Abstract
In order to further the insight into the explanation of changed performance in mental transformation under microgravity, we discuss the change of performance in mental transformation and its relationship with altered regional homogeneity (ReHo) in resting-state brain by using simulated weightlessness model. Twelve male subjects with age between 24 and 31 received resting-state fMRI scan and mental transformation test both in normal condition and immediately after 72 hours −6° head down tilt (HDT). A paired sample t-test was used to test the difference of behavior performance and brain activity between these two conditions. Compare with normal condition, subjects showed a changed performance in mental transformation with short term simulated microgravity and appeared to be falling. Meanwhile, decreased ReHo were found in right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and left inferior parietal lobule (IPL) after 72 hours −6° HDT, while increased ReHo were found in bilateral medial frontal gyrus (MFG) and left superior frontal gyrus (SFG) (P<0.05, corrected). Particularly, there was a significant correlation between ReHo values in left IPL and velocity index of mental transformation. Our findings indicate that gravity change may disrupt the function of right IFG and left IPL in the resting-state, among of which functional change in left IPL may contribute to changed abilities of mental transformation. In addition, the enhanced activity of the bilateral MFG and decreased activity of right IFG found in the current study maybe reflect a complementation effect on inhibitory control process.
Introduction
Human is the subject in space exploration activities, whether his activity is normal is the key point for all sorts of space exploration tasks, thus how the space environment influence individual activities had became a generally focus among space medical and psychological researchers. It is commonly believed that individual cognitive and behavioral activities would be impaired in space environment because there are negative factors like microgravity, radiation, noise and fatigue, etc [1], [2], [3], [4]. Among those negative factors, microgravity factor is the main difference between space environment and the earth environment, so relevant researches was a hot topic in recent years and there were many significative findings [5].
While exposed to microgravity environment, astronauts always face problems such as changed body positions and need to recognize objects and other astronauts’ gestures from various perspectives [6]. Due to the perspective change make the recognition process difficult, astronauts usually need to use the mental rotation strategy (mentally rotate an internal representation of the object) to help complicating it. If one couldn’t successfully completed the mental rotation process and following recognition process, he would fail to correctly recognize the object and sign around the cabin wall as well as crewmembers’ gestures, accordingly, it would bring obstacles to activity in the cabin and communication between crewmembers. So, the mental rotation ability plays a significant role in living in space and correlated researches are of great importance. Based on this kind of demand, several previous studies had done some excellent work on observing the variation of mental rotation ability under microgravity environment [7], [8], [9].
When Matsakis et al. initially discussed this issue in 1993, they suggested that the rate of mental rotation was increased in weightlessness as compared to pre-flight values by observing two astronauts’ performance in a mental rotation task [9]. Afterwards, Leone et al. came to a different conclusion in 1995. They declared that no deterioration of the mental rotation abilities of subjects occurring in space as compared to their own abilities prior to and after the flight [8]. In the above two studies, the applied stimuli are both nonsensical three-dimension objects, the results have limited value in practical application. While the stimuli are meaningful body parts, which commonly defined by the term mental transformation instead, it will be more close to practical application. However, it is a pity that research on mental transformation ability under microgravity has been seldom reported. As we have known, Grabherr et al. once made effort to discuss this issue by the method of parabolic flight. In their study, they found that task that requires the mental transformation of one’s own body or body parts became more difficult during microgravity [7].
Indeed, compared to previous mental rotation researches, mental transformation research has its merit in higher application value. Nevertheless, the amount of reported literature is quite a few and the recent study is tracking studies which just simply use behavioral test to discuss subjects’ mental transformation ability change. In this mean, the result is not very credible and just a phenomenological discuss in a relatively primitive stage. Meanwhile, parabolic flight model is usually applied to simulate the transient change induced by microgravity [10]. During the parabolic flight, there exists frequent gravity change, which may bring in mix factors such as acute stress. What’s more, due to the experiment environment is particular and advanced brain science facilities is nearly unable to fit in this environment [11], the internal neural mechanism of mental transformation ability change has not yet been clarified.
Fortunately, as a generally accepted technology in studies of microgravity among space science researchers [12], −6° head down tilt (HDT) bed rest could stimulate the microgravity effect of redistribution of individual body fluid toward head in a relative longer duration than parabolic flight, thus it makes it is possible to explore the effect of microgravity on individual cognitive and behavioral activities on earth. In addition, with the development of the experimental techniques of cognitive neuroscience, the application of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) technology in psychological research makes the exploration of the neural mechanism of the cognitive and behavioral processes possible [13], [14]. Especially, the resting-state fMRI technology could overcome the limitations of inconsistent results caused by different experimental designs and support more stable and effective evidence in the exploration of individual brain activities [15]. Moreover, previous studies have propose that resting-state fMRI data can be used as an alternative to task fMRI data, which is of great importance when task fMRI data are not easily or accurately acquired [16]. Thus, it had been accepted as an effective technology in the research of internal neural mechanism of individual cognitive and behavioral activities. For this technology, regional homogeneity (ReHo) is a widely used new method in resting-state fMRI research [17]. In ReHo analysis, Kendall's coefficient concordance (with value from 0 to 1) is used to calculate the coherence of time series of a given voxel with those of its nearest neighbors (26 voxels), thus we could learn the synchronization of the brain activities in a quantitative way. Our previous ALFF study has indicated that a gravity change-induced redistribution of body fluid may disrupt the baseline of local brain activity. But it should be noted that brain is a complex organ, it more emphasis on the interaction between neurons. Thus to clarify the brain activity change with short term microgravity from the perspective of functional connectivity (Such as ReHo, functional connectivity, brain network) is very meaningful. In addition, we could verify the result of the previous study in a different method by carry out the current study.
In the current study, to address the question how and why astronauts’ mental transformation ability change in microgravity, we combined the resting-state fMRI technology and −6° head down tilt bed rest technology together to find the difference of ReHo as well as behavior reaction change in a mental transformation test before and after 72 hours of −6° HDT bed rest. The results may support some evidences and references to further the insight into the explanation of changed performance in mental transformation under microgravity and rich the knowledge about protection of astronauts’ brain function.
Materials and Methods
2.1. Subjects
Sixteen health subjects were recruited in the present study; four subjects were excluded from the analysis because of excessive head motion (more than 2 mm or 2 degree in any axis). Totally, data of 12 subjects met the above mentioned criteria. Due to most of the astronauts are males, the subjects we selected are normal adult males. The valid subjects’ mean age is 26.4, range from 24 to 31. They were all right-handed as measured by Handedness Questionnaire [18]. Subjects reported no history of neurological injury, genetic mental disorders and substance abuse. With high resolution T1- and T2-weighted MRI examination, no subjects were found to have significant pathological change in brain. The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Fourth Military Medical University and all participants provided their written informed consent before the experiment.
2.2. Design
A pair comparison of subjects’ resting-state brain activities between normal condition and simulated microgravity condition were applied in the present study. The collected data of normal condition was subjects’ resting-state brain activities under normal seating posture condition. The collected data after simulated weightlessness condition was subjects’ resting-state brain activities after 72 hours −6° HDT bed rest. During the HDT bed rest, adequate water and foods were supplied to subjects and subjects’ head were forbidden to get out of bed in order to keep the redistribution of individual body fluid toward head. In order to avoid the fMRI scanning order of two conditions becoming an interference factor, the order of the scanning of the two conditions were balanced. Half of the subjects’ data of simulated microgravity condition were collected ten days after the data collection of normal condition while the others data collection order reversed. Moreover, in order to avoid the interference of biological rhythms, data collections were all carried out in the same time period in a day.
2.3. Behavioral Experiment
2.3.1. Experimental paradigm
Subjects were asked to complete the mental transformation test on a computer. The stimulus of hand pictures are shown in Fig. 1. At the beginning of each trial, a cross was presented in the center of the screen for a random duration (the duration ranging from 200 ms to 500 ms) by a commercial visual software (Stim2.0; Neuroscan Inc., USA). Initially the cross disappeared; the stimulus began to present until the subject complete the judgment by pressing the mouse button. Subjects’ task was to mental transformation the stimulus into upright position and judge whether the stimulus is right hand or not. There were six kind of orientation (0°, 60°120°, 180°, 240°, 300°) for both right and left hand pictures. Except for 0° and 180° stimulus repeated 60 times during the whole test, the other types stimulus all repeated 30 times. The sequences of trials were random by the computer.
2.3.2. Analysis of behavioral data
Those trials which were rightly responded were accepted for the further analysis. The reaction time (RT) between stimulus onset and response was averaged across clockwise and counterclockwise orientation angles. For previous studies have demonstrated that there was no statistical differences between counter- and clockwise stimulus orientations in the same angle [19], we merged them into four rotation conditions into the following linear regression analysis. The four conditions are: (i) stimuli not rotated (0°), (ii) 60° rotation (included stimuli rotated 60°/300°), (iii) 120° rotation (included stimuli rotated 120°/240°) and (iv) 180° rotation (included stimuli rotated 180°). Linear regression analysis on raw RTs as a function of the angle of rotation (angle of rotation ranging from 0° to 180°) was carried out. These linear regressions gave us the slope of the regression line. While rotated the same orientations, the higher slope means the subject needs more time to complicate the mental transformation process. Then the inverse of the slope corresponds to the velocity of the mental transformation, that is, if a subject has a higher slope in the regression analysis, he ought to have a slower velocity of the mental transformation.
2.3.3. Statistic
To explore the velocity of the mental transformation differences between the normal condition and simulated microgravity condition, a pair-sample t-test was performed.
2.4. Image Acquisition and Analysis
2.4.1
Data acquisitions. Additionally, all subjects’ image data were collected by an experienced radiologist with a Siemens 3.0T nuclear magnetic resonance equipment in the radiology department of the Xijing Hospital of Fourth Military Medical University. Subjects were told to lay on the scanning platform flatly and place the head to the assigned location cozily. Sponge earplugs were used to plug the ear hole in order to minimize the interference of noises and some foam padding were used to restrict head motion. A standard 16-channels head coil was used to collect the data. The subjects were told to try to keep the head motionless, to keep calm with eyes closed and to not think of anything in particular. (In addition, the data of functional images after 72 hours stimulated weightlessness was collected in head down tilt position. We placed a special foam pad on the platform of the scanner, then the subjects could keep head tilt down position during data collection.) The scanning sequence and parameters were using the following settings: anatomical images for location were T1-weighted images which used spin echo method (SE). TR = 500 ms, TE = 15 ms, flip angle (FA) = 90°, field of view (FOV) = 220 mm×220 mm, matrix = 256×144, slice thickness = 4 mm with no gap. the high definition three dimensional T1-weighted images of the whole brain were used MPRAG sequence. TR = 2530 ms, TE = 3.39 ms, inversion time = 1100 ms, FA = 7°, FOV = 256 mm×256 mm, matrix = 256×192, slice thickness = 1.33 mm with no gap, 128 slices. an echo planner imaging sequence was used to acquire functional images, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, FA = 90°, FOV = 200 mm×200 mm, matrix = 64×64, slice thickness = 3 mm, gap = 0.6 mm, 33 slices, this part of scan contained 240 time points and lasted 8 minutes.
2.4.2. Data preprocessing
Data Processing Assistant for Resting-State fMRI (DPARSF, http://www.restfmri.net/forum/DPARSF) V2.0 was used in data preprocessing [20]. The preprocessing steps were as follows: First, the data format of functional images was transform from DICOM to NFTI. Second, because of the subjects need some time to adjust, the first ten time points were discarded and 230 time points were left. Third, the left functional images were slice-time corrected, and aligned to the first image of each session for motion correction. Four subjects were excluded from the analysis because of excessive head motion (more than 2 mm or 2 degree in any axis). Fourth, the data was then spatially normalized with the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template (resampling voxel size = 3×3×3 mm3)in the Resting-State fMRI Data Analysis Toolkit (REST, www.restfmri.net) v1.0 [21]. Fifth, Linear trend was removed and then the fMRI data were temporally band-pass filtered (0.01 ≤ f ≤ 0.08 Hz) to reduce the low-frequency drift and physiological high-frequency respiratory and cardiac noise. Then head motion, white matter and CSF signals were regressing out before ReHo computation as previous studies suggested [22], [23], [24].
2.4.3. ReHo analysis
Kendall’s coefficient of concordance (KCC) was used to measure regional homogeneity or similarity of the ranked time series of a given voxel with its nearest 26 neighbor voxels in a voxel-wise way [25], [26]:
where W is the KCC among given voxels, ranging from 0 to 1; Ri is the sum rank of the ith time point; = ((n+1)K)/2 is the mean of the Ri; K is the number of time series within a measured cluster (27, one given voxel plus the number of its neighbors); and n is the number of ranks. DPARSF was used to calculate the KCC value of the time series of a given voxel with those of its nearest neighbors (26 voxels) in a voxel-wise analysis. The normalized ReHo map was got by divide the mean KCC value of all voxels from the KCC value of each voxel of the whole brain. Then the ReHo maps were spatially smoothed (4 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel).
2.4.4. Statistics
To explore the ReHo differences between the normal condition and simulated microgravity condition, a pair-sample t-test was performed on the normalized ReHo maps. The resultant statistical map was set at a combined threshold of P<0.005 and a minimum cluster size of 832 mm3 (13 voxels), corresponding to a corrected threshold of P<0.05, as determined by AlphaSim. For presentation purposes, the statistical maps were transformed to Talairach coordinates [27] and superimposed on the higher-resolution anatomical template available in REST. The difference areas found in paired-sample t-test was saved as masks separately in MRIcron (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/mricron). Then they were used to exacting out the mean ReHo values of these areas in Matlab7.1 (http://www.mathworks.cn/products/matlab/). Finally, these mean ReHo values was recorded into SPSS14.0 (http://www-01.ibm.com/software/cn/analytics/spss/products/statistics/) to calculate their correlations with velocity index of mental transformation.
Results
3.1. Behavioral Result in Mental Transformation Test
The mean accuracy in behavioral test under normal condition is 0.976 (SEM = 0.006), the mean accuracy in behavioral test after HDT is 0.973 (SEM = 0.004). There is no significant difference in mean accuracy in behavioral test between these two conditions (P = 0.736). The reaction times in mental transformation test are shown in Fig. 2. Regression analysis suggest that there are significant linear trend between reaction time and stimulus orientation across all subjects (P<0.05). Thus the slopes of the regression lines are valid. Compared to normal condition, subjects showed increased slop of mental transformation with short term simulated microgravity which may indicate declined mental transformation ability in microgravity. But from a statistical point of view, this difference was not significant at 0.05 level.
3.2. ReHo Results
ReHo results between the normal condition and simulated microgravity condition are shown in Fig. 3 and Tab. 1. Compared to normal condition, decreased ReHo was found in right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and left inferior parietal lobule (IPL) in simulated microgravity condition. In addition, increased ReHo were found in bilateral medial frontal gyrus (MFG) and left superior frontal gyrus (SFG) in simulated microgravity condition.
Table 1. Brain areas with significant ReHo differences between stimulated microgravity condition and normal condition.
Brain regions | Brodmann | Voxels size | Talairach coordinates | Peak-t | ||
area | (mm3) | X | Y | Z | value | |
Stimulated microgravity condition< Normal condition | ||||||
right IFG | 47 | 1216 | 17 | 11 | −14 | −5.41 |
left IPL | 40 | 896 | −34 | −53 | 40 | −4.61 |
Stimulated microgravity condition >Normal condition | ||||||
MFG | 10 | 4480 | 9 | 39 | 19 | 7.21 |
left SFG | 9 | 832 | −15 | 40 | 41 | 6.74 |
(A combined threshold of P<0.005 and a minimum cluster size of 832 mm3 (13 voxels), corresponding to a corrected threshold of P<0.05, as determined by AlphaSim).
3.3. The Correlations between ReHo Values and Velocity Index of Mental Transformation
In clusters for which significant group differences had been detected, we also tested for possible correlations between ReHo and velocity index of mental transformation. Finally, there was only a significant correlation between mean ReHo values in left inferior parietal lobule and velocity index of mental transformation in simulated microgravity condition(r = − 0.59, P<0.05; Fig. 4).
Discussion
The major finding of present study is the decreased ReHo in left parietal lobule with short term simulated microgravity and its significant correlation with velocity index of mental transformation. As a good quantitative indicator for reflecting the localized coherence of brain activity, ReHo could indicate the change of neural activities in certain brain areas [17], [28], [29], [30], [31], [32], [33], [34]. Thus, the ReHo changes found in the current study may reflect corresponding function changes, to some extent; it could help to clarify the internal neural mechanisms of mental transformation ability change under microgravity. Additionally, the declined trend of mental transformation ability and changed ReHo values in right inferior frontal gyrus, bilateral medial frontal gyrus and left superior frontal gyrus found with short term simulated microgravity are worthy discussed.
At behavioral level, a declined trend of mental transformation ability was found with short term simulated microgravity. This was accordance with the previous study carried out by Grabherr et al. with parabolic flight model [7]. They owed this result to the disturbed vestibular function induced by the missing of gravity information input. In contrast, the model to simulate microgravity used in the present study is different; the gravity information input still exists in HDT bed rest [12]. This difference may induce weaker damage to subjects in the present study than in Grabherr’s and could partly explain the insignificant between group differences in behavioral data found in the present study. Though it is, the result could support the idea of declined mental transformation ability with microgravity presented by Grabherr et al. in some extent. However, it is worth noting that with the gravity information input still existed in present study, there may be other contributors that contribute to the mental transformation ability change. The behavioral data in the present study is not enough to clarify this issue; we should look for some evidence from the image data.
As the result found in the present study, mean ReHo vaule in left inferior parietal lobule has a significant correlation with the velocity index of mental transformation. Accordingly, the declined function of left inferior parietal lobule revealed by the decreased ReHo in the left inferior parietal lobule with short term microgravity may play a significant role in mental transformation ability change. Amount of previous studies have provided credible evidence to this inference. Initially, Kosslyn et al. found the bilateral inferior parietal lobule activation in the mental rotation of hand pictures by positron emission tomography [35]. Later, an fMRI study carried out by Vingerhoets et al. similarly found the bilateral parietal areas activated during the mental transformation task [36]. Similar finding was reported again by Overney in 2005, they suggested that left parietal regions were strongly activated during mental transformation [37]. Thus, left inferior parietal lobule is with no doubt play an important role in the processing of mental representation (especially the mental representation of body parts, such as hand). The decreased ReHo found in this area with short term microgravity in the present study is ought to reflect a dysfunction there, which may in turn disturb the mental transformation process. Accordingly, the changed mental transformation ability found in the present study could be explained reasonably. Nevertheless, based on the data of the current study, we couldn’t conclude the reason of the dysfunction in left inferior parietal lobule yet. A possible reason is that the redistribution of individual body fluid toward head induced by simulated microgravity may affect the neuron metabolism in the left inferior parietal lobule, but this assumption lack direct evidence to support. An alternative assumption is that the dysfunction of left inferior parietal lobule is caused by the declined vestibular function. There are some indirect evidences to verify it in the previous studies. Several studies have reported the vestibular dysfunction in HDT bed rest [7]. Moreover, Overney et al. ever suggested that the parietal areas need to receive the input information input vestibular during the mental transformation process [37]. While vestibular function declined during simulated microgravity, the vestibular-cortical interactions would be disturbed in sequent, which may accordingly disturb the neural activity in left inferior parietal lobule (which plays an important role in mental transformation process). For now, the latter assumption seems to be more credible, but it is still need to be clarified in the future. Even though, the dysfunction in left inferior lobule with short term simulated microgravity and its relationship with changed mental transformation ability found in the present study is valuable, it will further the insight of brain activity change in microgravity and promote the development of corresponding countermeasures.
Moreover, it is worth paid attention to that declined function in right inferior frontal gyrus and enhanced function in bilateral medial frontal gyrus were also found in the present study. Previous studies [38], [39], [40], [41] have demonstrated that individual frontal cortical functioning was inhibited under microgravity and in turn impair several psychological processes such as inhibitory control etc. But enhanced frontal areas activity in microgravity had been seldom reported. As far as we know, amount of previous findings had suggested that right inferior frontal gyrus and medial frontal gyrus play critical roles in inhibitory control process [42], [43], [44], [45], [46], [47], [48], [49], [50]. Therefore, the enhanced activity of the bilateral medial frontal gyrus found in the current study maybe reflect a compensatory mechanism to the declined inhibitory control process. But due to there is no behavioral data for reference, whether this compensatory mechanism is stronger enough to cancel out the negative effect caused by the abnormal activity of the right inferior frontal gyrus is not clear yet. The relation of these two areas and their impact on inhibitory control is still a problem that worthy be addressed.
In addition, hyperfunction in left superior frontal gyrus after short term simulated microgravity was found in the present study. In previous studies, superior frontal gyrus has been related to multiple cognitive processes, such as task-switching, expression identification and complex motor control [51], [52], [53], [54], [55]l. Due to the lack of corresponding behavioral data, we could hardly clarify the exact role of hyperfunction in left superior frontal gyrus found in the present study; it needs to be addressed in the future studies.
The findings of the present finding are encouraging, but there still exist some limitation that need to be addressed. Firstly, because of technical limitations, the sampling rate (TR = 2 s) used in the data acquisition is low; therefore, simultaneously recording of cardiac rates is impossible [56], [57]. Although we could not eliminate the cardiac rate interference from our data, our findings may seem to be reliable after multiple corrections in some extent. Secondly, those findings of present study are gained from the simulated experiment on earth and there are some limitations when they are extended to microgravity environment. These findings are still needed to be tested in real microgravity environment in the near future.
Conclusion
In conclusion, gravity change may disrupt the function of right inferior frontal gyrus and left inferior parietal lobule in the resting-state, among of which functional change in left inferior parietal lobule may contribute to changed abilities of mental transformation. In addition, the enhanced activity of the bilateral medial frontal gyrus and decreased activity of right inferior frontal gyrus found in the current study maybe reflect a complementation effect on inhibitory control process. However, these results are gained from the simulated experiment on earth and there are some limitations when they are extended to microgravity environment. These findings are still needed to be tested in real microgravity environment in the near future.
Acknowledgments
We sincerely thank volunteers for their participation, and all the medical staff involved in data collecting.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by the National Science Foundation of China, Grant No.81071616 and No.81272174. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Eddy DR, Schiflett SG, Shebab RL (1998) Cognitive performance aboard the life and microgravity spacelab. Acta Astronautica 43: 193–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Morphew M (2001) Psychological and human factors in long duration spaceflight. MJM 6: 74–80. [Google Scholar]
- 3. Palinkas L (2001) Psychosocial issues in long-term space flight: Overview. Gravitational and Space Biology Bulletin 14: 25–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Sandal G (2001) Psychosocial issues in space: Future chalenges. Gravitational and Space Biology Bulletin 14: 47–54. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Grigoriev A, Egorov A (1992) General Mechanisms of the effect of weightlessness on the human body. Advance in Space Biology and Medicine 2: 1–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Jenkin MR, Dyde RT, Jenkin HI, Zacher JE, Harris LR (2011) Perceptual upright: The relative effectiveness of dynamic and static images under different gravity states. Seeing and Perceiving 24: 53–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Grabherr L, Karmali F, Bach S, Indermaur K, Metzler S, et al. (2007) Mental own-body and body-part transformations in microgravity. Journal of Vestibular Research 17: 279–287. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Leone G, Lipshits M, Gurfinkel V, Berthoz A (1995) Influence of graviceptives cues at different level of visual information processing: The effect of prolonged weightlessness. Acta Astronautica 36: 743–751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Matsakis Y, Lipshits M, Gurfinkel V, Berthoz A (1993) Effects of prolonged weightlessness on mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Experimental brain research 94: 152–162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Villard E, Garcia-Moreno FT, Peter N, Clement G (2005) Geometric visual illusions in microgravity during parabolic flight. Neuroreport 16: 1395–1398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Newberg A, Alavi A (1998) Changes in the central nervous system during long-duration space flight: implications for neuro-imaging. Advances in Space Research 22: 185–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Baisch F, Beck L, Karemaker J, Arbeille P, Gaffney F, et al. (1992) Head-down tilt bedrest. HDT'88–an international collaborative effort in integrated systems physiology. Acta physiologica scandinavica Supplementum 604: 1–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Ogawa S, Lee T, Kay A, Tank D (1990) Brain magnetic resonance imaging with contrast dependent on blood oxygenation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 87: 9868–9872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Ogawa S, Lee TM, Nayak AS, Glynn P (1990) Oxygenation-sensitive contrast in magnetic resonance image of rodent brain at high magnetic fields. Magnetic resonance in medicine 14: 68–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Biswal B, Zerrin Yetkin F, Haughton VM, Hyde JS (1995) Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar mri. Magnetic resonance in medicine 34: 537–541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Zou Q, Ross TJ, Gu H, Geng X, Zuo XN, et al.. (2012) Intrinsic resting-state activity predicts working memory brain activation and behavioral performance. Human Brain Mapping: doi: 10.1002/hbm.22136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 17. Zang Y, Jiang T, Lu Y, He Y, Tian L (2004) Regional homogeneity approach to fMRI data analysis. Neuroimage 22: 394–400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Annett M (1970) A classification of hand preference by association analysis. British journal of psychology 61: 303–321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Dalecki M, Hoffmann U, Bock O (2012) Mental rotation of letters, body parts and complex scenes: Separate or common mechanisms? Human movement science 31: 1151–1160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Yan C, Zang Y (2010) DPARSF: a MATLAB toolbox for “pipeline” data analysis of resting-state fMRI. Frontiers in systems neuroscience 4: 13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Song XW, Dong ZY, Long XY, Li SF, Zuo XN, et al. (2011) REST: a toolkit for resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data processing. PLoS One 6: e25031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2012) Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59: 2142–2154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Van Dijk KR, Sabuncu MR, Buckner RL (2012) The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage 59: 431–438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Zuo XN, Xu T, Jiang L, Yang Z, Cao XY, et al. (2013) Toward reliable characterization of functional homogeneity in the human brain: preprocessing, scan duration, imaging resolution and computational space. Neuroimage 65: 374–386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Liang P, Liu Y, Jia X, Duan Y, Yu C, et al. (2011) Regional homogeneity changes in patients with neuromyelitis optica revealed by resting-state functional MRI. Clinical Neurophysiology 122: 121–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kendall MG, Gibbons JD (1990) Rank correlation methods. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- 27.Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain: 3-dimensional proportional system: an approach to cerebral imaging: Thieme.
- 28. You H, Wang J, Wang H, Zang YF, Zheng FL, et al. (2011) Altered regional homogeneity in motor cortices in patients with multiple system atrophy. Neuroscience letters 502: 18–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Qiu C, Liao W, Ding J, Feng Y, Zhu C, et al. (2011) Regional homogeneity changes in social anxiety disorder: a resting-state fMRI study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 194: 47–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Wang D, Liu D, Li S, Zang Y (2012) Increased local synchronization of resting-state fMRI signal after episodic memory encoding reflects off-line memory consolidation. Neuroreport 23: 873–878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Lin X, Ding K, Liu Y, Yan X, Song S, et al. (2012) Altered spontaneous activity in anisometropic amblyopia subjects: revealed by resting-state FMRI. PLoS One 7: e43373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Ni L, Qi R, Zhang LJ, Zhong J, Zheng G, et al. (2012) Altered regional homogeneity in the development of minimal hepatic encephalopathy: a resting-state functional MRI study. PLoS One 7: e42016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Liu D, Yan C, Ren J, Yao L, Kiviniemi VJ, et al. (2010) Using coherence to measure regional homogeneity of resting-state fMRI signal. Frontiers in systems neuroscience 4: 24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Liu CH, Ma X, Li F, Wang YJ, Tie CL, et al. (2012) Regional homogeneity within the default mode network in bipolar depression: a resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging study. PLoS One 7: e48181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Kosslyn SM, Digirolamo GJ, Thompson WL, Alpert NM (1998) Mental rotation of objects versus hands: Neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. Psychophysiology 35: 151–161. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Vingerhoets G, De Lange FP, Vandemaele P, Deblaere K, Achten E (2002) Motor imagery in mental rotation: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 17: 1623–1633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Overney LS, Michel CM, Harris IM, Pegna AJ (2005) Cerebral processes in mental transformations of body parts: recognition prior to rotation. Cognitive brain research 25: 722–734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Benvenuti S, Bianchin M, Angrilli A (2011) Effects of simulated microgravity on brain plasticity: A startle reflex habituation study. Physiology & behavior 104: 503–506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Pattyn N, Migeotte PF, Morais J, Soetens E, Cluydts R, et al. (2009) Crew performance monitoring: Putting some feeling into it. Acta Astronautica 65: 325–329. [Google Scholar]
- 40. Rau H, Pauli P, Brody S, Elbert T, Birbaumer N (1993) Baroreceptor stimulation alters cortical activity. Psychophysiology 30: 322–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Vaitl D, Gruppe H, Stark R, Possel P (1996) Simulated micro-gravity and cortical inhibition: a study of the hemodynamic-brain interaction. Biological psychology 42: 87–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Aron AR, Fletcher PC, Bullmore ET, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW (2003) Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nature neuroscience 6: 115–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA (2004) Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in cognitive sciences 8: 170–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Dehaene S, Artiges E, Naccache L, Martelli C, Viard A, et al. (2003) Conscious and subliminal conflicts in normal subjects and patients with schizophrenia: the role of the anterior cingulate. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100: 13722–13727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Dosenbach NUF, Fair DA, Miezin FM, Cohen AL, Wenger KK, et al. (2007) Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104: 11073–11078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Dosenbach NUF, Visscher KM, Palmer ED, Miezin FM, Wenger KK, et al. (2006) A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron 50: 799–812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Horn N, Dolan M, Elliott R, Deakin J, Woodruff P (2003) Response inhibition and impulsivity: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 41: 1959–1966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S (2004) The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 306: 443–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Rubia K, Smith AB, Brammer MJ, Taylor E (2003) Right inferior prefrontal cortex mediates response inhibition while mesial prefrontal cortex is responsible for error detection. Neuroimage 20: 351–358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, et al. (2007) Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. The Journal of neuroscience 27: 2349–2356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Martino J, Gabarrós A, Deus J, Juncadella M, Acebes J, et al. (2011) Intrasurgical mapping of complex motor function in the superior frontal gyrus. Neuroscience 179: 131–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. McLellan T, Wilcke J, Johnston L, Watts R, Miles L (2012) Sensitivity to posed and genuine displays of happiness and sadness: A fMRI study. Neuroscience letters 531: 149–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Slagter H, Weissman D, Giesbrecht B, Kenemans J, Mangun G, et al. (2006) Brain regions activated by endogenous preparatory set shifting as revealed by fMRI. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 6: 175–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Crone EA, Wendelken C, Donohue SE, Bunge SA (2006) Neural evidence for dissociable components of task-switching. Cerebral Cortex 16: 475–486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Cutini S, Scatturin P, Menon E, Bisiacchi PS, Gamberini L, et al. (2008) Selective activation of the superior frontal gyrus in task-switching: an event-related fNIRS study. Neuroimage 42: 945–955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Liao Y, Zhang JS, Huang ZP, Xi YB, Zhang QR, et al. (2012) Altered baseline brain activity with 72 h of simulated microgravity- Initial evidence from resting-state fMRI. PLoS One 7: e52558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Zang Y, He Y, Zhu C, Cao Q, Sui M, et al. (2007) Altered baseline brain activity in children with ADHD revealed by resting-state functional MRI. Brain & development 29: 83–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]