Skip to main content
BMC Public Health logoLink to BMC Public Health
. 2013 May 24;13:496. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-496

Fear of crime and the environment: systematic review of UK qualitative evidence

Theo Lorenc 1,, Mark Petticrew 1, Margaret Whitehead 2, David Neary 2, Stephen Clayton 2, Kath Wright 3, Hilary Thomson 4, Steven Cummins 1,5, Amanda Sowden 3, Adrian Renton 6
PMCID: PMC3666893  PMID: 23705936

Abstract

Background

The fear of crime may have negative consequences for health and wellbeing. It is influenced by factors in the physical and social environment. This study aimed to review and synthesize qualitative evidence from the UK on fear of crime and the environment.

Methods

Eighteen databases were searched, including crime, health and social science databases. Qualitative studies conducted in the UK which presented data on fear of crime and the environment were included. Quality was assessed using Hawker et al.’s framework. Data were synthesized thematically.

Results

A total of 40 studies were included in the review. Several factors in the physical environment are perceived to impact on fear of crime, including visibility and signs of neglect. However, factors in the local social environment appear to be more important as drivers of fear of crime, including social networks and familiarity. Broader social factors appear to be of limited relevance. There is considerable evidence for limitations on physical activity as a result of fear of crime, but less for mental health impacts.

Conclusions

Fear of crime represents a complex set of responses to the environment. It may play a role in mediating environmental impacts on health and wellbeing.

Keywords: Crime/psychology, Fear, Review, Environment design, Qualitative research

Background

Most research on crime and health hitherto has focused on the direct health impacts suffered by victims of crime, particularly violent crime [1-3]. However, the indirect effects of crime and its broader harms on individuals and communities may also have important impacts on wellbeing. Fear of crime is of particular interest here, as it has been shown in several studies to have a modest, but consistently significant, association with health and wellbeing outcomes at the individual level, although there is still some controversy about the meaning of this association and the direction of causality underlying it [4-6].

A number of studies have found that fear is only weakly correlated with objective measures of crime, suggesting that fear of crime is not simply a response to high crime rates [7,8]. Fear appears to be more consistently associated with conditions in the physical environment, particularly signs of neglect such as litter and graffiti [4]; it has also been hypothesized to correlate with social factors such as social cohesion, although the findings here are more equivocal.

These findings suggest, first, that fear of crime may have an impact on health and wellbeing at a population level, independently of the direct impacts on crime on victims; and, second, that fear of crime is at least in part a response to factors in the social and physical environment. Therefore, fear of crime may be of interest to researchers in public health as a potential pathway mediating the effect of community-level environmental factors on health and wellbeing.

Qualitative research may of value in understanding the place of fear in individuals’ lives, and the determinants which shape it. From a public health perspective, qualitative research may also help to fill in the gaps in our understanding of how fear of crime influences wellbeing outcomes, and to gain a greater insight into how both relate to environmental factors. This can help to illuminate one area of the complex pathways through which environmental determinants impact on health and wellbeing outcomes.

Aim

The aim of this review was to synthesize qualitative evidence from the UK on fear of crime and the environment. The review focuses on UK evidence because qualitative research from other countries may be of limited applicability, and because a substantial body of good-quality qualitative evidence from the UK exists.

Methods

Searching

We searched the following databases. Searches were conducted between November 2010 and January 2011. All sources were searched from inception to the most current records.

•ASSIA

•CINAHL

•Conference Proceedings Citation Index

•Criminal Justice Abstracts

•Dissertation Abstracts

•EconLit

•Embase

•ERIC

•HMIC

•Inside Conferences

•Medline

•NCJRS

•PsycInfo

•Science Citation Index

•Social Policy & Practice

•Social Science Citation Index

•Sociological Abstracts

•Urban Studies Abstracts

The search strategy used took the following form:

((fear of crime) OR (crime) OR (antisocial behaviour)) AND ((built environment) OR (built environment interventions))

The full Medline search strategy can be found in web-only Additional file 1. Searches for other databases used a modified form of the Medline search strategy. No further limitations (e.g. by language or date of publication) were used in the searches.

The following additional sources were also used to locate studies:

•Google and Google Scholar (using a simplified version of the main search string and screening the first 50 hits from each);

•citation chasing from the studies included in the review;

•citation chasing from relevant systematic reviews located by the searches (i.e. which met all the inclusion criteria except that relating to study design);

•searches of websites of government bodies, research groups and other relevant organisations; and

•consultation with members of the research team and the Advisory Group.

Screening

Two reviewers coded an initial sample of records independently, with differences resolved by discussion and reference to a third reviewer where necessary. In total, 10% of the records were screened by two reviewers independently. The remaining abstracts were screened by one reviewer alone.

The following inclusion criteria were applied:

(1) Does the study report substantive data on the fear of crime?

(2) Does the study report substantive data on some aspect of the built environment?

(3) Is the study a primary qualitative study e.g. interviews, focus groups, ethnography?

(4) Was the study conducted in the UK?

The full text of all studies which met the inclusion criteria at abstract stage was retrieved and re-screened using the same criteria. Of the full text studies, 50% were screened independently by two reviewers, with differences resolved by discussion; the remainder were screened by one reviewer alone.

Data extraction and quality assessment

Data were extracted from the published study reports using a standardized form which included information on the context and setting of the study, the population, the methodology and the findings. Findings data were extracted only for direct quotes from participants cited in the study reports. Data extraction and quality assessment for all studies were carried out by a single reviewer and a sample double-checked in detail by a second reviewer.

Quality assessment for the qualitative review was carried out using Hawker et al.’s framework [9]. This tool allows for a systematic assessment by the reviewer for standard of reporting as well as appropriateness of methods. The tool includes an assessment of nine domains: abstract and title; introduction and aims; method and data; sampling; data analysis; ethics and bias; results; transferability/generalizability; implications; and usefulness. Each domain was scored from 1 (very poor) to 4 (good), giving an overall score between 9 and 36. Overall quality ratings were then assigned as follows: high quality (A), 30–36 points; medium quality (B), 24–29 points; low quality (C), 9–24 points. Studies were not excluded or given less weight in the synthesis on the basis of the quality assessment scores.

Data synthesis

The quotes reported in the identified publications were coded thematically using a broad coding framework. Although, as described above, the initial aim of the review was to focus on perceptions of the physical built environment, it became clear at an early stage in the analysis that the social environment would also need to be included to provide a coherent synthesis. Thus, the framework included the following categories: determinants of fear in the physical environment; determinants of fear in the social environment; and consequences of fear. A grounded theory approach was used to allow for the emergence of new themes or codes within the initial coding framework.

Results

The flow of literature is presented in Figure 1. Forty studies were included in the review. Table 1 shows the studies, where they were conducted, the primary research question or focus, the data collection methods used, the population and the quality rating assigned.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Flow of literature through the review.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the studies (N=40)

First author and reference Location Data collection methods* Research question or focus Population included QA
Airey [10,11]
Edinburgh
Individual interviews
Effects of place on wellbeing, esp. physical incivilities
Women aged 45-59
A
Alexander [12,13]
Newcastle-upon-Tyne
Focus groups and participatory methods
Effects of FoC on social inclusion and citizenship
Young people aged 16-25
C
Bannister [14]
Glasgow
Focus groups
Relations between physical environment and FoC
General population
C
Burgess [15-17]
Hertfordshire; nr Nottingham
Focus groups and participant observation
Perceptions of woodland and associated FoC
General population, esp. women
B
Cozens [18]
S Wales
Questionnaires, focus groups, virtual reality ‘walk-through’
Perceptions of safety in railway stations
General population
C
Crime Concern [19]
NR
Focus groups
Perceptions of safety in pedestrian journeys
General population
C
Crime Concern [20]
Various (England & Wales)
Focus groups, escorted journeys
Perceptions of safety and FoC on public transport
General population
C
Davis [21]
Birmingham
Questionnaires, focus groups
Perceptions of risk, esp. relating to transport
Children and young people aged 9-14
C
Day [22]
Glasgow and environs
Individual interviews, focus groups, observation
Effects of physical environment on wellbeing
Older people aged >60
A
Dixey [23]
Leeds
Individual interviews
Parents’ perceptions of child safety
Mothers of primary-school-aged children
B
Farrall [24-27]
London; Glasgow
Individual interviews
Perceptions of crime and the environment
General population
B
Girling [28-30]
Macclesfield; Prestbury (Cheshire)
Individual interviews, focus groups, observation
Perceptions of crime
General population
B
Goodey [31,32]
N England
Questionnaires, focus groups
Gender differences in FoC
Young people aged 11-16
C
Hollway [33,34]
NR
Individual interviews
Experiences of FoC
General population
C
Hopkins [35]
Glasgow
Individual interviews, focus groups
Experiences of FoC
Young Muslim men aged 16-25
C
Innes [36]
Blackpool; Oldham; London
Individual interviews
Perceptions of crime, anti-social behaviour and physical incivilities
General population
C
Jones [37]
NR
Focus groups
Perceptions of risk and constraints on behaviour; ethnic differences
Young women aged 11–14, most Asian
B
Koskela [38-40]
Edinburgh
Individual interviews
Relation between FoC and built environment
Women
B
Little [41]
Devon
Individual interviews
FoC in rural areas
Women
B
Mitchell [42]
NE England
Individual interviews
Mothers’ perceptions of risk for children
Young mothers aged 15-24
C
Moran [43-45]
Manchester; Lancaster
Individual interviews, focus groups
Fear of violence and its relation to spatiality
Lesbians and gay men
C
Nayak [46]
NE England
Questionnaires
Experiences of FoC
Young people aged 12-15
C
Nelson [47]
Cardiff; Gloucester; Worcester
Individual interviews
Perceptions of security shutters
General population
C
Pain [48,49]
Newcastle-upon-Tyne and environs
Individual and couple interviews
Perceptions of crime
Older people
C
Pain [50]
Newcastle-upon-Tyne
Focus groups
Perceptions of safety
General population
B
Pain [51,52]
Gateshead
Focus groups, questionnaires, participatory methods
Perceptions of risk and leisure time; role of mobile phones
Young people aged 10-16
C
Pain [53]
Northumberland
Focus groups, observation
Perceptions of street lighting and FoC
General population
B
Parry [54]
W Midlands
Focus groups
Effects of community factors on health
Young people aged 16–20 and older people aged >60
B
Seabrook [55]
N England
Pair interviews, participatory methods
Perceptions of risk, place and leisure time
Girls and young women aged 10–17
C
Squires [56]
Brighton
Individual interviews
Evaluation of CCTV system
General population
C
Taylor [57]
Manchester; Sheffield
Focus groups
Wellbeing and social change
General population
C
Trayers [58]
SW England
Focus groups
Views on planned neighbourhood renewal intervention
General population
A
Turner [59]
Glasgow and environs
Individual interviews, focus groups
Perceptions of risk and safety
Children and young people aged 8–14
A
Valentine [60-63]
Reading
Individual interviews, focus groups
Fear of male violence and perceptions of public space
Women
A
Valentine [64]
Peak District
Individual and couple interviews
Parents’ views of children’s safety in rural area
Parents of 8-11-year-old children
C
Walklate [65,66]
Salford
Individual interviews, focus groups, observation, content analysis
Perceptions of risk, FoC and community
General population
C
Waters [67]
Glamorgan; Loughborough
Questionnaires, focus groups, virtual reality ‘walk-throughs’
Perceptions of safety on university campuses
University staff and students
A
Waters [68,69]
S Wales
Focus groups, virtual reality ‘walk-throughs’
Perceptions of crime, FoC and community
Older people aged >65
A
Watson [70]
Leeds
Individual interviews, observation
Experiences of risk w/r/t leisure time
Young mothers
C
Whitley [71,72] London Individual interviews, focus groups, observation Impact of FoC on mental health General population; people with mental health problems A

*For mixed-methods studies, this column refers to the qualitative component only. Abbreviations: CCTV = closed-circuit television; FoC = fear of crime.

Determinants of fear in the physical environment

Several factors in the physical environment are described by participants as relevant to the fear of crime. Some participants see physical security measures, such as locks, fencing or secure entry systems, as reducing fear [27,71]. However, measures in public space such as shutters and security gates are often seen as increasing fear, and as creating an unpleasant and hostile atmosphere more generally [28,47,57,67]. Excessive security measures in the home are also seen as unwelcoming and depressing, with several participants using the metaphor of a fortress or prison [26,28,39,67,71]. In particular, several participants express a sense of anger at the need for such security measures [24,26,39].

Street lighting is also frequently discussed in the studies. Many participants report feeling more fearful in poorly lit locations [19,20,28,38,40,50,53,60],[67] and at night [27,28,36,41,47,57]. Lighting appears to be relevant to fear in two ways. First, it increases visibility and is thought to reduce potential hiding places for attackers [38,60,67]. Second, it gives a more pleasant and welcoming impression of the environment, partly by acting as an indicator of the presence of other people [20,67]. In some cases, participants report that the effects of lighting are outweighed by other factors which impact on fear: “I mean when I was a child we lived in the country and it was all dark lanes with no lights, but we never felt afraid” [60]. Participants in two studies express scepticism about the effectiveness of lighting as a fear reduction strategy [38,53]. Finally, specific aspects of lighting such as colour and brightness may also make be relevant to fear [19,50,53,67].

Lighting also relates to the sense that the environment allows visibility (what criminologists call ‘natural surveillance’). Places which are not visible because they are isolated [18,20,38], or sight-lines which are obstructed by vegetation, landscaping or poorly designed buildings are perceived to increase the risk of attack, and hence fear [19,20,38,67]. Such obstructions to visibility also create a feeling of being ‘trapped’; by contrast, a sense of ‘openness’ in the environment is reassuring [15,67].

Closed-circuit television (CCTV) is relatively rarely mentioned in the studies. A few participants express support for CCTV in general terms [18,20,28,50], but few say that it reduces fear, and several are sceptical about its effectiveness in reducing crime [20,47,50,56]. Several participants in Squires’ evaluation of CCTV are strongly critical of it, seeing it as an inadequate substitute for more substantive measures to reduce crime [56].

Dirt, decay, graffiti, litter and other signs of neglect of the environment (what criminologists call ‘physical incivilities’) are widely seen as drivers of fear [18-20,24,31,36,41,54,60,67],[69], for several reasons. Problems in the physical environment are seen to indicate a lack of commitment to social norms [24,67,69]. Problems such as graffiti or litter are associated with environmental indicators of socio-economic disadvantage (such as high-rise housing) as part of a more general sense of ‘rough’ areas [24,60,67]. More broadly, a pleasant physical environment is thought to contribute to an overall sense of wellbeing, and thus safety [24,54,71].

Finally, places where few other people are around, either because of the time of day or because of patterns of land use, are widely experienced as fear-inducing [15,19,20,38,40,50,67].

Determinants of fear in the social environment

An important factor relevant to fear is the extent to which one is familiar with an area. Many participants report feeling less fearful in their own area, or areas they know well, than elsewhere. “How true it is that one often feels safer in your local area… I just feel safer because it’s my local area and I know what happens there and I feel more confident” [19]. Participants describe factors which may increase fear for outsiders but are not seen as threatening by insiders. “I think it’s all right round here, I mean you see gangs of kids but they’re only young and it doesn’t bother me because it’s familiar, I mean I’ve always lived round here” [70]. In Bannister’s study, participants were asked to mark areas seen as unsafe on a map; most saw their own areas as safe and areas not known well as unsafe, such that a large proportion of residential areas were seen as unsafe by at least one participant, but none by all the participants [14].

Much of the protective effect of familiarity has to do with having strong social networks locally. Many participants report that they do not feel fearful in their own area because they know many people and are long-term residents in the area [26,27,36,48,60]. “Everyone knows everyone, so you’re not a stranger in your own town. And you just feel so safe, just in your own street and your own area” [36]. Conversely, strangers who come to the area from elsewhere are often the object of fear [27,28,60,64].

Young people, especially when ‘hanging about’ in groups, are often perceived as threatening [19,20,28,41,46,56,64], as are people using alcohol or drugs [19,20,22,24,36,50,60,69], especially large groups at pub closing time [28,45,50,60].

There is some evidence suggesting differences in fear between women and men. Women tend to express greater fear, and the focus of their fear is virtually always men [19,60]. To some extent this reflects differences in the crimes feared, with rape or sexual assault being the focus of fear for women [31,40,60], and perceived differences in physical vulnerability [48,60]. It may also be affected by ‘vicarious’ fear expressed by husbands or boyfriends or parents, which may lead to restrictions on women’s activities [21,27,31,37,48,60].

However, it also seems to reflect women’s experience of everyday harassment and relatively minor crimes such as indecent exposure [15,60]. Women’s fear appears to be more pervasive and inescapable than men’s, with several women participants expressing doubt that fear can ever be meaningfully addressed [40,60]. “You’re never safe at any time. If somebody wants to go out and attack a woman, they’ll do it” [40]. These points have parallels in the experience of ethnic minority participants [35] and lesbian and gay participants [43,45], several of whom also express a pervasive fear which is partly driven by the everyday experience of discrimination and harassment.

Consequences of fear

Relatively few participants see fear as having serious mental health impacts, although several report some degree of psychological stress as a result of fear [10,57,60]. Those who do report serious mental health consequences tend to be victims of serious violent crimes, particularly sexual or hate crimes [19,56,60], or people with pre-existing mental health problems [71]. Some participants also see fear as impacting on health as part of a broader nexus of disadvantage [10].

A much more widely perceived consequence of fear is to limit people’s activities, including social and cultural activities, sometimes leading to social isolation [12,20,50,54,55,57,71]. Participants from across the population report such limitations, but they appear to be more serious for women, older people and people with disabilities. Of particular concern from a health perspective are limitations on outdoor physical activity, especially walking and cycling, which are reported by several female participants [15,39,40,60]. Parents also report placing serious restrictions on children’s activities [21,23,37,39,60,64], even though both parents [23,64] and children [21,37] recognize the problematic effects of such restrictions on children’s independent mobility. Again, parental restrictions are often more serious for girls and young women [21,31,37,60].

More broadly, fear of crime is seen to contribute to the process by which disadvantaged areas gain a reputation as dangerous or ‘rough’, which can contribute to the social stigmatization of residents of those areas [10,12,24,56,73].

Discussion and conclusion

This is the first review to draw together the large body of UK qualitative evidence on fear of crime and the environment. Although this review is exploratory in nature and does not support strong conclusions, it helps to fill out the available theories and quantitative data which suggest that fear of crime is associated with poorer health outcomes, and that it may mediate determinants of health and wellbeing in the physical and social environment [4]. The findings of this review suggest some plausible pathways through which a number of factors in the physical and social environment may have an impact on fear, and in turn may influence wellbeing, particularly through restrictions on activities. Moreover, the findings suggest that fear of crime may play a role in generating health inequalities, since certain groups appear to be more seriously affected by fear; gender is the most obviously relevant dimension here, although age, ethnicity, sexuality and disability may all also play a role.

The relations between environmental factors and fear are complex. Aspects of the physical built environment are clearly relevant to fear to some extent, but fear often relates more directly to the environment’s social meanings than to its physical form. For example, familiarity with and social inclusion in a given context may largely nullify the potentially fear-inducing physical features of that context. Conversely, physical factors such as litter and graffiti increase fear mainly because they are taken to indicate low social cohesion and/or socio-economic disadvantage. (This applies particularly to residential areas; in public areas, such as shopping streets, parks, or public transport, the role of the physical environment may be greater). Nonetheless, it appears that most of the social factors which are relevant to fear of crime are spatially localised. This social mediation of physical cues also means that different people, or population subgroups, may come to different conclusions about the same physical environmental factors.

The social drivers of fear are complex and often contested. Several themes in the data, such as the fear of young people ‘hanging about', appear to represent a conflict between different group norms about the use of public space. Moreover, the findings on inequalities, particularly by gender, lend some support to theories of ‘spirit injury’ which posit an important role for systemic discrimination in the genesis of fear of crime [74,75]. Such theories suggest that the latent structural violence involved in maintaining social inequalities may be as important as the manifest violence measured in crime statistics in understanding fear and its impacts on wellbeing.

Although this review was carried out according to full systematic review methodology, it has some limitations. Only a thematic analysis, focusing on directly reported primary data, was carried out. The search terms and inclusion criteria, which focused on fear of crime and the physical built environment, may have excluded a number of relevant studies (e.g. studies which focused on the social environment alone). The primary studies are heterogeneous in many respects, and generalizations across them may have limited validity.

These limitations aside, this review suggests that fear of crime may have some role to play in mediating the impact of physical environmental factors on wellbeing, particularly by acting as a barrier to outdoor physical activity. However, the ways in which the environment influences fear appear to be be complex. The findings suggest that physical environmental change alone, and interventions which focus narrowly on crime reduction, are likely to have limited success in addressing fear and its effects on wellbeing. Approaches which engage with the broader social contexts of fear of crime – including socio-economic disadvantage and its symbolic meanings, and inequalities with respect to gender and ethnicity – appear to be more promising.

Competing interests

No authors have any competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MP and MW conceived the study, with input from HT, SCu, AS and AR. KW ran the literature searches. Screening and data extraction were carried out by TL, SCl and DN. Data analysis was carried out by TL. All authors contributed to the interpretation of the findings and approved the final report.

Pre-publication history

The pre-publication history for this paper can be accessed here:

http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2458/13/496/prepub

Supplementary Material

Additional file 1

MEDLINE search strategy.

Click here for file (177.6KB, pdf)

Contributor Information

Theo Lorenc, Email: theo.lorenc@lshtm.ac.uk.

Mark Petticrew, Email: mark.petticrew@lshtm.ac.uk.

Margaret Whitehead, Email: mmw@liverpool.ac.uk.

David Neary, Email: david.neary@liverpool.ac.uk.

Stephen Clayton, Email: stephen.clayton@liverpool.ac.uk.

Kath Wright, Email: kath.wright@york.ac.uk.

Hilary Thomson, Email: hilary@spshu.mrc.ac.uk.

Steven Cummins, Email: steven.cummins@lshtm.ac.uk.

Amanda Sowden, Email: ajs18@york.ac.uk.

Adrian Renton, Email: a.renton@uel.ac.uk.

Acknowledgements

This project was funded by the National Institute of Health Research (grant number 09/3000/14). The views expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect those of the NIHR or the Department of Health. We would like to thank the members of the project Advisory Group (Allan Brimicombe, John Middleton, David Nossiter, James Thomas, Rachel Tuffin and Sandra Walklate).

References

  1. McCabe A, Wine J. Framing the Debate: The impact of crime on public health. Birmingham: Public Health Alliance; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  2. McManus J, Mullett D. Better Health, Lower Crime: A briefing for the NHS and partner agencies. London: Nacro Crime and Social Policy Section; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  3. Robinson F, Keithley J. The impacts of crime on health and health services: A literature review. Health, Risk and Society. 2000;2:253–66. [Google Scholar]
  4. Lorenc T, Clayton S, Neary D, Whitehead M, Petticrew M, Thomson H, Cummins S, Sowden A, Renton A. Crime, fear of crime, environment, and mental health and wellbeing: Mapping review of theories and causal pathways. Health Place. 2012;18:757–765. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2012.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Jackson J, Stafford M. Public health and fear of crime: A prospective cohort study. Br J Criminol. 2009;49:832–47. [Google Scholar]
  6. Stafford M, Chandola T, Marmot M. Association between fear of crime and mental health and physical functioning. Am J Public Health. 2007;97:2076–81. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.097154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Farrall S, Gray E, Jackson J. Theorising the Fear of Crime: The Cultural and Social Significance of Insecurities about Crime. : ; Experience and Expression in the Fear of Crime Working Paper No. 5. Available from: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=1012393 (accessed 15 October 2012) [Google Scholar]
  8. Hale C. Fear of crime: A review of the literature. International Review of Victimology. 1996;4:79–150. [Google Scholar]
  9. Hawker S, Payne S, Kerr C, Hardey M, Powell J. Appraising the evidence: Reviewing disparate data systematically. Qual Heal Res. 2002;12:1284–1299. doi: 10.1177/1049732302238251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Airey L. “Nae as nice a scheme as it used to be”: lay accounts of neighbourhood incivilities and well-being. Health Place. 2003;9:129–37. doi: 10.1016/s1353-8292(03)00013-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Airey L. Locating health and illness: A study of women’s experiences in two contrasting Edinburgh neighbourhoods. Edinburgh, UK: University of Edinburgh; 2002. (PhD thesis). [Google Scholar]
  12. Alexander CL. In: Fear: Critical geopolitics and everyday life. Pain R, Smith SJ, editor. Ashgate: Aldershot; 2008. “Growing pains”? Fear, exclusion and citizenship in a disadvantaged UK neighbourhood; pp. 37–48. [Google Scholar]
  13. Alexander CL. Safety, fear and belonging: The everyday realities of civic identity formation in Fenham, Newcastle upon Tyne. ACME: An International E-Journal for Critical Geographies. 2008;7:173–198. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bannister J. In: Crime and the Urban Environment. Jones HR, editor. Ashgate: Aldershot; 1993. Locating fear: Environment and ontological insecurity; pp. 69–83. [Google Scholar]
  15. Burgess J. In: New Frontiers of Space, Bodies and Gender. Ainley R, editor. Routledge: London; 1998. But is it worth taking the risk? How women negotiate access to urban woodland: a case study; pp. 115–128. [Google Scholar]
  16. Burgess J. Focusing on fear: the use of focus groups in a project for the Community Forest Unit, Countryside Commission. Area. 1996;28:130–135. [Google Scholar]
  17. Burgess J. Growing in confidence: Understanding people’s perceptions of urban fringe woodlands. Cheltenham: Countryside Commission; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  18. Cozens P, Neale R, Whitaker J, Hillier D. Managing crime and the fear of crime at railway stations: A case study in South Wales (UK) Int J Transp Manag. 2003;1:121–132. [Google Scholar]
  19. Crime Concern, Social Research Associates. Personal Security Issues in Pedestrian Journeys. London: Department of Environment, Transport and the Regions; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  20. Crime Concern. People’s Perceptions of Personal Security and their Concerns about Crime on Public Transport: Research findings. London: Department for Transport; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  21. Davis A, Jones L. Environmental constraints on health: Listening to children’s views. Heal Educ J. 1996;55:363–374. [Google Scholar]
  22. Day R. Local Urban Environments and the Wellbeing of Older People. Glasgow: Scottish Centre for Research on Social Justice; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  23. Dixey R. Keeping children safe: The effect on parents’ daily lives and psychological well-being. J Heal Psychol. 1999;4:45–57. doi: 10.1177/135910539900400105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Farrall S, Jackson J, Gray E. Social Order and the Fear of Crime in Contemporary Times. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  25. Farrall S, Jackson J, Gray E. Everyday Emotion and the Fear of Crime: Preliminary findings from Experience and Expression. 2006. (Experience and Expression in the Fear of Crime Working Paper No. 1). Available from: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1012354 (accessed 15 October 2012)
  26. Gray E, Farrall S, Jackson J. Experience and expression: Conversations about crime, place and community. 2007. (Experience and Expression in the Fear of Crime Working Paper No. 6). Available from: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1012396 (accessed 15 October 2012)
  27. Gilchrist E, Bannister J, Ditton J, Farrall S. Women and the “fear of crime”: Challenging the accepted stereotype. Br J Criminol. 1998;38:283–298. [Google Scholar]
  28. Girling E, Loader I, Sparks R. Crime and Social Change in Middle England: Questions of order in an English town. London: Routledge; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  29. Loader I, Girling E, Sparks R. In: Crime, Risk and Insecurity. Hope T, Sparks R, editor. Abingdon: Routledge; 2000. After success? Anxieties of influence in an English village; pp. 65–82. [Google Scholar]
  30. Sparks R, Girling E, Loader I. Fear and everyday urban lives. Urban Studies. 2001;38:885–898. [Google Scholar]
  31. Goodey J. Boys don’t cry: Masculinities, fear of crime and fearlessness. Br J Criminol. 1997;37:401–418. [Google Scholar]
  32. Goodey J. In: Race, Class and Gender in Criminology. Schwarz M, Milovanovic D, editor. London: Garland; 1996. Adolescence and the socialization of gendered fear; pp. 267–291. [Google Scholar]
  33. Hollway W, Jefferson T. In: The Turn to Biographical Methods in Social Science. Chamberlayne P, Bornat J, Wengraf T, editor. London: Routledge; 2000. Anxiety, biography and the experience of locality; pp. 167–180. [Google Scholar]
  34. Hollway W, Jefferson T. In: Crime, Risk and Insecurity. Hope T, Sparks R, editor. London: Routledge; 2000. The role of anxiety in the fear of crime; pp. 31–49. [Google Scholar]
  35. Hopkins PE, Smith SJ. In: Fear: Critical geopolitics and everyday life. Pain R, Smith SJ, editor. Ashgate: Aldershot; 2008. Scaling segregation; racialising fear; pp. 103–116. [Google Scholar]
  36. Innes M, Jones V. Neighbourhood Security and Urban Change: Risk, resilience and recovery. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  37. Jones L. Inequality in access to local environments: the experiences of Asian and non-Asian girls. Heal Educ J. 1998;57:313–328. [Google Scholar]
  38. Koskela H, Pain R. Revisiting fear and place: Women’s fear of attack and the built environment. Geoforum. 2000;31:269–280. [Google Scholar]
  39. Pain R. Social geographies of women’s fear of crime. Trans Inst Br Geogr. 1997;22:231–44. [Google Scholar]
  40. Pain R. In: Crime and the Urban Environment. Jones H, editor. Aldersho: Avebury; 1993. Women’s fear of sexual violence: Explaining the spatial paradox; pp. 46–55. [Google Scholar]
  41. Little J, Panelli R, Kraack A. Women’s fear of crime: A rural perspective. J Rural Stud. 2005;21:151–163. [Google Scholar]
  42. Mitchell W. In: Young People, Risk and Leisure: Constructing identities in everyday life. Mitchell W, Bunton R, Green E, editor. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan; 2004. Risk, motherhood and children’s play spaces: the importance of young mothers’ experiences and risk management strategies; pp. 180–202. [Google Scholar]
  43. Moran LJ, Skeggs B, Tyrer P, Corteen K. The formation of fear in gay space: The “straights” story. Capital & Class. 2003;27:173–198. [Google Scholar]
  44. Moran LJ, Skeggs B. Sexuality and the Politics of Violence and Safety. London; New York: Routledge; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  45. Corteen K. Lesbian safety talk: Problematizing definitions and experiences of violence, sexuality and space. Sexualities. 2002;5:259–280. [Google Scholar]
  46. Nayak A. “Through children’s eyes”: Childhood, place and the fear of crime. Geoforum. 2003;34:303–315. [Google Scholar]
  47. Nelson AL. Security shutters: A double-edged sword. International Journal of Risk Security and Crime Prevention. 1998;3:11–19. [Google Scholar]
  48. Pain R. “Old age” and ageism in urban research: The case of fear of crime. International Journal of Urban and Regional Research. 1997;21:117–128. [Google Scholar]
  49. Pain R. Local contexts and the fear of crime: Elderly people in north-east England. Northern Economic Review. 1995;24:96–111. [Google Scholar]
  50. Pain R, Townshend T. A safer city centre for all? Senses of “community safety” in Newcastle upon Tyne. Geoforum. 2002;33:105–119. [Google Scholar]
  51. Pain R, Grundy S, Gill S, Towner E, Sparks G, Hughes K. “So long as I take my mobile”: Mobile phones, urban life and geographies of young people’s safety. International Journal of Urban and Regional Research. 2005;29:814–830. [Google Scholar]
  52. Pain R. Youth, age and the representation of fear. Capital & Class. 2003;27:151–171. [Google Scholar]
  53. Pain R, MacFarlane R, Turner K, Gill S. “When, where, if, and but”: Qualifying GIS and the effect of streetlighting on crime and fear. Environment and Planning A. 2006;38:2055–2074. [Google Scholar]
  54. Parry J, Mathers J, Laburn-Peart C, Orford J, Dalton S. Improving health in deprived communities: What can residents teach us? Critical Public Health. 2007;17:123–136. [Google Scholar]
  55. Seabrook T, Green E. In: Young People, Risk and Leisure: Constructing identities in everyday life. Mitchell WA, Bunton R, Green EE, editor. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan; 2004. Streetwise or safe? Girls negotiating time and space; pp. 129–141. [Google Scholar]
  56. Squires P. Independent Evaluation of the Installation of CCTV Cameras for Crime Prevention in the Whitehawk Estate, Brighton. Brighton: University of Brighton; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  57. Taylor I, Evans K, Fraser P. A Tale of Two Cities: Global change, local feeling and everyday life in the North of England. London: Routledge; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  58. Trayers T, Deem R, Fox KR, Riddoch CJ, Ness AR, Lawlor DA. Improving health through neighbourhood environmental change: Are we speaking the same language? A qualitative study of views of different stakeholders. J Public Health. 2006;28:49–55. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdi075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Turner KM, Hill M, Stafford A, Walker M. How children from disadvantaged areas keep safe. Heal Educ. 2006;106:450–464. [Google Scholar]
  60. Valentine G. Women’s fear of male violence in public space: A spatial expression of patriarchy. Reading, UK: University of Reading; 1989. (PhD thesis). [Google Scholar]
  61. Valentine G. The geography of women’s fear. Area. 1989;21:385–390. [Google Scholar]
  62. Valentine G. Women’s fear and the design of public space. Built Environment. 1990;16:288–303. [Google Scholar]
  63. Valentine G. Images of danger: Women’s sources of information about the spatial distribution of male violence. Area. 1992;24:22–29. [Google Scholar]
  64. Valentine G. A safe place to grow up? Parenting, perceptions of children’s safety and the rural idyll. J Rural Stud. 1997;13:137–148. [Google Scholar]
  65. Walklate S. Crime and community: Fear or trust? Br J Sociol. 1998;49:550–569. [Google Scholar]
  66. Evans K, Fraser P, Walklate S. Whom can you trust? The politics of “grassing” on an inner city housing estate. Sociol Rev. 1996;44:361–380. [Google Scholar]
  67. Waters J. Perceptions of Personal Safety on University Campuses. Glamorgan, UK: University of Glamorgan; 2006. (PhD thesis). [Google Scholar]
  68. Waters J, Neale R. Older people’s perceptions of personal safety in deprived communities: understanding the social causes of fear of crime. Quality in Ageing and Older Adults. 2010;11:48–56. [Google Scholar]
  69. Waters J, Neale R, Mears K. Design and Community Regeneration: investigating personal safety concerns of older people in socio-economically deprived communities in South Wales. Pontypridd: University of Glamorgan; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  70. Watson R. In: Young People, Risk and Leisure: Constructing identities in everyday life. Mitchell W, Bunton R, Green EE, editor. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan; 2004. Risking it? Young mothers’ experiences of motherhood and leisure; pp. 203–223. [Google Scholar]
  71. Whitley R, Prince M. Fear of crime, mobility and mental health in inner-city London, UK. Soc Sci Med. 2005;61:1678–88. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.03.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Whitley R, Prince M. Are inner-cities bad for your health? Comparisons of residents’ and third parties’ perceptions of the urban neighbourhood of Gospel Oak, London. Sociol Health Illn. 2005;27:44–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9566.2005.00431.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Walklate S, Evans K. Zero Tolerance or Community Tolerance? Managing crime in high crime areas. Aldershot: Ashgate; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  74. Walklate S. Imagining the Victim of Crime. Maidenhead: Open University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  75. Spalek B. Communities, Identities and Crime. Bristol: Policy; 2008. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Additional file 1

MEDLINE search strategy.

Click here for file (177.6KB, pdf)

Articles from BMC Public Health are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES