toys were on the table. The adult’s smiles and nods were used as a signal that the child could take a toy. When the adult was not smiling or nodding, any attempts to take a toy were blocked. In this way, the experimenters paired natural adult behavior with a rewarding activity. These types of social reinforcements motivate typically developing children to engage in initiating joint attention behaviors (Isaksen & Holth, 2009; Jones & Carr, 2004; Whalen & Schreibman, 2003), are naturalistic, and will continue to be present after the intervention. Isaksen & Hoth measured joint attention behaviors after a one-month follow-up interval and found that initiating joint attention skills maintained or improved for all 4 participants.

In summary, intervention studies demonstrate that children with ASD can improve their response to joint attention skills. However, initiating joint attention is much more challenging, most likely for motivational reasons. Studies such as those of Ingersoll (2012), Isaksen and Holth (2009), Naoi et al., (2008), and Taylor and Hoch (2008) are useful in their attempts to improve initiating joint attention without using extrinsic motivators, but their varied success underscores the difficulty of reinforcing social interactions for children with ASD. Finding a way to intrinsically motivate children with ASD to engage in initiating joint attention behaviors while they participate in interventions is important for long-term success.

Conclusions

This review has examined literature in the context of the SMH, including: oxytocin and ASD, administration of oxytocin and social behaviors, joint attention and ASD, and behavioral interventions to improve joint attention in ASD.

Although multiple neurotransmitters and neuropeptides have been implicated as candidates for improving intrinsic motivation for social interaction, oxytocin has consistently been seen as important for social motivation, interaction, and memory. Most of the literature linking oxytocin and social behavior derives from animal research, but there is a growing literature examining oxytocin administration in humans. Data from studies in which oxytocin has been administered to humans have strengthened the argument that oxytocin is important for social interaction. Because of the link between oxytocin and social behavior that has begun to emerge, oxytocin has begun to be used in interventions to improve social recognition in individuals with ASD.

Individuals with ASD have well documented deficits in joint attention (Bacon, et al., 1998; Charman et al., 1998; Dawson et al., 1998; Mundy et al., 1986; Sigman, et al., 1992). Improving joint attention deficits is important for improving social and linguistic functioning of individuals with ASD (Bono et al., 2004; Charman, 2003; Koegel, 2000; Lord, 2000; Siller & Sigman, 2002; Rogers & Lewis, 1989), and therefore has been a focus of multiple behavioral interventions (e.g. see White et al., 2011; Table 2). Although these interventions have been somewhat successful in improving joint attention, one key underlying issue has been individuals with ASD’s lack of intrinsic motivation to initiate joint attention and other social interactions (Jones & Carr, 2004; Whalen & Schreibman, 2003). Extrinsic rewards are often necessary for behavioral interventions to succeed, and when those are removed, individuals with ASD are no longer motivated to engage in joint attention behaviors, resulting in a failure of effects to maintain over the long term. Some theorists have proposed that increasing intrinsic motivation for social interaction might be a key component in improving symptoms of ASD (Dawson, 2008; Dawson & Bernier, 2007; Dawson et al., 2002; Dawson, et al., 2005; Grelotti et al., 2002).

However, one piece missing from the literature is integrating behavioral interventions and oxytocin administration in individuals with ASD. Behavioral interventions are important for increasing social behaviors, especially in young children with ASD. Oxytocin has been used experimentally to attempt to change social behaviors in ASD, but to our knowledge has not yet been used in a controlled treatments study, nor has it been used in combination with other intervention methods. By adding oxytocin administration to behavioral interventions, one important problem (lack of intrinsic motivation) that seems to hinder long-term success might be diminished. Interventions that combine behavioral interventions and oxytocin administration are crucial to long-term and generalized improvement of joint attention behaviors in young individuals with ASD (Dawson et al., 2012).

Experimental tests of such interventions would advance our understanding of how oxytocin mediates social symptoms of ASD in both the short and long term. Such experiments would need to be well-controlled, double-blind, and compare interventions plus oxytocin versus the same interventions with placebo. One issue would be when to administer oxytocin during the intervention, as well as what type of intervention to utilize. Multiple meta-analyses have emphasized the effectiveness of early intensive behavioral interventions (i.e. young children with ASD receiving 30–40 hours per week of structured behavioral interventions for over two years, e.g. Reichow, 2012; Eldevik et al., 2009). Although these interventions require a large time investment, they are highly successful in improving a variety of behavioral skills, as well as improving IQ. To utilize oxytocin for behavioral interventions, however, it would likely be more practical to administer a more short-term intervention similar to those reported by White et al., 2010 and in Table 2.

Based on previous animal studies (Ferguson, Aldag, Insel, & Young, 2001) as well as studies of the time course of oxytocin in saliva (Weisman et al., 2012) and order effects (Hollander et al., (2007), intervention should administer oxytocin about 45 minutes before each intervention session (rather than after or during sessions).

Directions for future research

We propose that two highly studied and important areas (behavioral intervention and studies on oxytocin administration) should be integrated in order to achieve optimal outcomes for children with ASD. Combining behavioral and pharmacological interventions has been highly successful in the alleviation of symptoms in other disorders (Ressler et al., 2004; Hoffman et al., 2006).

In the case of ASD, this novel approach to improving joint attention skills with both pharmacological and behavioral intervention is a necessary and worthwhile experimental direction. Theoretically, oxytocin administration will heighten patients’ intrinsic motivation to engage in social interactions, and the behavioral interventions will facilitate and teach social interactions. By improving intrinsic motivation to engage in social interactions, behavioral interventions can use only social rewards, and cease using any extrinsic motivators. Because the rewards will be entirely social, and intrinsic motivation will be improved (via oxytocin), interventions should have long-lasting effects. Using behavioral interventions in combination with oxytocin administration should increase the chances of long-term success and an improved understanding of the role of oxytocin in ASD.

Key points.

  • The social motivation hypothesis posits that individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are less motivated to engage in social behaviors than their typically developing peers, and this lack of motivation leads to later atypicalities in social behavior and cognition

  • The neurochemical oxytocin has received attention as a potential mechanism for social atypicalities in ASD

  • Administering oxytocin to individuals with and without ASD has been effective at improving social cognition

  • We propose that future studies should combine oxytocin administration and behavioral interventions to optimize outcomes for various social cognitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorder.

Acknowledgments

K.K.M.S. is supported by Autism Speaks’ Dennis Weatherstone Predoctoral Fellowship (7844). L.J.C. is supported by the National Institute of Health/National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NIH/NICHD R01 HD052804-01A2), and the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke/National Institute of Health (NINDS/NIH R01NS071580-01).

Footnotes

The authors have declared that they have no competing or potential conflicts of interest arising from the publication of this review.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. Washington, DC: Author; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  2. Andari E, Duhamel JR, Zalla T, Herbrecht E, Leboyer M, Sirigu A. Promoting social behavior with oxytocin in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107:4389–4394. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910249107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bacon AL, Fein D, Morris R, Waterhouse L. The responses of autistic children to the distress of others. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1998;28:129–142. doi: 10.1023/a:1026040615628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bakeman R, Adamson LB. Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Development. 1984;55:1278–1289. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bales KL, Perkeybile AM. Developmental experiences and the oxytocin receptor system. Hormones and Behavior. 2012;61:313–319. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bales KL, Perkeybile AM, Conoley OG, Lee MH, Guoynes CD, Downing GM, Mendoza SP. Chronic intranasal oxytocin causes long-term impairments in partner preference formation in male prairie voles. Biological Psychiatry. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.08.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baldwin DA. Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In: Moore C, Dunham PJ, editors. Joint Attention: its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1991. pp. 131–158. [Google Scholar]
  8. Baron-Cohen S. Perceptual role-taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1989;7:113–127. [Google Scholar]
  9. Baron-Cohen S. From attention–goal psychology to belief–desire psychology: the development of a theory of mind and its dysfunction. In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen D, editors. Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1993. pp. 59–82. [Google Scholar]
  10. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger Syndrome and High-functioning Autism. Journal of Child Psychological Psychiatry. 2001;42:241–251. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N, Hollander E, Ludwig NN, Kolevzon A, Ochsner KN. Oxytocin selectively improves empathic accuracy. Psychological Science. 2010;21:1426–1428. doi: 10.1177/0956797610383439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Baskerville TA, Douglas AJ. Dopamine and oxytocin interactions underlying behaviors: Potential contributions to behavioral disorders. CNS Neuroscience and Therapeutics. 2010;16:92–123. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2010.00154.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bates E. The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1979. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bell CJ, Nicholson H, Mulder RT, Luty SE, Joyce PR. Plasma oxytocin levels in depression and their correlation with the temperament dimension of reward dependence. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2006;20:656–660. doi: 10.1177/0269881106060512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bono MA, Daley YT, Sigman NM. Relations among joint attention, amount of intervention and language Gain in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2004;34:495–505. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-2545-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Born J, Lange T, Kern W, McGregor GP, Bickel U, Fehm HL. Sniffing neuropeptides: a transnasal approach to the human brain. Nature Neuroscience. 2002;5:514–616. doi: 10.1038/nn849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brooks R, Meltzoff AN. The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Developmental Science. 2005;8:535–543. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00445.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brooks R, Meltzoff AN. Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: a longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language. 2008;35:207–220. doi: 10.1017/s030500090700829x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Brüne M. Does the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) polymorphism (rs2254298) confer ‘vulnerability’ for psychopathology or ‘differential susceptibility’? Insights from evolution. BMC Medicine. 2012;10:38. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-10-38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Bruner J. Child’s talk. New York, NY: Norton; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  21. Cacioppo JT, Norris CJ, Decety J, Montelone G, Nusbaum H. In the eye of the beholder: Individual differences in perceived social isolation predict regional brain activation to social stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21:83–92. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Churchland PS, Winkielman P. Modulating social behavior with oxytocin: How does it work? What does it mean? Hormones and Behavior. 2012;61:392–399. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Clinicaltrials.gov. Oxytocin AND autism [Search] 2012 Retrieved (on 1 November 2012) from: http://www.clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/results?term=oxytocin+AND+autism&Search=Search.
  24. Campbell DB, Datta D, Jones ST, Batey Lee E, Sutcliffe JS, Hammock EA, Levitt P. Association of oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene variants with multiple phenotype domains of autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 2011;2:101–12. doi: 10.1007/s11689-010-9071-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Charman T. Why is joint attention a pivotal skill in autism? Philosophical Transaction of The Royal Society B. 2003;358:315–324. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Charman T, Swettenham J, Baron-Cohen S, Cox A, Baird G, Drew A. An experimental investigation of social-cognitive abilities in infants with autism: Clinical implications. Infant Mental Health Journal. 1998;19:260–275. [Google Scholar]
  27. Chen FS, Johnson SC. An Oxytocin receptor gene variant predicts attachment anxiety in females and autism-spectrum traits in males. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2011;3:93–99. [Google Scholar]
  28. Corkum V, Moore C. The origins of joint visual attention in infants. Developmental Psychology. 1998;34:28–38. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.1.28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Dawson G. Early behavioral intervention, brain plasticity, and the prevention of autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:775–803. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dawson G, Bernier R, Ring RH. Social attention: a possible early indicator of efficacy in autism clinical trials. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 2012;4:11. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-4-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Dawson G, Bernier R. Development of social brain circuitry in Autism. In: Coch D, Dawson G, Fischer KW, editors. Human behavior, learning, and the developing brain: Atypical development. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 28–56. [Google Scholar]
  32. Dawson G, Carver L, Meltzoff AN, Panagiotides H, McPartland J, Webb SJ. Neural correlates of face and object recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical development. Child Development. 2002;73:700–717. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dawson G, Meltzoff AN, Osterling J, Rinaldi J. Neuropsychological correlates of early symptoms of autism. Child Development. 1998;69:1276–85. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Dawson G, Webb SJ, Wijsma E, Schellenberg G, Estes A, Munson J, et al. Neurocognitive and electrophysiological evidence of altered face processing in parents of children with autism: implications for a model of abnormal development of social brain circuitry in autism. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:679–697. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Dichter GS, Richey AJ, Rittenberg AM, Sabatino A, Bodfish JW. Reward circuitry function in autism during face anticipation and outcomes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2012;42:147–160. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1221-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Dichter GS, Felder JN, Green SR, Rittenberg AM, Sasson NJ, Bodfish JW. Reward circuitry function in autism spectrum disorders. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2012;7:160–172. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Domes G, Heinrichs M, Michel A, Berger C, Herpertz SC. Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;61:731–733. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dunham PJ, Moore C. Current themes in research on joint attention. In: Moore C, Dunham PJ, editors. Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1995. pp. 15–28. [Google Scholar]
  39. Ebstein RP, Israel S, Lerer E, Uzefovsky F, Shalev I, Gritsenko I, Yirmiya N. Arginine vasopressin and oxytocin modulate human social behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2009;1167:87–102. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04541.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ebstein RP, Knafo A, Mankuta D, Hong Chew S, San Lai P. The contributions of oxytocin and vasopressin pathway genes to human behavior. Hormones and Behavior. 2012;61:359–379. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Eldevik S, Hastings RP, Hughes CJ, Jahr E, Eikesth S, Cross S. Meta-analysis of early intensive behavioral intervention for children with autism. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2009;38:439–450. doi: 10.1080/15374410902851739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Feifel D, Macdonald K, Nguyen A, Cobb P, Warlan H, Galangue B, Hadley A. Adjunctive intranasal oxytocin reduces symptoms in schizophrenia patients. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;68:678–680. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Feldman R. Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Hormones and Behavior. 2012;61:380–391. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ferguson JN, Aldag JM, Insel TR, Young LJ. Oxytocin in the medial amygdala is essential for social recognition in the mouse. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:8278–8285. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-20-08278.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Ferguson JN, Young LJ, Hearn E, Insel TR, Winslow J. Social amnesia in mice lacking the oxytocin gene. Nature Genetics. 2000;25:284–288. doi: 10.1038/77040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Ferraioli SJ, Harris SL. Teaching joint attention to children with autism through a sibling-mediated behavioral intervention. Behavioral Interventions. 2011;26:261–281. [Google Scholar]
  47. Gainer H, Lively MO, Morris M. Immunological and related techniques for studying neurohypophyseal peptide- processing pathways. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 1995;23:195–207. [Google Scholar]
  48. Gamer M, Zurowski B, Büchel C. Different amygdala subregions mediate valence related and attentional effects of oxytocin in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA. 2010;107:9400–9405. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000985107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Gomez JC, Sarria E, Tamarit J. The comparative study of early communication and theories of mind:ontogeny, phylogeny and pathology. In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen D, editors. Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1993. pp. 397–426. [Google Scholar]
  50. Gordon I, Martin C, Feldman R, Leckman JF. Oxytocin and social motivation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;1:471–492. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Graustella AJ, MacLeod C. A critical review of the influence of oxytocin nasal spray on social cognition in humans: Evidence and future directions. Hormones and Behavior. 2012;61:410–418. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Green LA, Fein D, Modahl C, Feinstein C, Waterhouse L, Morris M. Oxytocin and autistic disorder: alternations in peptide forms. Biological Psychiatry. 2001;50:609–613. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01139-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Gregory SG, Connelly JJ, Towers AJ, Johnson J, Biscocho D, Markunas CA, Pericak-Vance MA. Genomic and epigenetic evidence for oxytocin receptor deficiencies in autism. BMC Medicine. 2009;7:62. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-7-62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Guastella AJ, Einfeld SL, Gray KM, Rinehart NJ, Tonge BJ, Lambert TJ, Hickie IB. Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67:692–4. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Guastella AJ, Mitchell PB, Dadds MR. Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces. Biological Psychiatry. 2008;63:3–5. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Guastella AJ, Mitchell PB, Mathews F. Oxytocin enhances the encoding of positive social memories in humans. Biological Psychiatry. 2008;64:256–258. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Grelotti D, Gauthier I, Schultz R. Social interest and the development of cortical face specialization; what autism teaches us about face processing. Developmental Psychobiology. 2002;40:213–225. doi: 10.1002/dev.10028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Gruastella AJ, MacLeod C. A critical review of the influence of oxytocin nasal spray on social cognition in humans: Evidence and future directions. Hormones and Behavior. 2012;61:410–418. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Hollander E, Bartz JA, Chaplin W, Phillips A. Oxytocin increases retention of social cognition in autism. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;61:498–503. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Hoffman SG, Meuret AE, Smits JAJ, Simon NM, Pollack MH, Otto MW. Augmentation of exposure therapy with D-Cycloserine for social anxiety disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:298–304. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.3.298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Hurlemann R, Patin A, Onur OA, Cohen MX, Baumgartner T, Metzler S, Kendrick KM. Oxytocin enhances amygdala-dependent, socially reinforced learning and emotional empathy in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:4999–5007. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5538-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Ingersoll B. Brief Report: Effect of a focused imitation intervention on social functioning in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2012;42:1768–1773. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1423-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Insel TR, Frenald RD. How the brain processes social information: Searching for the social brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2004;27:697–722. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Insel TR, Hulihan TJ. A gender-specific mechanism for pair bonding: oxytocin and partner-preference formation in monogamous voles. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1995;109:782–789. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.109.4.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Insel TR, O’Brien DJ, Leckman JF. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and autism: is there a connection? Biological Psychiatry. 1999;45:145–157. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00142-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Isaksen J, Holth P. An operant approach to teaching joint attention skills to children with autism. Behavioral Interventions. 2009;24:215–236. [Google Scholar]
  67. Jacob S, Brune CW, Carter CS, Leventhal BL, Lord C, Cook EH., Jr Deletion of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) in Caucasian children and adolescents with autism. Neuroscience Letters. 2007;417:6–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Jones E, Carr E. Joint attention in children with autism: Theory and intervention. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. 2004;19:13–26. [Google Scholar]
  69. Jones W, Carr K, Klin A. Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts social disability in two-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2008;65:946–954. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.8.946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Kaale A, Smith L, Sponheim E. A randomized controlled trial of preschool-based joint attention intervention for children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53:97–105. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02450.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Kampe KK, Frith CD, Dolan RJ, Frith U. Reward value of attractiveness and gaze. Nature. 2001;413:589–602. doi: 10.1038/35098149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Kasari C, Gulsrud AC, Wong C, Kwon S, Locke J. Randomized controlled caregiver mediated joint engagement intervention for toddlers with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;40:1045–1056. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0955-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Kasari C, Freeman S, Paparella T. Joint attention and symbolic play in young children with autism: A randomized controlled intervention study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:611–620. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01567.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Kirsch P, Esslinger C, Chen Q, Mier D, Lis S, Siddhanti, Meyer-Lindenberg A. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:11489–11493. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3984-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Klin A, Jones W, Schultz R, Volkmar F, Cohen D. Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2002;59:809–816. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Koegel LK. Interventions to facilitate communication in autism. Journal of Autism and Devlopmental Disorders. 2000;30:383–391. doi: 10.1023/a:1005539220932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Kohls G, Schulte-Ruther M, Nehrkorn B, Muller K, Fink GR, Kampe-Becker I, Konrad K. Reward system dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2012:1–8. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss033. Advanced access published April 11, 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E. Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature. 2005;435:673–676. doi: 10.1038/nature03701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Landa RJ, Holman KC, O’Neill AH, Stuart EA. Intervention targeting development of socially synchronous engagement in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: a randomized control trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52:13–21. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02288.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Lawton K, Kasari C. Brief report: Longitudinal improvements in the quality of joint attention in preschool children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2012;42:307–312. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1231-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Leekam SR, Lopez B, Moore C. Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36:261–273. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.36.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Lerer E, Levi S, Israel S, Yaari M, Nemanov L, Mankuta D, Ebstein RP. Low CD38 expression in lymphoblastoid cells and haplotypes are both associated with autism in a family-based study. Autism Research. 2010;3:293–302. doi: 10.1002/aur.156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Lerer E, Levi S, Salomon S, Darvasi A, Yirmiya N, Ebstein RP. Association between the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and autism: relationship to Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales and cognition. Molecular Psychiatry. 2008;13:980–988. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Liu X, Kawamura Y, Shimada T, Otowa T, Koishi S, Sugiyama T, et al. Association of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene polymorphisms with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in the Japanese population. Journal of Human Genetics. 2010;55:137–141. doi: 10.1038/jhg.2009.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Liu Y, Wang ZX. Nucleus accumbens dopamine and oxytocin interact to regulate pair bond formation in female prairie voles. Neuroscience. 2003;121:537–544. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00555-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Lord C. Commentary: achievements and future directions for intervention in communication and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2000;30:393–398. doi: 10.1023/a:1005591205002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. MacDonald K, Feifel D. Oxytocin in schizophrenia: a review of evidence for its therapeutic effects. Acta Neuropsychiatrica. 2012;24:130–146. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2011.00634.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. McPartland J, Dawson G, Webb SJ, Panagiotides H, Carver LJ. Event-related brain potentials reveal anomalies in temporal processing of faces in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychological Psychiatry. 2004;45:1235–1245. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00318.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Modahl C, Green LA, Fein D, Morris M, Waterhouse L, Feinstein C, et al. Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children. Biological Psychiatry. 1998;43:270–7. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(97)00439-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Modi ME, Young LJ. The oxytocin system in drug discovery for autism: Animal models and novel therapeutic strategies. Hormones and Behavior. 2012;61:340–350. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Mundy P. Joint attention and social-emotional approach behavior in children with autism. Developmental Psychopathology. 1995;7:63–82. [Google Scholar]
  92. Mundy P, Gomes A. Individual differences in joint attention skill development in the second year. Infant Behavior and Development. 1998;21:469–482. [Google Scholar]
  93. Mundy P, Sigman M, Kasari C. A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1990;20:115–128. doi: 10.1007/BF02206861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Mundy P, Sigman M, Kasari C. The theory of mind and joint attention in autism. In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen D, editors. Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1993. pp. 181–203. [Google Scholar]
  95. Mundy P, Sigman M, Ungerer J, Sherman T. Defining the social deficits of autism: the contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1986;27:657–669. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1986.tb00190.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Mundy P, Sullivan L, Mastergeorge AM. A parallel and distributed-processing model of joint attention, social cognition and autism. Autism Research. 2009;2:2–21. doi: 10.1002/aur.61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Munesue T, Yokoyama S, Nakamura K, Anitha A, Yamada K, Hayashi K, Higashida H. Two genetic variants of CD38 in subjects with autism spectrum disorder and controls. Neuroscience Research. 2010;67:181–191. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2010.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Neuhaus E, Beauchaine TP, Bernier R. Neurobiological correlates of social functioning in autism. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010;30:733–48. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Pedersen CA, Gibson CM, Rau SW, Salimi K, Smedley KL, Casey RL, Penn DL. Intranasal oxytocin reduces psychotic symptoms and improves Theory of mind and social perception in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2011;132:50–53. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Reichow B. Overview of meta-analyses on early intensive behavioral intervention for young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2012;42:512–520. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1218-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Ressler KJ, Rothbaum BO, Tannenbaum L, Anderson P, Graap K, Zimand E, Davis M. Use of D-Cycloserine in phobic individuals to facilitate extinction of fear. Archive of General Psychiatry. 2004;61:1136–1144. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.11.1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Reibold M, Mankuta D, Lerer E, Israel S, Zhong S, Nemanov L, Ebstein RP. All-trans retinoic acid upregulates reduced CD38 transcription in lymphoblastoid cell lines from autism spectrum disorder. Molecular Medicine. 2011;7–8:799–806. doi: 10.2119/molmed.2011.00080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Riekkinen P, Legros JJ, Sennef C, Jolkkonen J, Smitz S, Soininen H. Penetration of DGAVP (Org 5667) across the blood-brain barrier in human subjects. Peptides. 1987;8:261–265. doi: 10.1016/0196-9781(87)90101-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Rogers S, Lewis H. An effective day treatment model for young children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1989;28:207–214. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198903000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Sauer C, Wörner C, Kirsch P, Montag C, Reuter M. Effects of a common variant in the CD38 gene on social processing in an oxytocin challenge study: possible links to autism. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1474–1482. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Scaife M, Bruner J. The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature. 1975;253:255–256. doi: 10.1038/253265a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Schultz W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;80:1–27. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Scott-Van Zeeland AA, Dapretto M, Ghahremani DG, Poldrack RA, Bookheimer Reward processing in autism. Autism Research. 2010;3:53–67. doi: 10.1002/aur.122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Sigman M, Kasari C. Joint attention across contexts in normal and autistic children. In: Moore C, Dunham PJ, editors. Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1995. pp. 189–203. [Google Scholar]
  110. Sigman M, Kasari C, Kwon J, Yirmiya N. Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. Child Development. 1992;63:7996–807. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Sigman M, Mundy P, Sherman T, Ungerer J. Social interactions of autistic, mentally retarded and normal children and their caregivers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1986;27:647–656. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1986.tb00189.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Sigman M, Ruskin E. Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome and developmental delays. Monograph of the Society for Research on Child Development. 1999;64:1–114. doi: 10.1111/1540-5834.00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Siller M, Sigman M. The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2002;32:77–89. doi: 10.1023/a:1014884404276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Skuse DH, Gallagher L. Genetic influences on social cognition. Pediatric Research. 2011;69:85R–91R. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e318212f562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Sullivan PF. Spurious genetic associations. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;61:1121–1126. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Tansey KE, Brookes KJ, Hill MJ, Cochrane LE, Gill M, Skuse D, Anney RJL. Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) does not play a major role in the aetiology of autism: Genetic and molecular studies. Neuroscience Letters. 2010;474:163–167. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.03.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Taylor BA, Hoch H. Teaching children with autism to respond to and initiate bids for joint attention. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 2008;41:377–391. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2008.41-377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Tomasello M. Joint attention as social cognition. In: Moore C, Dunham PJ, editors. Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1995. pp. 103–130. [Google Scholar]
  119. Tomasello M, Farrar MJ. Joint attention and early language. Child Development. 1986;57:1454–1463. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Tops M, Van Peer JM, Korf J, Wijers AA, Tucker DM. Anxiety, attachment, and cortisol predict plasma oxytocin. Psychophysiology. 2007;44:444–449. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00510.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Tost H, Kolachana B, Hakimi S, Lemaitre H, Verchinski BA, Venkata SM, Meyer-Lindenberg A. A common allele in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) impacts prosocial temperament and human hypothalamic-limbic structure and function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107:13936–13941. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1003296107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Vrtička P, Andersson F, Grandjean D, Sander D, Vuilleumier P. Individual attachment style modulates human amygdala and striatum activation during social appraisal. PloS One. 2008;3:e2868. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Walum H, Lichtenstein P, Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Ganiban JM, Spotts EL, Westberg L. Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene is associated with pair-bonding and social behavior. Biological Psychiatry. 2012;71:419–426. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Weisman O, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. Intranasal oxytocin administration is reflected in human saliva. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:1582–1586. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Wermter AK, Kamp-Becker I, Hesse P, Schulte-Körne G, Strauch K, Remschmidt H. Evidence for the involvement of genetic variation in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) in the etiology of Autistic disorders on high-functioning level. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B. 2009;153:629–639. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Whalen C, Schreibman L. Joint attention training for children with autism using behavior modification procedures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2003;44:456–468. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. White PJ, O’Reilly M, Streusand W, Levine A, Sigafoos J, Lancioni, Agular J. Best practices for teaching joint attention: A systematic review of the intervention literature. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2011;5:1283–1295. [Google Scholar]
  128. Wu S, Jia M, Ruan Y, Liu J, Guo Y, Shuang M, Zhang D. Positive association of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) with autism in the Chinese Han population. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;58:74–77. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Ylisaukko-oja T, Alarcon M, Cantor RM, Auranen M, Vanhala R, Kempas E, Peltonen L. Search for autism loci by combined analysis of autism genetic resource exchange and Finnish families. Annals of Neurology. 2006;59:145–155. doi: 10.1002/ana.20722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Young KA, Liu YY, Wang Z. The neurobiology of social attachment: A comparative approach to behavioral, neuroanatomic, and neurochemical studies. Comparative Biochemistry, and Physiology, Part C. 2008;148:401–410. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpc.2008.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Yrigollen CM, Han SS, Kochetkova A, Babitz T, Chang JT, Volkmar FR, Grigorenko EL. Genes controlling affiliative behavior as candidate genes for Autism. Biological Psychiatry. 2008;63:911–916. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.11.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Zink CF, Meyer-Lindenberg A. Human neuroimaging of oxytocin and vasopressin in social cognition. Hormones and Behavior. 2012;61:400–409. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES