Abstract
Objective
To identify family and infant characteristics associated with timing of introduction of two food types: core foods (nutrient-dense) and non-core foods (nutrient-poor) in a population-based sample of mothers and infants.
Method
Participants were 1861 mothers and infants from the Gemini twin birth cohort (one child per family). Family and infant characteristics were assessed when the infants were around 8 months old. Timing of introducing core and non-core foods was assessed at 8 and 15 months. As the distributions of timing were skewed, three similar-sized groups were created for each food type: earlier (core: 1–4 months; non-core: 3–8 months), average (core: 5 months; non-core: 9–10 months), and later introduction (core: 6–12 months; non-core: 11–18 months). Ordinal logistic regression was used to examine predictors of core and non-core food introduction, with bootstrapping to test for differences between the core and non-core models.
Results
Younger maternal age, lower education level, and higher maternal BMI were associated with earlier core and non-core food introduction. Not breastfeeding for at least 3 months and higher birth weight were specifically associated with earlier introduction of core foods. Having older children was specifically associated with earlier introduction of non-core foods.
Conclusion
There are similarities and differences in the characteristics associated with earlier introduction of core and non-core foods. Successful interventions may require a combination of approaches to target both food types.
Keywords: Infant feeding, core foods, non-core foods
Introduction
The transition from milk feeding to complementary foods is a critical period during which the developmental and nutritional needs of the infant must be met1. Despite potential health implications, many caregivers introduce solid foods before the recommended age2–4 or introduce non-recommended foods. By one year, a substantial proportion of infants consume foods high in fat, salt, or sugar, and low in nutrient density5,6.
Since 2001, the World Health Organisation has recommended exclusive breastfeeding for the first 6 months of life, followed by introduction of complementary foods alongside continued breastfeeding for up to two years of age or beyond7. Introduction of solid foods before 4 months may result in inadequate nutrient or energy intake due to displacement of milk in the diet, and could impose stress on the immature gastrointestinal, immune, and renal system8,9. Another potential risk is rapid infant weight gain10,11 and increased likelihood of obesity later in life12,13.
Current guidelines recommend that foods high in fat, salt or sugar and low in nutritional value should not be given in the first year of life14,15. Such foods have been described as ‘non-core’ or superfluous to a balanced diet16–18. A diet based on foods from the five ‘core’ food groups (carbohydrate-rich; vegetables; fruits; dairy products; high protein) meets essential nutrient requirements, and ‘non-core’ foods such as confectionery or savoury snacks are unnecessary18.
Studies to date have identified a range of family and infant characteristics associated with early solid food introduction, including socioeconomic status, education level, breastfeeding duration, perceived infant hunger, and infant temperament, size and gender19–23. However, relatively little attention has been paid to the timing of different food types. This is particularly relevant given the evidence that infancy is an important time for development of eating behaviours and infant dietary patterns track into childhood and beyond24–26. Foods given in this period can result in lasting taste preferences after as little as one exposure27 and have been linked to later food preference and consumption28–30.
Because infants’ first foods tend to be cereals, fruits, and vegetables2,31,32, which are core foods, studies of the timing of solid food introduction usually reflect introduction of core foods. In the few studies that have examined factors associated with non-core foods33,34, earlier age of any solid food introduction and presence of older children in the home predicted earlier intake, but there have been no specific comparisons with timing of core foods. In older children there is evidence of differential predictors of core and non-core food consumption35,36.
The aims of the present study were therefore to i) examine family and infant predictors of the timing of core and non-core food introduction, and ii) test whether different factors were associated with earlier introduction of core and non-core foods.
Methods
Sample
Data were from parent-infant dyads (one infant randomly selected per family) in the Gemini twin birth cohort (n=2402); a study of genetic and environmental influences on early weight gain37. The Gemini sample was recruited by the Office of National Statistics and included 36% of all eligible families with twin births in England and Wales during the recruitment period in 2007. Ethical approval was granted by the University College London Committee for the Ethics of non-National Health Service Human Research. The geographical distribution of participating families reflects the UK population density and the cohort is representative of national twin statistics for sex, zygosity, birth weight, and gestational age at birth37,38.
Family and infant characteristics were assessed when the children were on average 8.2 months old (SD=2.2, range 4–20). Questions on solid food timing were asked when the children were on average 8.2 and 15.8 months old (SD=1.1, range 14–27). The solid food questions were repeated in a second questionnaire because 55% of infants had not yet tried non-core foods at the time of the first questionnaire. Full data were available for 1861 parent-infant dyads.
Measures
Timing of core and non-core food introduction
In each questionnaire, parents were asked whether the infant had tried any of a list of foods and the age at which they first tried them. Core and non-core food groups were based on the Australian dietary guidelines18. Core foods were: baby rice, cereal, rusks or bread; fruit (fresh, frozen or tinned); vegetables (fresh, frozen or tinned); potatoes; meat (e.g. chicken, lamb, pork, beef); fish (fresh, frozen, or tinned); eggs; and dairy products (e.g. milk, cheese, yoghurt). Non-core foods were: savoury snacks (e.g. crisps, cheese biscuits); fried potatoes; processed meat (e.g. sausages, burger); sweet snacks (e.g. cakes, biscuits, ice cream); and confectionary (e.g. chocolate, fruit sweets). Age of core and non-core food introduction was taken as the earliest age (in months) that the infant had first tried each food type. Where available, responses were taken from the 8 month questionnaire. If responses were not available at baseline (e.g. if non-core foods had not been tried), data were from the 15 month questionnaire. This ensured that responses were given closer to the time of actual core and non-core food introduction. Using the two assessment points, mothers reported the timing of core food introduction on average 3.7 months (SD = 2.5, range = 0 – 16) after the event, and the timing of non-core food introduction on average 5.1 months (SD = 2.9, range = 0 – 18) after the event.
The distributions of timing of core and non-core food introduction were skewed, therefore three similar-sized groups were created for each food type: earlier (core: 1–4 months; non-core: 3–8 months), average (core: 5 months; non-core: 9–10 months) and later introduction (core: 6–12 months; non-core: 11– 18 months).
Potential predictors of timing
Family characteristics included maternal age, education, BMI, presence of older children in the home, and duration of breastfeeding. Maternal education was categorised as high (university education), intermediate (vocational or advanced high-school education), or low (no qualifications or basic high-school education). Self-reported height and weight were used to calculate maternal BMI (weight/height2). Presence of older children in the home was assessed using the question: ‘How many other children live in the home with your twins?’ This variable was categorised as none or one or more older children. Breastfeeding was assessed using two questions: ‘Which feeding method did you use in the first three months’ (response options: entirely breastfeeding, mostly breastfeeding with some bottle-feeding; equally breastfeeding and bottle-feeding; mostly bottle-feeding with some breastfeeding; almost entirely bottle-feeding; and entirely bottle-feeding); and ‘If you are no longer breastfeeding, when did you stop’ (response options: number of weeks after birth). Responses to the breastfeeding questions were categorised as: i) mothers who at least partly breastfed for at least 3 months and ii) mothers who never breastfed or stopped before 3 months.
Infant characteristics included birth weight, gender and a rating of appetite. Birth weights were taken by health professionals and recorded in the child’s personal health record. Parents were asked to photocopy the relevant pages of the health records or copy available measurements into the baseline questionnaire. Birth weight standard deviation scores (SDS) were calculated adjusting for exact age, sex and gestational age using British 1990 growth reference data and the LMS growth macro for excel39,40. Infant appetite was assessed using the item ‘How would you rate your twin’s appetite in the first 3 months’; measured on a 5-point scale (from 1=poor to 5=excellent). This measure was used to capture general appetite and has been validated against the Baby Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (BEBQ), a comprehensive measure of appetite during the milk feeding period41.
Statistical analyses
Univariate ordinal logistic regression was used to assess relationships between each family/infant variable and the timing of core and non-core foods. All variables were included in the multivariate models to determine which were independently associated with timing of core and non-core foods. To ensure equal numbers in the core and non-core models, cases with missing data on any of the study variables were excluded. Bootstrapping with 9999 iterations was used to test for differences between the regression coefficients for each family/infant characteristic in the core and non-core models42. SPSS 18.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used for the ordinal logistic regression analyses, and Stata version 10.1 (Stata Corp., College Station, TX, USA) was used for the bootstrapping.
Results
The total Gemini sample was 2402 families; 541 were excluded due to missing values on one or more of the study variables. There were no significant differences between the full sample (n=2402) and the sample used for analysis (n=1861) on any study variables (data not shown).
Characteristics of the study sample are included in Table 1. Maternal characteristics only are included as in 99% of cases, the mother was the child’s primary caregiver. Mothers had an average age of 33 years, a mean BMI of 25.1, 95% were married or cohabiting, and 45% were university educated. Almost a third of mothers had breastfed their babies for at least three months. Almost half (48%) had one or more older children. Infant characteristics are also shown in Table 1. Half the children were male, mean gestational age was 36.3 weeks, and mean birth weight SDS was −0.55; as expected for a twin population. The mean appetite score (3.5) was just above the mid-point of the scale.
Table 1.
Characteristics of the total sample (N = 1861, mean (SD) unless stated otherwise)
Family characteristics | |
Maternal age, yrs. | 33.86 (5.08) |
Marital status, n (%) | |
Married or cohabiting | 1774 (95.3) |
Divorced or separated | 16 (0.9) |
Single | 70 (3.8) |
Unknown | 1 (0.1) |
Maternal educational level, n (%) | |
High | 831 (44.7) |
Intermediate | 665 (35.7) |
Low | 365 (19.6) |
Maternal BMI | 25.07 (4.70) |
Breastfeeding, n (%) | |
At least 3 months | 581 (31.2) |
Never or stopped before 3 months | 1280 (68.8) |
Number of older children, n (%) | |
0 | 969 (52.1) |
1 or more | 892 (47.9) |
Infant characteristics | |
Gestational age, wks. | 36.30 (2.37) |
Gender, n (%) | |
Female | 928 (49.9) |
Male | 933 (50.1) |
Birth weight SDS | −0.55 (0.94) |
Appetite rating | 3.52 (1.16) |
Core food introduction | |
1 – 4 months | 567 (30.5) |
5 months | 638 (34.3) |
6 – 12 months | 656 (35.2) |
Non-core food introduction | |
3 – 8 months | 854 (45.9) |
9 – 10 months | 511 (27.5) |
11 – 18 months | 496 (26.7) |
The average age for introduction of core foods was 5.04 months (SD=1.03), and for non-core foods was 8.98 months (SD=2.34). On average, infants were introduced to non-core foods 3.9 months after being introduced to core foods; only 2.8% tried both at the same age; and less than 1% started non-core before core foods. At the time of the second questionnaire (average infant age=15.8 months), all infants had tried core food and 98.5% had tried non-core foods.
Almost a third of infants (30.5%) started core foods between 1 and 4 months, 34% at 5 months, and 35% after 6 months. Most of the infants who were introduced to core foods ‘later’ started at 6 months; only 3.5% started after 6 months. Almost half (46%) the infants were introduced to non-core foods between 3 and 8 months, 28% between 9 and 10 months, and 27% after 11 months. Figure 1 shows stronger clustering of the timing of core than non-core foods.
Figure 1.
Cumulative percentage of infants introduced to core and non-core foods by infant age.
Predictors of core food introduction
Lower maternal age and education, higher maternal BMI, and less than 3 months breastfeeding were associated with earlier core food introduction. Presence of older children in the home was not associated with timing of core foods. Infant characteristics associated with earlier core food introduction were: male gender, higher birth weight SDS, and a larger appetite. All characteristics associated with timing of core foods in univariate models remained significant in the multivariate model (see Table 2).
Table 2.
Multivariate analyses offamily and infant characteristics associated with earlier core and non-core food introduction1 (N = 1861)
Core food introduction | Non-core food introduction | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Adj OR | 95% CI (p value) | Adj OR | 95% CI (p value) | |
Family characteristics | ||||
Maternal age, yrs. | 0.93 | 0.91 – 0.95 (<0.001) | 0.94 | 0.93 – 0.96 (<0.001) |
Maternal education level | ||||
High | 1 | – | 1 | – |
Intermediate | 1.26 | 1.03 – 1.54 (0.024) | 1.27 | 1.04 – 1.56 (0.020) |
Low | 1.49 | 1.17 – 1.90 (0.001) | 1.34 | 1.04 – 1.71 (0.022) |
Maternal BMI (per unit increase) |
1.05 | 1.03 – 1.07 (<0.001) | 1.04 | 1.02 – 1.06 (<0.001) |
Breastfeeding | ||||
At least 3 months | 1 | – | 1 | – |
Never or stopped before 3 months |
1.48 | 1.21 – 1.80 (<0.001) | 1.13 | 0.93 – 1.38 (0.206) |
Number of older children | ||||
0 | 1 | – | 1 | – |
1 or more | 1.11 | 0.93 – 1.33 (0.228) | 1.35 | 1.13 – 1.61 (0.001) |
Infant characteristics | ||||
Gender | ||||
Female | 1 | – | ||
Male | 1.26 | 1.06 – 1.50 (0.008) | 1.11 | 0.93 – 1.32 (0.230) |
Birth weight SDS | 1.13 | 1.03 – 1.24 (0.009) | 0.95 | 0.87 – 1.04 (0.298) |
Appetite rating | 1.09 | 1.01 – 1.18 (0.024) | 1.03 | 0.96 – 1.11 (0.424) |
For the purposes of analyses, timing of core food introduction was coded as: 1 = later (6 – 12 months), 2 = average (5 months), and 3 = earlier (1 – 4 months). Timing of non-core food introduction was coded as: 1 = later (11 – 18 months), 2 = average (9 – 10 months), and 3 = earlier (3 – 8 months)
Adj OR = odds ratio adjusted for all listed variables; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval; 1 denotes the reference group; BMI = body mass index; Birth weight SDS = Weight standard deviation scores at birth calculated using 1990 UK weight reference data 43
Predictors of non-core food introduction
Lower maternal age and education, higher maternal BMI, less than three months breastfeeding, and older children in the home were associated with earlier non-core food introduction. None of the infant characteristics were related to timing of non-core food introduction. All characteristics associated with timing of non-core food introduction in univariate models, except for breastfeeding, remained significant in the multivariate model (see Table 2).
Because starting core foods preceded but correlated with timing of non-core foods (r=0.28), the non-core multivariate model was repeated including timing of core foods as an additional predictor (see Table 3). In this model, maternal education became borderline significant, but other effects remained the same.
Table 3.
Family and infant characteristics associated with earlier non-core food introduction1, including core food introduction as a covariate(N = 1861)
Adj OR | 95% CI (p value) | |
---|---|---|
Family characteristics | ||
Maternal age, yrs. | 0.96 | 0.94 – 0.98 (<0.001) |
Maternal education level | ||
High | 1 | – |
Intermediate | 1.22 | 1.00 – 1.50 (0.055) |
Low | 1.24 | 0.96 – 1.59 (0.093) |
Maternal BMI (per unit increase) | 1.03 | 1.01 – 1.05 (0.005) |
Breastfeeding | ||
At least 3 months | 1 | – |
Never or stopped before 3 months |
1.03 | 0.85 – 1.26 (0.758) |
Number of older children | ||
0 | 1 | – |
1 or more | 1.34 | 1.12 – 1.60 (0.001) |
Infant characteristics | ||
Gender | ||
Female | 1 | – |
Male | 1.05 | 0.88 – 1.25 (0.598) |
Birth weight SDS | 0.92 | 0.84 – 1.02 (0.101) |
Appetite rating | 1.02 | 0.94 – 1.10 (0.670) |
Core food introduction | ||
Later | 1 | – |
Average | 1.87 | 1.52 – 2.30 (<0.001) |
Earlier | 3.20 | 2.54 – 4.03 (<0.001) |
For the purposes of analyses, timing of core food introduction was coded as: 1 = later (6 – 12 months), 2 = average (5 months), and 3 = earlier (1 – 4 months). Timing of non-core food introduction was coded as: 1 = later (11 – 18 months), 2 = average (9 – 10 months), and 3 = earlier (3 – 8 months)
Adj OR = odds ratio adjusted for all listed variables; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval; 1 denotes the reference group
To examine the influence of postnatal growth on timing of core and non-core food introduction, the multivariate models were repeated replacing birth weight with 0–3 month weight gain. As for birth weight, greater 0–3 month weight gain was significantly associated with earlier core food introduction (OR=1.20, 95% CI=1.10–1.30, p < 0.001), and not with non-core food introduction (p=0.884). All other effects in the models remained the same as in the analyses using birth weight.
Differences between predictors of core and non-core food introduction
Bootstrapping was used to directly compare predictors of core and non-core food timing. Shorter duration of breastfeeding and higher birth weight SDS had significantly stronger associations with earlier core than non-core food introduction (p=0.030 and p=0.002, respectively). Presence of older children had a borderline significantly stronger association with earlier non-core than core food introduction (p=0.089). For other characteristics, there were no significant differences between the core and non-core models.
Discussion
This is the first study to directly compare family and infant characteristics associated with the timing of core and non-core food introduction. Consistent with recent surveys3,4, a substantial proportion of infants were given core foods earlier than currently recommended (i.e. six months). Non-core foods were introduced on average four months later than core foods, but nonetheless, three quarters of infants had non-core foods in the first year of life; contrary to current guidelines14,15.
The findings of this study are largely consistent with previous research on predictors of general solid food introduction19–23. Younger, less educated, heavier mothers who breastfed for less than three months introduced core foods earlier. Infants who were male, heavier, and had a heartier appetite were started on core foods earlier. This consistency with previous research is likely explained by the fact that the first foods introduced to infants tend to be core foods31,32; but given the potential role of non-core foods in excess weight, it is important to examine both food types.
Using bootstrapping, we could directly compare predictors of core and non-core food introduction and showed that higher maternal BMI and lower maternal age and education predicted earlier introduction of both core and non-core foods. Shorter breastfeeding duration and higher birth weight were associated with earlier core but not non-core food introduction. The specific association between shorter breastfeeding and earlier core food introduction is perhaps unsurprising given that core foods tend to be the first foods following milk feeding31,32. We had anticipated that infant size would relate to both food types, particularly if it is reflected in general food-oriented behaviour43,44. The finding was confirmed using weight gain from birth to three months. It is possible that weight beyond three months is relevant to consumption patterns of non-core food but not to the timing of introduction. Having older children showed a trend towards a stronger association with non-core than core food introduction. This may be because older children in the home provide more opportunities for the infant to be exposed to non-core foods. In addition, siblings may be significant role models for development of food habits and preferences26.
Understanding the similarities and differences in predictors of nutrient-dense and nutrient-poor foods may help to target infant feeding advice. For example, less educated families may need advice related to both core and non-core food introduction, but for infants who are breastfed for less than three months the risk of early introduction is primarily for core foods. Advising parents on how to deal with older children’s non-core food consumption may benefit strategies targeting introduction of these foods.
The strengths of this study include having a large, population-based sample, a range of family and infant characteristics, examination of core and non-core foods, and birth weight measurements taken by health professionals. Although some evidence suggests that feeding practices can differ between twin and singleton infants45, there is no direct evidence to suggest that the study findings would significantly differ in singletons. In contrast, research has shown that several factors associated with infant feeding practices, such as maternal age and education, are the same for twins and singletons46, and the findings of this study replicate previous research using different samples19–23. Measures of timing of solid food introduction were self-report, as in other studies in this area. As the data were collected retrospectively, there may be memory errors and bias; although collecting data at two times meant that parents reported their feeding habits closer to the time that they began using new foods. Research has demonstrated the validity of infant feeding practices recalled at 18 months postpartum47; and studies using retrospective reports of solid food introduction have replicated findings from concurrent data48. The cut-offs used to create the different timing groups were based on the distributions in this dataset, rather than recommended guidelines for optimal timing. This was because the timings were skewed and creating three similar sized groups produced a more detailed outcome measure that took into account the range of responses. In practice, the groups are close to the existing guidance (delay any solid food introduction until six months and avoid non-core food introduction in the first year of life) and results do not differ from studies that used other cut-offs to examine predictors of general solid food introduction3,22,23,49.
There is a still a need for clarification about the optimal timing of solid food introduction and the food types to be offered at different stages of weaning. This is important to ensure consistency for research purposes and so that caregivers can confidently use the guidelines in practice. In particular, high-quality randomised studies are needed to determine the long-term health outcomes of early introduction of core and non-core foods. There is evidence that early exposure to certain foods may increase preferences and later consumption27–29, which would have long-term health consequences. However, while there is evidence linking health outcomes to the timing of general solid food introduction10-13, few studies have focused on specific food groups, so it is unclear whether the health effects relate to core foods, non-core foods, or both. In terms of timing, adverse health outcomes have been associated with introduction to solid foods before four months; whereas evidence for adverse health outcomes with introduction between four and six months is limited50. This may be because few studies have considered a range of cut-offs or food types.
Conclusion
These findings suggest that parents who introduce core foods early into their infant’s diet tend also to introduce non-core foods early. Common predictors of core and non-core food introduction include maternal demographics. Infant characteristics were more strongly associated with core food introduction, whereas the presence of older siblings may be relevant to non-core food introduction. To ensure that all infants get the best nutritional start in life, strategies will need to consider parents, infants, and siblings.
Acknowledgements
The Gemini Study was funded by a grant from Cancer Research UK to Professor Wardle (C1418/A7974). Cancer Research UK had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis and interpretation of data, and preparation, review or approval of the manuscript. We would like to thank Dr David Boniface and Dr Chloe Taylor for their statistical support.
Abbreviations
- OR
Odds ratio
- CI
Confidence interval
- SDS
Standard deviation score
Footnotes
Financial disclosure: Nothing to disclose.
Conflicts of interest: None.
Author contributions Ms Schrempft, Dr van Jaarsveld, and Professor Wardle contributed to the conception and design of the study. Ms Schrempft conducted the analyses, with guidance and feedback from Dr van Jaarsveld and Professor Wardle. Ms Schrempft drafted and revised the manuscript with feedback from Dr van Jaarsveld, Dr Fisher and Professor Wardle. Each author approved the final version.
References
- 1.Illingworth RS, Lister J. The critical or sensitive period, with special reference to certain feeding problems in infants and children. Journal of Pediatr. 1964;65:839–848. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(64)80006-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Freeman V, Van’t Hof M, Haschke F. Patterns of milk and food intake in infants from birth to age 36 months: the Euro-growth study. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 2000;31(Suppl 1):S76–85. doi: 10.1097/00005176-200007001-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bolling K, Grant C, Hamlyn B, Thornton A. Infant feeding survey 2005. A survey conducted on behalf of The Information Centre for health and social care and the UK Health Departments by BMRB Social Research. Department of Health, Social Services and Public Safety; London, United Kingdom: 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Schiess S, Grote V, Scaglioni S, Luque V, Martin F, Stolarczyk A, et al. Introduction of complementary feeding in 5 European countries. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 2010;50:92–98. doi: 10.1097/MPG.0b013e31819f1ddc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Cribb VL, Warren JM, Emmett PM. Contribution of inappropriate complementary foods to the salt intake of 8-month-old infants. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2012;66:104–110. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2011.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Fox MK, Pac S, Devaney B, Jankowski L. Feeding infants and toddlers study: what foods are infants and toddlers eating? J Am Diet Assoc. 2004;104:s22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2003.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.World Health Organisation . Infant and Young Child Nutrition. Fifty-fourth World Health Assembly no. 54·2. WHO; Geneva: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Naylor AJ, Morrow AL. Developmental readiness of normal full term infants to progress from exclusive breastfeeding to the introduction of complementary foods: reviews of the relevant literature concerning infant immunologic, gastrointestinal, oral motor and maternal reproductive and lactational development. Wellstart International and the LINKAGES Project Academy for Educational Development; Washington DC: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Wilson AC, Forsyth JS, Greene SA, Irvine L, Hau C, Howie PW. Relation of infant diet to childhood health: seven year follow up of cohort of children in Dundee infant feeding study. BMJ. 1998;316:21–25. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7124.21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Baker JL, Michaelsen KF, Rasmussen KM, Sørensen TI. Maternal prepregnant body mass index, duration of breastfeeding, and timing of complementary food introduction are associated with infant weight gain. Am J Clin Nutr. 2004;80:1579–1588. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/80.6.1579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Sloan S, Gildea A, Stewart M, Sneddon H, Iwaniec D. Early weaning is related to weight and rate of weight gain in infancy. Child Care Hlth Dev. 2008;34:59–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2007.00771.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ong KK, Loos RJF. Rapid infancy weight gain and subsequent obesity: systematic reviews and hopeful suggestions. Acta Paediatr. 2006;95:904–908. doi: 10.1080/08035250600719754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Schack-Nielsen L, Sørensen TI, Mortensen EL, Michaelsen KF. Late introduction of complementary feeding, rather than duration of breastfeeding, may protect against adult overweight. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;91:619–627. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.27078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Department of Health . Weaning and The Weaning Diet. no 45. HMSO; London: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 15.United States Department of Agriculture (USDA) Infant Nutrition and Feeding: A Guide for Use in the WIC and CSF Programs. USDA; Washington, DC: 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bell AC, Kremer PJ, Magarey AM, Swinburn BA. Contribution of ‘noncore’ foods and beverages to the energy intake and weight status of Australian children. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2005;59:639–645. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rangan AM, Randall D, Hector DJ, Gill TP, Webb KL. Consumption of ‘extra’ foods by Australian children: types, quantities and contribution to energy and nutrient intakes. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2008;62:356–364. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Smith A, Kellet E, Schmerlaib Y. The Australian Guide to Healthy Eating. Commonwealth Department of Health and Family Services; Canberra: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Anderson AS, Guthrie C, Alder EM, Forsyth S, Howie PW, Williams FLR. Rattling the plate--reasons and rationales for early weaning. Health Educ Res. 2001;16:471–479. doi: 10.1093/her/16.4.471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Wasser H, Bentley M, Borja J, Goldman DG, Thompson A, Slining M, Adair L. Infants perceived as ‘fussy’ are more likely to receive complementary foods before 4 months. Pediatrics. 2011;127:229–237. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-0166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Fewtrell MS, Lucas A, Morgan JB. Factors associated with weaning in full term and preterm infants. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2003;88:F296–F301. doi: 10.1136/fn.88.4.F296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lande B, Andersen LF, Baerug A, Trygg KU, Lund-Larsen K, Veierød MB, et al. Infant feeding practices and associated factors in the first six months of life: the Norwegian infant nutrition survey. Acta Paediatr. 2003;92:152–161. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2003.tb00519.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Scott JA, Binns CW, Graham KI, Oddy WH. Predictors of the early introduction of solid foods in infants: results of a cohort study. BMC Pediatr. 2009;9 doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-9-60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Bounds W, Ziegler P, Reidy K. Do food-related experiences in the first 2 years of life predict dietary variety in school-aged children? J Nutr Educ Behav. 2002;34:310–315. doi: 10.1016/s1499-4046(06)60113-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wang Y, Bentley ME, Zhai F, Popkin BM. Tracking of dietary intake patterns of Chinese from childhood to adolescence over a six-year follow-up period. J Nutr. 2002;132:430–438. doi: 10.1093/jn/132.3.430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Birch LL, Fisher JO. Development of eating behaviors among children and adolescents. Pediatrics. 1998;101:539–549. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Birch LL, Gunder L, Grimm-Thomas K, Laing DG. Infants’ consumption of a new food enhances acceptance of similar foods. Appetite. 1998;30:283–295. doi: 10.1006/appe.1997.0146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Beauchamp GK, Moran M. Dietary experience and sweet taste preference in human infants. Appetite. 1982;3:139–152. doi: 10.1016/s0195-6663(82)80007-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Cooke LJ, Wardle J, Gibson EL, Sapochnik M, Sheiham A, Lawson M. Demographic, familial and trait predictors of fruit and vegetable consumption by pre-school children. Public Health Nutr. 2004;7:295–302. doi: 10.1079/PHN2003527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Coulthard H, Harris G, Emmett P. Long-term consequences of early fruit and vegetable feeding practices in the United Kingdom. Public Health Nutr. 2010;13:2044–2051. doi: 10.1017/S1368980010000790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Giovannini M, Riva E, Banderali G, Scaglioni S, Veehof SH, Sala M, et al. Feeding practices of infants through the first year of life in Italy. Acta Paediatr. 2004;93:492–497. doi: 10.1080/08035250410025591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Norris FJ, Larkin MS, Williams CM, Hampton SM, Morgan JB. Factors affecting the introduction of complementary foods in the preterm infant. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2002;56:448–454. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Grummer-Strawn LM, Scanlon KS, Fein SB. Infant feeding and feeding transitions during the first year of life. Pediatrics. 2008;122:S36–S42. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1315d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Koh GA, Scott JA, Oddy WH, Graham KI, Binns CW. Exposure to non-core foods and beverages in the first year of life: results from a cohort study. Nutr Diet. 2010;67:137–142. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Johnson L, Van Jaarsveld CHM, Wardle J. Individual and family environment correlates differ for consumption of core and non-core foods in children. Br J Nutr. 2011;105:950–959. doi: 10.1017/S0007114510004484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.McGowan L, Croker H, Wardle J, Cooke LJ. Environmental and individual determinants of core and non-core food and drink intake in preschool-aged children in the United Kingdom. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2012;66:322–328. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2011.224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Van Jaarsveld CHM, Johnson L, Llewellyn C, Wardle J. Gemini: a UK twin birth cohort with a focus on early childhood weight trajectories, appetite and the family environment. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2010;13:72–78. doi: 10.1375/twin.13.1.72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Office for National Statistics . Review of the Registrar General on births and patterns of family building in England and Wales. National Statistics; Newport, UK: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Freeman JV, Cole TJ, Chinn S, Jones PR, White EM, Preece MA. Cross sectional stature and weight reference curves for the UK, 1990. Arch Dis Child. 1995;73:17–24. doi: 10.1136/adc.73.1.17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Cole TJ. Software for LMS method: LMS Growth PC. http://homepage.mac.com/tjcole/FileSharing1.html.
- 41.Llewellyn CH, Van Jaarsveld CHM, Johnson L, Carnell S, Wardle J. Development and factor structure of the Baby Eating Behaviour Questionnaire in the Gemini birth cohort. Appetite. 2011;57:388–396. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.05.324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Carpenter J, Bithell J. Bootstrap confidence intervals: when, which, what? A practical guide for medical statisticians. Stat Med. 2000;19:1141–1164. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0258(20000515)19:9<1141::aid-sim479>3.0.co;2-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Schachter S. Obesity and eating. Internal and external cues differentially affect the eating behavior of obese and normal subjects. Science. 1968;161:751–756. doi: 10.1126/science.161.3843.751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Johnson SL, Birch LL. Parents’ and children’s adiposity and eating style. Pediatrics. 1994;94:653–661. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Geraghty SR, Pinney SM, Sethuraman G, Roy-Chaudhury A, Kalkwarf HJ. Breast milk feeding rates of mothers of multiples compared to mothers of singletons. Ambul Pediatr. 2004;4:226–231. doi: 10.1367/A03-165R1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Östlund Å , Nordström M, Dykes F, Flacking R. Breastfeeding in preterm and term twins—maternal factors associated with early cessation: a population-based study. J Hum Lact. 2010;26:235–241. doi: 10.1177/0890334409359627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Launer LJ, Forman MR, Hundt GL, Sarov B, Chang D, Berendes HW, et al. Maternal recall of infant feeding events is accurate. J Epidemiol Commun H. 1992;46:203–206. doi: 10.1136/jech.46.3.203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Griffiths LJ, Smeeth L, Hawkins SS, Cole TJ, Dezateux C. Effects of infant feeding practice on weight gain from birth to 3 years. Arch Dis Child. 2009;94:577–582. doi: 10.1136/adc.2008.137554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Tarrant R, Younger K, Sheridan-Pereira M, White M, Kearney J. Factors associated with weaning practices in term infants: a prospective observational study in Ireland. Br J Nutr. 2010;104:1544–1554. doi: 10.1017/S0007114510002412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.European Food Safety Authority (EFSA) Panel on Dietetic Products. Nutrition and Allergies (NDA) Scientific Opinion on the appropriate age for introduction of complementary feeding of infants. EFSA Journal. 2009;7:1423. [Google Scholar]