Abstract
Odors elicit spatiotemporal patterns of activity in the brain. Spatial patterns arise from the specificity of the interaction between odorants and odorant receptors expressed in different olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs), but the origin of temporal patterns of activity and their role in odor coding remain unclear. We investigate how physiological aspects of ORN response and physical aspects of odor stimuli give rise to diverse responses in Drosophila ORNs. We show that odor stimuli have intrinsic dynamics that depend on odor type and strongly affect ORN response. Using linear–nonlinear modeling to remove the contribution of the stimulus dynamics from the ORN dynamics, we study the physiological properties of the response to different odorants and concentrations. For several odorants and receptor types, the ORN response dynamics normalized by the peak response are independent of stimulus intensity for a large portion of the dynamic range of the neuron. Adaptation to a background odor changes the gain and dynamic range of the response but does not affect normalized response dynamics. Stimulating ORNs with various odorants reveals significant odor-dependent delays in the ORN response functions. However, these differences can be dominated by differences in stimulus dynamics. In one case the response of one ORN to two odorants is predicted solely from measurements of the odor signals. Within a large portion of their dynamic range, ORNs can capture information about stimulus dynamics independently from intensity while introducing odor-dependent delays. How insects might use odor-specific stimulus dynamics and ORN dynamics in discrimination and navigation tasks remains an open question.
Introduction
Insects and vertebrates olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) project to segregated regions of the brain called glomeruli, in which they make synaptic connections with second-order neurons according to the odorant receptor they express (Su et al., 2009). Most odorants elicit distinct patterns of activity in the population of ORNs and in the glomeruli, suggesting that odors are encoded in spatial combinatorial maps (Malnic et al., 1999; Duchamp-Viret et al., 2000; Hallem and Carlson, 2006).
In locusts, flies, and moths, odor-evoked firing patterns are organized not only in space but also in time (Stopfer et al., 2003; Daly et al., 2004; Lei et al., 2004; Wilson et al., 2004). Fruit flies with only one functional class of ORNs can discriminate between odors (DasGupta and Waddell, 2008). These and other studies in vertebrates (Mozell, 1964b; Friedrich and Laurent, 2001; Spors et al., 2006; Schaefer and Margrie, 2007; Junek et al., 2010; Smear et al., 2011) suggested that information about odor stimuli might be encoded in temporal patterns in addition to spatial patterns of neural activity. In the locust, independent information about identity and intensity of odor stimuli can be extracted from the analysis of spatiotemporal patterns of activity of projection neurons (PNs; the neural targets of ORN) (Stopfer et al., 2003), even in the presence of temporally structured stimuli (Brown et al., 2005). Temporally diverse PN responses likely originate from temporally diverse ORN responses (Raman et al., 2010), raising the question of what determines the dynamics of the ORN responses.
In Drosophila, the diversity of ORN response dynamics has been attributed to the kinetics of interaction between odorants and receptors (Hallem and Carlson, 2006; Nagel and Wilson, 2011). However, characterization of the ORN response dynamics has been complicated by their dependency on odor concentration (Hallem and Carlson, 2006). Moreover, with few exceptions (Nagel and Wilson, 2011), previous studies have assumed that stimulus dynamics are independent of odor identity, but recent results show that response dynamics to a mixture of excitatory and inhibitory odorants might be affected by the odor-specific dynamics of the stimulus itself (Su et al., 2011) and depend on odor-dependent physico-chemical processes (Andersson et al., 2012). Hence, odor-receptor identity, odorant concentration, and odor-specific stimulus dynamics may all contribute to ORN response dynamics.
We combined single-sensillum recordings from the fly antennae and maxillary palps with direct measurements of the time-dependent concentration of the odor. To separate physiological aspects of ORN response from physical aspect of the odor-dependent stimulus dynamics, we used linear–nonlinear (LN) modeling. We found that, for a large portion of the ORN dynamic range, response dynamics tend to be phasic and independent of stimulus and background intensities. Near the bottom of the dynamic range, ORN response dynamics tend to be more tonic but transition to phasic as odor concentration is increased. Saturating stimuli tend to elicit prolonged responses. Removal of stimulus dynamics from ORN response revealed that different odorants induce different response delays (up to 18 ms in our dataset) and only marginally affect the shape of the response function.
Materials and Methods
Fly stocks.
Recordings from the ORNs ab3A, ab2A, ab7A, and pb1A were performed on adult female Canton-S flies 5 d after eclosion. To facilitate sorting of ab3B spikes, we killed the ab3A neuron by expressing UAS–reaper (w; UAS–rpr; Or22a–GAL4) (Su et al., 2011); recordings were performed on adult females flies 7 d after eclosion.
Electrophysiology.
Single sensillum recordings were performed as described previously (de Bruyne et al., 2001). Electrical signal was amplified using an Iso-DAM amplifier (World Precision Instruments), bandpass filtered (300 Hz to 2kHz), digitized at 10 kHz (NI-USB6221 digital acquisition board), and acquired in LabView. Spikes were sorted using a custom MATLAB routine.
Odor delivery.
All odor dilutions were prepared in paraffin oil (volumetric ratio). (1) For single puffs (see Figs. 1, 3, 5), as described previously (de Bruyne et al., 2001), 50 μl of odor dilution was placed in a paper filter contained in a Pasteur pipette; a three-way solenoid valve redirected an airstream (3 ml/s) through the pipette that was manually placed into a hole on the side of the main delivery tube in which a clean airstream (30 ml/s) was continuously running (see Fig. 1a). This setup allows the delivery of multiple concentrations of odor on the same neuron. When the photoionization detector (PID) was not used, the main airstream was humidified by passing it through a bottle containing distilled water. (2) For random stimulation (see Figs. 7–9), 20 ml of odor dilution were placed in a bottle (100 ml). A small three-way solenoid valve (The Lee Company) was placed in a hole on the side of the main delivery tube and was used to redirect the odor airstream from the exhaust to the delivery tube. Before starting recordings, the odor bottle was placed in the setup for 10–15 min to let the odor gas-phase reach equilibrium. We used a magnetic stirrer to facilitate equilibration of the liquid-phase preparation. A new solenoid valve was used for each odor. (3) For adaptation to a background (see Fig. 4), a constant background was delivered through the bottle into the main airstream, and puffs of increasing concentration were delivered through the Pasteur pipette as described above. Note that odor dilutions prepared in the Pasteur pipette do not result in the same gas-phase concentration as odor dilution prepared in the bottle. For methyl butyrate, the secondary airstream was 3 ml/s, and different background intensities were obtained with different dilutions of odor in paraffin oil (10−4, 3 × 10−4, 10−3). For ethyl acetate, we used two dilutions (10−2, 10−3), but we observed a slow drift in the background intensity due to the slow depletion of odor concentration in the bottle. The drift is not significant within a trial but becomes significant when comparing different trials. Therefore, for the data analysis of Figure 4, we grouped the available trials based on PID measurements of the background stimulus. For ethyl butyrate, we used a fixed dilution (10−5) and changed the secondary flow rate to obtain different background intensities (0.5 and 1.2 ml/s). A lower flow rate allows maintaining a stable background for longer time. We checked that the specific method used to change the background intensity does not affect the results. (4) “Natural” stimuli used for Figure 2 were generated as depicted in Figure 2, a and b.
PID measurements.
A fast-response mini-PID (200a; Aurora Scientific) was used to measure gas-phase concentration of the odor stimuli (Vetter et al., 2006; Schuckel et al., 2008). Linearity of the PID response was tested using a tracer gas (propylene). Timescales plotted in Figures 1 and 2 were obtained by fitting Aon, off e−t/τon, off + Aon, off1 e−t/τon, off1 to the rising and decaying phase of the PID traces. The first exponential captures the amplitude and the speed of the response to a change in stimulus, and the corresponding parameters have been plotted in Figures 1 and 2. The second exponential has a much smaller amplitude (A1 ≪ A) and a slower timescale (τ1 ≫ τ).
Data analyses.
Data analysis was performed in MATLAB. Unless otherwise specified, peristimulus time histograms (PSTH) were calculated as the average firing rate across n trials in 10 ms bins smoothed by a 50 ms squared window (n = 10–12 trials). PID measurements were averaged in the same way (n = 4–12 trials). For normalized responses, basal activity (mean activity in 1 s before stimulus) was subtracted before dividing by peak response. In Figure 3e, for each odor–receptor combination, we considered all the trials (n = 10–12) and three dilutions within the dynamic range. We defined rid(t) as the normalized time-dependent response of neuron i to concentration d and calculated the variance between different trials averaged over all the dilutions as Ptrial = 〈〈〈(rid(t) − 〈rid(t)〉i)2〉i〉t〉d and the variance between different dilutions averaged over all trials as Pdil = 〈〈〈(rid(t) − 〈rid(t)〉d)2〉d〉t〉i. In Figure 6, ab3A firing rate was normalized, and the first 3 s after the stimulus onset were considered for the analysis. Each time bin corresponds to a dimension, and the time-dependent firing rate for each odor represents a point in an n-dimensional space (n = 301 time bins). Principal component analysis (PCA) was used to visualize the distribution of firing traces in the temporal space and was performed using the MATLAB routine “princomp. ” Dendrograms were built from pairwise Euclidean distances between the normalized traces using the MATLAB routine “dendrogram.”
LN model.
An LN model assumes that the neuron response can be decomposed into a linear filter k(t) and a static nonlinear function N such that the response rLN(t) to a time-dependent stimulus s(t) is rLN(t) = N(∫−∞ts(t′)k(t − t′)dt′) (Dayan and Abbott, 2001). We extracted linear filters from data by performing a linear regression to minimize the squared error between predicted rlin(t) = ∫−∞ts(t′)k(t − t′)dt′ and measured response r(t). A random stimulus was generated by opening and closing the valve placed on the delivery tube according to a pseudorandom sequence of zeros (closed) and ones (open) (Geffen et al., 2009; Nagel and Wilson, 2011). The state of the valve was updated at time intervals of length τ. We considered values of τ between 30 and 100 ms. The full sequence was L = 30–60 s long depending on the dataset. The resulting stimulus had a flat power spectrum up to a cutoff frequency imposed by the minimum length of the on and off intervals, τ. Because the input stimulus has relatively low power at high frequencies, estimation of the filter is very sensitive to high-frequency noise. Filters were regularized using elasticnet method (Zou and Hastie, 2005) that allows both L1 and L2 constraints to the linear regression: K = argminK (||R − ΣK||2 + λ2 ||K||2 + λ1 ||K||1), where K is the linear filter, R is the neuron response, Σ is the matrix of the time-shifted stimulus, and λ1 and λ2 are regularization parameters. We used the same set of parameters for all the datasets that we compare. Linear filters were normalized by their maximum value (normalization by the variance gave similar results but made more difficult visualization of the change in the nonlinear function in Fig. 7f). The nonlinear function N was estimated by plotting the measured firing rate against the predicted linear response and fitting the data with a Hill function, r = 1/(1 + (H/rlin)n), where H is the half maximum. For filter estimation, the PSTH was calculated on 3 ms time bins and smoothed with a 30 ms squared window. The accuracy of the filter prediction was calculated as the ratio between the residual of the fit and the noise in the response (Geffen et al., 2009), where PN = 〈〈(ri(t) − r(t))2〉i)t, PR = 〈(r(t) − fLN(t))2〉t, and r(t) = 〈ri(t)〉i. A value of NR smaller than one indicates a model prediction that is more accurate than the variability in the data. We used two-thirds of the length of the recording to estimate the filter and the remaining one-third to test the prediction.
Results
Laboratory odor stimuli exhibit a large diversity of odor-specific temporal dynamics
In a previous study, we showed that stimulus dynamics can depend on odor identity (Su et al., 2011). We wondered how diverse are the dynamics of odor stimuli delivered in the laboratory for a larger set of monomolecular odors. We selected 27 compounds from the panel used previously (Hallem and Carlson, 2006) that contains odorants from different chemical classes that are relevant to the ecology of the fruit fly. Odor dilutions were prepared in a Pasteur pipette, and the time-dependent concentration of odorant reaching the fly position was measured with a PID (Fig. 1a and Materials and Methods). The timescales of the rise and decay phases of the PID traces are broadly distributed (Fig. 1b,c) between 30 ms and 1 s. Rise and decay phases are correlated but not identical. Stimulus dynamics is fairly independent of concentration for “fast” odors, but we measured differences up to hundreds of milliseconds for different concentrations of “slow” odors (Fig. 1d).
These observations suggest the presence of nonlinearity in the delivery system, likely associated with interactions between odor molecules and the surfaces of the delivery system. In the absence of any interaction, we would expect the system to be linear with the concentration of odor, and the on and off timescales to be similar (except for a 10% difference due to the different flow rates during the rising and decaying phases of the puff) and independent of concentration. In contrast, when interactions with surfaces become significant, this linear scaling breaks down; the rates of adsorption and desorption of odorant to surfaces depend on the relative coverage of the surface by odor molecules, which itself is a nonlinear function of the gas-phase concentration [e.g., a hyperbolic function in the simple case of a Langmuir adsorption isotherm (Langmuir, 1932), but more complex isotherms are also possible (Rabe et al., 2011)]. Hence, in this case, we expect the stimulus dynamics to depend on the odor concentration in the gas phase (Fig. 1d).
The concentration of odor in gas phase depends on the concentration in liquid phase and scales linearly with vapor pressure through the Raoult's law (Teixeira et al., 2009). Accordingly, stimulus dynamics also depends on the vapor pressure of the odorant (Fig. 1e). In particular, all odors with long rising time (>100 ms) had low vapor pressure (<1 mmHg). We noticed that the three outliers (in orange) are all acyclic monoterpenes, suggesting that other properties of the odorants (e.g., binding affinity to the tube surface) likely contribute to the time dependency of the odor signal as well.
Although the temporal dynamics of the stimulus reaching the fly is likely determined by the particular delivery method used (Vetter et al., 2006; Andersson et al., 2012), we found consistent differences between slow and fast odors when using different variations of our delivery system (Su et al., 2011). To make such comparisons easier throughout the text, we use “cold” blue colors to plot PID measurements of “slow” odorants and “warm” red colors for “fast” odorant.
What about more natural conditions? We considered two situations likely to occur in nature. First, we investigated stimulus dynamics in the absence of strong wind, mimicking what might happen in the head space of a flower, for example, when an insect is at close range. We placed 50 μl of odor dilution on a paper filter and recorded the gas-phase concentration of the head space 1.5 cm above it (Fig. 2a). In this situation, odor dynamics is governed by evaporation from the liquid source, transport by convection and advection by small flow due to the PID suction. We observed a continuum of dynamics ranging from fast depletion for ethyl acetate to slow increase for diethyl succinate (Fig. 2a). In this case, the dependency of the stimulus dynamics on vapor pressure likely reflects the linear relationship between evaporation rate (defined as minus the rate of change of the odor concentration in liquid phase) and vapor pressure, which has been experimentally observed (van Wesenbeeck et al., 2008), as well as deviations due to interactions between the paraffin oil and the odorant molecules in the liquid phase (Teixeira et al., 2009).
Next we asked whether differences between slow and fast odors would still be noticeable if the odor was transported downstream from the source by wind but still in the vicinity of surfaces. We blew a continuous airstream onto a paper filter containing 50 μl of odorant dilution and measured the odor concentrations 3 cm downstream from the odor source (Fig. 2b). To determine whether an ORN can detect these differences, we recorded the response of ab3A simultaneously with the stimulus concentration (Fig. 2b). Although the stimulus signal depends on the position of the detector and the particular configuration of the experiment, we found consistent differences in stimulus dynamics between a fast and a slow odorant. The overall trend of the stimulus concentration seems to reflect the evaporation dynamics shown in Figure 2a (fast depletion for methyl butyrate and slow increase for diethyl succinate). To quantify the statistics of the fluctuations around the evaporation trend, we calculated the autocorrelation function of the fluctuations around the mean for the last 70 s of the traces. The decay time in the autocorrelation function of the PID signal was longer for the slow odorant diethyl succinate than for the fast odorant methyl butyrate. Importantly, the autocorrelation functions for the ab3A responses show similar differences (Fig. 2c). This result suggests that the physico-chemical properties of odorant molecules can affect the temporal statistics of odor signals that reach the sensory system and that ORNs can detect such differences. Farther away from the source and away from surfaces, these differences should be eliminated by turbulent mixing. Note that, even away from surfaces, the edges of an odorant plume might still exhibit small delays between compounds coming from the same source because of different diffusivities of the odorants. What physiological properties enable the neuron to capture such differences in the temporal statistics of odor signals?
ORN response dynamics are independent of stimulus intensity for a large part of their dynamic range
Previous studies have shown that the response of ORNs to odor puffs exhibit diverse dynamics ranging from tonic to phasic depending on the particular odor–receptor combination (Hallem and Carlson, 2006; Nagel and Wilson, 2011). We reasoned that, to compare the response of ORNs to different odors, we should first characterize the dependence of the response on concentration, thus taking into account the fact that ORNs have odor-specific sensitivities (Ito et al., 2009). For this purpose, we considered only fast odorants for which stimulus dynamics is fairly independent from the particular dilution used (Figs. 1d, 3c).
ab3A response to 500 ms puffs (see Materials and Methods) of ethyl acetate is tonic at low concentrations, more phasic at concentrations close to the half-maximal response, and prolonged for saturating stimuli (Fig. 3a). A poststimulus silent period can be observed with intermediate concentrations. We measured the peak firing rate (Fig. 3b, black) and the degree of adaptation (Fig. 3b, green) defined as the decrease in firing rate relative to the peak, as a function of the odor dilution. The peak response follows the typical sigmoidal dependency on concentration and shows saturation for the two highest dilutions used. Surprisingly, the degree of adaptation is nearly constant for all the dilutions in the middle of the dynamic range in which the peak response is nearly linear with log changes in concentration (between 10 and 90% of the maximum firing rate, p > 0.6, ANOVA).
To analyze the response dynamics independently from the amplitude, we plotted the time-dependent firing rate normalized by its peak response. In the middle of the dynamic range (Fig. 3b, arrows), the curves are remarkably similar (Fig. 3c, p > 0.05, ANOVA performed on each time bin). Thus, not only the degree of adaptation but the entire response dynamics are independent of odor dilution. We used the PID to measure the intensity of the stimulus in the vicinity of the fly. A change in the dilution of ethyl acetate only affected the amplitude of the stimulus reaching the fly but not its dynamics (Fig. 3c).
We asked whether the invariance of normalized responses (Fig. 3d) was a property of the population average or a property of the individual ORNs. For all 12 neurons examined individually within their dynamic range, the normalized response dynamics are remarkably similar (Fig. 3d, first column, three individual cells shown). For four ORNs (ab2A, ab3A, ab3B, and ab7A) and the different odorants tested, normalized responses of single neurons are also concentration invariant (Fig. 3d). Moreover trial-to-trial variability of the normalized response dynamics is equal or larger than dilution-to-dilution variability (Fig. 3e and Materials and Methods), suggesting that, for concentrations in the middle of the dynamic range, invariance to stimulus intensity is a robust feature of ORN responses.
In summary, we identified three regimes of response dynamics. Stimuli at the lower end of the dynamic range usually elicit a tonic response that is probably concentration dependent (Nagel and Wilson, 2011). Response becomes more phasic and concentration invariant for stimuli in the middle of the dynamic range. Stimuli near the upper end of the neuron dynamic range elicit saturated responses characterized by prolonged dynamics.
Independence of ORN response dynamics with background intensity
In nature, odor stimuli are often encountered on top of a background. This poses a challenge to sensory systems, which must remain sensitive to changes in the signal over a wide range of background intensities. We asked whether the invariance of the normalized ORN response dynamics with respect to odor intensity still holds in the presence of a background of the same odorant.
We adapted ab3A to a stable concentration of methyl butyrate, and superimposed on this background, we administered short puffs of increasing concentration of the same odorant (Fig. 4a and Materials and Methods). Prolonged stimulation with methyl butyrate caused the neuron to adapt within few seconds to a stationary firing rate that is concentration dependent. When normalized by the peak response, the dynamics are independent of odor intensity (Fig. 4a), confirming the results obtained with short puffs (Fig. 3c).
Adaptation to the background induces a shift in the dose–response curve toward higher doses and decreases the dynamic range (Fig. 4b), similarly to what has been found in frog olfactory cells (Reisert and Matthews, 1999). Remarkably, in the middle of the dynamic range of the different adapted states (Fig. 4b, arrows), ab3A responds to odor puffs with firing dynamics that are very similar across background intensities when normalized by peak amplitude (Fig. 4c). Apart from one time point (Fig. 4c, black point bottom left), the differences between the curves are not significant (p > 0.05; Fig. 4c, bottom, tested on 30 ms time bins). For ethyl acetate and ethyl butyrate, there was no significant difference (p > 0.05) between the curves for all the time points (Fig. 4, second and third columns).
Thus, adaptation capabilities of ab3A are responsible for generating response dynamics that are independent of stimulus intensity, even in the presence of background stimuli. These results suggest that the dynamics of odor stimuli may be encoded in the ORN response independently of stimulus intensity.
Odor-dependent stimulus dynamics strongly affect ORN response dynamics
We showed in Figure 1 that the odor stimulus reaching the fly exhibited different temporal profiles depending on the odor used. We therefore asked to what extent ORN dynamics depend on stimulus dynamics in addition to the different physiological processes that can affect the response.
We measured the responses of ab3A, ab3B, and ab7A to five odorants ranging from fast to slow (Fig. 5). For fast odorants (methyl butyrate and propyl acetate in red), the stimulus measured at the fly closely follows the opening and closing of the valve, whereas ORNs (ab3A, ab7A, and ab3B) show a phasic response with a degree of adaptation ∼0.5 (as defined above). In contrast, for slow odorants, PID signals and neuron responses do not follow the opening and closing of the valve as well. ORN responses to slow odorants tend to be more tonic (lower degree of adaptation) and exhibit prolonged dynamics that reflects the long decay of the odor after the closing of the valve (ab3A and ab3B responses to 1-octen-3-ol and diethyl succinate).
Although for fast odors a change in dilution only affects the amplitude of the stimulus (Figs. 1d, 3c, 5, top), for slower odors a change in dilution can affect both the amplitude and the dynamics of the stimulus (Figs. 1d, 5, bottom). Thus, unlike for fast odors (Figs. 3, 5 top), the ORNs responses to puffs of slow odorants should not be expected to rescale with liquid dilution because the signal arriving on the fly does not rescale (see ab3A and ab3B responses to 1-octen-3-ol and diethyl succinate; Fig. 5, bottom). However, we expect the ORN response to rescale with the intensity of the odorant reaching the fly for both slow and fast odors as considered below (see Fig. 10). For very slow odorants, the dependency of stimulus dynamics on odor dilution can cause concentration-dependent delays in the neuron response to odor puffs (diethyl succinate; Fig. 5a), possibly related to the time needed for the odor concentration to reach the neuron detection threshold.
Although these examples indicate that odor-dependent stimulus dynamics does affect ORN response dynamics, Figure 5 also shows that physiological processes alone can introduce differences in the ORN dynamics as reported previously (Nagel and Wilson, 2011): for example, the responses of ab3A and ab3B to 1-octen-3-ol are different, with ab3B response being more tonic.
To quantify to what extent stimulus dynamics might affect ORN response dynamics, we asked how easy it would be to discriminate between odorants on the basis of the time-dependent activity of ab3A only. Following the same approach that was used in previous studies (Stopfer et al., 2003; Raman et al., 2010), we performed PCA and hierarchical clustering on ab3A responses to the four odorants used in Figure 5a. Because of the similarity of ab3A response to different concentrations of the same odorant, responses to the same odor fall very close to each other in the PCA plot (Fig. 6a). Responses to different concentrations of diethyl succinate are relatively more distant from each other than for other odorants, probably because of the stronger concentration dependency of the stimulus dynamics. Responses to all the concentrations of the same odor cluster together, and different odor types rank according to their characteristic “speed” (Fig. 6a and Materials and Methods). In contrast, ab3A responses to four odors with more similar dynamics do not cluster accordingly to odor type (Fig. 6b).
Using an LN model to extract ab3A response function
The data shown so far indicate that the observed ORN response dynamics is the result of the convolution of the odor-dependent stimulus dynamics and the ORN response function. To separate this two contributions, we estimated LN models (Dayan and Abbott, 2001; Kim et al., 2011; Nagel and Wilson, 2011) for the delivery system and for ab3A responses.
An LN model consist of a linear filter k(t) and a static nonlinear transformation N (see Materials and Methods). We approximated a white-noise stimulus by randomly opening and closing a three-way solenoid valve to redirect an odor stream into the delivery tube (Fig. 7a, top trace). The valve state (On–Off) was updated every τ = 30 ms (see Materials and Methods). In the following, we refer to τ as the “stimulus correlation time ” (the time for the autocorrelation function to decay to zero). We performed simultaneous recordings of the odor concentration reaching the fly together with ab3A firing activity (Fig. 7).
First, we built an LN model for the odor stimulus using the binary state of the valve as the input and the PID signal as the output (Fig. 7b). The resulting linear filter k(t) peaks at 56 ms, consistent with the air travel time, and exhibits a long tail (∼200 ms, hereafter “integration time”) indicative of some integration of the signal within the delivery tube. The function N is a straight line revealing that the delivery system is linear.
Next, we built a LN model using the state of the valve as the input and ab3A response as the output (Fig. 7c). Comparing the time-to-peak of this filter with the time-to-peak of the filter obtained for the signal (Fig. 7b), we estimated a 12 ms delay between the stimulus arrival and the ORN response. The function N has the shape of a rectifier (Fig. 7c) and was fitted with a Hill function (see Materials and Methods).
Finally, we built an LN model using the PID signal as input and ab3A firing rates as output (Fig. 7d). In this case, the ORN response function is deconvolved from the dynamical properties of the stimulus, and the filter only reflects the effects of physiological processes. We call this filter the response function of the ORN. As reported previously (Kim et al., 2011; Nagel and Wilson, 2011), the filter shows a positive and a negative lobe, suggesting that the neuron is taking a derivative of the odor stimulus. The area under the filter is not zero, in agreement with the fact that this ORN does not return to baseline activity after a step stimulus (Fig. 4a). The integration time for this ORN (estimated as the width of the filter) is ∼100 ms, much shorter than the integration time of the delivery system. Cross-validation (see Materials and Methods) shows that the estimated LN model can accurately predict the response of ab3A (NR = 0.41) (Fig. 7a, bottom, red).
Adaptive properties of ab3A response to a flickering stimulus
To what extent does the LN model depend on the statistics of the stimulus signal? Figure 7e shows LN models for the odor signal and ab3A extracted using stimulus sequences with different correlation times (τ = 30, 50, and 100 ms). The LN model for the valve-to-odor transformation is independent of τ, suggesting that our method to estimate the model is not affected by the stimulus correlation time (for the range of τ values considered here). In contrast, ab3A response function does exhibit a small dependency on τ. The filter becomes slightly wider as the correlation time of the input signal is increased, suggesting that the neuron may adapt its response kinetics to the dynamics of the stimulus (Hosoya et al., 2005). We will investigate this issue further in future studies.
The short ∼100 ms integration time of ab3A response function (Fig. 7d) is consistent with the initial phasic response of this neuron to step increases in methyl butyrate (Figs. 4a, 5a). However, it is too short to explain the slower adaptation of the ab3A response to prolonged stimuli reported in Figure 4b. To quantify how the neuron might adapt to the flickering stimulus, we followed a procedure used for the analysis of adaptation in the retina (Baccus and Meister, 2002). We estimated LN models for the early (first 10 s) and the late (last 10 s) response of ab3A to the flickering signal (Fig. 7f). Although the linear filter k(r) remains invariant, the nonlinear function N becomes less steep as the neuron adapts to the flickering stimulus. This result is consistent with our finding that the presence of an odor background induces a change in the dose response of ab3A but does not affect ab3A response dynamics (Fig. 4). We obtained similar results for the response of ab3A to ethyl acetate, 1-octen-3ol (Fig. 7f), 2-butanone, and 1-pentanol (data not shown), as well as for the response of the maxillary palp neuron pb1A to 2-butanone and isoamyl acetate (Fig 7f).
The change observed in the nonlinear function during adaptation depends on the particular odor–receptor combination: for example, pb1A shows greater adaptation to 2-butanone than to isoamyl acetate. Note that the early response function was estimated from the first 10 s of the time series, and it is very likely that some adaptation already takes place during this time. The limited amount of data does not allow us to extract LN models for segments shorter than 10 s.
Different odorants induce small temporal delays in the response functions of two ORNs located in the antenna and the maxillary palp
Finally, we investigated how the response function of ab3A and pb1A depend on odor identity. We generated a flickering stimulus of the slow odorant 1-octen-3-ol and compared the PID and ab3A responses (Fig. 8a) with those obtained with the fast odorant methyl butyrate (Fig. 7a). With 1-octen-3-ol, the PID trace is smoother and ab3A response appears more tonic. Neither odor concentration nor neuron firing goes to zero when the valve is closed (Fig. 8a). To identify the origin of such differences, we compared the filters obtained for the two odorants. The valve-to-odor filter peaks at the same time for both odorants but exhibits a longer tail for 1-octen-3-ol (Fig. 8b, left) with an integration time twice as long as for methyl butyrate, consistent with the slower stimulus dynamics of that odorant. The valve-to-ORN filter also shows a longer integration time with 1-octen-3-ol (Fig. 8b, middle). In addition, we observe a small 4 ms delay in the peak of the filter, which is consistent across single-neuron recordings (data not shown). Interestingly, apart from the same 4 ms delay, the filters obtained for the odor-to-ORN transformation (the response function of ab3A) exhibit less difference between the two odorants than those obtained for the valve-to-odor transformation (Fig. 8b, right). In particular the longer tail observed with 1-octen-3-ol in the valve-to-ORN filter is gone.
These results suggest that the large differences observed in ab3A dynamics between the two odorants (Figs. 7a, 8a) might be dominated by differences in stimulus dynamics. If this is true, it should be possible to use a single filter to predict the response to both odorants once a measure of the stimulus is provided. To test this hypothesis, we used PID measurements of 1-octen-3-ol concentration (appropriately scaled and shifted by 4 ms) as input into the LN model of ab3A obtained with methyl butyrate (Fig. 8c). The prediction of the response to the slow odor is remarkably good (NR = 0.44), comparable with the one obtained for methyl butyrate itself (NR = 0.41). Importantly, it was better than the prediction obtained doing the same cross-prediction with the valve-to-ORN filters (NR = 0.62). We attempted to extract similar LN models for other slow odors that activate ab3A, such as γ-hexalactone and diethyl succinate. However, ab3A is very sensitive to these odorants whereas the PID is not, and we were unable to obtain reliable measurements of the stimulus to use as input to the LN model.
We examined the response function of ab3A to a total of five odorants: (1) methyl butyrate; (2) 1-octen-3-ol; (3) ethyl acetate; (4) 2-butanone; and (5) 1-pentanol (Fig. 9a–d). To reduce the effect of cell-to-cell and fly-to-fly variability in our comparison, we tested two odors on each neuron and compared LN models obtained for each odor pair. A correlation time of 100 ms was used for the flickering stimulus. For 1-octen-3-ol, we obtained results similar to those measured with a 30 ms correlation time in the input (Fig. 8): apart from a small temporal shift, the filter extracted for methyl butyrate can be used to predict ab3A response to 1-octen-3-ol (Fig. 9a). For ethyl acetate, 2-butanone, and 1-pentanol, we do not observe any difference in the valve-to-odor filters (apart from a small shift due to the different position of the valves on the delivery tube) (Fig. 9b–d, top). The odor-to-ORN filters estimated for pairs of odors have different response delays with shifts of up to 12 ms and exhibit only minor differences in their shapes (Fig. 9b–d, middle). Given that the stimulus contribution was deconvolved from the ORN response, these differences in response delay can be entirely attributed to physiological processes. Once shifted in time by the appropriate amount, the filters obtained with methyl butyrate (in red) can be used to predict with good accuracy the response of ab3A to the other odorants (Fig. 9b–d, bottom) and this despite small differences in the filters shape. We note a larger difference for the ab3A 1-pentanol response. Consistent with the finding that the linear response of ab3A was invariant over the course of the flickering stimulus experiment (Fig. 7f), we found that the response delays measured for pairs of odorants at the very beginning of the flickering stimulus were the same as those measured at the end of the flickering stimulus. These odor-dependent latencies in the response of ab3A remained invariant, although the neuron sensitivity was adapting to the flickering stimulus.
We also measured the response function of pb1A, a neuron located in the maxillary palp, to 2-butanone and isoamyl acetate. Despite the different vapor pressure (Fig. 2b, 71 mmHg for 2-butanone and 5 mmHg for isoamyl acetate), we did not observe any difference in the dynamics of the two odor stimuli (Fig. 9e, top), likely because of the very low concentration of 2-butanone used to elicit a firing rate comparable with isoamyl acetate (∼100 Hz). The linear filter for the response of pb1A (Fig. 9e, bottom) has a smaller negative lobe than what was found for ab3A, suggesting that the response function depends on the neuron identity as reported previously (Nagel and Wilson, 2011). Like for ab3A, the main difference between the response functions of pb1A to the two odorants is a small delay (18 ms) between the corresponding filters. We demonstrated this by predicting the response of pb1A to a flickering stimulus of isoamyl acetate using the filter measured for 2-butanone (appropriately shifted in time by 18 ms) (NR = 0.49). A previous study reported larger differences in the responses of pb1A to these two odorants (Nagel and Wilson, 2011). Differences are to be expected if one of the two odorants is used at concentrations near the bottom of the dynamic range in which neural responses are close to the tonic-to-phasic transition. Indeed, the responses of pb1A to higher odorant concentrations (Nagel and Wilson, 2011, their Fig. 6) exhibit smaller differences between these two odorants. Another likely source of differences stems from the dependency of the ORN response on the correlation time in the stimulus (Fig. 7e). The cutoff frequency of the flickering stimulus is 100 ms in this study, much shorter that in the study by Nagel and Wilson (2011). Finally, the dependency of the stimulus dynamics on the delivery system configuration, odor type, and concentration may also contribute to the differences. For example, we noticed that the small difference in odor stimulus that we observed between methyl butyrate and 1-pentanol when delivered through the Pasteur pipette (Fig. 5a) is absent when the odor is delivered using a bottle (Fig. 9d, top). Similarly, ab3A response does not show a silent period after an odor puff, but it does show silent periods after the closing of the valve during the flickering odor stimulation. These observations support the importance of taking stimulus dynamics and neuron adaptation in account to study the contribution of physiological processes to ORN response dynamics.
Discussion
Here we identified three key aspects of ORN response dynamics. Our first result is that ORN response dynamics are independent of stimulus intensity in the middle of the neuron dynamic range (Fig. 3). Near the bottom of the dynamic range, responses become more tonic and often depend on concentration. For saturating stimuli, we observed prolonged responses that do not rescale. Adaptation at the level of both transduction and firing could be involved in the concentration invariance observed here (Liu and Wang, 2001). In photoreceptors and vertebrate ORNs, increases in stimulus intensity elicit faster response kinetics (Baylor and Nunn, 1986; Reisert and Matthews, 1999). Similarly, local field potential recordings from Drosophila ORNs show an increase in onset kinetics (Nagel and Wilson, 2011). Additional investigations are necessary to understand the mechanisms underlying the concentration invariance of the firing rate. In vision, adaptation to a background is usually accompanied by an acceleration of response kinetics (Baylor and Hodgkin, 1974; Burns and Baylor, 2001). In contrast, adaptation to a background in both vertebrate and insect ORNs has been shown to slow down the kinetics of the response to low stimulus concentrations (Reisert and Matthews, 1999; Nagel and Wilson, 2011). Here we show that adaptation to a background shifts and reduces the dynamic range, but in addition, we find that, for a large part of the adapted dynamic range, response dynamics to short puffs remain independent of the concentration of stimulus and background (Figs. 4, 7f).
These results suggest that, within a population of ORNs, neurons activated in this regime are able to encode the dynamics of the stimulus independently of its intensity. Hence, for each odorant, a relatively small number of ORNs might be sufficient to reliably capture the stimulus dynamics (Geffen et al., 2009). This property could be useful to maintain olfactory-driven navigation capabilities independent of signal intensity and background. A previous study (Asahina et al., 2009) suggested that, when climbing an odor gradient, Drosophila larvae rely on the response of various individual ORNs with distinct but overlapping dynamic ranges. A similar principle enhances the capability of bacteria to navigate chemical environments (Sourjik and Berg, 2002; Lazova et al., 2011). Additional investigations are needed to understand the role of low, saturated, and super-sustained responses (Montague et al., 2011). Concentration invariance of behavior may be achieved by processing in the antennal lobe (Asahina et al., 2009). Local inhibitory neurons in the antennal lobe have been shown to perform a normalization of the activity of a single glomerulus by the total activity of the ORN population (Olsen et al., 2010). These observations pose the question of whether processing in the Drosophila antennal lobe may provide the means to extract concentration-invariant information from temporal patterns of activity in the ORN population.
Our second result is that, under several conditions, ORN responses can be strongly affected by the odor-specific dynamics of the stimulus (Figs. 1, 2, 5, 6). In one case, we show that a single LN model can be used to predict the temporal response of an ORN to different odors, providing direct measurements of the odor stimuli reaching the fly (Figs. 8c, 9). In Figure 10, we use simulations to explore how the differences in stimulus dynamics might affect ORN dynamics. Assuming a single response function, ORN responses can be more or less phasic depending on the dynamics of the input stimulus (Fig. 10a). Consistent with what we observed in Figure 3, within the linear regime of the model, the ORN responds to different stimulus intensities with the same dynamics (Fig. 10b). Provided that the stimulus dynamics is concentration invariant, the model predicts concentration-invariant responses for fast and slow odorants (Fig. 10b,c). However, no rescaling of the ORN response should be expected when both the dynamics and intensity of the stimulus change (Fig. 10d). This seems similar to what we observed with most slow odorants we tested (Figs. 1d, 5). A better control of the stimulus is required to investigate experimentally the concentration dependency of the ORN response dynamics for slow odorants. We showed recently that ORNs respond to mixtures of excitatory and inhibitory odorants with distinct dynamics that reflect the physico-chemical properties of the odorants (Su et al., 2011). The LN model for ab3A can qualitatively account for the measured response dynamics solely from the different dynamics of the constituent odorants, assuming the contribution of the excitatory and inhibitory odorant to be additive (Fig. 11). Differences in stimulus dynamics alone could play an important role in the perception of odor mixtures.
Our third result is that, when differences in stimulus dynamics are carefully removed from the neuronal responses, ORN response dynamics still exhibit odor-dependent delays (Fig. 9). Other studies (Hallem and Carlson, 2006; Raman et al., 2010; Montague et al., 2011; Nagel and Wilson, 2011) have reported larger differences in the response of a single ORN to different odorants, suggesting that a bigger dataset might reveal greater variability than what we observed here. However, with few exceptions (Montague et al., 2011; Nagel and Wilson, 2011), most previous studies assumed stimuli to be independent of odor type and considered only one or few concentrations of the stimulus. Here we first quantified the dose response of each ORN–odor pair and then identified a regime of the dynamic range over which dynamics are concentration invariant. The response dynamics for different ORN–odor pairs were then compared within this regime. Moreover, we quantified the response function of the delivery system for each odorant and removed its contribution from the ORN response, obtaining response functions that only depend on physiological processes. Within this limited dataset, the shape of the response function only depends mildly on odorant identity but shows odor-dependent delays with differences up to 18 ms (Fig. 9). These delays remained constant between the beginning and end of the flickering stimulus, although the neuron sensitivity adapted to the stimulus (Fig. 7f). Odor-specific response delays have been observed previously in vertebrates ORNs and M/T cells (Spors et al., 2006; Junek et al., 2010). In the fly, odor-dependent delays could originate at different levels of the transduction process. For example, there could be rate-limiting events in the capture of odorant, their transport by odorant binding proteins, and their interaction with ORs.
Can odor-dependent differences in stimulus dynamics and odor-dependent delays in ORN response affect behavior?
Several experiments suggest that animals can use temporal information to distinguish between two olfactory stimuli. For example, mice can discriminate small delays of the order of 10 ms in the activation of the ORNs (Smear et al., 2011). Bees can discriminate two odor stimuli time shifted by only few milliseconds (Szyszka et al., 2012). Flies can discriminate between two odors using a single ORN type (DasGupta and Waddell, 2008). Moreover, odor speed affects the capacity of humans to discriminate mixtures of odorants and tastants (Mozell, 1964a; Kuznicki and Turner, 1988; Laing et al., 1994; Jinks and Laing, 1999).
It is widely accepted in perfumery that the composition of a fragrance changes over time as a result of differential evaporation of the individual components (Carles, 1962; Teixeira et al., 2009). Here we show that the concentration of the individual components can fluctuate differently depending on their physico-chemical properties. In ecology, much attention has been paid to the passive transport of odorants by turbulent flows (Murlis et al., 1992; Cardé and Willis, 2008; Riffell et al., 2008). While most of these works focused on situations in which the temporal dynamics of the stimulus are dominated by those of the air flow away from surfaces, here we examined what happens in the vicinity of surfaces. This situation is ecologically relevant to the fruit fly that spends a large amount of its lifetime walking (Carey et al., 2006). Interactions between odorant and surfaces introduce retardations in the stimulus dynamics that are odor specific (Figs. 1, 2a,c,d), and our results suggest that insects may be able to detect such relative temporal differences downwind of a nearby source (Fig. 2c). Behavioral experiments suggest that animals use fluctuations in odor concentration to identify different odor sources (Hopfield and Gelperin, 1989; Hopfield, 1991). It would be interesting in the future to investigate whether odor-dependent dynamics might be used to reveal the presence of different components within the same source.
Odor dependent differences in stimulus dynamics may affect tracking performance as well. For example the structure of an odor plume can greatly modify the way a moth tracks an odor signal down to its source (Baker et al., 1985; Mafraneto and Carde, 1994). Tracking capability in flies may also depend on odor identity (Krishnan et al., 2011), but whether this is attributable to differences in the input stimulus, ORN response, or downstream processing remains unclear.
The temporal aspect of ORN responses may add coding capacity to the spatial representation of odors at the periphery of the olfactory system. We have shown how ORN odor-specific dynamics originate in part from the stimulus itself in addition to the interactions between odorants and the periphery of the olfactory system. Whether and how the olfactory system uses the diversity in stimulus dynamics to enhance odor discrimination and tracking remains an open question.
Footnotes
This work was supported by research awards from the Whitehall Foundation and the Raymond and Beverley Sackler Institute for Biological, Physical, and Engineering Sciences (T.E.) and National Institutes of Health grants (J.C.). We thank D. Mathew for help with electrophysiology, P. van Tassel, C. Crews, J. Gustafson, and E. Paulson for discussions about odorant surface interactions and chemistry, C. Su for the w;UAS–rpr;Or22a–GAL4 line, S. Gorur-Shandilya for help with data analysis, T. Mora and members of the Emonet and Carlson laboratories for discussion, R. Alexander, A. Bernacchia, D. Clark, K. Nagel, K. Dave, and R. Wilson for critical reading of this manuscript.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Andersson MN, Schlyter F, Hill SR, Dekker T. What reaches the antenna? How to calibrate odor flux and ligand-receptor affinities. Chem Senses. 2012;37:403–420. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjs009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asahina K, Louis M, Piccinotti S, Vosshall LB. A circuit supporting concentration-invariant odor perception in Drosophila. J Biol. 2009;8:9. doi: 10.1186/jbiol108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baccus SA, Meister M. Fast and slow contrast adaptation in retinal circuitry. Neuron. 2002;36:909–919. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01050-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker TC, Willis MA, Haynes KF, Phelan PL. A pulsed cloud of sex-pheromone elicits upwind flight in male moths. Physiol Entomol. 1985;10:257–265. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1985.tb00045.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Baylor DA, Hodgkin AL. Changes in time scale and sensitivity in turtle photoreceptors. J Physiol. 1974;242:729–758. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1974.sp010732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baylor DA, Nunn BJ. Electrical properties of the light-sensitive conductance of rods of the salamander Ambystoma tigrinum. J Physiol. 1986;371:115–145. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp015964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown SL, Joseph J, Stopfer M. Encoding a temporally structured stimulus with a temporally structured neural representation. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1568–1576. doi: 10.1038/nn1559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burns ME, Baylor DA. Activation, deactivation, and adaptation in vertebrate photoreceptor cells. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:779–805. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardé RT, Willis MA. Navigational strategies used by insects to find distant, wind-borne sources of odor. J Chem Ecol. 2008;34:854–866. doi: 10.1007/s10886-008-9484-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey JR, Papadopoulos N, Kouloussis N, Katsoyannos B, Müller HG, Wang JL, Tseng YK. Age-specific and lifetime behavior patterns in Drosophila melanogaster and the Mediterranean fruit fly, Ceratitis capitata. Exp Gerontol. 2006;41:93–97. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2005.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carles J. A method of creation in perfumery. Soap Perfumery Cosmetics. 1962;35:328–335. [Google Scholar]
- Daly KC, Wright GA, Smith BH. Molecular features of odorants systematically influence slow temporal responses across clusters of coordinated antennal lobe units in the moth Manduca sexta. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92:236–254. doi: 10.1152/jn.01132.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DasGupta S, Waddell S. Learned odor discrimination in Drosophila without combinatorial odor maps in the antennal lobe. Curr Biol. 2008;18:1668–1674. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P, Abbott LF. Theoretical neuroscience: computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- de Bruyne M, Foster K, Carlson JR. Odor coding in the Drosophila antenna. Neuron. 2001;30:537–552. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00289-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duchamp-Viret P, Duchamp A, Chaput MA. Peripheral odor coding in the rat and frog: quality and intensity specification. J Neurosci. 2000;20:2383–2390. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-06-02383.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedrich RW, Laurent G. Dynamic optimization of odor representations by slow temporal patterning of mitral cell activity. Science. 2001;291:889–894. doi: 10.1126/science.291.5505.889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geffen MN, Broome BM, Laurent G, Meister M. Neural encoding of rapidly fluctuating odors. Neuron. 2009;61:570–586. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hallem EA, Carlson JR. Coding of odors by a receptor repertoire. Cell. 2006;125:143–160. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.01.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopfield JF, Gelperin A. Differential conditioning to a compound stimulus and its components in the terrestrial mollusk Limax-maximus. Behav Neurosci. 1989;103:329–333. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.103.2.329. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hopfield JJ. Olfactory computation and object perception. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1991;88:6462–6466. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.15.6462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hosoya T, Baccus SA, Meister M. Dynamic predictive coding by the retina. Nature. 2005;436:71–77. doi: 10.1038/nature03689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito I, Bazhenov M, Ong RC, Raman B, Stopfer M. Frequency transitions in odor-evoked neural oscillations. Neuron. 2009;64:692–706. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jinks A, Laing DG. Temporal processing reveals a mechanism for limiting the capacity of humans to analyze odor mixtures. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 1999;8:311–325. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00034-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Junek S, Kludt E, Wolf F, Schild D. Olfactory coding with patterns of response latencies. Neuron. 2010;67:872–884. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim AJ, Lazar AA, Slutskiy YB. System identification of Drosophila olfactory sensory neurons. J Comput Neurosci. 2011;30:143–161. doi: 10.1007/s10827-010-0265-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krishnan P, Duistermars BJ, Frye MA. Odor identity influences tracking of temporally patterned plumes in Drosophila. BMC Neurosci. 2011;12:62. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-12-62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuznicki JT, Turner LS. Temporal dissociation of taste mixture components. Chem Senses. 1988;13:45–62. doi: 10.1093/chemse/13.1.45. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Laing DG, Eddy A, Francis GW, Stephens L. Evidence for the temporal processing of odor mixtures in humans. Brain Res. 1994;651:317–328. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90712-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langmuir I. Vapor pressures, evaporation, condensation and adsorption. J Am Chem Soc. 1932;54:2798–2832. doi: 10.1021/ja01346a022. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lazova MD, Ahmed T, Bellomo D, Stocker R, Shimizu TS. Response rescaling in bacterial chemotaxis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:13870–13875. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1108608108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lei H, Christensen TA, Hildebrand JG. Spatial and temporal organization of ensemble representations for different odor classes in the moth antennal lobe. J Neurosci. 2004;24:11108–11119. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3677-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu YH, Wang XJ. Spike-frequency adaptation of a generalized leaky integrate-and-fire model neuron. J Comput Neurosci. 2001;10:25–45. doi: 10.1023/A:1008916026143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mafraneto A, Carde RT. Fine-scale structure of pheromone plumes modulates upwind orientation of flying moths. Nature. 1994;369:142–144. doi: 10.1038/369142a0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Malnic B, Hirono J, Sato T, Buck LB. Combinatorial receptor codes for odors. Cell. 1999;96:713–723. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)80581-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montague SA, Mathew D, Carlson JR. Similar odorants elicit different behavioral and physiological responses, some supersustained. J Neurosci. 2011;31:7891–7899. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6254-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mozell MM. Evidence for sorption as a mechanism of the olfactory analysis of vapours. Nature. 1964a;203:1181–1182. doi: 10.1038/2031181a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mozell MM. Olfactory discrimination: electrophysiological spatiotemporal basis. Science. 1964b;143:1336–1337. doi: 10.1126/science.143.3612.1336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murlis J, Elkinton JS, Carde RT. Odor plumes and how insects use them. Annu Rev Entomol. 1992;37:505–532. doi: 10.1146/annurev.en.37.010192.002445. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nagel KI, Wilson RI. Biophysical mechanisms underlying olfactory receptor neuron dynamics. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:208–216. doi: 10.1038/nn.2725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olsen SR, Bhandawat V, Wilson RI. Divisive normalization in olfactory population codes. Neuron. 2010;66:287–299. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabe M, Verdes D, Seeger S. Understanding protein adsorption phenomena at solid surfaces. Adv Colloid Interface Sci. 2011;162:87–106. doi: 10.1016/j.cis.2010.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raman B, Joseph J, Tang J, Stopfer M. Temporally diverse firing patterns in olfactory receptor neurons underlie spatiotemporal neural codes for odors. J Neurosci. 2010;30:1994–2006. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5639-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reisert J, Matthews HR. Adaptation of the odour-induced response in frog olfactory receptor cells. J Physiol. 1999;519:801–813. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.0801n.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riffell JA, Abrell L, Hildebrand JG. Physical processes and real-time chemical measurement of the insect olfactory environment. J Chem Ecol. 2008;34:837–853. doi: 10.1007/s10886-008-9490-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer AT, Margrie TW. Spatiotemporal representations in the olfactory system. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:92–100. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schuckel J, Meisner S, Torkkeli PH, French AS. Dynamic properties of Drosophila olfactory electroantennograms. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol. 2008;194:483–489. doi: 10.1007/s00359-008-0322-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smear M, Shusterman R, O'Connor R, Bozza T, Rinberg D. Perception of sniff phase in mouse olfaction. Nature. 2011;479:397–400. doi: 10.1038/nature10521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sourjik V, Berg HC. Receptor sensitivity in bacterial chemotaxis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:123–127. doi: 10.1073/pnas.011589998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spors H, Wachowiak M, Cohen LB, Friedrich RW. Temporal dynamics and latency patterns of receptor neuron input to the olfactory bulb. J Neurosci. 2006;26:1247–1259. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3100-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stopfer M, Jayaraman V, Laurent G. Intensity versus identity coding in an olfactory system. Neuron. 2003;39:991–1004. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2003.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Su CY, Menuz K, Carlson JR. Olfactory perception: receptors, cells, and circuits. Cell. 2009;139:45–59. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Su CY, Martelli C, Emonet T, Carlson JR. Temporal coding of odor mixtures in an olfactory receptor neuron. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:5075–5080. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1100369108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szyszka P, Stierle JS, Biergans S, Galizia CG. The speed of smell: odor-object segregation within milliseconds. PLoS One. 2012;7:e36096. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teixeira MA, Rodriguez O, Mata VG, Rodrigues AE. The diffusion of perfume mixtures and the odor performance. Chem Eng Sci. 2009;64:2570–2589. doi: 10.1016/j.ces.2009.01.064. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- van Wesenbeeck I, Driver J, Ross J. Relationship between the evaporation rate and vapor pressure of moderately and highly volatile chemicals. Bull Environ Contam Toxicol. 2008;80:315–318. doi: 10.1007/s00128-008-9380-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vetter RS, Sage AE, Justus KA, Cardé RT, Galizia CG. Temporal integrity of an airborne odor stimulus is greatly affected by physical aspects of the odor delivery system. Chem Senses. 2006;31:359–369. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjj040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson RI, Turner GC, Laurent G. Transformation of olfactory representations in the Drosophila antennal lobe. Science. 2004;303:366–370. doi: 10.1126/science.1090782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zou H, Hastie T. Regularization and variable selection via the elastic net. J R Stat Soc B Biol Sci. 2005;67:301–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9868.2005.00503.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]