In March 2012 Henry et al. published a paper that explored whether or not the consumption of thiamethoxam via nectar could be a causal factor of Colony Collapse Disorder (CCD) in honeybees. In the first part of their report, Henry et al. (2012) measured the homing success after “ecologically relevant” thiamethoxam exposure and compared it to non-thiamethoxam exposed, control homing rates [see Guez (2013) for a critique of this aspect of their work]. In the second part of their report, they applied their homing study results to the honeybee population dynamic model devised by Khoury et al. (2011), and from that they concluded that dietary thiamethoxam intoxication may potentially contribute to CCD.
Khoury et al.'s (2011) model is build upon the hypothesis that colony failure occurs when bee death rate become unsustainable at the colony level, and the salient assumption that mortality within the hive is negligible. Khoury et al.'s (2011) model allows the evolution of the honeybee hive population to be projected over time. Model outputs are dependent on the total hive population (at the start, and then at any given time), the queen's egg laying rate (L), an eclosion rate that is directly dependent upon the hive population and modulated via the parameter w (the larger w the lower the eclosion rate), and the forager mortality rate or forager homing failure (m). In Khoury et al.'s (2011) model therefore, the population growth of the colony is controlled mainly by the parameters L and w [but see Cresswell and Thompson (2012) for a critique of the choice of w], whereas population decline is dependent on m, the forager mortality rate or forager homing failure.
In order to model the population dynamic under dietary thiamethoxam exposure, Henry et al.'s (2012) undertaking was to calculate the homing failure due to pesticide exposure (mhf). mhf was then used to increase the value of m for population projection under a dietary thiamethoxam exposed scenario, compared to the “normal” homing failure m postulated in the non-thiamethoxam exposed scenario. In my previous critic of Henry et al. (2012) I pointed out that the way in which Henry et al. calculated mhf was incorrect given the authors claim that mhf “[…] estimates the proportion of exposed foragers that might disappear due solely to post-exposure homing failure, all other sources of mortality or homing failure set apart (natural mortality, predation, manipulation stress).”
In their answer to my critic (Guez, 2013) Henry and Decourtye (2013) maintain that the formula used in Henry et al. (2012) to calculate homing failure in honeybees post pesticide exposure (mhf) is correct. In this note I show that the calculated mhf value is largely impacted by assumptions, and that regardless of which assumption is taken, Henry et al. (2012) formula for calculating honeybee homing failure post-pesticide exposure is incorrect.
In their original report Henry et al. (2012) propose that:
(1) |
whereas Guez (2013) contends that it should be:
(2) |
To evaluate the validity of the formula used to calculate mhf it is important to understand that mhf is used to estimate the m parameter of the Khoury et al. (2011) population dynamic model after pesticide intoxication. The parameter m represents the attrition of foragers with time and is expressed in individuals.day−1. In the absence of pesticide exposure Henry et al. (2012) fixed this parameter to m = 0.154 individuals.day−1, assuming an expected forager lifespan of about 6.5 days in line with previously published results [see Henry et al. (2012) for details]. However, with pesticide exposure, it is difficult to discern exactly how the m parameter was estimated in Henry et al.'s (2012) original publication.
In their supplemental material Henry et al. (2012) write as follows (emphasis added):
We ran simulations under the hypotheses of (i) constant forager death rate with no forager exposure, and (ii) forager death rate raised by post-exposure homing failure mhf during a 30-days oilseed rape flowering period […]. In the later configuration, exposed foragers were assigned a probability of disappearance combining daily death rate and the additional mortality due solely to post-exposure homing failure.
From this statement it is clear that: (i) mhf was used by Henry et al. (2012) to raise the homing failure rate after pesticide exposition, and (ii) mhf represents the post-exposure homing failure.
Using the mhf formula put forward by Henry et al. (2012) and Henry and Decourtye (2013)
If we use the equation put forward by Henry et al. (2012) (Equation 1), mhf is expressed as a proportional decrease in homing success in the treatment group relative to the control, as highlighted in the comment of Henry and Decourtye (2013). In the case of the upper bound mhf determined in Experiment 2, Henry et al. (2012) found that there was 0.316 less homing success in the treatment group than in the control group. In other words, there was 31.6% less homing success with treatment. However, since the parameter m of Khoury et al. (2011) is intended to represent the attrition of foragers with time, what is needed is the proportional increase or decrease in homing failure of the treatment given the control, not the decrease in homing success of the treatment given the control. As counterintuitive as it may be, these two values (i.e., the proportional increase in homing failure and the proportional decrease in homing success) are not numerically equivalent.
Taking Experiment 2 in Henry et al. (2012) as an example, homing success was reported as 0.83 and 0.57 individuals.day−1 in the respective control and treatment groups, meaning that 0.316 less individuals.day−1 returned to their hive in the treatment group relative to the control. Indeed, [treatment homing success] = 0.83 − (0.83 × 0.316) = 0.57 individuals.day−1. However, these homing success rates also mean that 0.17 and 0.43 individuals.day−1 failed to return in the respective control and treatment groups ([homing failure] = 1 − [homing success]). This translates into a 1.55 fold (155%) additional increase in homing failure rate relative to the control (0.17 individuals.day−1), and represents a treatment homing failure of 0.43 individuals.day−1:
If mhf expresses the proportional increase in homing failure in the treatment given the control it should be calculated as follows:
(31) |
and not as proposed by Henry et al. (2012):
(1) |
Therefore, mhf should equal 0.59 and 1.55 for Experiments 1 and 2 respectively, not 0.102 and 0.310 as stated in Henry et al. (2012). The mhf values reported by Henry et al. (2012) are therefore erroneous and stem from the use of an incorrect formula that calculates the proportional decrease in homing success instead of a proportional increase in homing failure in the treatment relative to the control. In their supplemental material Henry et al. (2012) claim that the mhf value they calculated “[…] estimates the proportion of exposed foragers that might disappear due solely to post-exposure homing failure, all other sources of mortality or homing failure set apart (natural mortality, predation, manipulation stress)” (emphasis added). On the contrary, it assumes that the additional mortality due to pesticide exposure is proportional to all other sources of mortality or homing failure. This is also regardless of the fact that Henry et al. (2012) failed to note that the proportional decrease in homing success was not equivalent to the proportional increase in homing failure in the treatment given the control. In fact, if mhf is expressed as in Equation 3, we assume that the additional homing failure with pesticide exposure is proportional to the observed, non-exposed homing failure. The same is true if we erroneously use Equation 1 to calculate mhf as Henry et al. (2012). Thus, if mhf is expressed as a proportional increase of homing failure give the “normal” attrition rate, it impacts upon how the m parameter of Khoury et al. (2011) need to be estimated:
Therefore, the m parameter of Khoury et al.'s (2011) population dynamic model should be (mhf × 100%) larger following pesticide exposure than the m parameter that is used under non pesticide exposure conditions.
Using the mhf formula put forward by Guez (2013)
The homing failure of the control group reflects normal attrition or mortality and the homing failure attributable directly to experimental stress. For instance:
(4) |
In contrast, homing failure observed in the treatment group is due not only to natural mortality and the homing failure attributable directly to experimental stress, but also to the homing failure that is induced by exposure to pesticides:
(5) |
If mhf is the homing failure that is solely induced by pesticide treatment, and given Equations 4 and 5, then:
In this form mhf does indeed “[…] estimates the proportion of exposed foragers that might disappear due solely to post-exposure homing failure, all other sources of mortality or homing failure set apart (natural mortality, predation, manipulation stress)” as suggested by Henry et al. (supplemental material, 2012; emphasis added).
Importantly, since [Control homing success], [Control homing failure], [Treatment homing success], and [Treatment homing failure] are expressed in individuals.day−1, mhf is also expressed in individuals.day−1. In using Equation 2 to calculate mhf, we assume that pesticide exposure increases homing failure by a set amount and is not proportional to any “normal” homing failure. If so, the m parameter of the Khoury et al. (2011) model should be estimated using:
Assumptions and consequences
The choice of Equations 2 or 3 as the basis for the calculation of mhf is solely dependent on the assumptions taken. If we choose to use Equation 2, we assume that the homing failure attributable to the exposure of a given dose of pesticide is a fixed value regardless of the “normal” homing failure [assumption (a)]. In this case we assume that most of the homing failure observed in the control is due to natural predation. However, if we choose Equation 3 we assume that the homing failure attributable to pesticide exposure is not only a fixed value in function of the dose of pesticide, but is proportional to the level of the “normal” homing failure [assumption (b)]. For example, we would assume that most of the natural homing failure is due to an aging population of foragers which are more susceptible to the effects of pesticides in comparison to young foragers. We favor assumption (a), although others may disagree.
Nonetheless, and notwithstanding Guez's (2013) previous critic of Henry et al. (2012), if we use the correct formula as set forth herein, choosing assumptions (a) or (b) has only a minimal impact on Khoury et al.'s (2011) model projection, as exemplified by the green and red curves in Figure 1. However, the difference in model projections that are obtained under assumptions (a) and (b) can become quite significant if the assumed “normal homing failure” (here 0.154 individuals.day−1) is far removed from the experimentally determined control homing probability. For example, let us imagine that due to a heightened experimental stress the control homing probability is only 0.7 individuals.day−1 and the treatment homing probability is 40% less (i.e., 0.42 individuals.day−1). If we use assumption (a), after 30 days of exposure we project more than 4000 less individuals in the hive than if we use assumption (b) (see Figure 1, continuous and dotted black lines). Thus, our assumptions are not without consequence on our model projections, highlighting the need for researchers to explicit all assumptions, and to describe exactly how such assumptions would impact upon outputs within the model used. This is particularly important for models projections that claim to have direct ecological significance, such as the one presented by Henry et al. (2012). Without this crucial information and an understanding of the consequences of various assumptions on model projection, model end points cannot be accurately interpreted and thus cannot be used for accurate or meaningful decision making in the field.
Conclusion
This contribution highlights the influence of assumptions on model projections and the importance of making explicit not only any assumptions, but also how these influence outputs within a given model. It also highlights that the formula used to calculate mhf in Henry et al. (2012) (Equation 1), is conceptually flawed since it calculates the proportional decrease in post exposure homing success given the control instead of the proportional increase in post exposure homing failure. These two values are not numerically equivalent and thus, even without taking into account Guez's (2013) critique, the model projections presented by Henry et al. (2012) are erroneous and cannot be used as a realistic evaluation of the potential impact of dietary thiamethoxam exposure on foraging honeybees. Furthermore, if as Henry et al. (2012) claimed mhf was intended to solely represent the post-exposure homing failure: “all other sources of mortality or homing failure set apart (natural mortality, predation, manipulation stress),” mhf should have been calculated using Equation 3 as previously described in Guez (2013). Nonetheless, and regardless of the assumptions made by Henry et al. (2012) when calculating mhf, the mhf value presented by Henry et al. (2012) is erroneous as the equation used to calculate it is conceptually flawed. Thus, Henry et al.'s (2012) model projection should not be used as the basis for any meaningful regulatory decision making about the potential risks posed by dietary thiamethoxan intoxication in honeybees.
Acknowledgment
I would like to thank C. Conway for her feedback during the elaboration of this paper.
Footnotes
1It is important to note that Equation 3 is not equivalent to Equation 1.
2Cresswell and Thomson implementation is available at is available online from the Exeter Research and Institutional Content archive (ERIC) at http://hdl.handle.net/10036/3648. To run simulation under assumption (a) [mpesticide = mnon − exposure + (mnon − exposure × mhf)] one need to modify how the parameter m is calculated. To this end the formula contained in cell B3 should be changed to “= background + (B16 × background).”
References
- Cresswell J. E., Thompson H. M. (2012). Comment on “A common pesticide decreases foraging success and survival in honey bees.” Science 337, 1453 10.1126/science.1224618 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guez D. (2013). A common pesticide decreases foraging success and survival in honey bees: questioning the ecological relevance. Front. Physiol. 4:37 10.3389/fphys.2013.00037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry M., Beguin M., Requier F., Rollin O., Odoux J.-F., Aupinel P., et al. (2012). A common pesticide decreases foraging success and survival in honey bees. Science 336, 348–350 10.1126/science.1215039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry M., Decourtye A. (2013). Ecological relevance in honey bee pesticide risk assessment: developing context-dependent scenarios to manage uncertainty. Front. Physiol. 4:62 10.3389/fphys.2013.00062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khoury D. S., Myerscough M. R., Barron A. B. (2011). A quantitative model of honey bee colony population dynamics. PLoS ONE 6:e18491 10.1371/journal.pone.0018491 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]