Abstract
Objective
This study investigated adolescent and caregiver reports of ADHD symptoms in a sample of clinically referred inner-city adolescents.
Method
Participants (N = 168) included youth ages 12–18 (54% male, 98% ethnic minority) and their caregivers who each completed diagnostic interviews of ADHD symptoms and assessments of perceived need for ADHD treatment and correlated behavior problems.
Results
Informants showed poor agreement on DSM-IV diagnostic categories and also dimensional scales, Inattention/Disorganization (I/D) and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity (H/I). Both caregiver and adolescent reports of I/D symptoms, but not H/I symptoms, were related to perceived need for ADHD treatment. Caregiver reports were linked to behavioral correlates typically associated with ADHD: I/D symptoms correlated with planning/organization and socioemotional deficits, and H/I symptoms correlated with externalizing and behavior regulation deficits. In contrast, adolescent reports of I/D were related to internalizing and externalizing problems, and their reports of H/I correlated with externalizing only. Few gender effects were found.
Conclusion
Study results underscore the developmental salience of I/D symptoms and have implications for ADHD diagnosis and treatment planning for adolescents.
Keywords: adolescent ADHD, informant agreement, perceived need for treatment, ADHD correlates, ADHD subtypes
Research on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in adolescent populations has gained considerable momentum over the past decade. Studies that extend ADHD research beyond childhood into the teenage years include research on longitudinal trajectories and correlates of ADHD symptoms (e.g., Todd et al., 2008), diagnosis of ADHD and co-occurring disorders in high-risk samples (e.g., Malone et al., 2010), and treatment responsiveness to stimulant medications alone (e.g., McGough et al., 2006) or medications combined with behavioral interventions (e.g., Riggs et al., 2011). There is now consensus that ADHD is a prevalent mental health problem across the adolescent developmental span (Taylor, 2009).
The current study focuses on assessment of ADHD in a sample of clinically referred inner-city adolescents. The growing research base on adolescent ADHD has underscored numerous challenges related to symptom assessment and clinical diagnosis for this age group. One important issue is cross-informant agreement between caregivers and teens. Among the most robust findings in clinical child research is that, across a diverse range of ages, ethnicities, and symptom profiles, different informants routinely provide discrepant ratings for emotional and behavioral problems (Comer & Kendall, 2004; De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2004; Yeh & Weisz, 2001), including ADHD classifications (e.g., Hartung, McCarthy, Milich, & Martin, 2005; Rowland et al., 2008; Serra-Pinheiro, Mattos, & Regalla, 2008; Valo & Tannock, 2010). It has also been shown that discrepancies in symptom ratings are larger for parent-teen dyads than for parent-child dyads (Achenbach, McConaughy, & Howell, 1987), which may be related to developmental differences in type of symptom being rated, informant awareness of problematic behaviors, and cross-situational consistency in symptoms (Achenbach, 2011; Hartung et al., 2005). Many advocate the position that modest correspondence does not reflect measurement error but rather ecologically valid differences in reporter perspective that constitute a rich source of nosological and clinical data to be carefully mined (Achenbach, 2011; De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005). The current study follows this logic by exploring differences in the profiles of ADHD correlates reported by caregivers versus their teenagers.
Another key assessment issue is the developmental fit of ADHD subtypes for adolescent populations. The DSM-IV (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—Fourth Edition; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) recognizes three subtypes of ADHD: Inattentive (ADHD-I), characterized by symptoms related to inattention, disorganization, and distractibility; Hyperactive/Impulsive (ADHD-HI), characterized by restlessness, motor and verbal excesses, and impulsivity; and Combined (ADHD-C), which describes youth who meet criteria for both ADHD-I and ADHD-HI. There is substantial support for this nosology in children (see Nigg, Tannock, & Rohde, 2010) and emerging consensus for adolescents as well (e.g., Hudziak et al., 1998; Rohde et al., 2001). Nevertheless, there appear to be several developmental caveats to the ADHD subtype nosology. The DSM-IV criteria are weighted heavily toward behaviors that are more common and/or evident at younger ages (Sibley et al., 2012). For example, the motor restlessness observed in early childhood may convert to cognitive restlessness in teen and young adult years that is not well represented in DSM descriptions (Weyandt et al., 2003). And the age of onset criterion, which requires that symptoms be evident by age 7, is susceptible to retrospective reporting error and under-identifies teens with late-onset symptoms (Todd, Huang, & Henderson, 2008). Some have argued that these and related measurement shortcomings are partially responsible for the marked decline in ADHD prevalence rates during adolescence, especially with regard to hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms (Valo & Tannock, 2010), and lead to under-identification of ADHD in adolescence (Sibley et al., 2012). Thus there is clearly need for additional research describing the psychometric and clinical properties of ADHD subtypes in adolescents.
The current study addressed these key issues in adolescent ADHD assessment in three ways. First, it calculated caregiver-adolescent agreement on ADHD symptoms using both (a) categorical DSM variables based on diagnostic thresholds that yield either a positive or negative diagnosis and (b) dimensional DSM variables based on summed symptom counts that yield normally distributed scores for Inattention/Disorganization (I/D) and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity (H/I). Several recent studies have shown that continuous variables derived from summing the number of endorsed ADHD-I and ADHD-HI symptoms demonstrate equivalent or superior construct validity (Hartung et al., 2005) and reliability and predictive validity properties (Lahey & Willcutt, 2010; Todd et al., 2008; Volk, Todorov, Hay, & Todd, 2009) compared to diagnostic scores, indicating that research on informant agreement should examine both kinds of variables.
Second, this study assessed the relation between endorsed ADHD symptoms and perceived need for ADHD treatment separately for caregiver and teen reports. Perceived need for treatment is one key element of overall treatment motivation, alongside related elements such as perceived barriers to participation, readiness to change, and attitudes about counseling and the treatment system (Heflinger & Hinshaw, 2010; McKay & Bannon, 2004; Neff & Zule, 2000; Yeh et al., 2003). Several studies have demonstrated that caregiver perceived need predicts youth participation in mental health services (e.g., Angold et al., 1998; Shin & Brown, 2009; Yeh et al., 2005), including participation in ADHD services among school-age children (Bussing et al., 2003). However, few have assessed adolescent-perceived need (Logan & King, 2001); notably, such studies have found low concordance between parents and teens on receptivity to (Bussing et al., 2011) and perceived need for (Williams, Lindsey, & Joe, 2011) ADHD services. The current study featured an index of perceived need for ADHD treatment (based on Shen, McLellan, & Merrill, 2000) that was specifically anchored to endorsement of the DSM-IV ADHD items by each informant. This permitted analysis of how strongly each informant’s ADHD symptom ratings predicted his/her perceived need for ADHD services.
Third, this study examined correlates of ADHD symptoms in related domains of adolescent functioning. This afforded a developmentally rich comparison of adolescent versus caregiver constructions of the subtype dimensions as they relate to comorbid problems and behavioral correlates of ADHD. Behaviors commonly linked with adolescent ADHD include: externalizing symptoms such as oppositionality (Barkley et al., 1991; van Lier, van der Ende, Koot, & Verhulst, 2007) and antisocial behavior (Abikoff & Klein, 1992; Satterfield & Schell, 1997); internalizing symptoms such as anxiety and depression (Bauermeister et al., 2007; Hinshaw, Owens, Sami, & Fargeon, 2006; Kessler et al., 2005); and indices of executive functioning such as self-regulation and planning (Barkley, 1997; Coolidge, Thede, & Young, 2000; Holmes et al., 2010). But there remains a paucity of research on contrasts between caregiver versus teen reports of co-occurring problems in ADHD samples (Bauermeister et al., 2007) that can inform the differential meaning of each informant’s symptom ratings.
There are three specific study hypotheses: (1) Caregivers and target adolescents will show low agreement on ADHD diagnostic thresholds (ADHD-I, ADHD-HI, ADHD-C) and also symptom counts (I/D and H/I scores); (2) Caregivers will report higher levels of perceived need for the adolescent’s ADHD treatment than will adolescents themselves, and for both informants, amount of perceived need will be related to amount of I/D and H/I symptoms; (3) I/D and H/I ratings by both informants will be related to behavioral traits that frequently co-occur with adolescent ADHD: externalizing symptoms, internalizing symptoms, callous-unemotional traits, and executive functioning. Of particular interest will be differences between caregiver and adolescent reports in the strength of these relations; the existing literature with adolescent-only samples is not sufficient to formulate specific hypotheses. Gender effects will also be tested in all analyses. Although parents rate boys higher than girls in ADHD symptoms across youth populations (Gershon, 2002; Rescorla et al., 2007), large-sample studies limited to adolescents have generally failed to find consistent gender differences across a variety of reporting sources (e.g., Conners, Sitarenios, Parker, & Epstein, 1998; Hartung et al., 2005; Novik et al., 2006; Rohde et al., 2001; Serra-Pinheiro et al., 2008). Thus it remains important to investigate the influence of gender on caregiver and teen reports of ADHD symptoms and correlates in this age group (Monuteaux, Mick, Faraone, & Biederman, 2010).
Method
Participants
Study participants were 168 adolescents and one primary caregiver per teen. Participants included both males (54%) and females (46%) and averaged 15.1 years of age (SD = 1.3). Self-reported ethnicities were Hispanic (60%), African American (22%), multiracial (11%), White (2%), and other (5%). Households were headed by a single parent (58%), two parents (28%), or grandparents (14%). Among caregivers, 67% graduated high school, 60% worked full- or part-time, 50% earned less than $15,000 per year, and 19% received public assistance, 55% reported a history of child welfare involvement, 35% reported at least one household member had ever used illegal drugs regularly, and 18% reported at least one member had been involved in illegal activities. Adolescents were referred primarily from schools (76%) but also from community-based family service agencies (10%), juvenile justice or child welfare sources (8%), and other sources (5%). Per study inclusion criteria (see below), every adolescent participant was diagnosed with at least one DSM-IV disorder, using the conventional “Or” principle of counting either positive adolescent or positive parent report (see Valo & Tannock, 2010). Overall diagnosis rates were as follows: Oppositional Defiant Disorder = 84%, Conduct Disorder = 53%, ADHD-I = 52%, Major Depressive Disorder and/or Dysthymia = 40%, alcohol and/or substance abuse/dependence = 28%, ADHD-C = 28%, Posttraumatic Stress Disorder = 19%, and Generalized Anxiety Disorder = 16%. Moreover, 88% were diagnosed with more than one disorder; of these comorbid cases, 7% met criteria for ODD + CD only, with the remaining 93% showing some other comorbidity profile.
Demographic characteristics and main study variables for the full sample and separately by gender are depicted in Table 1. Boys and girls did not differ on demographic variables, with the exception of age: boys were significantly older than girls (t(157) = 2.13, p < .05). Significant gender differences were also found for three of the main study variables: Girls self-reported more internalizing behaviors (t(124.4) = −5.00, p < .001) and externalizing behaviors (t(164) = −2.53, p < .05) than boys, and caregivers reported greater planning/organizational skills in their girls compared to boys (t(148) = 2.75, p < .01).
Table 1.
Full Sample | Boys | Girls | |
---|---|---|---|
N | 167 | 89 | 78 |
Age in Years (M/SD)* | 15.1 (1.3) | 15.3 (1.4) | 14.8 (1.3) |
Ethnicity | |||
Hispanic | 60% | 54% | 65% |
African American | 22% | 24% | 19% |
Other | 19% | 21% | 15% |
Family Composition | |||
Single Parent | 58% | 54% | 63% |
Two Parents | 28% | 29% | 26% |
Grandparents | 14% | 17% | 11% |
Caregiver Characteristics | |||
Graduated High School | 67% | 65% | 69% |
Worked Full-or Part-Time | 60% | 60% | 59% |
Annual Income less than $15K | 50% | 49% | 50% |
Received Public Assistance | 19% | 14% | 24% |
Household History Characteristics | |||
Child Welfare Involvement | 55% | 49% | 61% |
One Member Regularly Used Drugs | 35% | 38% | 30% |
One Member Involved in Illegal Activities | 18% | 19% | 17% |
Participant Referral Source | |||
School | 76% | 77% | 76% |
Other | 24% | 23% | 24% |
Adolescent Externalizing Symptoms | |||
CBCL Externalizing Score (M/SD) | 19.2 (11.8) | 19.6 (12.4) | 18.8 (11.2) |
YSR Externalizing Score (M/SD)* | 14.4 (8.7) | 12.8 (7.6) | 16.2 (9.5) |
Adolescent Internalizing Symptoms | |||
CBCL Internalizing Score (M/SD) | 13.1 (8.5) | 12.4 (7.7) | 13.9 (9.3) |
YSR Internalizing Score (M/SD)*** | 10.0 (7.9) | 7.3 (5.6) | 13.2 (8.9) |
Adolescent Behavioral Characteristics | |||
ICU Total Score (M/SD) | 33.0 (12.1) | 32.5 (12.8) | 33.5 (11.2) |
BRIEF Behavior Regulation Index (M/SD) | 47.9 (10.8) | 48.5 (10.2) | 47.1 (11.6) |
BRIEF Plan/Organize Scale (M/SD)** | 23.7 (5.3) | 24.8 (5.2) | 22.4 (5.3) |
Note.
p < .05,
p < .01,
p < .001
CBCL = Child Behavior Checklist; YSR = Youth Self-Report; ICU = Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits; BRIEF = Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function.
Study Recruitment, Participation Rates, and Procedures
Study recruitment procedures were designed to identify adolescents with untreated mental health disorders and to assist interested families in initiating available treatment services. To recruit adolescents with unmet treatment needs, research staff developed a community-based referral network of high schools, family service agencies, and youth programs in inner-city areas within a large northeastern city. Staff made regular on-site visits and phone calls to referral partners to maintain communication about current and potential cases. There were five study referral criteria: (a) target adolescent was between 12 and 18 years old; (b) adolescent lived with an adult family member who acted as primary caregiver; (c) adolescent was observed or suspected by referral source to have significant behavioral problems that impaired functioning; (d) adolescent problems were deemed beyond the scope of routine services available at the referral site (e.g., guidance/counseling services in schools, case management in family agencies); and (e) adolescent was not currently enrolled in behavioral treatment. Network partners made referrals to research staff during site visits and also by phone and confidential email. Staff then contacted referred families by phone to offer a home-based family research interview to assess the reason for study referral and discuss current developmental challenges.
A total of 675 adolescents were referred by network partners at the time of this study. Of these, 225 (33%) could not be recruited by research staff because the contact information was invalid or because the family did not respond to repeated voice messages. The contacted sample differed from the uncontacted sample in age (uncontacted had a higher proportion older than 15 years: χ2(1) = 7.9, p < .01), and referral source (uncontacted had a higher proportion referred by schools: χ2(1) = 7.6, p < .01). Of the 450 cases (67% of all referrals) successfully contacted, 359 (80%) completed a home-based family eligibility screen, whereas 91 (20%) refused due to disinterest (18%) or lack of time (2%). A full description of the demographic and clinical characteristics of the screen interview sample is available elsewhere (Hogue & Dauber, in press). Of the 359 families successfully screened, 168 (47%) met criteria for at least one DSM-IV Axis I diagnosis and subsequently completed the baseline interview; this group constitutes the sample for the current study. Reasons for attrition from the baseline interview included inability to re-establish contact with the family after the screen interview (31%), disinterest in participating (11%), or failed to meet criteria for a DSM-IV diagnosis (4%). No demographic differences were found between the study sample (who completed both screen and baseline interviews) and the attrition sample (who were successfully screened but did not complete a baseline interview).
Interviews were conducted by research staff primarily in the home but also in other locations upon request. Caregivers and teens were consented and interviewed separately; caregivers consented for themselves and their adolescents, and adolescents assented for themselves. Participants were informed that a federal Certificate of Confidentiality from the National Institutes of Health was obtained to protect their confidentiality. Assessment measures consisted of structured clinical interviews and audio computer-assisted self-report measures. Caregiver assessments were administered in the preferred language: 77% English, 23% Spanish. Caregivers and teens each received $35 in vouchers for completing the eligibility screen and $40 each for completing the baseline interview. After interview completion, interested families were linked to appropriate treatment services by research staff using intensive family-based engagement strategies (McKay & Bannon, 2004). The study was conducted under approval by the governing Institutional Review Board.
Measures
ADHD Symptoms and Diagnoses
ADHD symptoms and diagnoses were assessed using the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI, Version 5.0; Sheehan et al., 1998). Adolescents and caregivers were interviewed separately. The MINI is a brief structured diagnostic interview that assesses DSM-IV diagnoses in adolescent and adult populations. The MINI is specifically designed to be administered by lay interviewers and has demonstrated solid interrater and test-retest reliability on two international samples of psychiatric and nonpsychiatric patients (Lecrubier et al., 1997) and also excellent convergent validity with both the SCID and the CIDI (Lecrubier et al., 1997; Sheehan et al., 1997; Sheehan et al., 1998). ADHD variables used in the current study include both dimensional symptom counts and ADHD diagnostic categories. Two dimensional variables were calculated: total number of symptoms endorsed (out of 9) on the Inattentive/Disorganized (I/D) subscale, and total number of symptoms endorsed (of 9) on the Hyperactive/Impulsive (H/I) subscale. The three DSM-IV ADHD diagnostic categories were also included: ADHD-Inattentive subtype (ADHD-I), ADHDHyperactive/ Impulsive subtype (ADHD-HI), and ADHD-Combined subtype (ADHD-C).
Perceived Need for Treatment
To assess perceived need for treatment (PNT), we adopted two items from the Addiction Severity Index supported by strong reliability and validity data (McLellan et al., 1992; Shen, et al., 2000). Whenever either an adolescent or caregiver reported at least one symptom of ADHD that caused significant impairment, the informant was asked: “During the past month, how much have you been troubled or bothered by these [ADHD] symptoms you just reported?” Participants responded “Not at all” (0), “A little” (1), or “A lot” (2). If an informant reported a score of 1 or 2, he or she was then asked: “Is treatment in this area important to you, and if so, how much?” and given the same response choices: “Not at all” (0), “A little” (1), or “A lot” (2). The final ADHD PNT score was computed by summing the troubled and bothered score and the treatment importance score (summed scores had psychometric properties that were slightly favorable compared to multiplicative scores; Hogue & Dauber, in press). ADHD PNT scores were calculated separately for adolescent and caregivers.
Externalizing and Internalizing Symptoms
The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; caregiver report; Achenbach, 1991a) and Youth Self-Report (YSR; Achenbach, 1991b) are parallel measures of youth behavioral and emotional problems supported by extensive evidence encompassing reliability, validity, and clinical utility (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). These measures have been administered to a wide range of clinical and ADHD adolescent populations (e.g., Biederman et al., 2001, Doyle, Mick, & Biederman, 2007, McConaughy & Achenbach, 1994). Each measure contains a summary scale of Externalizing (delinquent and aggressive) and Internalizing (withdrawn, anxious/depressed, somatic complaints) symptoms used in the current study; high internal consistency (α = .87–.93) was found for all four scales in the sample.
Sociopathy
Adolescent sociopathy was assessed using the 24-item, parent-report Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (ICU; Essau, Sasagawa, & Frick, 2006). The ICU is a 24-item questionnaire that assesses adolescents’ callous-unemotional traits. The ICU was developed using items from the Callous-Unemotional (CU) subscale of the Antisocial Process Screening Device (Frick & Hare, 2001), which is a widely used scale to assess antisocial traits in youth. The construct validity of the ICU was supported in a large community sample (n =1,443) of German adolescents (Essau et al., 2006) as well an American sample (n = 248) of juvenile offenders (Kimonis et al., 2008). In both samples, the total scale showed internal consistency (alpha = .77 and .81, respectively); expected associations with aggression, delinquency, personality traits, emotional reactivity, and psychosocial impairment; and strong validity associations with aggression, delinquency, personality traits, emotional reactivity, and psychosocial impairment. Internal consistency in the study sample was high (α = .88).
Behavior Regulation and Planning
The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) is a caregiver-report measure of behavioral problems that are linked to executive functioning and commonly observed in ADHD youth (Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000, MaCandless & O' Laughlin, 2007).Gioia et al. (2000) report good convergent and discriminant validity between the BRIEF and similar behavioral rating systems as well as test-retest reliability statistics ranging from .79 to .88 during a 2-week period; internal consistency ranging from α = .80 to .98; and interrater reliability between parent and teacher responses of r = .32. The BRIEF has been validated on ADHD outpatient samples (Mares, McLuckie, Schwartz, & Saini, 2007) and teens with mixed clinical diagnoses (Gioia Isquith, Retzlaff, & Espy, 2002). This study used two BRIEF scales, the Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) global scale (comprised of the Inhibition, Behavioral Shift, and Emotional Control subscales) and the Plan/Organize scale. Internal consistency in our sample was α = .93 for the BRI and α = .85 for the Plan/Organize scale. Higher scores indicate poorer functioning.
Statistical Analyses
Preliminary analyses included descriptive statistics of adolescent and caregiver reports on the ADHD dimensional scales (I/D and H/I) and perceived need for treatment (PNT) scores, as well as mean comparisons of gender, age, and ethnicity differences on these variables using independent samples t-tests to identify possible covariates for subsequent analyses. We examined adolescent and caregiver agreement on ADHD categorical diagnoses using Kappa coefficients and on ADHD dimensional scales using Pearson’s correlations. This was followed by hierarchical regressions testing the degree to which I/D and H/I scores predicted PNT scores for each informant, conducted separately for adolescent and caregiver reports, as follows: In Step 1, gender was entered as a covariate; in Step 2, the ADHD dimensional scales were entered simultaneously to examine the effect of each on PNT while controlling for the effects of the other; in Step 3, the dimensional interaction term (I/D*H/I) and gender interaction terms (I/D*Sex, H/I*Sex) were entered to explore moderator effects. Finally, we analyzed the relation between ADHD symptoms and related behavioral variables, controlling for gender effects. A series of hierarchical multiple regressions were conducted to examine the individual and interactive effects of I/D and H/I scores on the following dependent variables (DVs): externalizing symptoms, internalizing symptoms, callous-unemotional traits, school engagement, and executive functioning. Two sets of regressions were conducted, one for adolescent-reported and one for caregiver-reported ADHD scores. A separate regression was conducted for each DV: In Step 1, gender was entered as a covariate; in Step 2, the two ADHD dimensional scales were entered simultaneously to examine the effect of each on study DVs while controlling for the effects of the other; in Step 3, the interaction term (I/D*H/I) was tested to examine moderating effects for ADHD dimensional symptoms. We did not test three-way interactions involving gender effects because the study was under-powered to detect three-way interactions, and there were no a priori hypotheses to guide interpretation of significant findings.
Results
Descriptive Statistics for the ADHD Dimensional Scales (I/D and H/I) and Perceived Need for Treatment (PNT)
Descriptive statistics on the I/D and H/I dimensional scales and PNT for adolescent and caregiver reports are depicted in Table 2 for the full sample, as well as separately by gender. As seen in the table, the dimensional scales and PNT scores were normally distributed in the full sample and in the gender groups for both adolescent and caregiver reports. Gender differences were found on the I/D scale for caregiver report, with caregivers indicating more I/D symptoms in their boys than in their girls (t(165) = 1.96, p = .05). No gender differences were found for H/I symptoms, and no significant age or ethnicity differences were found for either scale. No gender, age, or ethnicity differences were found for adolescent or caregiver PNT.
Table 2.
Adolescent Report | Caregiver Report | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | M | SD | Skew (SE) |
Kurtosis (SE) |
N | M | SD | Skew (SE) |
Kurtosis (SE) |
|
Inattentive/Disorganized Scale | ||||||||||
Full Sample | 167 | 4.3 | 2.4 | −.25 (.19) | −.87 (.37) | 167 | 5.6 | 2.6 | −.64 (.19) | −.72 (.37) |
Boys | 89 | 4.0 | 2.4 | −.09 (.26) | −.90 (.51) | 89 | 5.9* | 2.5 | −.79 (.26) | −.45 (.51) |
Girls | 78 | 4.7 | 2.3 | −.45 (.27) | −.66 (.54) | 78 | 5.2* | 2.7 | −.47 (.27) | −.94 (.54) |
Hyperactive/Impulsive Scale | ||||||||||
Full Sample | 166 | 3.4 | 2.3 | .32 (.19) | −.72 (.38) | 167 | 3.1 | 2.6 | .64 (.19) | −.59 (.37) |
Boys | 88 | 3.3 | 2.1 | .45 (.26) | −.16 (.51) | 89 | 3.1 | 2.6 | .60 (.26) | −.56 (.51) |
Girls | 78 | 3.6 | 2.5 | .18 (.27) | −1.1 (.54) | 78 | 3.1 | 2.7 | .69 (.27) | −.58 (.54) |
Perceived Need for Treatment (PNT) | ||||||||||
Full Sample | 158 | .81 | 1.3 | 1.2 (.19) | .11 (.38) | 156 | 2.9 | 1.6 | −1.1 (.19) | −.49 (.39) |
Boys | 83 | .64 | 1.1 | 1.6 (.26) | 1.5 (.52) | 85 | 2.9 | 1.5 | −1.2 (.26) | −.05 (.52) |
Girls | 75 | 1.0 | 1.4 | .92 (.28) | −.72 (.55) | 71 | 2.8 | 1.6 | −.92 (.29) | −.85 (.56) |
Note.
There was a significant difference between boys and girls on the Inattentive/Disorganized Scale according to caregiver report, (t (165) = 1.96, p = .05). No other significant gender, age, or ethnicity differences were found on any scale for either adolescent or caregiver reports.
Paired samples t-tests were conducted to examine differences in levels of I/D versus H/I symptoms reported by adolescents and caregivers in the full sample and separately by gender. Significantly more inattention symptoms than hyperactivity symptoms were reported by both adolescents and caregivers in the full sample and within each gender group: Adolescent report full sample: t(165) = 4.5, p < .001; Caregiver report full sample: t(166) = 11.1, p < .001; Adolescent report boys: t(87) = 2.9, p < .01; Caregiver report boys: t(88) = 9.2, p < .001; Adolescent report girls: t(77) = 3.5, p < .01; Caregiver report girls: t(77) = 6.5, p < .001. Paired samples t-tests were also conducted to compare adolescent and caregiver PNT for the 146 cases that had available scores from both informants. Caregivers reported higher PNT than adolescents in the full sample (t (145) = −12.2, p < .001), as well as among boys (t (77) = −10.5, p < .001) and girls (t (67) = −6.9, p < .001).
Bivariate correlations of adolescent and caregiver reports on each dimensional scale as well as scale correlations within informant were examined for the full sample and for gender, ethnicity, and age subgroups. Findings are presented for the full sample only, as correlations in the subgroups mirrored those for the full sample. Low correlations were found between adolescent and caregiver reports on both I/D (r = .14, ns) and H/I (r = .16, p < .05), indicating weak agreement between informants on ADHD symptoms and supporting the decision to conduct remaining study analyses separately by reporter. I/D and H/I scales were moderately correlated for both adolescent (r = .37, p < .001) and caregiver reports (r = .41, p < .001).
Cross-Informant Agreement on the ADHD Diagnostic Categories
Agreement between adolescent and caregiver on the ADHD diagnostic subtypes (ADHD-I, ADHD-HI, ADHD-C) is shown in Table 3. The data reveal a modest 36% rate of absolute agreement overall: For 61 of the 168 participants, teens and parents gave identical reports, including 25% of the sample (n = 42) who jointly reported no ADHD diagnosis. One-tenth of the sample (n = 16) agreed on the ADHD-I diagnosis, and only 2% (n = 3) agreed on ADHD-C. An additional 11% were in partial agreement, wherein one reporter indicated ADHD-C while the other indicated either ADHD-I or ADHD-HI. The largest example of partial agreement (n = 11; 7% of the sample) resulted from an adolescent reporting ADHD-C while his/her caregiver reported ADHD-I. Only 4 participants were diagnosed as ADHD-HI by either reporter, with no agreement among these. Finally, we identified the percentage of pair agreement for each “diagnostic decision”: No Diagnosis, ADHD-I, and ADHD-HI. For No Diagnosis, 33% of those cases with at least one registered report of no diagnosis were matched with a second report of no diagnosis. Pair agreement was 30% for ADHD-I cases and 9% of ADHD-HI cases, respectively. Cohen’s κ (see Bakeman & Gottman, 1997) was calculated as a measure of cross-informant agreement for each diagnosis that controls for chance agreement. Guidelines for interpreting values of κ are provided by Landis and Koch (1977): κ between .40 – .60 is considered moderate, .60 – .80 substantial, and > .80 excellent. For the current study, κ = .05 (SE = .07, ns) for ADHD-I, κ = −.03 (SE = .07, ns) for ADHD-C, κ = −.04 (SE = .01, ns) for ADHD-HI, and κ = .04 (SE = .07, ns) for any positive ADHD diagnosis, indicating virtually nonexistent diagnostic agreement between adolescent and caregiver beyond chance occurrence.
Table 3.
Adolescent Report | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Caregiver Report |
No Diagnosis | ADHD-I | ADHD-HI | ADHD-C | Total |
No Diagnosis | 42 (25%) | 13 (8%) | 6 (4%) | 6 (4%) | 67 |
ADHD-I | 37 (22%) | 16 (10%) | 4 (2%) | 11 (7%) | 68 |
ADHD-HI | 2 (1%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 2 (1%) | 4 |
ADHD-C | 19 (11%) | 6 (4%) | 0 (0%) | 3 (2%) | 28 |
Total | 100 | 35 | 10 | 22 | 168 |
Note.
ADHD-I = ADHD-Inattentive Subtype; ADHD-HI = ADHD-Hyperactive/Impulsive Subtype; ADHD-C = ADHD-Combined Subtype. Numbers in bold type indicate absolute agreement and numbers in italicized type indicate partial agreement. Percentages in parentheses are drawn from the sample size of N = 168. Total figures for each diagnosis according to caregiver and adolescent reports are provided in the final column and row, respectively.
Relation of ADHD Dimensional Scales to Perceived Need for ADHD Treatment
Hierarchical regressions were conducted to examine the degree to which I/D and H/I symptoms predicted adolescent and caregiver PNT. Results are presented in Table 4. In order to account for the potential influence of DSM-IV diagnoses other than ADHD on the perceived need for ADHD treatment, the total number of co-occurring diagnoses was included in Step 1 as a covariate, along with gender. Step 2 included simultaneous entry of I/D and H/I dimensional scores, and Step 3 included interactions among I/D and H/I and gender. Adolescent-reported ADHD symptoms were used in the analysis predicting adolescent PNT, and caregiver-reported symptoms were used to predict caregiver PNT. Higher number of co-occurring diagnoses was associated with higher adolescent PNT (β = .21, p < .05) but not with caregiver PNT. Higher I/D symptoms predicted greater adolescent-reported PNT (β = .33, p < .001) and also caregiver-reported PNT (β = .48, p < .001). Additionally, there was a significant I/D by H/I interaction predicting caregiver PNT (β = −.61, p < .05). Probing this interaction using a mean-split strategy revealed that caregiver-reported I/D predicted caregiver PNT more strongly when the H/I score was low (β = .54, p < .001). When H/I was high, the predictive strength of I/D was diminished (β = .22, p = .07). H/I symptoms did not predict either adolescent PNT or caregiver PNT. No moderating effects of gender were found.
Table 4.
Adolescent PNT (N = 158) | Caregiver PNT (N = 156) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | SE(B) | β | P | B | SE(B) | β | P | |
Stepl: | ||||||||
Gender | .21 | .20 | .08 | Ns | −.21 | .26 | −.07 | Ns |
Number of CODs1 | .19 | .08 | .21 | >05 | .09 | .10 | .07 | Ns |
Step 2: | ||||||||
ADHD-I/D | .18 | .05 | .33 | >.001 | .31 | .05 | .48 | >.001 |
ADHD-H/I | −.06 | .05 | −.11 | Ns | .06 | .04 | .10 | Ns |
Step 3: | ||||||||
ADHD-I/D*Gender | .04 | .09 | .13 | Ns | −.07 | .10 | −.20 | Ns |
ADHD-H/I*Gender | −.03 | .09 | −.11 | Ns | .13 | .09 | .37 | Ns |
ADHD-I/D*ADHD-H/I | −.02 | .02 | −.21 | Ns | −.04 | .02 | −.61 | >.05 |
Note.
PNT = Perceived Need for Treatment; COD = Co-Occurring Diagnosis; I/D = Inattentive/Disorganized Scale; H/I = Hyperactive/Impulsive Scale
In order to account for potential influence of co-occurring diagnoses (i.e., diagnoses other than ADHD) on PNT, the total number of co-occurring diagnoses for each adolescent was included as a covariate.
For Adolescent PNT, R2 change was .06 for Step 1 (p > .01), .09 for Step 2 (p < .001), and .01 for Step 3 (ns).
For Caregiver PNT, R2 change was .01 for Step 1 (ns), .28 for Step 2 (p < .001), and .04 for Step 3 (p = .06).
Relation of ADHD Dimensional Scales to Co-Occurring Problems
As described above, a series of hierarchical regressions were conducted to examine the relations between I/D and H/I scales and adolescent behavior problems, callous-unemotional traits, school engagement, and executive functioning, controlling for gender. Separate analyses were conducted for adolescent and caregiver reports and results are presented in Table 5. Adolescent-reported I/D symptoms were associated with greater adolescent-reported internalizing symptoms (β = .29, p < .001) and externalizing symptoms (β = .24, p < .01). Adolescent-reported H/I symptoms were associated with more externalizing symptoms, based on both adolescent report (β = .31, p < .001) and caregiver report (β = .19, p < .05). Finally, a significant I/D by H/I interaction was found for callous-unemotional traits (β = .73, p < .01). Probing this interaction revealed a trend-level effect of I/D symptoms on callous-unemotional traits only when H/I symptoms were also high (β = .24, p < .10). Although it did not reach statistical significance, the effect of I/D symptoms on callous-unemotional traits in the presence of low H/I symptoms was in the opposite direction (β = −.16, p = .17). Thus, it may be that higher I/D symptoms are associated with more sociopathy traits when H/I symptoms are also high, but with lower sociopathy traits when H/I symptoms are low.
Table 5.
Step 1: | Step 2: | Step 3: | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | I/D and H/I Scales | I/D*H/I | ||||||
Adolescent Report |
||||||||
B (SE) | β | I/D: B (SE) | I/D: β | H/I: B (SE) | H/I: β | B (SE) | β | |
YSR Internalizing | 5.7 (1.1) | .37*** | .97 (.25) | .29*** | .07 (.26) | .02 | −.01 (.11) | −.02 |
YSR Externalizing | 3.2 (1.3) | .19* | .90 (.28) | .24** | 1.2 (.29) | .31*** | .15 (.11) | .28 |
CBCL Internalizing | 1.6 (1.3) | .10 | .31 (.30) | .09 | −.40 (.32) | −.11 | −.06 (.13) | −.11 |
CBCL Externalizing | −.72 (1.9) | −.03 | −.78 (.42) | −.16 | 1.0 (.44) | .19* | .16 (.17) | .22 |
Invent. Callous-Unemotional (ICU) | .62 (2.1) | .03 | −.04 (.49) | −.01 | .06 (.51) | .01 | .55 (.20) | .73** |
Behavior Regulation Index (BRI) | −1.3 (1.9) | −.06 | −.21 (.43) | −.05 | −.14 (.45) | −.03 | .07 (.18) | .11 |
Plan/Organize | −2.3 (.86) | −.22** | .25 (.20) | .11 | −.10 (.21) | −.04 | .13 (.08) | .40 |
Caregiver Report |
||||||||
B (SE) | β | I/D: B (SE) | I/D: β | H/I: B (SE) | H/I: β | B (SE) | β | |
YSR Internalizing | 5.8 (1.1) | .37*** | −.02 (.24) | −.01 | −.27 (.24) | −.09 | .10 (.10) | .28 |
YSR Externalizing | 3.3 (1.3) | .19* | .14 (.29) | .04 | .08 (.28) | .03 | .02 (.12) | .06 |
CBCL Internalizing | 1.4 (1.3) | .08 | .33 (.28) | .10 | .08 (.28) | .03 | −.03 (.12) | −.07 |
CBCL Externalizing | −.86 (1.8) | −.04 | .01 (.36) | .00 | 1.8 (.36) | .41*** | .07 (.15) | .12 |
Invent. Callous-Unemotional (ICU) | .98 (2.1) | .04 | .84 (.43) | .18* | .83 (.41) | .18* | .25 (.18) | .44 |
Behavior Regulation Index (BRI) | −1.4 (1.9) | −.07 | .75 (.33) | .19* | 1.8 (.33) | .43*** | .12 (.14) | .24 |
Plan/Organize | −2.4 (.86) | −.22** | 1.1 (.15) | .55*** | −.02 (.15) | −.01 | .05 (.06) | .19 |
Note.
p < .05,
p < .01,
p < .001
Adolescent Report: YSR Internalizing Step 1 R2Δ = .134***, Step 2 R2Δ = .089***, Step 3 R2Δ = .000; YSR Externalizing Step 1 R2Δ = .034*, Step 2 R2Δ = .210***, Step 3 R2Δ = .009; CBCL Internalizing Step 1 R2Δ = .009, Step 2 R2Δ = .012, Step 3 R2Δ = .001; CBCL Externalizing Step 1 R2Δ = .001, Step 2 R2Δ = .040*, Step 3 R2Δ = .005; ICU Step 1 R2Δ = .001, Step 2 R2Δ = .000, Step 3 R2Δ = .056**; BRI Step 1 R2Δ = .004, Step 2 R2Δ = .004, Step 3 R2Δ = .001; Plan/Organize Step 1 R2Δ = .047**, Step 2 R2Δ = .010, Step 3 R2Δ = .017.
Caregiver Report: YSR Internalizing Step 1 R2Δ = .137***, Step 2 R2Δ = .009, Step 3 R2Δ = .005; YSR Externalizing Step 1 R2Δ = .036*, Step 2 R2Δ = .003, Step 3 R2Δ = .000; CBCL Internalizing Step 1 R2Δ = .007, Step 2 R2Δ = .013, Step 3 R2Δ = .000; CBCL Externalizing Step 1 R2Δ = .001, Step 2 R2Δ = .168***, Step 3 R2Δ = .001; ICU Step 1 R2Δ = .002, Step 2 R2Δ = .092**, Step 3 R2Δ = .013; BRI Step 1 R2Δ = .004, Step 2 R2Δ = .275***, Step 3 R2Δ = .004; Plan/Organize Step 1 R2Δ = .049**, Step 2 R2Δ = .292***, Step 3 R2Δ = .003.
Caregiver-reported I/D symptoms were associated with more callous-unemotional traits (β = .18, p < .05), as well as higher scores on the behavior regulation index (β = .19, p < .05) and the plan/organize scale (β = .55, p < .001). Caregiver-reported H/I symptoms were associated with greater externalizing symptoms (caregiver reported) (β = .41, p < .001), more callousunemotional traits (β = .18, p < .05), and higher scores on the behavior regulation index (β = .43, p < .001). No interactions between I/D and H/I were found for caregiver reported symptoms.
Discussion
This study is among the first to examine youth-caregiver agreement, perceived need for treatment, and co-occurring problems related to ADHD dimensional symptoms among clinically referred adolescents. There were three main study findings. First, there was markedly low agreement between caregivers and teens on ADHD diagnostic categories and dimensional scales. This replicates previous research demonstrating weak cross-reporter correspondence across a range of youth behavior problems, including ADHD (e.g., Rowland et al., 2008; Sayal & Goodman, 2009). These data confirm that parents and teens make highly discrepant judgments about adolescent ADHD symptoms as contained in the DSM-IV.
A second main finding was that I/D symptoms, but not H/I symptoms, predicted perceived need for ADHD treatment on the part of both caregivers and teens. This finding was robust even when controlling for other psychiatric diagnoses and for level of H/I symptoms. Also, among caregivers only, I/D symptoms were more strongly related to perceived treatment needs when relatively fewer H/I symptoms were identified. These results underscore the clinical salience of the I/D subtype in adolescent populations: Not only are I/D symptoms more prevalent than H/I symptoms, they appear to be strongly related to family perceptions about counseling needs. This finding takes on added significance in light of emerging data suggesting that both parent and adolescent perceptions about the need for behavioral treatment are important predictors of treatment participation (e.g., Williams et al., 2011), including treatment for ADHD specifically (Bussing et al., 2011).
The third main finding was that caregiver and adolescent reports of ADHD produced notably different profiles of behavioral correlates. Caregiver reports of I/D symptoms were related to executive functioning and socioemotional deficits—planning/organization, behavior regulation, sociopathy—that are typically associated with ADHD; in contrast, adolescent reports of I/D were related exclusively to internalizing and externalizing problems. Similarly, caregiver reports of H/I matched the fuller spectrum of behavioral excess associated with the H/I construct—externalizing and behavior regulation deficits—whereas adolescent reports of H/I were related to externalizing problems only. On the one hand these data support the validity, and thus the utility, of caregiver data for diagnosing both dimensions of adolescent ADHD (Sibley et al., 2012). On the other hand, findings raise intriguing questions about the informative value of adolescent reports. According to the teenagers themselves, their ADHD symptoms were associated with broad-based psychological symptoms but not with the self-regulation and self-organizational deficits thought to be endemic to ADHD youth (Coolidge et al., 2000; Dendy, 2006; Holmes et al., 2010). The profile of adolescent I/D correlates was particularly complex, intertwined most strongly with internalizing problems and also related to callous-unemotional traits among those teens with elevated H/I symptoms; in contrast, ADHD was not associated with internalizing problems according to caregivers. Because I/D is arguably the key dimension for diagnosis and treatment planning among adolescents, more research on the convergent and discriminant validity of adolescent-reported I/D symptoms is certainly warranted. Such research would add to our understanding of the conceptual and psychometric underpinnings of ADHD-I in adolescents, as well as provide new insights into the clinical (e.g., Dendy, 2006) and research (Carr, Henderson, & Nigg, 2010; Diamond, 2005; Milich, Balentine, & Lynam, 2001) debates as to whether ADHD-I and ADHD-C are two variants of the same disorder or altogether separate clinical syndromes.
Despite poor agreement on symptom counts and differential profiles of behavioral correlates, caregivers and teens were uniform in reporting higher levels of I/D symptoms compared to H/I symptoms, and this was equally true for boys and girls. This confirms research showing that I/D is far more prevalent than H/I among teens (e.g., Sibley et al., 2012; Todd et al., 2008), unlike younger children, who tend to exhibit relatively equivalent levels of each dimension (e.g., Valo & Tannock, 2010). Interestingly, only caregivers reported higher levels of I/D for boys than girls, and neither informant reported gender differences for H/I. Results overall support the assertion that ADHD creates significant multi-domain impairment for teenage girls as well as boys (Hinshaw et al., 2006; Monuteaux et al., 2010; Owens et al., 2009).
Study Strengths and Limitations
Several features of the study design strengthen the validity of reported findings. The assessment design included both adolescent and caregiver reports of most study variables, and the analytic plan encompassed both dimensional (i.e., symptom counts) and categorical (i.e., diagnostic subtypes) constructs for ADHD. By including both I/D and H/I scores in all dimensional analyses, we were able to calculate the unique effect of each score on predicted variables, independent of the other dimension. Analyses also controlled for gender differences in individual levels of ADHD symptoms and for gender effects on ADHD symptom-correlate relations. Space limitations precluded analysis of individual and family characteristics that may be related to caregiver-adolescent reporting discrepancies. There is keen interest in whether informant discrepancies about behavioral symptoms are related to the developmental course of symptoms over time (e.g., De Los Reyes, Goodman, Kliewer, & Reid-Quinones, 2010) and to treatment planning (e.g., Hawley & Weisz, 2003). However, such analyses require theory-driven examination of multiple facets of observed discrepancies (De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005), which was beyond the scope of this study.
Common source variance was an unavoidable influence on the analyses of ADHD correlates: For most (but not all) significant effects, caregiver-reported ADHD symptoms predicted caregiver-reported correlates, and likewise for adolescent report. However, the impact of common source variance was at least partly mitigated by the inclusion of both dimensional scales as simultaneous predictors in regression analyses: Each scale acted as a statistical control for the other, partialling out common variance associated with reporter identity prior to calculation of unique scale effects. Also note that there were two “crossover” effects: adolescent-reported ADHD symptoms predicted caregiver-reported externalizing problems and sociopathy. It would be possible to eliminate the common variance confound altogether by utilizing a multitrait-multimethod analytic approach that fully models reporter effects across a combined-informant data set (e.g., Greenbaum, Dedrick, Prange, & Friedman, 1994; Phillips, Lonigan, Driscoll, & Hooe, 2002). However, whereas this approach might better inform the construct validity of ADHD reports, it would negate our ability to examine the internal diagnostic logic that distinguishes adolescent from caregiver reports and to accentuate differences in reporter perspective that may have strong ecological validity and clinical implications (Achenbach, 2011; Valo & Tannock, 2010).
The study design was limited by the absence of teacher reports of ADHD symptoms. Teacher reports are not required for making a DSM-IV ADHD diagnosis—the only contextual stipulation is that impairment be reported for multiple settings—and acquiring teacher data for teens is complicated by the fact that several teachers are involved with any given student during a routine school day (Evans, Serpell, Schultz, & Pastor, 2007). Nevertheless, in their longitudinal study comparing gold-standard childhood ADHD diagnoses to diagnoses made in adolescence,Sibley et al. (2012) found that the optimal reporting source for accurate diagnosis of adolescent ADHD is parent report combined with teacher report. Finally, it is not known whether study findings are generalizable to adolescents and families who have different points of entry to care and different cultural backgrounds. It would be worthwhile to replicate and extend study analyses with other clinical and community-based adolescent ADHD populations.
Implications for Treatment Policy and Practice
Study findings underscore the need for ADHD assessment and treatment services for inner-city teens identified by school-based referral sources as having unmet behavioral health problems (Atkins et al., 2006). In the study sample both caregivers and adolescents themselves expressed serious concerns about all varieties of ADHD symptoms: poor attention span, disorganization, impulsivity, and so forth. Because ADHD often co-occurs with learning disabilities (e.g., Bental & Tirosh, 2007; McGrath et al., 2011), adolescents meeting criteria for ADHD will likely require a combination of behavioral, scholastic, and perhaps pharmacological interventions to meet expectancies for school achievement. As such, school guidance personnel and allied mental health disciplines may have to significantly upgrade training efforts in ADHD assessment, referral, and intervention procedures to properly meet this need (Jitendra, DuPaul, Someki, & Tresco, 2008; Kent et al., 2011; Leslie et al., 2008; Young & Amarasinghe, 2010).
All evidence to date points to the particular importance of assessing the Inattention/Disorganization dimension—ADHD-I, often referred to as ADD—in adolescents. Indeed the salience of teacher-report data for diagnosing adolescent ADHD (Sibley et al., 2012) may stem in large part from the fact that I/D symptoms, which are more prevalent than H/I symptoms among teens, create relatively greater impairment in school settings than home settings. Because secondary education places a premium on student initiative and selforganization, teachers in these settings may be (much) better informed than parents about the debilitating effects of ADHD symptoms (Dendy, 2011). In lieu of obtaining reports directly from hard-to-reach teachers with limited knowledge of individual students, diagnosticians might seek access to available school records pertaining to the teen’s history of disciplinary problems, special education services, or counseling services. Such documents often refer directly or indirectly to ADHD symptoms and related problems and can thereby provide cross-situational support for, or competing hypotheses to, an ADHD diagnosis.
Along these same lines, in order to encourage appropriate help-seeking among families, diagnosticians should carefully assess the evidence of I/D symptoms and formulate I/D-relevant treatment recommendations for teens undergoing ADHD evaluation. Many families may be less knowledgeable about the established benefits of medication and other interventions for the I/D dimension (see Smith, Waschbusch, Willoughby, & Evans, 2000; Wilens et al., 2006), including families who previously pursued services for childhood H/I symptoms. Targeted, family-centered support of help-seeking for ADHD symptoms in teens may be especially important among ethnic minority populations, who, for a variety of reasons, generally underutilize behavioral services (Cauce et al., 2002; Eirladi et al., 2006; Hervey-Jumper et al., 2008).
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01 DA019607 and K02 DA026538). The authors would like acknowledge the dedicated work of the CASALEAP research staff: Cynthia Arnao, Molly Bobek, Daniela Caraballo, Benjamin Goldman, Diana Graizbord, Jacqueline Horan, Candace Johnson, Emily McSpadden, and Catlin Rideout.
References
- Abikoff H, Klein RG. Attention-deficit hyperactivity and conduct disorder: Comorbidity and implications for treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1992;60:881. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.60.6.881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont; 1991a. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM. Manual for the Youth Self-Report and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1991b. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM. Commentary: Definitely More Than Measurement Error: But How Should We Understand and Deal With Informant Discrepancies? Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2011;40:80–86. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2011.533416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, McConaughy SH, Howell CT. Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: Implications of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychological Bulletin. 1987;101:213. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed., text revision. Washington, DC: APA; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Angold A, Messer SC, Stangl D, Farmer EM, Costello EJ, Burns BJ. Perceived parental burden and service use for child and adolescent psychiatric disorders. American Journal of Public Health. 1998;88:75–80. doi: 10.2105/ajph.88.1.75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atkins MS, Frazier SL, Birman D, Abdul Adil J, Jackson M, Graczyk PA, Talbott E, Farmer AD, Bell CC, McKay MM. School-based mental health services for children living in high poverty urban communities. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research. 2006;33:146–159. doi: 10.1007/s10488-006-0031-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bakeman R, Gottman JM. Observing interaction: An introduction to sequential analysis. 2nd edition. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin. 1997;121:65. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barkley RA, Anastopoulos AD, Guevremont DC, Fletcher KE. Adolescents with ADHD: patterns of behavioral adjustment, academic functioning, and treatment utilization. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1991;30:752–761. doi: 10.1016/s0890-8567(10)80010-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauermeister JJ, Shrout PE, Ramírez R, Bravo M, Alegría M, Martínez-Taboas A, et al. ADHD correlates, comorbidity, and impairment in community and treated samples of children and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2007;35:883–898. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9141-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bental B, Tirosh E. The relationship between attention, executive functions and reading domain abilities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and reading disorder: a comparative study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:455–463. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01710.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Greene RW, Braaten E, Doyle AE, Faraone SV. Long-term stability of the Child Behavior Checklist in a clinical sample of youth with ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 2001;30:492–502. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3004_06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bussing R, Zima BT, Gary FA, Mason DM, Leon CE, Sinha K, Garvan CW. Social networks, caregiver strain, and utilization of mental health services among elementary school students at high risk for ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42:842–850. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046876.27264.BF. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bussing R, Zima BT, Mason DM, Porter PC, Garvan CW. Receving treatment for Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Do the perspectives of adolescents matter? Journal of Adolescent Health. 2011;49:7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.08.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carr L, Henderson J, Nigg JT. Cognitive control and attentional selection in adolescents with ADHD versus ADD. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2010;39:726–740. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.517168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cauce AM, Domenech-Rodríguez M, Paradise M, Cochran BN, Shea JM, Srebnik D, Baydar N. Cultural and contextual influences in mental health help seeking: A focus on ethnic minority youth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:44–55. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.1.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Comer JS, Kendall PC. A symptom-level examination of parent-child agreement in the diagnosis of anxious youths. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2004;43:878–886. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000125092.35109.c5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conners CK, Sitarenios G, Parker JDA, Epstein JN. The revised Conners' Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1998;26:257–268. doi: 10.1023/a:1022602400621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coolidge FL, Thede LL, Young SE. Heritability and the comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with behavioral disorders and executive function deficits: A preliminary investigation. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2000;17:273–287. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1703_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Los Reyes A, Goodman KL, Kliewer W, Reid-Quinones K. The longitudinal consistency of mother-child reporting discrepancies of parental monitoring and their ability to predict child delinquent behaviors two years later. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2010;39:1417–1430. doi: 10.1007/s10964-009-9496-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Los Reyes A, Kazdin AE. Informant discrepancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: A critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for future study. Psychological Bulletin. 2005;131:483–509. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Los Reyes A, Kazdin AE. Measuring informant discrepancies in clinical child research. Psychological Assessment. 2004;16:330–334. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.16.3.330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dendy CAZ. Teenagers with ADD and ADHD: A Guide for Parents and Professionals. 2nd Edition. Bethesda, MD: Woodbine House; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Dendy CAZ. Teaching Teens with ADD, ADHD & Executive Function Deficits: A Quick Reference Guide for Teachers and Parents. 2nd edition. Bethesda. MD: Woodbine House; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Diamond A. Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attentiondeficit/ hyperactivity disorder (with hyperactivity) Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:807. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doyle R, Mick E, Biederman J. Convergence between the Achenbach Youth Self-Report and Structured Diagnostic Interview Diagnoses in ADHD and Non-ADHD Youth. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2007;195:350–352. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000253732.79172.43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eiraldi RB, Mazzuca LB, Clarke AT, Power TJ. Service utilization among ethnic minority children with ADHD: A model of help-seeking behavior. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research. 2006;33:607–622. doi: 10.1007/s10488-006-0063-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Essau CA, Sasagawa S, Frick PJ. Callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of adolescents. Assessment. 2006;13:454–469. doi: 10.1177/1073191106287354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans SW, Serpell ZN, Schultz BK, Pastor D. Cumulative benefits of secondary school-based treatment of students with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Review. 2007;36:256–273. [Google Scholar]
- Frick PJ, Hare RD. The Antisocial Process Screening Device. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Gershon J. A meta-analytic review of gender differences in ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2002;5:143. doi: 10.1177/108705470200500302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Retzlaff PD, Espy KA. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in a clinical sample. Child Neuropsychology. 2002;8:249–257. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.4.249.13513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Test review: Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology. 2000;6:235–238. doi: 10.1076/chin.6.3.235.3152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenbaum PE, Dedrick RF, Prange ME, Friedman RM. Parent, teacher, and child ratings of problem behaviors of youngsters with serious emotional disturbances. Psychological Assessment. 1994;6:141–148. [Google Scholar]
- Hartung CM, McCarthy DM, Milich R, Martin CA. Parent–adolescent agreement on disruptive behavior symptoms: A multitrait-multimethod model. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2005;27:159–168. [Google Scholar]
- Hawley KM, Weisz JR. Child, parent, and therapist (dis)agreement on target problems in outpatient therapy: The therapist’s dilemma and its implications. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2003;71:62–70. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.71.1.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heflinger CA, Hinshaw S. Stigma in child and adolescent mental health services research: Understanding professional and institutional stigmatization of youth with mental health problems and their families. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research. 2010;37:61–70. doi: 10.1007/s10488-010-0294-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hervey-Jumper H, Douyon K, Falcone T, Franco KN. Identifying, evaluating, diagnosing, and treating ADHD in minority youth. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2008;11:522–528. doi: 10.1177/1087054707311054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinshaw SP, Owens EB, Sami N, Fargeon S. Prospective follow-up of girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into adolescence: Evidence for continuing cross-domain impairment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2006;74:489. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hogue A, Dauber S. Diagnostic profiles among urban adolescents with unmet treatment needs: Comorbidity and perceived need for treatment. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders. doi: 10.1177/1063426611407500. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes J, Gathercole SE, Alloway TP, Elliott JG, Hilton KA. The diagnostic utility of executive function assessments in the identification of ADHD in children. Child and Adolescent Mental Health. 2010;15:37–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-3588.2009.00536.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hudziak JJ, Heath AC, Madden PF, Reich W, Bucholz KK, Slutske W, Todd RD. Latent class and factor analysis of DSM-IV ADHD: a twin study of female adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1998;37:848–857. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199808000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jitendra AK, DuPaul GJ, Someki F, Tresco KE. Enhancing academic achievement for children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Evidence from school based intervention research. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2008;14:325–330. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Adler LA, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners CK, Faraone SV, et al. Patterns and predictors of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder persistence into adulthood: results from the national comorbidity survey replication. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:1442–1451. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kent KM, Pelham WE, Molina BSG, Sibley MH, Waschbusch DA, Yu J, Gnagy EM, Biswas A, Babinski DE, Karch KM. The academic experience of male high school students with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39:451–462. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9472-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimonis ER, Frick PJ, Skeems JL, Marsee MA, Cruise K, Munoz LC, Aucoin KJ, Morris AM. Assessing callous– unemotional traits in adolescent offenders: Validation of the Inventory of Callous–Unemotional Traits. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry. 2008;31:241–252. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2008.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahey BB, Willcutt EG. Predictive Validity of a Continuous Alternative to Nominal Subtypes of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder for DSM–V. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2010;39:761–775. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.517173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977;33:159–174. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lecrubier Y, Sheehan DV, Weiller E, Amorim P, Bonora I, Sheehan KH, et al. The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): A short diagnostic structured interview: Reliability and validity according to the CIDI. European Psychiatry. 1997;12:224–231. [Google Scholar]
- Leslie LK, Lambros KM, Aarons GA, Haine RA, Hough RL. Schoolbased service use by youth with ADHD in public-sector settings. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders. 2008;16:163. [Google Scholar]
- Logan DE, King CA. Parental facilitation of adolescent mental health service utilization: A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2001;8:319–333. [Google Scholar]
- Malone PS, Van Eck K, Flory K, Lamis DA. A mixture-model approach to linking ADHD to adolescent onset of illicit drug use. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46:1543–1555. doi: 10.1037/a0020549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mares D, McLuckie A, Schwartz M, Saini M. Executive function impairments in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Do they differ between school and home environments? Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;52:527–534. doi: 10.1177/070674370705200811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCandless S, O'Laughlin L. The clinical utility of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in the diagnosis of ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2007;10:381–389. doi: 10.1177/1087054706292115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McConaughy SH, Achenbach TM. Comorbidity of empirically based syndromes in matched general population and clinical samples. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1994;35:1141–1157. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01814.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGough JJ, McBurnett K, Bukstein O, Wilens TE, Greenhill L, Lerner M, et al. Once-daily OROS methylphenidate is safe and well tolerated in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology. 2006;16:351–356. doi: 10.1089/cap.2006.16.351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGrath LM, Pennington BF, Shanahan MA, Santerre Lemmon LE, Barnard HD, Willcutt EG, Olson RK. A multiple deficit model of reading disability and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: searching for shared cognitive deficits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52:547–557. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02346.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKay MM, Bannon WM. Engaging families in child mental health services. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2004;13:905–921. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2004.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLellan AT, Kushner H, Metzger D, Peters R, Grissom G, Pettinati H, Argeriou M. The fifth edition of the Addiction Severity Index. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 1992;9:199–213. doi: 10.1016/0740-5472(92)90062-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milich R, Balentine AC, Lynam DR. ADHD Combined Type and ADHD Predominantly Inattentive Type are distinct and unrelated disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2001;8:463–488. [Google Scholar]
- Monuteaux MC, Mick E, Faraone SV, Biederman J. The influence of sex on the course and psychiatric correlates of ADHD from childhood to adolescence: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51:233–241. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02152.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neff JA, Zule W. Development of a measure of treatment readiness for out of treatment drug users: Psychometric properties and construct validity. Substance Use & Misuse. 2000;35:585–599. doi: 10.3109/10826080009147473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nigg JT, Tannock R, Rohde LA. What Is to Be the Fate of ADHD Subtypes? An Introduction to the Special Section on Research on the ADHD Subtypes and Implications for the DSM–V. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2010;39:723–725. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.517171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nøvik TS, Hervas A, Ralston SJ, Dalsgaard S, Rodrigues Pereira R, Lorenzo MJ. Influence of gender on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Europe– ADORE. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;15:15–24. doi: 10.1007/s00787-006-1003-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owens EB, Hinshaw SP, Lee SS, Lahey BB. Few girls with childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder show positive adjustment during adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2009;38:132. doi: 10.1080/15374410802575313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips BM, Lonigan CJ, Driscoll K, Hooe ES. Positive and negative affectivity in children: A multitrait-multimethod investigation. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2002;31:465–479. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3104_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla L, Achenbach T, Ivanova MY, Dumenci L, Almqvist F, Bilenberg N, et al. Behavioral and emotional problems reported by parents of children ages 6 to 16 in 31 societies. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders. 2007;15:130. [Google Scholar]
- Riggs PD, Winhusen T, Davies R, Leimbeger JD, Mikulich-Gilbertson S, Klein C, Liu D. Randomized controlled trial of osmotic-release methylphenidate with cognitive-behavioral therapy in adolescents with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and substance use disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2011;50:903–914. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rohde LA, Barbosa G, Polanczyk G, Eizirik M, Rasmussen ER, Neuman RJ, Todd RD. Factor and latent class analysis of DSM-IV ADHD symptoms in a school sample of Brazilian adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40:711–718. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200106000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowland AS, Skipper B, Rabiner DL, Umbach DM, Stallone L, Campbell RA, Sandler DP. The shifting subtypes of ADHD: classification depends on how symptom reports are combined. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36:731–743. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9203-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Satterfield JH, Schell A. A prospective study of hyperactive boys with conduct problems and normal boys: adolescent and adult criminality. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36:1726–1735. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199712000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sayal K, Goodman R. Do parental reports of child hyperkinetic disorder symptoms at school predict teacher ratings? European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009;18:336–344. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0735-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serra-Pinheiro MA, Mattos P, Angélica Regalla M. Inattention, hyperactivity, and oppositional–defiant symptoms in Brazilian adolescents. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2008;12:135. doi: 10.1177/1087054708314620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Janavs J, Weiller E, Keskiner A, Dunbar GC. The validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) according to the SCID-P and its reliability. European Psychiatry. 1997;12:232–241. [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1998;59:22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shen Q, McLellan AT, Merrill JC. Client’s perceived need for treatment and its impact on outcome. Substance Abuse. 2000;21:179–192. doi: 10.1080/08897070009511431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shin SH, Brown TA. Racial and ethnic disparities in caregiver strain and the use of child mental health services: A structural equation model. Psychiatric Services. 2009;60:1036–1045. doi: 10.1176/ps.2009.60.8.1039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sibley MH, Pelham WE, Molina BSG, Gnagy EM, Waschbusch DA, Garefino AC, Karch KM. Diagnosing ADHD in adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2012;80:139–150. doi: 10.1037/a0026577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith BH, Waschbusch DA, Willoughby MT, Evans S. The efficacy, safety, and practicality of treatments for adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2000;3:243–267. doi: 10.1023/a:1026477121224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor E. Developing ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry. 2009;50:126–132. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01999.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todd RD, Huang H, Henderson CA. Poor utility of the age of onset criterion for DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Recommendations for DSM-V and ICD-11. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;49:942–949. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01892.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todd RD, Huang H, Todorov AA, Neuman RJ, Reiersen AM, Henderson CA, Reich WC. Predictors of stability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes from childhood to young adulthood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008;47:76–85. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31815a6aca. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valo S, Tannock R. Diagnostic instability of DSM-IV ADHD subtypes: Effects of informant source, instrumentation, and methods for combining symptom reports. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2010;39:749–760. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.517172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Lier PAC, Der Ende J, Koot HM, Verhulst FC. Which better predicts conduct problems? The relationship of trajectories of conduct problems with ODD and ADHD symptoms from childhood into adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:601–608. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01724.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volk HE, Todorov AA, Hay DA, Todd RD. Simple identification of complex ADHD subtypes using current symptom counts. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009;48:441–450. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819996ba. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weyandt LL, Iwaszuk W, Fulton K, Ollerton M, Beatty N, Fouts H, Greenlaw C. The Internal Restlessness Scale. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2003;36:382. doi: 10.1177/00222194030360040801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilens TE, McBurnett K, Bukstein O, McGough J, Greenhill L, Lerner M, et al. Multisite controlled study of OROS methylphenidate in the treatment of adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 2006;160:82–90. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.160.1.82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams CD, Lindsey M, Joe S. Parent-adolescent concordance on perceived need for mental health services and its impact on service use. Children and Youth Services Review. 2011;33:2253–2260. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2011.07.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeh M, McCabe K, Hough RL, Dupuis D, Hazen A. Racial/ethnic differences in parental endorsement of barriers to mental health services for youth. Mental Health Services Research. 2003;5:65–77. doi: 10.1023/a:1023286210205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeh M, McCabe K, Hough RL, Lau A, Fatme F, Garland A. Why bother with beliefs? Examining relationships between race/ethnicity, parental beliefs about causes of child problems, and mental health service use. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2005;73:800–807. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.5.800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeh M, Weisz JR. Why are we here at the clinic? Parent-child (dis)agreement on referral problems at outpatient treatment entry. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2001;69:1018–1025. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.69.6.1018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young S, Myanthi-Amarasinghe J. Practitioner Review: Non-pharmacological treatments for ADHD: A lifespan approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51:116–133. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02191.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]