Skip to main content
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience
. 2013 May 30;7:56. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00056

Spatial learning of female mice: a role of the mineralocorticoid receptor during stress and the estrous cycle

Judith P ter Horst 1, Jiska Kentrop 1, Marit Arp 1, Chantal J Hubens 1, E Ron de Kloet 1,*, Melly S Oitzl 1
PMCID: PMC3667238  PMID: 23754993

Abstract

Corticosterone facilitates behavioral adaptation to a novel experience in a coordinate manner via mineralocorticoid (MR) and glucocorticoid receptors (GR). Initially, MR mediates corticosterone action on appraisal processes, risk assessment and behavioral flexibility and then, GR activation promotes consolidation of the new information into memory. Here, we studied on the circular holeboard (CHB) the spatial performance of female mice with genetic deletion of MR from the forebrain (MRCaMKCre) and their wild type littermates (MRflox/flox mice) over the estrous cycle and in response to an acute stressor. The estrous cycle had no effect on the spatial performance of MRflox/flox mice and neither did the acute stressor. However, the MRCaMKCre mutants needed significantly more time to find the exit and made more hole visit errors than the MRflox/flox mice, especially when in proestrus and estrus. In addition, stressed MRCaMKCre mice in estrus had a shorter exit latency than the control estrus MRCaMKCre mice. About 70% of the female MRCaMKCre and MRflox/flox mice used a hippocampal (spatial, extra maze cues) rather than the caudate nucleus (stimulate-response, S-R, intra-maze cue) strategy and this preference did neither change over the estrous cycle nor after stress. However, stressed MRCaMKCre mice using the S-R strategy needed significantly more time to find the exit hole as compared to the spatial strategy using mice suggesting that the MR could be needed for the stress-induced strategy switch toward a spatial strategy. In conclusion, the results suggest that loss of MR interferes with performance of a spatial task especially when estrogen levels are high suggesting a strong interaction between stress and sex hormones.

Keywords: MR mutant mice, spatial performance, estrous cycle, stress, strategy

Introduction

Corticosterone modulates learning and memory processes. This action exerted by the hormone is mediated by high affinity mineralocorticoid receptors (MR) and low affinity glucocorticoid receptors (GR) that operate in complementary fashion (Reul and De Kloet, 1985; De Kloet et al., 1998, 2005). The MR is involved in the initial phase of memory formation by appraisal of information, response selection and behavioral flexibility (Oitzl and De Kloet, 1992; Lupien and McEwen, 1997; Berger et al., 2006). Via GR memory consolidation is affected (Oitzl and De Kloet, 1992; Lupien and McEwen, 1997; Oitzl et al., 2010).

Stress is known to affect the performance of both sexes differently in spatial learning tests. In males stress impairs spatial performance (Schwabe et al., 2010a,b) but does not seem to affect or improves spatial learning in females (Bowman et al., 2001; Conrad et al., 2004; Kitraki et al., 2004; ter Horst et al., 2013). As said before the MR is involved in the initial phase of memory formation. Male mice carrying a genetic deletion of MR in the forebrain, the MRCaMKCre mice (Berger et al., 2006), needed more time to learn the spatial circular hole board (CHB) task than their control littermates (MRflox/flox mice) (ter Horst et al., 2012c). During stress the latency to find the exit hole decreased in the MRflox/flox mice, whereas the mutants were not further affected in their performance. A recent study revealed that chronic stress not only affects cognitive performance, but also can change the strategy to learn a task by switching between memory systems by the use of specific brain circuits (Dias-Ferreira et al., 2009). Other studies showed that the switch between memory systems occurs also after exposure to acute stressors. To demonstrate this the CHB can be solved by either using a hippocampus-based “cognitive” spatial strategy or a caudate nucleus-based “habit,” also indicated as a stimulus-response (S-R) strategy (Schwabe et al., 2010a; ter Horst et al., 2012c). After an acute stressor or corticosterone administration, the behavioral strategy can change from spatial to S-R in male rodents (rats: Kim and Baxter, 2001; mice: Schwabe et al., 2008, 2010a; ter Horst et al., 2012c). However, there are profound sex differences. Female mice use both strategies and in response to stress they switch to a spatial strategy, especially when in the estrus phase (ter Horst et al., 2013). The MR is shown to be involved in the choice of the learning strategy (Schwabe et al., 2010a), since the administration of a mineralocorticoid antagonist could prevent in males the stress-induced switch of spatial to habit learning. When stressed male MRCaMKCre mice used the S-R strategy their performance was actually improved as compared to the mutants using the spatial strategy, therefore preventing further deterioration of performance (ter Horst et al., 2012c).

These findings raised the question how genetic MR deletion in the forebrain would affect performance of the female mouse in the CHB over the various stages of the estrous cycle with and without prior exposure to an acute stressor. For instance, during a fear conditioning task female MRCaMKCre mice were unable to extinguish fear memory whereas male mice could (Brinks et al., 2009; ter Horst et al., 2012a), suggesting that loss of MR in the forebrain enhanced sex differences in cognitive and emotional behaviors. To address this question female MRCaMKCre and MRflox/flox mice were subjected to 10 min acute restraint stress 30 min before the training on the CHB started. We report here that, while in the proestrus and estrus stage of the cycle the CHB performance of the control female MRCaMKCre mice was impaired, the exposure to the acute stressor did improve spatial performance, but only of the estrus MRCaMKCre mice.

Materials and methods

Animals

Female MR forebrain deficient mice (MRCaMKCre; 5–6 months) and female MR forebrain intact littermates (MRflox/flox) were bred in the animal facility of Leiden University. The MRCaMKCre mice and their littermates were obtained by breeding MRflox/flox with MRflox/wtCaMKCre mice. A modified MR allele (MRflox) was generated in embryonic stem cells of 129Ola mice and the CaMKCre transgene was injected in FVB/N mice (Casanova et al., 2001). The MRflox allele and CaMKCre transgene were backcrossed into C57BL/6J over multiple generations. MRCaMKCre mice have a MR deficiency in the forebrain from postnatal day 12 onwards. For detailed description of the design and breeding of MRCaMKCre mice see (Berger et al., 2006). For this study we have four groups: control MRCaMKCre (n = 30), control MRflox/flox (n = 25), stress MRCaMKCre (n = 18) and stress MRflox/flox (n = 21) mice. One week before the behavioral experiment the mice were moved to the experimental room and housed individually in Macrolon cages (translucent plastic: 44 × 22 × 17 cm) with sawdust bedding, a tissue for nest building, tap water and food ad-libitum, at 20°C with controlled humidity humidity under a 12 h: 12 h light/dark cycle, lights on at 7.30 h. Experiments were performed between 08.30 and 12.30 h (during the non-active phase of the mice) and were approved by the committee on Animal Health and Care from Leiden University, The Netherlands, in accordance with the EC Council Directive of November 1986 (2010/63/EU).

Restraint stress

Thirty minutes before the first training trial mice were immobilized for 10 min in a narrow cylinder (transparent Plexiglas; diameter 2.5 cm, 8 cm long) that still allowed breathing but no further movement. Immobilization was performed in a room adjacent to the experimental room. Thereafter, mice returned to their home cage in the experimental room for 20 min.

Experimental design

Apparatus

The CHB is a revolvable round board (gray Plexiglas, 110 cm in diameter, situated 1 m above the floor) with 12 holes at equal distances from each other, 10 cm from the rim of the board. Holes are 5 cm in diameter and can be closed by a lid at a depth of 5 cm. Only when the mouse puts its head over the edge of the hole, the mouse can see whether it is open or not. If open, the hole is the exit to the animal's home cage via a S-shaped tunnel (15 cm long; 5 cm diameter). Numerous cues in the room allow spatial orientation (Schwabe et al., 2010a; ter Horst et al., 2012c). A bottle located close to a hole provides a proximal stimulus, relevant for S-R learning. Total training of the mice on the CHB lasted 1 h and 30 min (6 trials) followed 15 min later by a test trial to reveal the learning strategy.

General procedure

Free exploration trial (FET). In the week prior to the FET, mice were trained three times to climb through a tunnel. For the FET mice were placed on the CHB for 5 min. All holes were covered with a lid. A transparent water filled bottle (0.5 L, 22 cm high, 5 cm diameter) was placed next to the hole which is the exit hole during the training trials. At the end of the 5 min, the exit hole was opened and the mouse was gently guided by the experimenter toward the exit hole using an iron grid (20 × 6 cm). This initial exploration trial served to estimate possible differences in mouse exploration behavior. Training trials started one week later.

Training trials. Each training trial started by placing the mouse in a gray cylinder (Plexiglas; 25 cm high; 10 cm diameter), located at the center of the CHB. After 5 s, the cylinder was lifted and the animal could explore the board and exit through the tunnel. If a mouse did not enter the exit hole within 120 s, it was gently guided there by the experimenter along the small iron grid. The CHB was cleaned after each trial with 1% acetic acid solution and turned clockwise until another hole was at the location of the exit to avoid an influence of odor cues. The home cage was placed under the exit hole and was not visible to the mouse on the board.

Six successive training trials were given with an inter-trial interval of 15 min. The position of the exit hole was fixed with respect to the distal extra-maze cues in the room. Also, the proximal intra-maze cue (the bottle) was always at the same position next to the exit hole in all six training trials. This set-up allowed that the location of the exit hole could be acquired both by using the relation between spatial cues and by association with a single cue: the bottle.

Revealing the learning strategy. Trial 7, which started 15 min after the sixth training trial, was a test trial to detect the learning strategy (Figure 1). The exit hole used during the training trials remained open but the bottle was moved to the opposite part of the board and an additional exit hole was opened. Leaving the board through the exit hole used in the training showed the use of a spatial strategy. Using the hole at the novel location and next to the bottle, reflects the use of a S-R strategy. To control for possible odor cues in trial 7, we divided the bedding of the home cage of the mouse over two cages and placed the cages under both exit holes.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Estrous cycle and stress affect the latency to exit hole (first visit). MRCaMKCre (MR forebrain deficient) and MRflox/flox (MR forebrain intact) mice performed six trials on the circular hole board. (A) All mice; (B) Estrus phase; (C) Diestrus phase; (D) proestrus phase. Control MRCaMKCre mice (n = 30) need more time to find the exit hole compared to MRflox/flox mice (n = 25), especially in the proestrus (n = 6) and estrus (n = 10) phase. Stress has no effect on the latency to exit hole in MRCaMKCre (n = 18) and control mice (n = 21); however, it improves the performance of estrus MRCaMKCre mice (n = 6). Black circles with the solid line are control MRCaMKCre mice and black squares with a solid line represent control MRflox/flox mice. White circles with a dotted line are stressed MRCaMKCre mice and white squares with the dotted line represent stressed MRflox/flox mice. *p < 0.05, compared to MRCaMKCre same estrous cycle phase; $p < 0.05, compared to estrus within MRCaMKCre mice; p < 0.05, compared to control estrus MRCaMKCre mice.

Behavioral observations

Behavior was digitally recorded during the FET and training trials and analyzed with Ethovision XT 6.1 (Noldus Information and Technology BV, Wageningen, The Netherlands). This image analysis system sampled the position of the mouse 12.5 times per second. The following performance parameters were measured for all trials and scored by Ethovision: latency of first visit to exit hole (exit latency: sec), velocity (cm/s), distance moved (cm), latency to leave the board (escape latency: sec) and latency to leave the center (sec; center is 30 cm in diameter). The following parameters were hand scored by the researcher: total number of holes visited (a visit was counted, when the animal put at least its nose in the hole), hole visit errors (the number of holes visited before the exit hole) and perseveration (%). Perseveration was calculated when the mouse visited the same hole twice in a row or when there was one other hole between the two holes (e.g., hole 6, 6 or holes 6, 7, 6). Percentage perseveration is expressed by the number of perseverations divided by the total holes visited.

Estrous cycle

Smears were taken after the FET and after the training (ter Horst et al., 2013). The mouse was placed on top of its cage, lifted slightly by its tail and the head of the smear loop (size 1 μl; Greiner Bio-one) was gently inserted above the major labia in the cloaca and carefully rubbed along the ventral/rostral side of the cloaca. Cells were transferred to a drop of water on a glass microscope slide, air-dried and stained with Giemsa (Sigma) to facilitate identification of the cycle phase. The four phases are proestrus, estrus, metestrus and diestrus. We did not encounter the metestrus phase which is the shortest and most difficult to detect phase (Caligioni, 2009) so therefore it is not included in this study.

Statistical analysis

Data are presented as mean ± SEM. Statistical analysis include Chi-square and GLM repeated measures when appropriate. Reported p-values are two-tailed. P < 0.05 was accepted as statistically significant. Statistical analyses were performed with SPSS software (version 17.0; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL).

From two naive MRCaMKCre females the estrous cycle phase could not be determined. The number of stressed MRCaMKCre and MRflox/flox mice in the proestrus phase is too low (n = 1–2) to allow test statistics. However, these mice were still included in the “Total” analysis but not used for further sub-analysis.

Results

Behavior of MRCaMKCre mice differed from the littermate MRflox/flox mice during free exploration and training trials.

Free exploration trial (FET)

The exploration pattern of MRCaMKCre mice differed from MRflox/flox (Table 1). MRCaMKCre mice were less active than MRflox/flox mice, expressed by slower velocity, shorter walking distance, longer latencies to leave the center and visited less holes (all p < 0.05). Furthermore, MRCaMKCre mice persistently returned to holes visited one or two holes earlier (% perseverance, MRCaMKCre mice vs. MRflox/flox, p < 0.001). These genotype differences in exploration were independent of the estrous cycle. Within MRCaMKCre mice proestrus females showed significantly less perseveration compared to females in other phases [F(2, 33) = 4.025, p = 0.027; proestrus vs. estrus: p = 0.016; proestrus vs. diestrus p = 0.01]. The estrous cycle did not affect exploration in the MRflox/flox mice (data not shown).

Table 1.

Behavioral data from the free exploration trial (FET).

MRCaMKCre (n = 48) MRflox/flox (n = 46)
Velocity (cm/s) 3.57 ± 0.2*** 4.79 ± 0.2
Distance (m) 10.66 ± 0.5*** 14.33 ± 0.6
Latency to leave center (s) 13.04 ± 2.7* 6.86 ± 1.2
Total holes visited (n) 11.44 ± 0.8*** 16.07 ± 0.9
Perseveration holes visited (%) 32.01 ± 2.4*** 18.34 ± 1.9
***

p < 0.0001,

*

p < 0.05.

Training trials

The phase of the estrous cycle had an effect on performance during the training in MRCaMKCre mice but not in their MR intact littermates, the MRflox/flox mice. Statistic data is displayed in Table 2.

Table 2.

Repeated measurement (ANOVA) results for behavioral performance parameters during the training on the circular hole board tested for genotype (G), stress (S), and estrous cycle in within controls (ES) effects.

Behavioral parameters F df P
EXIT LATENCY
G Control 13.103 1.53 0.001*
G Stress 9.450 1.37 0.004*
G × S 0.044 1.90 0.833
S Within MRCaMKCre 0.148 1.46 0.702
S Within MRflox/flox 0.622 1.44 0.622
ES Within MRCaMKCre 3.716 2.25 0.039*
ES Within MRflox/flox 2.269 2.22 0.127
ES Control proestrus 10.976 1.15 0.005*
ES Control estrus 22.502 1.19 <0.0001*
ES Control diestrus 0.299 1.13 0.594
ES Stress estrus 3.805 1.15 0.07
ES Stress diestrus 6.107 1.17 0.024*
HOLE VISIT ERRORS
G Control 5.740 1.53 0.020*
G Stress 9.275 1.37 0.004*
G × S 0.503 1.90 0.480
S Within MRCaMKCre 0.104 1.46 0.748
S Within MRflox/flox 3.785 1.44 0.058
ES Within MRCaMKCre 3.479 2.25 0.046*
ES Within MRflox/flox 1.584 2.22 0.228
LATENCY TO LEAVE CENTER
G Control 16.299 1.53 <0.0001*

E.g., control proestrus represents the comparison between control MRCaMKCre and MRflox/flox mice in the proestrus phase

*

= significant.

Exit latency

Both control genotypes (MRCaMKCre and MRflox/flox littermates) learned the task as demonstrated by the decrease of latencies of first visit of the exit hole over trials [Figure 1A; training: F(5, 265) = 12.271, p < 0.0001]. However, MRflox/flox mice had significantly shorter latencies than MRCaMKCre mice (Table 2). Taking the phase of the estrous cycle into account showed longer latencies in some estrous phases in MRCaMKCre mice but not in the MRflox/flox mice. MRCaMKCre mice in estrus have significantly longer latencies than diestrus MRCaMKCre mice (p = 0.016). Comparing the MRCaMKCre mice to the MRflox/flox mice in different estrous cycle phases shows that proestrus and estrus MRCaMKCre mice have significantly longer latencies to the exit hole than proestrus and estrus MRflox/flox mice; there were no genotype differences in the diestrus phase (Table 2).

After stress both genotypes learned the task as the exit latency was decreasing over trials [Figure 1A; training: F(5, 185) = 7.619, p < 0.0001]. Stressed MRCaMKCre mice need significantly more time to find the exit hole than stressed MRflox/flox mice. In the stress group we had enough females of both genotypes in the estrus and the diestrus phase but not in the proestrus phase of the estrous cycle. Within the stressed MRflox/flox and the stressed MRCaMKCre mice no estrous cycle effect on the latency to exit hole was found. However, between genotype comparison revealed that stressed diestrus MRCaMKCre mice performed significantly worse on the CHB compared to stressed diestrus MRflox/flox mice whereas a trend was seen in stressed estrus mice (Table 2).

Stress did not affect the performance of both genotypes compared to the controls based on the exit latency (Figure 1). However, stressed estrus MRCaMKCre mice displayed significantly shorter latencies to the exit than control estrus MRCaMKCre mice [F(1, 16) = 4.662, p = 0.046], whereas stressed diestrus MRCaMKCre had longer latencies than control diestrus MRCaMKCre mice [F(1, 18) = 4.304, p = 0.053]. Estrus or diestrus stressed MRflox/flox did not behave differently from naive controls in the same estrous phase.

Other behavioral parameters

Similar results as in the exit latency are found for hole visit errors (Figure 2). Control and stressed MRCaMKCre mice made more errors than the MRflox/flox mice. An estrous cycle effect in hole visit errors was found in control MRCaMKCre mice but not in control MRflox/flox mice. MRCaMKCre mice in estrus (p = 0.025) and proestrus (p = 0.049) made significantly more errors than diestrus MRCaMKCre mice. Time to leave the center might also influence the latencies to exit hole. Indeed, MRCaMKCre mice took longer to leave the center than MRflox/flox mice (data not shown). Especially estrus MRCaMKCre mice remained longer in the center compared to proestrus (p = 0.021) and diestrus (p = 0.003) MRCaMKCre mice. The same significant differences between both genotypes were also found in velocity and time to leave the board (data not shown).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Hole visit errors in MR forebrain deficient mice during the circular hole board training. (A) All mice; (B) Estrus phase; (C) Diestrus phase; (D) proestrus phase. Control MRCaMKCre mice make more errors before finding the exit hole compared to MRflox/flox mice, especially in the estrus phase. Stress did not affect hole visit errors, however, stressed MRCaMKCre mice still made more errors than stressed MRflox/flox mice. Black circles with the solid line are control MRCaMKCre mice and black squares with a solid line represent control MRflox/flox mice. White circles with a dotted line are stressed MRCaMKCre mice and white squares with the dotted line represent stressed MRflox/flox mice. *p < 0.05, compared to MRCaMKCre same estrous cycle phase; $p < 0.05, compared to estrus within MRCaMKCre mice.

Test trial

Strategy (Figure 3)

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Percentage of mice using Spatial or Stimulus-response strategies revealed by the test trial. No effect of the estrous cycle on strategies in (A) control and (B) stressed mice. Numbers in the bars represent the number of mice using the strategy. Black bar denotes the spatial strategy and the gray bar the stimulus–response strategy. D, diestrus; E, estrus; P, proestrus.

To reveal the strategy the intra-maze cue (bottle) was moved to the opposite side of the board. The mice could now exit the board through the original exit hole (spatial strategy) or the new exit hole marked by the bottle (S-R strategy). Independent of genotype control non-stressed mice applied either the spatial (70–80%) or the S-R (20–30%) strategy [χ2(1) = 2.509 p = 0.285]. No stress effect was seen on strategy use [χ2(1) = 0.06 p = 0.807] and similar data was seen for the estrous cycle phase.

Performance

No genotype difference in latency to exit hole was seen during the test trial. Within the control MRCaMKCre mice we found an effect of the estrous cycle [F(2, 25) = 3.669 p = 0.04; data not shown] with estrus mice taking more time to locate the exit hole compared to proestrus and diestrus mice (40 vs. 10–20 s, p = 0.049; p = 0.023; respectively). The estrous cycle did not affect the performance of the control MRflox/flox mice during the test trial. Stress did not change the latency to an exit hole in both genotypes.

Corrected exit latency

Longer latencies to leave the center could be interpreted as a sign of anxiety interfering with learning. In order to be able to answer the question whether we saw a learning deficit in control and stressed MRCaMKCre or anxiety, the latency to leave the center was subtracted from the latency of first visit of the exit hole. When corrected MRCaMKCre mice still needed significantly more time to find the exit hole compared to the MRflox/flox mice [control: F(1, 53) = 6.014 p = 0.018; stress: F(1, 37) = 8.734 p = 0.005; Figure 4).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Corrected exit latency. Subtraction of the latencies to leave the center from the latency of the first visit of the exit hole still resulted in longer latencies to locate the exit in MRCaMKCre mice compared to MRflox/flox mice, independent of stress. Black circles with the solid line are control MRCaMKCre mice and black squares with a solid line represent control MRflox/flox mice. White circles with a dotted line are stressed MRCaMKCre mice and white squares with the dotted line represent stressed MRflox/flox mice. *p < 0.05, compared to MRCaMKCre mice.

Spatial vs. S-R performance

The performance of mice over the training trials was grouped according to the learning strategy as revealed by the test trial. Stressed MRCaMKCre mice using the S-R strategy performed significantly worse over the first 5 trials compared to MRCaMKCre mice using the spatial strategy [F(1, 16) = 4.930 p = 0.041; Figure 5) but also compared to stressed MRflox/flox mice using the S-R strategy [F(1, 6) = 20.171 p = 0.004]. Learning was comparable between stressed MRCaMKCre and MRflox/flox mice using the spatial strategy [F(1, 29) = 2.415 p = 0.131]. No performance differences between spatial and S-R strategy users were seen in the control MRCaMKCre and MRflox/flox mice and in the stressed MRflox/flox mice [F(1, 28) = 1.098 p = 0.304; F(1, 23) = 0.648 p = 0.429; F(1, 19) = 0.542 p = 0.47; respectively].

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Exit latency of stressed mice grouped according to the strategy revealed by the test trial. Stressed MRCaMKCre mice using the stimulus-response strategy (n = 4) need more time to find the exit hole compared to MRCaMKCre mice using the spatial strategy (n = 14). Strategy (stimulus-response n = 4, spatial n = 17) did not affect the performance of stressed MRflox/flox mice. Black up-pointing triangles are MRCaMKCre mice and black down-pointing triangles are MRflox/flox mice which used a S-R strategy. White up-pointing triangle represent MRCaMKCre and down-pointing white triangles represent MRflox/flox mice which used a spatial strategy. *p < 0.05, compared to MRCaMKCre S-R strategy users. S-R, stimulus-response.

Discussion

In the present study we investigated in female mice the effect of forebrain MR deficiency, estrous cycle and an acute stressor on spatial performance and strategy use by using the CHB. We showed the following findings: Firstly, MR deficiency in the forebrain resulted in longer latencies to the exit hole and more hole visit errors. Secondly, the estrous cycle phases did not affect spatial performance in the wild type mice (MRflox/flox) but did in the mutants. The MRCaMKCre mice in the proestrus and the estrus phase had longer latencies to the exit hole compared to MRCaMKCre mice in diestrus. Thirdly, prior stressor exposure did not affect performance on the CHB of the MRCaMKCre or MRflox/flox mice. And finally, stressed estrus MRCaMKCre mice learned the CHB significantly faster than control estrus MRCaMKCre mice.

Around 70% of the control female mice (MRCaMKCre and MRflox/flox mice) favored the spatial strategy over the S-R strategy. Stress and estrous cycle did not influence the strategy use. However, stressed MRCaMKCre mice using the S-R strategy needed significantly more time to find the exit hole compared to spatial strategy users.

Loss of mineralocorticoid receptor impairs spatial learning

Female MRCaMKCre mice needed significantly more time to find the exit hole on the CHB compared to the MRflox/flox mice. However, detailed analysis showed that the MRCaMKCre mice stayed longer in the center than MRflox/flox mice before moving toward the exit hole. Could it be that they are slower to start their exploration or is this maybe an anxiety response? MR forebrain deficiency caused no differences in anxiety (Berger et al., 2006). Although, MR overexpression in mice resulted in reduced anxiety (Lai et al., 2007; Rozeboom et al., 2007). When the exit latency was corrected for the time spent in the center we still saw a significantly increased latency to the exit hole in the MRCaMKCre females compared to MRflox/flox mice. This implies that the impairment found in the MRCaMKCre mice is mainly due to a slower start of exploration than due to increased anxiety. Male MRCaMKCre mice also needed more time to find the exit hole compared to MRflox/flox littermates on the CHB (ter Horst et al., 2012c). However, male MRCaMKCre mice did not remain longer in the center (ter Horst, unpublished observation). Furthermore, in the Morris water maze MRCaMKCre mice were found to be more passive and needed more time to learn the task (Berger et al., 2006), which is comparable to the behavior seen on the CHB.

Another observation was that the MRCaMKCre mice made more hole visit errors than MRflox/flox mice. MRCaMKCre mice were reported to make also more re-entry errors on the radial arm maze, especially females (Berger et al., 2006). In addition, during the free exploration trial female MRCaMKCre mice were found to return to the same holes repeatedly (perseveration) while the total number of visited holes was comparable to the MRflox/flox mice. To support this finding, male MRCaMKCre mice were also reported to show this intense exploration or hyper-responsiveness (ter Horst et al., 2012c). Moreover, in a novel object recognition task male and female MRCaMKCre mice persistently explored the novel object more than their MR forebrain intact littermates. Furthermore, high emotional arousal was observed in stressed female MRCaMKCre mice compared to MRflox/flox mice (Brinks et al., 2009). Alternatively, MR forebrain overexpressing mice also showed a persistent searching for an absent platform in the water maze suggesting perseveration (Harris et al., 2013).

The present study, therefore, shows that MR forebrain deficiency impairs spatial performance in females most likely due to a change in exploration patterns.

The phase of the estrous cycle affects the spatial performance of control MRCaMKCre

The main question was whether the phase of the estrous cycle could interfere with spatial learning. Here, we saw that control MRflox/flox mice were not affected in their spatial performance by the estrous cycle phases. In concordance, previously the estrous cycle phase did not interfere with the spatial learning in C57BL/6J female mice on the CHB (ter Horst et al., 2013), which is the background strain of the MRCaMKCre mice (Berger et al., 2006). Furthermore, Berry et al. (1997) found no estrous cycle effect on spatial performance. However, the investigators that did find an effect of the estrous cycle phase on spatial learning reported either an impairment in the proestrus (Bowman et al., 2001; Pompili et al., 2010) or in the estrus phase (Frye, 1995; Healy et al., 1999; Frick and Berger-Sweeney, 2001) depending on the task, species and strain.

Sex hormones fluctuate during the estrous cycle. During the proestrus phase estrogen levels are high whereas during the estrus phase a small peak in progesterone is found but the estrogen levels are at their lowest at the end of this phase (Walmer et al., 1992). In these phases spatial learning was impaired in the MRCaMKCre mice. Being in the diestrus phase of the estrous cycle did not affect the spatial performance in the MRCaMKCre mice. During the proestrus synapse formation and dendritic spine numbers in hippocampal CA1 neurons in rats is highest whereas in the estrus it rapidly decreases and intermediate in the diestrus (Woolley et al., 1990a; Shors et al., 2001). Carey et al. (1995) elegantly demonstrated that estradiol decreases hippocampal MR mRNA expression and binding capacity. Progesterone binds to MR, and acts as an antagonist (Carey et al., 1995). In addition, progesterone produces an increase in MR mRNA in vivo but only after estrogen pretreatment (Castren et al., 1995). Clearly there is a strong interaction between sex- and stress hormones with regard to MR function. MR forebrain deficiency impairs spatial learning in mice, especially at proestrus (high estrogen levels) and estrus (decreasing estrogen levels with a small peak in progesterone levels).

Acute stress did not influence spatial performance in female mice independent of the MR

Mice were acutely stressed in this study by a 10 min restraint starting 30 min before the first training trial. Previous studies with male mice showed that corticosterone levels were increased after the acute stress (Schwabe et al., 2010a; ter Horst et al., 2012c). The lack of effect of the acute stress on the learning of the CHB in female mice is supported by data from the background strain, C57BL/6J, which also did not show a stress effect on spatial performance in female mice (ter Horst et al., 2013).

In female mice and rats the effect of stress on spatial learning depends on the type, duration, and timing of the stressor (ter Horst et al., 2012b). Furthermore, pretraining familiarizes the animals to the task and apparatus and reduces novelty stress (Bucci et al., 1995; Warren and Juraska, 1997). Chronic stressed (isolation or restraint) female rats showed impaired spatial performance (Daniel et al., 1999; Conrad et al., 2003). However, chronic restraint stress as well as acute stress could also improve spatial learning (Bowman et al., 2001; Conrad et al., 2004; Kitraki et al., 2004). Stressed female mice by predator odor showed improved learning in the Morris water maze (Galliot et al., 2010) while acute restraint stress did not affect their performance on the CHB (ter Horst et al., 2013).

In contrast to females, both chronic and acute stress impaired spatial performance in male rats (Luine et al., 1994; Diamond et al., 1999; Bowman et al., 2001; Conrad et al., 2003, 2004; Kitraki et al., 2004). Also male mice showed impaired spatial performance on the CHB after acute (Schwabe et al., 2010a) and chronic stress (Schwabe et al., 2008). However, acute stress did not worsen the spatial performance in the male MRCaMKCre mice but did so in their littermates (ter Horst et al., 2012c). A possible explanation for this could be that the MRCaMKCre mice already have increased basal corticosterone levels when compared to MRflox/flox mice (ter Horst et al., 2012c), which was also found in female MRCaMKCre mice (ter Horst et al., 2012a).

Chronic stress produced hippocampal CA3 synapse loss and dendritic retraction in males (Woolley et al., 1990b; McLaughlin et al., 2007). In female rats, chronic stress either failed to influence CA3 dendritic arbors or showed a mild hippocampal CA3 dendritic retraction (Galea et al., 1997; McLaughlin et al., 2010). However, chronic stress combined with estradiol increased the dendritic complexity of the CA1 region in either ovariectomized or gonadally intact females (McLaughlin et al., 2010; Conrad et al., 2012). This suggests that sex hormones might prevent stress induced hippocampal CA3 induced neuronal loss and in parallel impair spatial performance.

Interaction effect of acute stress and the estrous cycle phase on spatial learning in MRCaMKCre

Acute stress did affect the performance of the MRCaMKCre mice on the CHB depending on the estrous cycle phase. This interaction effect of acute stress and the estrous cycle phase was not observed in the littermates. Stressed estrus MRCaMKCre females showed improved performance on the CHB compared to naive MRCaMKCre mice in estrus. The opposite effect was seen in diestrus stressed MRCaMKCre females: decreased performance compared to naive MRCaMKCre mice in diestrus. Thus, in absence of MR, the impact of acute stress is larger with better performance in estrus and worse performance in diestrus. Whether this aggravation of the stress effect is because of altered stress-induced circulating sex steroid levels (Burgess and Handa, 1992; Shors et al., 1999) or because of dysregulation or a shift of the estrous cycle (Pollard et al., 1975; Herzog et al., 2009) remains to be investigated.

Problem-solving strategy is not affected by sex and stress hormones

To solve the CHB task mice can either use a spatial or a S-R strategy (Schwabe et al., 2010a). The spatial strategy relies on multiple stimuli in the surrounding environment and on the functionality of the hippocampus (White and McDonald, 2002). The S-R strategy makes use of one single proximal stimulus and requires the functionality of the caudate nucleus (Packard and Knowlton, 2002; Schwabe et al., 2010a). Male C57BL/6J mice have a preference for the spatial strategy (Schwabe et al., 2008, 2010a; Bettis and Jacobs, 2009) as do male MRCaMKCre mice (ter Horst et al., 2012c). Here, control MRflox/flox and MRCaMKCre females used either the spatial or the S-R strategy, as did female C57BL/6J mice (Bettis and Jacobs, 2009; ter Horst et al., 2013) and female rats (Tropp and Markus, 2001; Korol et al., 2004; Pleil and Williams, 2010; Hawley et al., 2012). Clearly, there are sex differences in strategy use.

In this study the estrous cycle phase did not affect strategy choice in control, hence non-stressed, female mice, as was seen before on the CHB in C57BL/6J mice (ter Horst et al., 2013). In female rats the phases of the estrous cycle did not affect the strategy on the T-maze (Hawley et al., 2012). However, others reported that female rats in the proestrus phase predominantly used the spatial strategy in a T-maze task (Korol et al., 2004; Pleil and Williams, 2010) and in the estrus phase were strongly affected by the intra-maze cue in a water maze (Sava and Markus, 2005). Cognition and strategy use in female mice and rats depend on multiple factors such as the learning task self (simple or complex) but also in which memory phase is tested (acquisition or memory retrieval).

Stress did not affect the strategy choice in female MRCaMKCre and MRflox/flox mice on the CHB. So far the effect of stress on strategy choice has scarcely been examined in females. In C57Bl/6J female mice a stress-induced switch toward spatial was seen, especially in estrus females, thereby improving their performance on the CHB (ter Horst et al., 2013). That we did not find a stress-induced switch toward a spatial strategy in this study could result from the fact that the majority of the mice (70–80%) already used a spatial strategy, whereas in the previous study 50% of the mice employed a spatial strategy. We do not know why in the present study the mice preferred the spatial strategy. Nevertheless, the switch from S-R toward a spatial strategy after cortisol administration was also found in women (Schwabe et al., 2009). Acute and chronic stress in males make them switch toward a more S-R strategy (Schwabe et al., 2007, 2008, 2010a). Interestingly, in males the MR was found to be involved in the stress-induced switch from spatial to the S-R strategy and this switch rescued their performance (Schwabe et al., 2010a; ter Horst et al., 2012c). Even though we did not see a stress-induced strategy switch in this study, acutely stressed female MRCaMKCre mice using a S-R strategy performed significantly worse on the CHB compared to spatial strategy users. This suggests that in females the MR is needed for the stress-induced switch toward a spatial strategy and this switch might rescue their performance.

Conclusion

Loss of MR in the forebrain results in an impaired spatial performance particularly in proestrus and estrus. This suggests that the MR is necessary for spatial learning when estrogen and progesterone levels are fluctuating. Stress does not influence the spatial performance of controls but it does improve learning in stressed estrus MRCaMKCre mice. Stress or the estrous cycle do not affect the strategy use of both MRCaMKCre and MRflox/flox mice in this study. However, stressed MRCaMKCre using the S-R strategy were significantly impaired on the CHB suggesting that the MR could be needed for the stress-induced strategy switch toward a spatial strategy.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the European Science Foundation to Judith P. ter Horst, E. Ron de Kloet, and Melly S. Oitzl (Eurocores, 07-EuroSTRESS-FP-005), NWO-DFG –IRTG -DN 95-420 to Melly S. Oitzl and the Royal Netherlands Academy of Sciences KNAW (to E. Ron de Kloet) -Dr. J. Dobberke 2010-09 to Judith P. ter Horst.

References

  1. Berger S., Wolfer D. P., Selbach O., Alter H., Erdmann G., Reichardt H. M., et al. (2006). Loss of the limbic mineralocorticoid receptor impairs behavioral plasticity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 195–200 10.1073/pnas.0503878102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Berry B., McMahan R., Gallagher M. (1997). Spatial learning and memory at defined points of the estrous cycle: effects on performance of a hippocampal-dependent task. Behav. Neurosci. 111, 267–274 10.1037/0735-7044.111.2.267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bettis T. J., Jacobs L. F. (2009). Sex-specific strategies in spatial orientation in C57BL/6J mice. Behav. Processes 82, 249–255 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bowman R. E., Zrull M. C., Luine V. N. (2001). Chronic restraint stress enhances radial arm maze performance in female rats. Brain Res. 904, 279–289 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02474-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brinks V., Berger S., Gass P., De Kloet E. R., Oitzl M. S. (2009). Mineralocorticoid receptors in control of emotional arousal and fear memory. Horm. Behav. 56, 232–238 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bucci D. J., Chiba A. A., Gallagher M. (1995). Spatial learning in male and female Long-Evans rats. Behav. Neurosci. 109, 180–183 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Burgess L. H., Handa R. J. (1992). Chronic estrogen-induced alterations in adrenocorticotropin and corticosterone secretion, and glucocorticoid receptor-mediated functions in female rats. Endocrinology 131, 1261–1269 10.1210/en.131.3.1261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Caligioni C. S. (2009). Assessing reproductive status/stages in mice. Curr. protoc. Neurosci. Appendix 4:Appendix 4I. 10.1002/0471142301.nsa04is48 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Carey M. P., Deterd C. H., De Koning J., Helmerhorst F., De Kloet E. R. (1995). The influence of ovarian steroids on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal regulation in the female rat. J. Endocrinol. 144, 311–321 10.1677/joe.0.1440311 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Casanova E., Fehsenfeld S., Mantamadiotis T., Lemberger T., Greiner E., Stewart A. F., et al. (2001). A CamKIIalpha iCre BAC allows brain-specific gene inactivation. Genesis 31, 37–42 10.1002/gene.1078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Castren M., Patchev V. K., Almeida O. F., Holsboer F., Trapp T., Castren E. (1995). Regulation of rat mineralocorticoid receptor expression in neurons by progesterone. Endocrinology 136, 3800–3806 10.1210/en.136.9.3800 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Conrad C. D., Grote K. A., Hobbs R. J., Ferayorni A. (2003). Sex differences in spatial and non-spatial Y-maze performance after chronic stress. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 79, 32–40 10.1016/S1074-7427(02)00018-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Conrad C. D., Jackson J. L., Wieczorek L., Baran S. E., Harman J. S., Wright R. L., et al. (2004). Acute stress impairs spatial memory in male but not female rats: influence of estrous cycle. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 78, 569–579 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.04.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Conrad C. D., McLaughlin K. J., Huynh T. N., El-Ashmawy M., Sparks M. (2012). Chronic stress and a cyclic regimen of estradiol administration separately facilitate spatial memory: relationship with hippocampal CA1 spine density and dendritic complexity. Behav. Neurosci. 126, 142–156 10.1037/a0025770 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Daniel J. M., Roberts S. L., Dohanich G. P. (1999). Effects of ovarian hormones and environment on radial maze and water maze performance of female rats. Physiol. Behav. 66, 11–20 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00272-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. De Kloet E. R., Joels M., Holsboer F. (2005). Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 463–475 10.1038/nrn1683 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. De Kloet E. R., Vreugdenhil E., Oitzl M. S., Joels M. (1998). Brain corticosteroid receptor balance in health and disease. Endocr. Rev. 19, 269–301 10.1210/er.19.3.269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Diamond D. M., Park C. R., Heman K. L., Rose G. M. (1999). Exposing rats to a predator impairs spatial working memory in the radial arm water maze. Hippocampus 9, 542–552 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dias-Ferreira E., Sousa J. C., Melo I., Morgado P., Mesquita A. R., Cerqueira J. J., et al. (2009). Chronic stress causes frontostriatal reorganization and affects decision-making. Science 325, 621–625 10.1126/science.1171203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Frick K. M., Berger-Sweeney J. (2001). Spatial reference memory and neocortical neurochemistry vary with the estrous cycle in C57BL/6 mice. Behav. Neurosci. 115, 229–237 10.1037//0735-7044.115.1.229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Frye C. A. (1995). Estrus-associated decrements in a water maze task are limited to acquisition. Physiol. Behav. 57, 5–14 10.1016/0031-9384(94)00197-D [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Galea L. A., McEwen B. S., Tanapat P., Deak T., Spencer R. L., Dhabhar F. S. (1997). Sex differences in dendritic atrophy of CA3 pyramidal neurons in response to chronic restraint stress. Neuroscience 81, 689–697 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00233-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Galliot E., Levaillant M., Beard E., Millot J. L., Pourie G. (2010). Enhancement of spatial learning by predator odor in mice: involvement of amygdala and hippocampus. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 93, 196–202 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.09.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Harris A. P., Holmes M. C., De Kloet E. R., Chapman K. E., Seckl J. R. (2013). Mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptor balance in control of HPA axis and behaviour. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 648–658 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.08.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hawley W. R., Grissom E. M., Barratt H. E., Conrad T. S., Dohanich G. P. (2012). The effects of biological sex and gonadal hormones on learning strategy in adult rats. Physiol. Behav. 105, 1014–1020 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.11.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Healy S. D., Braham S. R., Braithwaite V. A. (1999). Spatial working memory in rats: no differences between the sexes. Proc. Biol. Sci. 266, 2303–2308 10.1098/rspb.1999.0923 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Herzog C. J., Czeh B., Corbach S., Wuttke W., Schulte-Herbruggen O., Hellweg R., et al. (2009). Chronic social instability stress in female rats: a potential animal model for female depression. Neuroscience 159, 982–992 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kim J. J., Baxter M. G. (2001). Multiple brain-memory systems: the whole does not equal the sum of its parts. Trends Neurosci. 24, 324–330 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01818-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kitraki E., Kremmyda O., Youlatos D., Alexis M., Kittas C. (2004). Spatial performance and corticosteroid receptor status in the 21-day restraint stress paradigm. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1018, 323–327 10.1196/annals.1296.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Korol D. L., Malin E. L., Borden K. A., Busby R. A., Couper-Leo J. (2004). Shifts in preferred learning strategy across the estrous cycle in female rats. Horm. Behav. 45, 330–338 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lai M., Horsburgh K., Bae S. E., Carter R. N., Stenvers D. J., Fowler J. H., et al. (2007). Forebrain mineralocorticoid receptor overexpression enhances memory, reduces anxiety and attenuates neuronal loss in cerebral ischaemia. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 1832–1842 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05427.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Luine V., Villegas M., Martinez C., McEwen B. S. (1994). Repeated stress causes reversible impairments of spatial memory performance. Brain Res. 639, 167–170 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91778-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lupien S. J., McEwen B. S. (1997). The acute effects of corticosteroids on cognition: integration of animal and human model studies. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 24, 1–27 10.1016/S0165-0173(97)00004-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. McLaughlin K. J., Gomez J. L., Baran S. E., Conrad C. D. (2007). The effects of chronic stress on hippocampal morphology and function: an evaluation of chronic restraint paradigms. Brain Res. 1161, 56–64 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.05.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. McLaughlin K. J., Wilson J. O., Harman J., Wright R. L., Wieczorek L., Gomez J., et al. (2010). Chronic 17beta-estradiol or cholesterol prevents stress-induced hippocampal CA3 dendritic retraction in ovariectomized female rats: possible correspondence between CA1 spine properties and spatial acquisition. Hippocampus 20, 768–786 10.1002/hipo.20678 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Oitzl M. S., Champagne D. L., Van Der Veen R., De Kloet E. R. (2010). Brain development under stress: hypotheses of glucocorticoid actions revisited. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 853–866 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Oitzl M. S., De Kloet E. R. (1992). Selective corticosteroid antagonists modulate specific aspects of spatial orientation learning. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 62–71 10.1037/0735-7044.106.1.62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Packard M. G., Knowlton B. J. (2002). Learning and memory functions of the Basal Ganglia. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 563–593 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142937 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Pleil K. E., Williams C. L. (2010). The development and stability of estrogen-modulated spatial navigation strategies in female rats. Horm. Behav. 57, 360–367 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.01.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Pollard I., White B. M., Bassett J. R., Cairncross K. D. (1975). Plasma glucocorticoid elevation and desynchronization of the estrous cycle following unpredictable stress in the rat. Behav. Biol. 14, 103–108 10.1016/S0091-6773(75)90374-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Pompili A., Tomaz C., Arnone B., Tavares M. C., Gasbarri A. (2010). Working and reference memory across the estrous cycle of rat: a long-term study in gonadally intact females. Behav. Brain Res. 213, 10–18 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Reul J. M., De Kloet E. R. (1985). Two receptor systems for corticosterone in rat brain: microdistribution and differential occupation. Endocrinology 117, 2505–2511 10.1210/endo-117-6-2505 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Rozeboom A. M., Akil H., Seasholtz A. F. (2007). Mineralocorticoid receptor overexpression in forebrain decreases anxiety-like behavior and alters the stress response in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 4688–4693 10.1073/pnas.0606067104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Sava S., Markus E. J. (2005). Intramaze cue utilization in the water maze: effects of sex and estrous cycle in rats. Horm. Behav. 48, 23–33 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.01.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Schwabe L., Dalm S., Schachinger H., Oitzl M. S. (2008). Chronic stress modulates the use of spatial and stimulus-response learning strategies in mice and man. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 90, 495–503 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.07.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Schwabe L., Oitzl M. S., Philippsen C., Richter S., Bohringer A., Wippich W., et al. (2007). Stress modulates the use of spatial versus stimulus-response learning strategies in humans. Learn. Mem. 14, 109–116 10.1101/lm.435807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Schwabe L., Oitzl M. S., Richter S., Schachinger H. (2009). Modulation of spatial and stimulus-response learning strategies by exogenous cortisol in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 358–366 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.09.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Schwabe L., Schachinger H., De Kloet E. R., Oitzl M. S. (2010a). Corticosteroids operate as a switch between memory systems. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1362–1372 10.1162/jocn.2009.21278 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Schwabe L., Schachinger H., De Kloet E. R., Oitzl M. S. (2010b). Stress impairs spatial but not early stimulus-response learning. Behav. Brain Res. 213, 50–55 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Shors T. J., Chua C., Falduto J. (2001). Sex differences and opposite effects of stress on dendritic spine density in the male versus female hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 21, 6292–6297 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Shors T. J., Pickett J., Wood G., Paczynski M. (1999). Acute stress persistently enhances estrogen levels in the female rat. Stress 3, 163–171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. ter Horst J. P., Carobrez A. P., Van Der Mark M. H., De Kloet E. R., Oitzl M. S. (2012a). Sex differences in fear memory and extinction of mice with forebrain-specific disruption of the mineralocorticoid receptor. Eur. J. Neurosci. 36, 3096–3102 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08237.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. ter Horst J. P., De Kloet E. R., Schachinger H., Oitzl M. S. (2012b). Relevance of stress and female sex hormones for emotion and cognition. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 32, 725–735 10.1007/s10571-011-9774-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. ter Horst J. P., Van Der Mark M. H., Arp M., Berger S., De Kloet E. R., Oitzl M. S. (2012c). Stress or no stress: mineralocorticoid receptors in the forebrain regulate behavioral adaptation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 98, 33–40 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. ter Horst J. P., Kentrop J., De Kloet E. R., Oitzl M. S. (2013). Stress and estrous cycle affect strategy but not performance of female C57BL/6J mice. Behav. Brain Res. 241, 92–95 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.11.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Tropp J., Markus E. J. (2001). Sex differences in the dynamics of cue utilization and exploratory behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 119, 143–154 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00345-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Walmer D. K., Wrona M. A., Hughes C. L., Nelson K. G. (1992). Lactoferrin expression in the mouse reproductive tract during the natural estrous cycle: correlation with circulating estradiol and progesterone. Endocrinology 131, 1458–1466 10.1210/en.131.3.1458 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Warren S. G., Juraska J. M. (1997). Spatial and nonspatial learning across the rat estrous cycle. Behav. Neurosci. 111, 259–266 10.1037/0735-7044.111.2.259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. White N. M., McDonald R. J. (2002). Multiple parallel memory systems in the brain of the rat. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 77, 125–184 10.1006/nlme.2001.4008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Woolley C. S., Gould E., Frankfurt M., McEwen B. S. (1990a). Naturally occurring fluctuation in dendritic spine density on adult hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J. Neurosci. 10, 4035–4039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Woolley C. S., Gould E., McEwen B. S. (1990b). Exposure to excess glucocorticoids alters dendritic morphology of adult hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Brain Res. 531, 225–231 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90778-A [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES