Abstract
The present article is concerned with studies on magnitude estimations that strived to uncover the underlying mental representation(s) of magnitudes. We point out a number of methodological differences and shortcomings that make it difficult drawing general conclusions. To solve this problem, we propose a taxonomy by which those studies could be classified, taking into account central methodological aspects of magnitude estimation tasks. Finally, we suggest perspectives for future research on magnitude estimations, which might abandon the hunt for the mathematical model that explains estimations best and turn, instead, to investigate the underlying principles of estimations (e.g., strategies) and ways of their improvement.
Keywords: magnitude estimations, mental representation, number line, symbolic and non-symbolic magnitudes, estimation biases
Introduction
There is an ongoing debate among researchers concerned with magnitude estimations on how the relationship between subjective estimations and objective magnitudes may be described best. This issue is important as poor estimation performance – such as in number line estimation tasks – is associated with limited mathematical abilities in children (e.g., Booth and Siegler, 2008; Geary et al., 2008). Furthermore, the characteristics of the underlying mental representation of magnitudes including its systematic biases are often directly inferred from the estimations (e.g., Siegler and Opfer, 2003).
Initially, two fundamental models have been proposed on how magnitudes might be mentally represented. The logarithmic ruler model (Dehaene et al., 1990; Dehaene, 1997) assumes that magnitudes are represented with constant variability on a mental number line. However, representations of larger numbers are located closer to each other and thus overlap compared to smaller numbers. The accumulator model, in contrast, states that magnitudes are represented equidistantly but with proportionally increasing variability (i.e., scalar variability: Gibbon and Church, 1981; Whalen et al., 1999; Huntley-Fenner, 2001). Studies promoting the logarithmic model usually employed relative magnitude estimation tasks such as identifying the larger of two numbers (e.g., Dehaene et al., 1990), while other studies used absolute magnitude estimations that required the approximate transformation between two magnitudes (e.g., generating 23 key presses without counting: Whalen et al., 1999). Only absolute magnitude estimations will be considered further in this article as only these represent estimations in a narrower sense, that is, “a process of translating between alternative quantitative representations, at least one of which is inexact” (Siegler and Booth, 2005, p. 198).
Based on the initial accounts, Siegler and colleagues (e.g., Siegler and Opfer, 2003) investigated how the estimation pattern of magnitudes develops using the number line task. Participants usually mark the position of given numbers on a number line, ranging for instance from 0 to 100 or from 0 to 1000. It has been demonstrated that in young children and for relatively large number ranges, in particular, the estimation pattern exhibits a logarithmic shape, whereas for small number ranges and in older children and adults, the pattern is linear and quite exact, without scalar variability.
This research was the starting point for further studies aiming to explain typical biases in numerical estimations of children and adults. Alternative models to a logarithmic model with constant variability and a linear model with scalar variability have been proposed, that is, segmented linear models (e.g., Ebersbach et al., 2008; Moeller et al., 2009) or a cyclic power model (e.g., Barth and Paladino, 2011). Moreover, a simple power model, adopted from psychophysical research (e.g., Stevens, 1957), was put forward to describe systematic biases in adults' numerical estimations (e.g., Crollen et al., 2011). A debate has started and is still going on about which model is best suited to explain the relationship between estimations and actual magnitudes (e.g., Barth and Paladino, 2011; Opfer et al., 2011; Ashcraft and Moore, 2012; Bouwmeester and Verkoeijen, 2012).
In the present article, we aim at emphasizing that studies employing absolute magnitude estimations to investigate the characteristics of the mental representation of magnitudes are often hardly to compare as they involve a broad range of methodological approaches that apparently may lead to different outcomes concerning the shape, accuracy, or variability of the estimations. To address this problem, we propose a taxonomy by which many of the conducted (and future) studies may be classified, which might help to evaluate the comparability, reliability, and validity of studies. Finally, implications for future research will be suggested.
Lacking comparability of the studies
Studies involving absolute magnitude estimations differ broadly with regard to the tasks, the stimuli, and the methods of analysis. Hence, even additional studies might provide no further clarity on children's and adults' estimation abilities and the nature of their underlying mental representations as long as apples and pears are collected into the same basket. In the following, we will give some examples that are directly related to the taxonomy proposed later.
First, estimations can be conceived as numerical or non-numerical (Siegler and Booth, 2005). Numerical estimations involve a magnitude in a symbolic format (i.e., a number word or a numeral) that has to be transferred approximately into another – symbolic or non-symbolic – magnitude (e.g., telling the number of dots), or vice versa. This type will be referred to as symbolic estimations in the following. Non-numerical – or non-symbolic estimations, in contrast, refer to the approximate transformation between two non-symbolic magnitudes (e.g., reproducing a number of dots by key presses). Crollen et al. (2011) showed that symbolic and non-symbolic estimations of adults differ both qualitatively and quantitatively. Symbolic estimations yielded typical biases – that is, under- or overestimations, respectively – that could be well described by a power function. Non-symbolic estimations (i.e., reproduction task), in contrast, were relatively accurate and were described best by a largely linear function.
The differences between symbolic and non-symbolic estimations might be explained by the assumption of format-dependent representations of magnitudes (e.g., Dehaene, 1992; Cohen Kadosh et al., 2011; Lyons et al., 2012; for a review see Cohen Kadosh et al., 2008), although a format-independent representation has been proposed, too (e.g., McCloskey et al., 1985; Barth et al., 2003; Walsh, 2003). Evidence for format-dependent representations comes from fMRI measures showing that different formats activate distinct brain regions (e.g., Vogel et al., 2013). Furthermore, Roggeman et al. (2007) provided evidence that (at least small) symbolic magnitudes are mentally represented by place codes, that is, as activation of a specific position on the mental number line, corresponding to the target magnitude. Non-symbolic magnitudes, in contrast, are represented by summation codes, that is, as activation of a whole segment of the number line up to the corresponding position of the target magnitude. Place codes reflect a local and thus more precise activation on the number line than summation codes and might thus explain a higher accuracy of symbolic compared to non-symbolic estimations. Furthermore, it has been assumed that different transformation paths exist between distinct representational codes (e.g., bi-directional mapping model: Castronovo and Seron, 2007) that might differently affect children's estimations, in particular, whose number knowledge is not fully developed yet. They might perform poorer in symbolic estimations that require the comprehension or production of number symbols, compared to non-symbolic estimations. Evidence for this assumption stems from children's magnitude comparisons (see Rousselle and Noël, 2007) as well as from differential effects of language characteristics on number line estimations (Helmreich et al., 2011). However, most of the studies so far that strived at examining the mental representation of magnitudes involved only symbolic estimations, which is in particular true for research with children (for an exception see Mejias et al., 2012). It might be worthwhile to directly compare the performance in symbolic and non-symbolic estimations and to relate it to the symbolic number knowledge. Sasanguie et al. (2012) have for instance found that children's performance in both a symbolic and a non-symbolic number line task were highly correlated but that only the symbolic task performance was associated with math performance–even if controlled for non-symbolic task performance (see also Sasanguie et al., 2013).
Furthermore, different types of tasks were used within symbolic estimations, such as position-to-number tasks (or perception tasks), where symbolic numbers have to be assigned to given non-symbolic magnitudes (e.g., Ashcraft and Moore, 2012) and number-to-position tasks (or production tasks), where non-symbolic magnitudes have to be generated that match given symbolic numbers (e.g., Barth and Paladino, 2011). Crollen et al. (2011) have shown that both tasks yield opposing biases (i.e., over-estimations in the production task and underestimations in the perception task) and different error rates in adults. A poorer performance in a production-like task compared to a perception-like task was also reported for children (Mundy and Gilmore, 2009; Mejias et al., 2012).
In addition, the target stimuli to be estimated differed. Continuous stimuli, such as in the number line paradigm (e.g., Siegler and Opfer, 2003), and discrete stimuli (e.g., numbers of dots, Crollen et al., 2011) have been used. Boyer et al. (2008) showed that children perform better in proportional judgments of liquids when they were presented as continuous amounts than by discrete units–probably as discrete magnitudes allured them to apply counterproductive counting mechanisms and suppressed a more intuitive approach. Moreover, children were also more accurate in comparing continuous (i.e., lengths of bars) than discrete magnitudes (i.e., numbers of dots; Barth et al., 2009).
Taken together, research so far has shown that the estimation type (i.e., symbolic vs. non-symbolic), task type (i.e., perception, production), and target type (continuous vs. discrete) might differently affect the shape and accuracy of magnitude estimations as well as the direction of the biases in terms of under- and overesti-mations. The next two issues refer to the variability and, again, to the shape of the estimations and, relatedly, to the inferred shape of the underlying mental representation of magnitudes.
Magnitude estimations can be bounded (e.g., number line tasks with lower and upper anchor points: Siegler and Opfer, 2003) or unbounded with no upper anchor cue (e.g., Booth and Siegler, 2006, Exp. 1; Whalen et al., 1999; Cohen and Blanc-Goldhammer, 2011). This issue is relevant in particular for the question of whether or not the estimations exhibit the signature of scalar variability. It seems likely that only unbounded tasks with no upper anchor cue would yield scalar variability as they do not allow for adjusting large estimations to an upper limit (Ebersbach et al., 2008). Furthermore, young children who lack an understanding of large numbers might fail to use the upper numerical anchor and their estimations thus might exhibit scalar variability, too, in cases where the upper anchor is unfamiliar.
Moreover, within the bounded number line tasks, many studies provided a lower and an upper anchor, such as 0 or 1 and 100 (e.g., Siegler and Opfer, 2003; Siegler and Booth, 2004; Booth and Siegler, 2006; Laski and Siegler, 2007; Opfer and Siegler, 2007; Ebersbach et al., 2008; Opfer and Thompson, 2008; Thompson and Opfer, 2008; Ashcraft and Moore, 2012; Ebersbach, in press), while in other studies the location of an additional reference point was explicitly referred to in the pre-test (e.g., the location of 50 on a number line of 0–100; Barth and Paladino, 2011; Bouwmeester and Verkoeijen, 2012; Slusser et al., 2013). The explicit indication of an additional reference point might have affected the shape of the estimations and facilitated the calibration of the estimations around the additional reference point. As a result, estimations might be best described by a cyclic power model with relative accurate estimations near the reference points, while the absence of a third reference point might rather yield a better fit with a logarithmic model.
Methodological taxonomy
So far, we illustrated methodological differences between studies that might account for the often heterogeneous findings concerning the shape, variability, and accuracy of magnitude estimations. To solve this shortcoming, we propose a taxonomy into which each of the used paradigms might be classified (see Figure 1). This taxonomy accounts for (1) the question of whether symbolic numerals (or number words) are involved in the estimations or not (i.e., symbolic vs. non-symbolic estimations), (2) the type of the estimation tasks (i.e., perception, production, reproduction), (3) the type of the target stimuli (i.e., discrete vs. continuous), (4) the potential range of the estimations (i.e., bounded vs. unbounded), and (5) whether an additional reference point was provided or not. For instance, a classical number line paradigm (e.g., Siegler and Opfer, 2003) involves symbolic/numerical estimations in terms of a production task, in which the target stimuli are continuous, the estimation range is bounded by anchors and no additional reference point is provided.
Suggestions for future research
First of all, it could be useful to systematically manipulate the methodological aspects proposed in the taxonomy in magnitude estimation tasks. This might allow determining if previous findings concerning the shape, variability, and accuracy of magnitude estimations and their underlying mental representation, as well as the emergence of systematic estimation skills in the course of development are generalizable or if they apply only to certain paradigms. If the latter was true, the different paradigms might tap different mental representations or, perhaps a certain paradigm might be no reliable and valid instrument to investigate the underlying mental representation (cf. Moeller and Nuerk, 2011).
Second, when aiming at identifying the mathematical model that explains magnitude estimations best, one has to take into account that model fits are affected, amongst others, by the number of trials, the number of parameters of the model, the question of whether a constant is estimated or not, and by the intra-individual variability of the estimations, which is relatively large in young children, in particular. One way to account for the model errors as well as for the number of free parameters would be using the Akaike information criterion (AIC), though it refers only to the relative fit of alternative models.
Third, shortcomings in deciding which model describes the shape of estimations of individual participants best should be prevented. Previous approaches largely differed, ranging from inferential statistics (i.e., comparing adjusted R2 values or the absolute values of the residuals of each model by t-tests: Siegler and Opfer, 2003; Moeller et al., 2009; Berteletti et al., 2012; though it cannot even be assumed that these parameters are Gaussian: May et al., 1989; Edgington and Onghena, 2007) to pure descriptive accounts (i.e., comparing R2 values or likelihoods of each model by visual inspection: Thompson and Opfer, 2008; Barth and Paladino, 2011; Cohen and Blanc-Goldhammer, 2011; Ashcraft and Moore, 2012). A descriptive approach provides maximally a heuristic but not a reliable decision rule (Glover and Dixon, 2004). Even slight differences between concurrent model fits might be overvalued if estimations were, for instance, classified as being linear only because the fit of a linear model is R2 = 0.731 and that of an alternative model is R2 = 0.730. In this regard, a statistically based classification method seems necessary to avoid arbitrary results (see also Moeller and Nuerk, 2011; Bouwmeester and Verkoeijen, 2012). In addition, if individual data will be analyzed in future research, a larger number of trials should be used to get more reliable data – although it also needs to be considered that a multitude of trials might reduce participants' motivation and yield interfering learning effects.
A fourth remark refers to the assumption, made implicitly or explicitly, that linear relationships between estimations and actual magnitudes, as reflected for instance by a better fit of a linear model (e.g., Siegler and Opfer, 2003), are the “idealized developmental endpoint of numerical estimation” (Ashcraft and Moore, 2012, p. 256; see also Hollands et al., 2002). Even if estimations rather obey a linear function, they might significantly and systematically deviate from the actual values, depending on the slope and intercept of the fitted linear function. Thus, even if equidis-tance between neighboring numbers is assumed, the estimations might deviate fundamentally from the actual values (cf. Moeller and Nuerk, 2011). In turn, estimations that are better explained by a power or logarithmic model might correspond on average better to the actual magnitudes than a linear model. However, the use of the best-fitting function might be questioned if the functions make similar predictions with respect to the observable estimation behavior (cf. Wagenaar, 1975; Dehaene, 2001; Thompson and Opfer, 2008; Cantlon et al., 2009). It thus might be useful to consider not only the shape of the estimations but also the accuracy in terms of both absolute and simple deviations from the actual values, as well as the variability of the estimations (see also Holloway and Ansari, 2008; White and Szucs, 2012).
Given the current state of research, future research might rather focus on conditions that lead to biased magnitude estimations and on how these estimations and the underlying “number sense” (Dehaene, 1997) might be improved. First attempts were already provided in the field of developmental research, where number games have proven to support equidistance in the mental representation of numbers (e.g., Wilson et al., 2006; Siegler and Ramani, 2008; Whyte and Bull, 2008). Other approaches might include promoting the familiarity with (e.g., Ebersbach et al., 2008) and the embodiment of numbers (e.g., Fischer et al., 2011) as potential precursors of an appropriate representation of the number system. Furthermore, the nature of estimation processes might be inspected further, such as when and how anchor cues are used or internally created–in particular in the course of development (see Schneider et al., 2008; Ashcraft and Moore, 2012; White and Szucs, 2012). It has been shown that adults use anchors to adjust their numerical estimations (Izard and Dehaene, 2008), but studies on whether children are able to do so and how their use of anchors might be affected (e.g., number knowledge, working memory) are rare (for exceptions see Newman and Berger, 1984; Petitto, 1990). Thus, the question of how and which estimation strategies are applied should be addressed. In addition, as strategies affect estimations (e.g., Ashcraft and Moore, 2012) one might question the fundamental assumption underlying the use of estimation paradigms, namely that estimations are a probate instrument to tap the underlying mental representation at all (Gescheider, 1988; Moeller and Nuerk, 2011). To sum up, we put forward a taxonomy that might contribute to a better comparability of studies on absolute magnitude estimations. We propose that the research focus might switch from trying to identify the model that describes the estimations best toward conditions and strategies that lead to estimation biases and toward procedures that might ward off these biases.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The preparation of this article was supported by a Grant of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG: EB462/1-1) to the first author and a GOA Grant 2012/010 from the Research Fund K.U. Leuven, Belgium.
References
- Ashcraft M. H., Moore A. M. (2012). Cognitive processes of numerical estimation in children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 111, 246–267 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barth H., Baron A., Spelke E., Carey S. (2009). Children's multiplicative transformations of discrete and continuous quantities. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 103, 441–454 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barth H., Kanwisher N., Spelke E. (2003). The construction of large number representations in adults. Cognition 86, 201–221 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00178-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barth H. C., Paladino A. M. (2011). The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift. Dev. Sci. 14, 125–135 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00962.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berteletti I., Lucangeli D., Zorzi M. (2012). Representation of numerical and non-numerical order in children. Cognition 124, 304–313 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Booth J. L., Siegler R. S. (2006). Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. Dev. Psychol. 41, 189–201 10.1037/0012-1649.41.6.189 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Booth J. L., Siegler R. S. (2008). Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. Child Dev. 79, 1016–1031 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01173.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouwmeester S., Verkoeijen P. P. J. L. (2012). Multiple representations in number line estimation: a developmental shift or cases of representations? Cogn. Instr. 30, 246–260 10.1080/07370008.2012.68938418066732 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Boyer T. W., Levine S. C., Huttenlocher J. (2008). Development of proportional reasoning: where young children go wrong. Dev. Psychol. 44, 1478–1490 10.1037/a0013110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cantlon J. F., Cordes S., Libertus M. E., Brannon E. M. (2009). Comment on “Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures”. Science 323, 5910 10.1126/science.1164773 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castronovo J., Seron X. (2007). Numerical estimation in blind subjects: evidence of the impact of blindness and its following experience. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 33, 1089–1106 10.1037/0096-1523.33.5.1089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen D. J., Blanc-Goldhammer D. (2011). Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 331–338 10.3758/s13423-011-0059-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen Kadosh R., Lammertyn J., Izard V. (2008). Are numbers special? An overview of chronometric, neuroimaging, developmental, and comparative studies of magnitude representation. Prog. Neurobiol. 84, 132–147 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.11.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen Kadosh R., Bahrami B., Walsh V., Butterworth B., Popescu T., Price C. J. (2011). Specialisation in the human brain: the case of numbers. Front. Hum. Neurosci., 5:62 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crollen V., Castronovo J., Seron X. (2011). Under- and over-estimation. Exp. Psychol. 58, 39–49 10.1027/1618-3169/a000064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S. (1992). Varieties of numer-ical abilities. Cognition 44, 1–42 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90049-N [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S. (1997). The Number Sense. New York: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S. (2001). Subtracting pigeons: logarithmic or linear? Psychol. Sci. 12, 244–246 10.1111/1467-9280.00343 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S., Dupoux E., Mehler J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 16, 626–641 10.1037/0096-1523.16.3.626 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebersbach M. (in press). Evidence for a spatial-numerical association in kindergartners using a number line task. J. Cogn. Dev. [Google Scholar]
- Ebersbach M., Luwel K., Frick A., Onghena P., Verschaffel L. (2008). The relationship between the shape of the mental number line and familiarity with numbers in 5- to 9-year-old children: evidence for a segmented linear model. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 99, 1–17 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edgington E. S., Onghena P. (2007). Randomization Tests, 4th Edn Boca Raton, FL: Chapman and Hall/CRC [Google Scholar]
- Fischer U., Moeller K., Bientzle M., Cress U., Nuerk H.-C. (2011). Sensori-motor spatial training of number magnitude representation. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 177–183 10.3758/s13423-010-0031-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geary D. C., Hoard M. K., Nugent L., Byrd-Craven J. (2008). Development of number line representations in children with mathematical learning disability. Dev. Neuropsychol. 33, 277–299 10.1080/87565640801982361 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gescheider G. A. (1988). Psychophysical scaling. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 39, 169–200 10.1146/annurev.ps.39.020188.001125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbon J., Church R. M. (1981). Time left: linear versus logarithmic subjective time. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 7, 87–107 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glover S., Dixon P. (2004). Likelihood ratios: a simple and flexible statistic for empirical psychologists. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 11, 791–806 10.3758/BF03196706 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helmreich I., Zuber J., Pixner S., Kaufmann L., Nuerk H.-C., Moeller K. (2011). Language effects on children's mental number line: how cross-cultural differences in number word systems affect spatial mappings of numbers in a non-verbal task. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 42, 598–613 10.1177/0022022111406026 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hollands J. G., Tanaka T., Dyre B. P. (2002). Understanding bias in pro-portion production. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 3, 563–574 10.1037/0096-1523.28.3.563 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holloway I. D., Ansari D. (2008). Domain-specific and domain-general changes in children's development of number comparison. Dev. Sci. 11, 644–649 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00712.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huntley-Fenner G. (2001). Children's understanding of number is similar to adults' and rats': numerical estimation by 5–7-year-olds. Cognition 78, B27–B40 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00122-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izard V., Dehaene S. (2008). Calibrating the mental number line. Cognition 106, 1221–1247 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laski E. V., Siegler R. S. (2007). Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. Child Dev. 76, 1723–1743 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01087.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyons I. M., Ansari D., Beilock S. L. (2012). Symbolic estrangement: evidence against a strong association between numerical symbols and the quantities they represent. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 635–641 10.1037/a0027248 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- May R. B., Masson M. E. J., Hunter M. A. (1989). Randomization tests: viable alternatives to normal curve tests. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 21, 482–483 10.3758/BF03202823 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McCloskey M., Caramazza A., Basili A. (1985). Cognitive mechanisms in number processing and calculation: evidence from dyscal-culia. Brain Cogn. 4, 171–196 10.1016/0278-2626(85)90069-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mejias S., Mussolin C., Rousselle L., Grégoire J., Noël M.-P. (2012). Numerical and non-numerical estimation in children with and without mathematical learning disabilities. Child Neuropsychol. 18, 550–575 10.1080/09297049.2011.625355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moeller K., Nuerk H.-C. (2011). Psychophysics of numerical representation: why seemingly logarithmic representations may rather be multi-linear. J. Psychol. 219, 64–70 10.1027/2151-2604/a000048 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moeller K., Pixner S., Kaufmann L., Nuerk H.-C. (2009). Children's early mental number line: logarithmic or decomposed linear? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 103, 503–515 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mundy E., Gilmore C. K. (2009). Children's mapping between symbolic and nonsymbolic representations of number. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 103, 490–502 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman R. S., Berger C. F. (1984). Children's numerical estimation: flexibility in the use of counting. J. Educ. Psychol. 78, 55–64 10.1037/0022-0663.76.1.55 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Opfer J. E., Siegler R. S. (2007). Representational change and children's numerical estimation. Cogn. Psychol. 55, 169–195 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Opfer J. E., Siegler R. S., Young C. J. (2011). The powers of noise-fitting: reply to Barth and Paladino. Dev. Sci. 14, 1194–1204 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01070.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Opfer J. E., Thompson C. A. (2008). The trouble with transfer: insights from microgenetic changes in the representation of numerical magnitude. Child Dev. 79, 790–806 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01158.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petitto A. (1990). Development of numberline and measurement concepts. Cogn. Instr. 7, 55–78 10.1207/s1532690xci0701_316716212 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Roggeman C., Verguts T., Fias W. (2007). Priming reveals differential coding of symbolic and non-symbolic quantities. Cognition 105, 380–394 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rousselle L., Noël M. P. (2007). Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: a comparison of symbolic vs. non-symbolic number magnitude processing. Cognition 102, 361–395 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sasanguie D., de Smedt B., Defever E., Reynvoet B. (2012). Association between basic numerical abilities and mathematics achievement. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30, 344–357 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02048.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sasanguie D., Göbel S. M., Moll K., Smets K., Reynvoet B. (2013). Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number-space mappings: what underlies mathematics achievement? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 114, 418–431 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider M., Heine A., Thaler V., Torbeyns J., De Smedt B., Verschaffel L., et al. (2008). A validation of eye movements as a measure of elementary school children's developing number sense. Cogn. Dev. 23, 409–422 10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.07.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Siegler R. S., Booth J. L. (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child Dev. 75, 428–444 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00684.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegler R. S., Booth J. L. (2005). Development of numerical estimation: a review, in Handbook of Mathematical Cognition, ed. Campbell J. I. D. (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; ), 197–212 [Google Scholar]
- Siegler R. S., Opfer J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation: evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychol. Sci. 14, 237–243 10.1111/1467-9280.02438 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegler R. S., Ramani G. B. (2008). Playing linear numerical board games promotes low-income children's numerical development. Dev. Sci. 11, 655–661 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00714.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slusser E. B., Santiago R. T., Barth H. C. (2013). Developmental change in numerical estimation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142, 193–208 10.1037/a0028560 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychol. Rev. 64, 153–181 10.1037/h0046162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson C. A., Opfer J. E. (2008). Costs and benefits of representational change: effect of context on age and sex differences in magnitude estimation. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 101, 20–51 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogel S. E., Grabner R. H., Schneider M., Siegler R. S., Ansari D. (2013). Overlapping and distinct brain regions involved in estimating the spatial position of numerical and non-numerical magnitudes: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 51, 979–989 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagenaar W. A. (1975). Stevens vs Fechner: a plea for dismissal of the case. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 39, 225–235 10.1016/0001-6918(75)90037-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 7, 483–488 10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whalen J., Gallistel C. R., Gel-man R. (1999). Non-verbal counting in humans: the psychophysics of number representation. Psychol. Sci. 10, 130–137 10.1111/1467-9280.00120 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- White S. L. J., Szucs D. (2012). Representational change and strategy use in children's number line estimation during the first years of primary school. Behav. Brain Funct. 8, 1 10.1186/1744-9081-8-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whyte J. C., Bull R. (2008). Number games, magnitude representation, and basic number skills in preschoolers. Dev. Psychol. 44, 588–596 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.588 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson A. J., Revkin S. K., Cohen D., Cohen L., Dehaene S. (2006). An open trial assessment of “The Number Race”, an adaptive computer game for remediation of dyscalculia. Behav. Brain Funct. 2, 20 10.1186/1744-9081-2-20 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]