Abstract
Background
Impairments in executive function characterize offenders with antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) and offenders with psychopathy. However, the extent to which those impairments are associated with ASPD, psychopathy, or both is unknown.
Methods
The present study examined 17 violent offenders with ASPD and psychopathy (ASPD+P), 28 violent offenders with ASPD without psychopathy (ASPD−P), and 21 healthy non-offenders on tasks assessing cool (verbal working memory and alteration of motor responses to spatial locations) and hot (reversal learning, decision-making under risk, and stimulus-reinforcement-based decision-making) executive function.
Results
In comparison to healthy non-offenders, violent offenders with ASPD+P and those with ASPD−P showed similar impairments in verbal working memory and adaptive decision-making. They failed to learn from punishment cues, to change their behaviour in the face of changing contingencies, and made poorer quality decisions despite longer periods of deliberation. Intriguingly, the two groups of offenders did not differ significantly from the non-offenders in terms of their alteration of motor responses to spatial locations and their levels of risk-taking, indicated by betting, and impulsivity, measured as delay aversion. The performance of the two groups of offenders on the measures of cool and hot executive function did not differ, indicating shared deficits.
Conclusions
These documented impairments may help to explain the persistence of antisocial behaviours despite the known risks of the negative consequences of such behaviours.
Introduction
Most violent crimes are committed by a small group of males who display persistent antisocial and aggressive behaviour from childhood onwards [1], [2]. This life-long pattern of behaviour is indexed by DSM-IV diagnoses of Conduct Disorder (CD) prior to age 15 and Antisocial Personality Disorder (ASPD) in adulthood [3]. Life-long patterns of risk taking and impulsivity are central features of ASPD [4]. Illegal behaviours persist despite repeated criminal sanctions. Neuropsychological deficits in executive function (EF) reflecting the higher order cognitive control of thought, action, and emotion [5] have been hypothesized to be central to the onset and persistence of severe antisocial and aggressive behaviour [6]–[11].
There is accumulating evidence that men with ASPD represent a heterogeneous population with respect to personality traits, aggressive behaviour, offending patterns, [4], and engagement with, and response to, cognitive-behavioural offender rehabilitation programs [12], [13]. While all within this population present an early onset of antisocial behaviour that remains stable over the life-span, a sub-group additionally present psychopathy (ASPD+P), as defined by the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; [14], [15]). Psychopathy is a syndrome characterized by a constellation of affective, interpersonal, and behavioural features [14], including a lack of empathy, callousness, shallow affect and a failure to take responsibility for one's actions, and a pathological interpersonal style involving grandiosity, glibness, superficial charm, and the manipulation of others [16]. Much research has demonstrated that in comparison to offenders without psychopathy, those with ASPD+P begin offending at a younger age [17], more often engage in instrumental aggression [18], and acquire more convictions or charges for violent offences [19], [20].
Consistent with the differences in personality traits and aggressive behaviour that distinguish adult men with ASPD+P and those with ASPD and not psychopathy (ASPD−P), recent evidence suggests that the two groups show distinct emotional impairments [20], affective processing [21], brain response to emotional stimuli when engaged in goal-directed behaviour [22], and structural brain anomalies [23]. Those with ASPD−P are hypothesized to present a low threshold for engaging in reactive aggressive behaviour towards others due to a hyper-sensitivity to threat, both real and perceived, as evidenced by hyperactivity in the amygdala [6]. By contrast, individuals with ASPD+P show hypo-activity in the amygdala in response to threat (e.g., [24]). Importantly, these distinct phenotypes emerge early in childhood [25]. The lack of responsiveness in the amgydala, especially to fearful faces, among adults with ASPD+P (e.g., [26]) and children showing the antecedents of this condition (e.g., [27], [28]) may underlie their impairment in stimulus-reinforcement learning central to passive avoidance paradigms [29]. This impairment has been hypothesized to be a core deficit of ASPD+P that emerges in childhood and limits learning not to engage in instrumental antisocial and aggressive behaviour and learning to engage in prosocial behaviour [29]. Evidence from developmental studies examining children and adolescents is indeed consistent with the notion that different EF impairments are associated with distinct forms of antisocial behaviour and patterns of aggressive behaviour [30], [31].
Cool EF refers to top-down processes subsumed primarily by the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and ventrolateral PFC that are distinctly cognitive in nature and usually elicited by abstract, decontextualized problems. Working memory, response inhibition, planning, sustained attention, and attentional set-shifting are considered to be cool EF [32], [33]. By contrast, hot EF refers to cognitive processes that have an affective, motivational, or incentive/reward component; these processes are generally considered to be subsumed by ventromedial pathways connecting mesolimbic reward circuitry, including the amygdala and striatum, to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) [34]. Appraising the motivational significance of a stimulus in affective decision-making paradigms and reappraising it in response reversal paradigms are considered hot EF [32], [35].
The extent to which cool and hot EF differ in the two types of violent offenders is difficult to determine from the extant literature as few studies have directly compared EF of violent offenders with ASPD−P and ASPD+P. Most previous studies compared offenders with ASPD+P to offenders without psychopathy([36], [37], but see [38]), while others compared individuals with ASPD from the community who had not been assessed for psychopathy (but see [39], [40] for two studies on violent offenders) to either healthy non-offenders [41]–[44] or patients without ASPD with substance use disorders [45], [46].
A large number of studies suggest that offenders with ASPD+P, in comparison to offenders without psychopathy, do not present impairments in cool EF such as attentional set-shifting, planning, and verbal working memory indexing the functional integrity of the DLPFC [47]–[53]. These studies, however, show that offenders with ASPD+P present impairments in cool EF such as response inhibition (e.g., [50]) and in hot EF tasks such as response reversal, behavioural extinction, and affective decision-making indexing the functional integrity of the VMPFC (e.g., [51], [52], [54], [55]). Further evidence of hot EF impairments among offenders with ASPD+P comes from studies showing that they make more commission errors (i.e., responses to stimuli paired with negative reinforcement) than non-psychopathic offenders on passive avoidance learning tasks assessing stimulus-reinforcement-based decision-making.
Only two studies have been published that examined violent offenders with clearly delineated ASPD−P [39], [40]. The majority of investigations of cool EF suggest that men with ASPD−P perform like healthy men on attentional set-shifting tasks, planning and measures of verbal or spatial working memory, all dependent on DLPFC functioning ([39], [41], [45], [56]; but see [57], [58]). By contrast, there is consistent evidence that men with ASPD−P are characterized by hot EF deficits as indicated by their impaired performance on tasks such response reversal and affective decision-making indexing the functional integrity of the VMPFC [43], [44], [46], [57].
To date, only one study [40] has compared violent offenders with ASPD+P and violent offenders with ASPD−P to healthy participants on tasks assessing cool EF (the Stockings of Cambridge planning task, attentional set-shifting on the intra-dimensional/extradimensional [ID/ED] set-shifting task, behavioural inhibition on a Go/No-Go task) and hot EF (response reversal components of the ID/ED task). Based on their scores on the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version (PCL: SV; [59]) the violent offenders with ASPD were divided into three groups (‘low’ = PCL: SV ≤15, ‘medium’ = PCL: SV = 16–19; and ‘high’ = PCL: SV >19). Results indicated that, regardless of psychopathy scores, offenders with ASPD, as compared to the healthy participants, exhibited subtle impairments in cool EF (planning, attentional set-shifting, response inhibition), but no hot EF impairment (i.e., reversal learning). In correlational analyses psychopathy scores were not related to performance on any of the tasks. Taken together, the results of this investigation suggested that violent offenders with ASPD+P and those with ASPD−P exhibit similar cool EF impairments as measured by attentional set-shifting, and similar performance on one index of hot EF (reversal learning). However, in view of the results Dolan [40] concluded that “further studies using a range of DLPFC and VMPFC tasks” (p.8) were needed.
Knowledge of cool and hot EF that are impaired or preserved in each type of violent offender could be used to improve the effectiveness of rehabilitation programs aimed at reducing recidivism [60]. While cognitive-behavioural programs have been shown to reduce criminal recidivism [61], [62], offenders with ASPD+P fail to benefit [12], [13]. Further, such knowledge will contribute to unravelling the etiology of persistent violent behaviour that is a prerequisite for preventing it.
The present study employed a broad range of neuropsychological tests to assess both cool and hot EF among violent offenders with ASPD+P, violent offenders with ASPD−P, and healthy non-offenders. Tests of EF were selected because they have been validated in studies of subjects with lesions in specific brain regions, all but one (Cambridge Gamble Task [CGT]) have been used in previous studies of ASPD+P or ASPD−P, they index processes that have been shown to play an important role in the display of aggressive behaviour (e.g., impairment in working memory), or to be related to core features of either ASPD+P or ASPD−P (e.g., insensitivity to punishment, impulsivity and risk-taking) (Detailed justifications available online in Text S1).
Both violent offenders with ASPD+P and ASPD−P exhibit life-long antisocial behaviour, but they are characterized by differences in personality traits, aggressive behaviour, emotion processing, and in response to interventions aimed at reducing antisocial/criminal behaviour. Consequently, we reasoned that they would show both similarities and differences in neurocognitive performance. First, we hypothesized that both the violent offenders with ASPD+P and those with ASPD−P would show similarly poor performance on two tests assessing cool EF (the Digit Span – Backward and Spatial Alternation Task), and on several tests of hot EF, (CGT, more reversal errors on the Probabilistic Response Reversal Task, more commission on the Passive Avoidance Learning Task) as compared to healthy non-offenders. Second, we hypothesized that the two groups of violent offenders would show one important difference in performance on these tests of hot EF. Consistent with much previous evidence and theorizing about psychopathy [6], [37], we hypothesized that the violent offenders with ASPD+P would make more commission errors on the Passive Avoidance Task than the violent offenders with ASPD−P.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Violent male offenders with ASPD and healthy male non-offenders with English as a first language were recruited from the community for a study of the neurobiological correlates of persistent aggression. Diagnostic interviews indicated that none had a life-time history of severe mental illness or a substance use disorder in the past month, and showed that all obtained a score of 70 or higher on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III; [63]).
Violent offenders with ASPD
Violent offenders were recruited from the National Probation Service. Probation officers identified potential participants with convictions for violent offences (murder, rape, attempted murder, and grievous bodily harm) confirmed by official criminal records. Offenders with a diagnosis of ASPD who obtained a total PCL-R score ≥25 were assigned to the ASPD+P group (n = 17), and those with a score ≥25 were assigned to the ASPD−P group (n = 28).
Healthy non-offenders
Non-offenders (n = 21) were recruited by means of advertisements in local newspapers and notices in the community. Those retained for the study had no criminal record, no mental disorder other than past substance misuse, and a PCL-R score of 24 or less.
Classification measures
Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV
All participants completed the Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, I and II, (SCID; [64]) administered by trained forensic psychiatrists to provide life-time and current DSM-IV diagnoses.
Psychopathy checklist – revised
The PCL-R [16] consists of 20 items that are scored by a trained rater on the basis of a file review and a semi-structured interview. Each of the 20 items is scored on a three-point scale (0–2), with the total score ranging from 0 to 40. Consistent with a validation study [65], a score of 25 or higher identified the syndrome of psychopathy among these European offenders. Forensic psychiatrists and psychologists trained to use the PCL-R administered interviews and extracted information from files in order to rate the scale. Interviews were videotaped and a random 25% sample was rerated by a second trained psychologist. Intra-class correlation coefficient values for PCL-R total scores were acceptable (0.81). Scores for the four facets and total scores were calculated [66].
Neuropsychological measures (Detailed descriptions available in Text S1)
Digit Span – Backward [63]
The Digit Span – Backward is a verbal subtest of the WAIS-R used to measure of verbal working memory [67]. The raw score was used as the dependent variable.
Spatial Alternation Task [47]
This task assesses the alteration of motor responses to spatial locations on the basis of reinforcement information. Two red cars appeared on either side of the computer screen on each trial. The participant had to learn that the side on which the £20 note was located was being alternated after each correct response. The dependent variable was the number of errors committed before achieving 12 consecutive correct responses.
Probabilistic Response Reversal Task [54]
In the acquisition phase, the task assesses the ability to learn stimulus-response associations and, in the reversal phase, the task assesses the ability to alter stimulus-response associations as a function of contingency change. The reinforcement contingencies were probabilistic: the ‘correct’ pair was not always rewarded and the ‘incorrect’ pair was not always punished. There were two test pairs that changed contingency (reversing pairs) and four ‘dummy’ or non-reversing pairs. The two reversing pairs had the following probabilistic contingencies: 100–0; 80–20. The dependent variable was the number of errors committed before reaching the learning criterion of eight consecutive correct responses. If the participants did not meet the learning criterion, total errors made were analysed.
CGT [68]
On each trial, the participant was given 100 points and presented with a row of 10 boxes across the top of the screen, some of which were red and some of which were blue. The ratio of red:blue boxes varied from 1∶9 to 9∶1 in a pseudo-random order. The participant was instructed that the computer had hidden a token in one of the boxes, and that they must guess whether the token had been hidden in of the red or one of the blue boxes. On each trial, the participant first selected the colour (decision stage) and then betted a proportion of his total points on his colour decision (gambling stage). Each bet was presented for 2.5 seconds and offered in descending (95%, 75%, 50%, 25%, 5% of current points) or ascending (5%, 25%, 50%, 75%, 95% of the current points) sequences. After the bet was placed, the hidden token was revealed and the bet was added to or subtracted from the total score. The five principal dependent measures were: (1) Deliberation Time defined as the mean latency in milliseconds from presentation of the coloured boxes to the participant's response; (2) Quality of Decision-Making defined as the proportion of trials on which the participant chose to gamble on the more likely outcome, i.e. the colour of the greatest number of boxes; (3) Risk Taking defined as the percentage of the current points that the participant bet. To maintain the independence of betting behaviour and choice behaviour, analyses were limited to the trials where the participants selected the colour of the majority of boxes, i.e. trials on which they had more chance of winning than losing; (4) Risk Adjustment was defined as the degree to which a participant varies their risk-taking in response to the ratios of red to blue boxes within each trial; and (5) Delay Aversion was defined as the difference between risk-taking scores in the descending and the ascending conditions. High bets in both ascending and descending conditions reflect genuine risk-taking behaviour, whereas betting early in both the ascending and descending conditions reflects impulsivity (the participant does not wait for the bet to increase in the ascending condition or to decrease in the descending condition).
Passive Avoidance Learning Task [36]
The goal was to learn to respond to stimuli that lead to reward and to avoid responding to stimuli that lead to punishment. The participant was presented with 10 blocks of eight trials of distinct number identity. Each number was presented once during a block. Four numbers were associated with punishment (the S –) and four with reward (the S +). Participants were randomly assigned to one of two versions of the task: the numbers that were the S+ and the S – for one task were the S – and the S + in the other task, respectively. Reinforcement values were plus or minus 1, 700, 1400, and 2000 points for the four different S +/S –. The dependent variables were the number of passive avoidance (commission) errors (i.e., when participants approached a S−) and the number of omission errors (i.e., when the participants did not approach a S+).
Ethics statement
The study was approved by the Joint South London and Maudsley and the Institute of Psychiatry NHS Research and Ethics Committee (reference 06/Q0706/87).
Procedure and apparatus
At the first interview, the study was fully explained both verbally and in writing to potential participants. After all of their questions were answered, participants signed consent forms. All potential participants who declined to participate or otherwise did not participate were not disadvantaged in any other way by not participating in the study. Participants included in the study were reimbursed at minimum hourly wage for each hour of testing completed. Participants were strongly encouraged to desist from using substances two weeks prior to participation and during the period of testing.
After all diagnostic interviews were completed, an appointment was scheduled for neuropsychological testing. Participants were reminded not to use drugs/alcohol prior to testing and that on arrival at the laboratory saliva and urine samples would be taken. Each participant was tested individually in a quiet interview room. The computer administered tasks were presented on a Dell Inspiron 510 m Laptop computer with a 15-in. (38.1 cm) colour monitor with participants seated about 0.5 m from the computer.
Data analytic strategy
Data for quality of decision-making on the CGT were highly negatively skewed, with many participants selecting the likely outcome on the large majority of the trials. Normality could not be achieved using an arcsin transformation, thus data for quality of decision-making were analysed using non-parametric Kruskal –Wallis tests in each condition (ascending versus descending), collapsing across box ratio, and in each ratio (9∶1, 8∶2, 7∶3, 6∶4) collapsing across conditions. Significant between-group effects were followed-up using pair-wise post-hoc tests. Deliberation time data were positively skewed, with many participants responding quickly. The distribution of these values was successfully normalised using a logarithmic (log-10) transformation [69]. Data presented in the tables and figures are untransformed.
Continuous variables that conformed to parametric assumptions were analysed using Student's t-test, univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) or repeated measures ANOVA. The Welch t' and F' tests and the Greenhouse-Geisser correction were applied where assumptions about homogeneity of variance and sphericity were violated, respectively [69]. Significant between-group effects were followed-up using pair-wise comparisons with Fisher's LSD procedure, which is the most powerful technique for post-hoc tests involving three groups [69], [70]. Effect sizes are reported as partial eta-squared (ηp 2; small ≥.01, medium ≥.06, large ≥.14) [69]. Categorical variables were analysed using Chi square tests. Results were considered statistically significant at p<.05, two-tailed. Not all the participants completed all the tasks, so degrees of freedom vary slightly across analyses.
Results
Final sample
The characteristics of the three groups are reported in Table 1. The three groups were similar with respect to age, IQ, and ethnicity. As intended, there were significant differences between all three groups on total and 4-facet PCL-R scores. The non-offenders additionally differed from the offenders by presenting significantly fewer conduct disorder symptoms prior to age 15, lower scores for proactive and reactive aggression, and less substance misuse. Consistent with previous studies, the ASPD+P offenders, as compared to the ASPD−P offenders, presented significantly more symptoms of conduct disorder prior to age 15, were significantly younger at first conviction for a violent offence, obtained higher scores for proactive aggression and similar scores for reactive aggression, and there was a trend suggesting that they had accumulated more convictions for violent crimes and they had higher scores for proactive aggression. The proportions of ASPD+P and ASPD−P offenders with substance use disorders were similar.
Table 1. Comparisons of Sociodemographic, Clinical, and Behavioural Characteristics of Non-offenders, Violent Offenders with ASPD−P, and Violent Offenders with ASPD+P.
Measure | Non-offenders (n = 21) | ASPD–P (n = 28) | ASPD+P (n = 17) | Group differences |
Age in years | 35.0 (8.2) | 35.8 (8.4) | 40.0 (9.0) | F(63) = 1.87 |
Full Scale IQ | 95.1 (11.0) | 91.9 (10.2) | 88.9 (9.9) | F(63) = 1.72 |
% Caucasian | 61.9 | 67.9 | 41.2 | χ2(2) = 3.22 |
% with PD other than ASPD | ||||
Cluster A | 0 | 10.7 | 17.6 | χ2(2) = 3.69 |
Cluster B | 0 | 14.3 | 23.5 | χ2(2) = 5.1# |
Cluster C | 0 | 7.1 | 11.8 | χ2(2) = 2.38 |
PCL–R total | 3.8 (2.8)a | 16.7 (4.1)b | 28.3 (2.1)c | F(63) = 260.73*** |
PCL–R Interpersonal facet | 0.4 (1.0)a | 1.7 (1.4)b | 3.9 (1.3)c | F(63) = 36.14*** |
PCL–R Affective facet | 0.6 (0.9)a | 2.9 (1.8)b | 6.2 (2.9)c | F(63) = 63.37*** |
PCL–R Lifestyle | 1.9 (1.5)a | 5.1 (2.1)b | 6.9 (1.8)c | F(63) = 34.91*** |
PCL–R Antisocial | 0.3 (0.6)a | 5.8 (2.1)b | 8.6 (1.4)c | F(63) = 134.60*** |
CD symptoms Counts | 0.7(1.2)a | 4.4 (2.8)b | 7.6 (3.4)a | F(63) = 32.60*** |
Age at first violent convictions | n/a | 23.4 (8.1)a | 16.8 (3.3)b | t' (43) = −3.75** |
Number of violent convictions | n/a | 4.7 (3.4) | 6.9 (5.2) | t(43) = 1.70# |
RPAQ Aggression total | 7.3 (3.4)a | 17.4 (9.1)b | 22.3 (11.3)b | F(62) = 15.90*** |
Proactive aggression | 2.3 (3.3)a | 8.4 (5.4)b | 12.5 (7.1)c | F(62) = 17.66*** |
Reactive aggression | 4.9 (3.1)a | 9.0 (5.8)b | 11.5 (6.5)b | F(62) = 7.49** |
% Alcohol | ||||
Abuse | 11.8 | 25.0 | 26.7 | χ2(2) = 1.39 |
Dependence | 5.9a | 39.3b | 26.7b | χ2(2) = 6.04* |
% Cannabis | ||||
Abuse | 5.9 | 14.3 | 13.3 | χ2(2) = .78 |
Dependence | 11.8 | 32.0 | 25.0 | χ2(2) = 2.27 |
% Cocaine | ||||
Abuse | 0 | 0 | 6.7 | χ2(2) = 3.05 |
Dependence | 0 | 20.0 | 25.0 | χ2(2) = 4.48 |
% Stimulants | ||||
Abuse | 0 | 3.6 | 0 | χ2(2) = 1.16 |
Dependence | 0 | 4.2 | 0 | χ2(2) = 1.23 |
% Sedatives | ||||
Abuse | 0 | 3.6 | 0 | χ2(2) = 1.16 |
Dependence | 0 | 4.0 | 0 | χ2(2) = 1.18 |
% Opioid | ||||
Abuse | 0 | 3.6 | 13.3 | χ2(2) = 3.21 |
Dependence | 0 | 12.0 | 8.3 | χ2(2) = 2.14 |
% Hallucinogenics | ||||
Abuse | 0 | 7.1 | 0 | χ2(2) = 2.37 |
Dependence | 0 | 0 | 0 | n/a |
Note. Unless otherwise stated, means are presented with standard deviations in parentheses for each group. Means with different superscripts within each row indicate a significant difference. PD = Personality Disorder; ASPD–P = Antisocial Personality Disorder without Psychopathy; ASPD+P = Antisocial Personality Disorder with Psychopathy; n/a = Not Applicable; PCL–R = Psychopathy Checklist – Revised (Hare, 2003); RPAQ = Reactive Proactive Aggression Questionnaire (Raine et al., 2006). One offender with ASPD–P did not complete the RPAQ Aggression Questionnaire.
p<.10. ** p<.01. *** p<.001.
Digit Span – Backward
In line with the a priori hypothesis, there was a statistically significant group difference in scores on the Digit Span-Backward task, F' (2, 37.15) = 3.57, p = .038, ηp 2 = .11. Post-hoc tests indicated that the non-offenders (M = 7.33, SD = 3.76) repeated more digits than the ASPD+P (M = 4.69, SD = 2.21; p = .009) and the ASPD−P group, but only at a trend level (M = 5.75, SD = 2.62; p = .07). Scores for the two ASPD groups did not differ (p = .26).
Spatial Alternation Task
No participant failed the task. Contrary to the a priori hypothesis, scores for the three groups on the Spatial Alteration Task did not differ, F(2, 62) = .36, p = .70, ηp 2 = .01 (ASPD+P: M = 3.12, SD = 2.52; ASPD−P: M = 3.96, SD = 7.13; non-offenders: M = 4.52, SD = 3.16). Five participants (one ASPD+P, one ASPD−P and two non-offenders) were identified as outliers with respect to their groups, but removing them from the analyses did not alter the pattern of results, F(2, 57) = 1.38, p = .26, ηp 2 = .05.
Probabilistic Response Reversal Task
All participants reached the learning criterion for the acquisition and reversal phases of the pair with the 100–0 contingency. However, three non-offenders failed to reach the criterion for the acquisition of the pair with the 80–20 contingency. In line with Budhani et al. [54], their data were excluded from the analyses since it was unclear if these participants had learned the stimulus-response association so that response reversal could be examined. In addition, data from one ASPD+P and three ASPD−P offenders and three non-offenders were excluded from the analyses as their scores were more than 2.5 standard deviations above their respective group means.
A 3 (group: ASPD+P, ASPD−P, non-offenders) ×2 (contingency: 100–0 versus 80–20) ×2 (phase: acquisition versus reversal) mixed model ANOVA revealed that there was a statistically significant main effect of group, F(2, 52) = 3.94, p = .03, ηp 2 = .13. Post-hoc tests indicated that men with ASPD−P (M = 15.7, SD = 6.7; p = .01) and those with ASPD+P, albeit at a trend level (M = 14.1, SD = 8.6; p = .07), committed more errors than the non-offenders (M = 9.6, SD = 3.5). Scores of ASPD+P and ASPD−P offenders did not differ (p = .45). There was a highly significant main effect of phase, F(1, 52) = 51.64, p<.001, ηp 2 = .50, indicating that participants committed more errors during the reversal phase (M = 10.6, SD = 6.9) than the acquisition phase (M = 2.9, SD = 2.7) (Figure 1). In addition, there was a highly significant main effect of contingency, F(1, 52) = 49.23, p<.001, ηp 2 = .49. As can been seen from Figure 1, participants committed more errors on the stimulus pair with a 80–20 contingency (M = 10.3, SD = 6.8) than on the stimulus pair with a 100–0 contingency (M = 1.8, SD = 0.2). There was also a significant phase by contingency interaction, F(1, 52) = 10.84, p = .002, ηp 2 = .17. Importantly, there were also a significant group by phase interaction, F(2, 52) = 58.38, p = .02, ηp 2 = .15, and a significant group by phase by contingency interaction, F(1, 52) = 49.23, p = .049, ηp 2 = .11. While the three groups committed a similar number of errors for the two contingency pairs in the acquisition phase and the reversal phase of the 100–0 contingency pair, in comparison to the non-offenders (M = 4.3, SD = 3.5), offenders with ASPD−P (M = 10.5, SD = 6.7; p = .005) and those with ASPD+P, albeit at a trend level (M = 8.1, SD = 7.8; p = .10), committed more errors on the reversal phase of the of the 80–20 contingency pair (Figure 1). The comparison between ASPD+P and ASPD−P was not significant (p = .26).
CGT
Deliberation time
A 3 (group: ASPD+P, ASPD−P, non-offenders) ×4 (ratio: 9∶1, 8∶2, 7∶3, 6∶4) mixed model ANOVA on deliberation time indicated that participants took more time to make decisions on trials with less favourable ratios as indicated by a significant main effect of ratio, F(3, 189) = 6.38, p<.001, ηp 2 = .09 (Figure 2). There was a statistically significant main effect of group, F(2, 63) = 5.69, p = .005, ηp 2 = .15. Both offenders with ASPD+P (M = 3629.6, SD = 1285.5; p = .001) and those with ASPD−P (M = 3060.5, SD = 1103.2; p = .04) took more time to make decisions than the non-offenders (M = 2571.7, SD = 991.0). In addition, there was a significant group by ratio interaction, F(6, 189) = 3.53, p = .002, ηp 2 = .10. There were differences in deliberation time between some of the ratios among men with ASPD+P (9∶1 vs. 7∶3, p = .009; 9.1 vs. 6∶4, p = 004) and among those with ASPD−P (6∶4 vs.7∶3, p = .008; 6∶4 vs. 8∶2, p = .001; 6∶4 vs. 9∶1, p = .048), but this was not observed for the non- offender group (all ps >.69).
Quality of decision-making
There were statistically significant group differences in the quality of decision-making in both the ascending, χ2 (2) = 6.74, p = .034, ηp 2 = .10, and the descending, conditions, χ2 (2) = 9.42, p = .009, ηp 2 = .14. Follow-up post-hoc tests for the ascending condition indicated significant differences between the non-offenders and both the ASPD−P (p = .02) and ASPD+P offenders (p = .04). No differences were detected in scores of the two ASPD groups (p = 1) (Figure 2). Similarly, follow-up post-hoc-tests for the descending condition revealed significant differences between the non-offenders and both the ASPD+P (p = .01) and ASPD−P offenders (p = .01), while the two ASPD groups did not differ from each other (p = .72). The quality of decision-making did not differ across groups for the ratios 9∶1, χ2 (2) = 3.61, p = .17, ηp 2 = .05, and 8∶2, χ2 (2) = 1.14, p = .57, ηp 2 = .02, but there were statistically significant group differences in the quality of decision-making for the less favourable ratios 7∶3, χ2 (2) = 7.33, p = .03. ηp 2 = .11, and 6∶4, χ2 (2) = 8.98, p = .01, ηp 2 = .14. Post-hoc tests for the ratio 7∶3 revealed that, while the there was no difference between the two ASPD groups (p = .97), there was a significant difference between the non-offenders and both the ASPD−P offenders (p = .01) and ASPD+P offenders (p = .03). A similar pattern was observed for the ratio 6∶4: while the there was no difference between the two ASPD groups (p = .87), there was a significant difference between the non-offenders and both the ASPD−P offenders (p = .01) and ASPD+P offenders (p = .02).
Risk-taking, risk adjustment, and delay aversion
A 3 (group: ASPD+P, ASPD−P, non-offenders) ×4 (ratio: 9∶1, 8∶2, 7∶3, 6∶4) ×2 (condition: ascending versus descending) ANOVA on risk-taking identified a significant main effect of condition, F(1, 61) = 90.48, p<.001, ηp 2 = .60, and of ratio, F(1.52, 92.41) = 83.13, p<.001, ηp 2 = .58. (The degree of freedom for the repeated ANOVA is 61 instead of 63 because the risk-taking measure could not be calculated for one ASPD+P and one ASPD−P, as they bet on the colour in the minority (i.e. the less likely outcome) – see description of how Risk-taking is calculated in Text S1.) Participants bet more on the descending condition and less as the ratio of boxes became less favourable (Figure 2). The main effect of group and the interactions terms were not statistically significant (all Fs <2.23). A one-way ANOVA on the risk adjustment measure indicated that there was a trend for a group difference, F(2, 63) = 2.97, p = .058, ηp 2 = .60, suggesting that the two ASPD groups adjusted their betting less than the non-offenders (Figure 2). Finally, there was no main effect of group on the delay aversion measure, F(2, 61) = .12, p = .99, ηp 2 = .00, indicating no group difference in impulsivity.
Passive Avoidance Learning Task
Following R.J.R. Blair et al [36], each initial presentation of a stimulus was treated as a learning trial, so results from the first block were omitted from the analysis. A 3 (group: ASPD+P, ASPD−P, non-offenders) ×4 (punishment values: 1, 700, 1400, 2000) ×9 (blocks) model ANOVA was performed on the number of commission errors. There was a statistically significant main effect of block, F(5.91, 366.63) = 13.67, p<.001, ηp 2 = .18, indicating a decrease in the number of commission errors as the task progressed (Figure 3). The main effect of group fell short of statistical significance, F(2, 62) = 2.92, p = .06, ηp 2 = .09, suggesting that the two ASPD groups tended to make more commission errors than the non-offenders. There was no statistically significant main effect of punishment or interaction effects (all Fs <1.1).
A 3 (group: ASPD+P, ASPD−P, non-offenders) ×4 (reward values: 1, 700, 1400, 2000) mixed model ANOVA conducted on the omission errors revealed a main effect of reward, F(2.47, 153.26) = 2.94, p = .045, ηp 2 = .05. As illustrated in Figure 3, participants made fewer omission errors for smaller reward values. There was also a main effect of block, F(5.25, 325.23) = 15.83, p<.001, ηp 2 = .20. As can be seen from Figure 3, participants made more errors as the task progressed. While the main effect of group was not significant, F(2, 62) = 1.17, p = .32, ηp 2 = .04, there was a significant group by reward interaction, F(4.94, 153.26) = 2.65, p = .03, ηp 2 = .08. While the performance of the ASPD−P and the non-offender groups was not influenced by the level of reward, the ASPD+P offenders committed fewer errors at the lowest level of reward (+1) as compared to levels +700 and +2000 (ps = .018 and .008, respectively). The group by block interaction was not statistically significant, F(10.49, 325.25) = 1.69, p = .08, ηp 2 = .05.
Discussion
The present study is the first to use a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests to assess cool and hot EF, comparing two groups of violent offenders and one group of healthy non-offenders. A summary of the results is presented in Table 2. As hypothesized, both the violent offenders with ASPD+P and those with ASPD−P showed similarly poor performance on the Digit Span – Backward test assessing cool EF, and on several tests of hot EF as compared to healthy non-offenders. However, infirming our second hypothesis, the ASPD+P offenders did not make more commission errors than the ASPD−P offenders on the Passive Avoidance Learning Task. In fact, the performance of the two groups of violent offenders did not differ on any of the tasks.
Table 2. Summary of Task Performance of Non-offenders, Violent Offenders with ASPD−P, and Violent Offenders with ASPD+P.
Neuropsychological measure | Group and Interaction Effects | Post-hoc† |
Digit Span – Backward | Group | Non-offenders > ASPD+P, ASPD−P# |
Spatial Alternation Task | – | – |
Passive Avoidance Learning Task | ||
Commission errors | Group# | Non-offenders > ASPD+P#, ASPD−P# |
Omission errors | – | – |
Probabilistic Response Reversal | Group | Non-offenders > ASPD+P#, ASPD−P |
Acquisition errors | – | – |
Reversal errors 80–20 pair | Group x Phase | Non-offenders > ASPD+P#, ASPD−P |
Cambridge Gamble Task | ||
Deliberation time | Group | Non-offenders > ASPD+P, ASPD−P |
Quality of decision-making | Group ↑↓ | Non-offenders > ASPD+P, ASPD−P |
Group ratio 7∶3 | Non-offenders > ASPD+P, ASPD−P | |
Group ratio 6∶4 | Non-offenders > ASPD+P, ASPD−P | |
Risk-taking | – | – |
Risk adjustment | Group# | Non-offenders > ASPD+P#, ASPD−P# |
Delay aversion | – | – |
Note. Better performance > worse performance.
The performance of the ASPD+P and ASPD−P did not differ on any of the tasks.
Trend for group difference.
– No statistically significant group difference.
↑ Ascending condition.
↓ Descending condition.
Importantly, violent offenders with a life-long history of antisocial and aggressive behaviour as compared to non-offenders matched for age, IQ and ethnicity, showed deficits in an array of both cool and hot EF. Both offenders with ASPD+P and those with ASPD−P showed impaired performance on the Digit Span – Backward, a measure of working verbal memory indexing cool EF. Impaired verbal working memory limits reflection during problem solving, particularly in situations requiring adaptive social responses [7], [71], [72]. While a previous study reported that offenders with ASPD+P performed similarly to offenders without psychopathy on this task [53], the results of the present study show that, when compared to healthy non-offenders, both offenders with ASPD+P and those with ASPD−P display deficits in verbal working memory as do persistently aggressive children and adults [11], [72]. Interestingly, two previous studies did not find a verbal working memory deficit among men with ASPD−P with no history of criminality or substance misuse [43], [73]. Taken together, the results of the present study and the extant literature may be interpreted to suggest that impaired verbal working memory is associated with a life-long pattern of aggressive behaviour.
While both groups of violent offenders showed poorer verbal working memory than the non-offenders, on another test of cool EF, the Spatial Alternation Task, they showed no impairment. This finding is consistent with results of the only previous study to assess offenders with this task in which offenders with and without psychopathy performed similarly [47]. Our results extend the previous findings by showing that, while the role of the DLPFC in the alteration of motor responses to spatial locations on the basis of reinforcement information is not impaired in offenders with ASPD+P and those with ASPD−P, they do exhibit impairments in verbal working memory, another cool EF subsumed by the DLPFC.
The two groups of violent offenders also showed impairments in hot EF as compared to the non-offenders. In the reversal phase of the Probabilistic Response Reversal Task, offenders with ASPD−P committed significantly more errors than the non-offenders in the condition where the stimulus-response association was less clear (i.e., 80–20 contingency pair). The offenders with ASPD+P showed a trend in the same direction. A previous study of offenders using the same paradigm reported that those with psychopathy, as compared to those without psychopathy, committed more errors on the reversal phase of the 100–0 and 80–20 pairs [54]. However, the finding that the ASPD−P offenders showed impairments on the response reversal task is consistent with a previous study [74] showing that offenders with moderate PCL-R scores (between 21–29; insufficient to warrant a diagnosis of psychopathy in the U.S.) were impaired in response reversal in comparison to offenders without psychopathy (PCL-R scores range: 0–20).
On the CGT, the two groups of offenders, as compared to the non-offenders, also displayed poorer quality of decision-making despite increased deliberation times and a strong trend for less modulation of their betting as the probability of loss increased, but similar levels of impulsivity and risk-taking. The two groups of violent offenders, like the non-offenders, deliberated longer before making a decision as the box ratio became less favourable, thereby showing an understanding of the trial-by-trial probabilities and of the increased risk of losing points. This pattern of results – long delay and poor decision making - resembles that observed among patients with lesions in the VMPFC (e.g., [68], [75]; but see [76]). Thus, although they were aware of the increased risk of loss, the offenders failed to adjust their behaviour to the increasing risk of losing points, just as they persist in engaging in antisocial behaviour despite knowing that it will likely lead to negative consequences.
Perhaps one of the most novel aspects of this study are the results of the delay aversion and risk taking measures, which indicate that the two groups of ASPD offenders were no more impulsive or risk-taking (at least not at the two most favourable ratios) than the non-offenders. These results are likely due to the fact that the CGT is a decision-making task in which outcome probabilities and the associated risks are explicit. By contrast, the few studies that have examined affective decision-making of men with ASPD+P or ASPD−P and shown increased risk-taking behaviour used the Iowa Gambling Task in which outcome probabilities are unknown. This latter task relies on the integrity and coordination of several processes, including stimulus-reinforcement learning, reversal learning, set-shifting, and working memory [77]. Since men with ASPD, regardless of psychopathy scores, are known to be impaired on some of these processes, this might explain results of previous studies of risk-taking on the Iowa Gambling Task. Much evidence indicates that men with ASPD, regardless of psychopathy, show impulsive behaviour in the form of impaired response inhibition (e.g., [50], [57]). Results of the present study suggest that they may not display impulsivity defined as delay aversion. A previous study examined delay aversion among offenders with and without psychopathy [78]. Low anxious psychopaths, in comparison to low anxious non-psychopaths, delayed gratification less often in the condition that involved rewards and punishments, but not in the condition that involved rewards only. Clearly, additional research examining different forms of impulsivity in relation to ASPD and psychopathy is warranted.
On another test of hot EF, the Passive Avoidance Learning Task, the violent offenders were impaired as compared to the non-offenders. There was a trend (p = .06) indicating that the two offender groups made more commission errors than the non-offenders, but, contrary to our second hypothesis, no evidence of an increased number of commission errors in the ASPD+P group compared to the ASPD−P group. As hypothesized, there was no evidence of a group difference in omission errors. These results show that this failure to learn from punishment cues characterizes not only violent offenders with ASPD+P, but also those with ASPD−P. The results of the present study suggest that both violent offenders with ASPD+P and those with ASPD−P have difficulty in stimulus-punishment associations.
Violent offenders with ASPD, both those with and without additional diagnoses of psychopathy, showed impairments in verbal working memory, and in adaptive affective decision-making. They failed to learn from punishment cues, to change their behaviour in the face of changing contingencies, and made poorer quality decisions despite longer periods of deliberation before such decisions. The combination of these impairments may go some way towards explaining why violent offenders with ASPD with and without psychopathy are characterized by irresponsibility, recklessness, persistent aggressive behaviour, and engagement in multiple other types of antisocial behaviours despite knowing that such behaviour will likely lead to negative consequences for themselves and/or others [14].
These findings need to be replicated. The absence of statistically significant differences in performance on any of the neuropsychological tasks between the violent offenders with ASPD+P and those with ASPD−P suggests shared deficits in cool and hot EF, at least based on the tasks used and the processes they index. These results are consistent, however, with our structural brain imaging findings on an overlapping sample showing differences between violent offenders with ASPD+P and those with ASPD−P in gray matter volume of the superior/medial prefrontal cortex and temporal poles, but no differences in gray matter volume the amygdala, VMPFC or DLPFC [23]. Additionally, reduced fractional anisotropy in the right uncinate fasciculus (the primary white matter tract connecting the VMPFC and the anterior temporal lobes) has been demonstrated in both violent men with ASPD+P [79], [80] and those with ASPD−P [81]. Thus, the results from the present study are consistent with this emerging evidence from brain imaging studies and might reflect that fact that, while the offenders with ASPD+P scored twice as high as the offenders with ASPD−P on the PCL-R facet 1 and facet 2 indexing the core interpersonal and affective features of the syndrome of psychopathy, these two groups of violent offenders share many characteristics, most importantly a childhood onset of conduct problems that persist into adulthood and violent behaviour. The present results suggest that both offenders with ASPD+P and those with ASPD−P present similar EF impairments despite differences in the types of aggressive behaviour in which they engage, personality traits, emotion processing, and structural and functional brain anomalies.
Several methodological limitations should be considered in interpreting the results of the present study. One, there may have been a lack of statistical power to detect group differences resulting from the relatively small number of violent offenders with ASPD+P. The number of participants, however, was similar to many of the previous neuropsychological studies in the field (e.g., [51], [54], [82]). Two, the use of the validated PCL-R cut-off score for European offenders to identify the syndrome of psychopathy may have lessened the likelihood of observing cognitive impairments. However this is unlikely as the pattern of results generally showed that impairments previously reported as characterizing offenders with psychopathy also characterized those with ASPD−P. Three, as is evident from the review of the literature and the present results, findings about psychopathy depend to a large extent on the comparison group used in each study. Therefore, all analyses were re-run excluding six offenders with ASPD−P whose total PCL-R scores were between 24 and 20. Again, no significant group differences between offenders with ASPD+P and ASPD−P were found. Four, the violent offenders with ASPD, like almost all people with ASPD [83], had a history of substance misuse. While objective tests assured that the participants were not tested when intoxicated, it is possible that past substance misuse led to some of the deficits in performance that were observed. Finally, the use of digits as central stimuli in two of the tasks (i.e., the Digit Span – Backward and the Passive Avoidance Learning Task) may not have been ideal for testing individuals with low levels of education.
This study also has several strengths. One, it is the first study to directly contrast various aspects of hot EF in violent offenders with ASPD+P and violent offenders with ASPD−P and to compare test performance to that of healthy non-offenders. Two, this is the first study to include offenders who were convicted of several violent crimes, diagnosed by forensic psychiatrists using standardized, validated interview protocols, and examined using a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests that assessed both cool and hot EF. Three, this study was the first to examine affective decision-making under risk among men with ASPD+P and men with ASPD−P. Finally, the three groups did not differ in terms of age, IQ, and ethnicity.
The findings from this study provide novel evidence that, in comparison to healthy non-offenders, violent offenders with ASPD+P and violent offenders with ASPD−P present impairments in both cool and hot EF such as verbal working memory, response reversal, affective decision-making under risk, and stimulus-reinforcement-based decision-making. The performance of the two groups of offenders on these tasks did not differ suggesting shared deficits in EF, at least based on the tasks used and the processes they index. The combination of these impairments may help to explain why violent offenders with ASPD, both those with and without psychopathy, persist in engaging in antisocial behaviours despite knowing the risks of negative consequences to themselves and/or others [14]. Crucially, given the differences in their responses to cognitive-behavioural rehabilitation programs aimed at reducing violence and recidivism [12], [13], additional research is needed to further understanding of the neurobiological and psychological similarities and differences in these two types of offenders. Functional magnetic resonance imaging, which has never been used to directly compare these two groups of violent offenders, would be highly informative in this regard since it can detect subtle alterations in neural processing that may not be observable with behavioural indices.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We thank the participants for their time, the National Probation Service for their collaboration and the following persons who recruited and assessed participants: Ms. Sam Prior, Ms. Clare Goodwin, Mr. William Wainwright, Ms. Rebecca Brewer, Ms Sarah Gregory, Mr. Ruben Azevedo, Mr. Francis Vergunst, Ms. Lucy Butler, Ms. Leila Niknejad, Dr. Anna Plodowski, Dr. Philip Baker, Dr. Timothy Rogers, Dr. Preethi Chabbra, Dr. Stephen Attard, Dr. Seema Sukhwal, Dr. Nathan Kolla, Dr. Paul Wallang, and Dr. Clare Conway.
Funding Statement
This research was funded by research grants from the Department of Health (the National Forensic Mental Health R&D programme), the Ministry of Justice (a DSPD programme grant), the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, South London, Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, and Institute of Psychiatry (King's College London). SDB was supported by an MRC Ph.D. studentship and a research fellowship from the Swiss National Science Foundation (FNS PA00P1_139586), EV is supported by ESRC (RES-062-23-2202). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Kratzer L, Hodgins S (1999) A typology of offenders: A test of Moffitt's theory among males and females from childhood to age 30. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health 9: 57–73. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Harrington H, Milne BJ (2002) Males on the life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways: follow-up at age 26 years. Development and Psychopathology 14: 179–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.AmericanPsychiatricAssociation (2000) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.,Text revision). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
- 4.De Brito SA, Hodgins S (2009) Antisocial personality disorder. In: McMurran M, Howard R, editors. Personality, personality disorder, and risk of violence: An evidence based approach United Kingdom: Wiley. 133–154. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Zelazo PD, Müller U (2002) Executive function in typical and atypical development In: Goswami U, editor. Handbook of childhood cognitive development. Malden, MA: Blackwell. 445–469. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Blair RJR, Mitchell DGV, Blair KS (2005) The psychopath: Emotion and the brain. Oxford: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- 7. Giancola PR (1995) Evidence for dorsolateral and orbital prefrontal cortical involvement in the expression of aggressive behavior. Aggressive Behavior 21: 431–450. [Google Scholar]
- 8. Moffitt TE (1993) The neuropsychology of conduct disorder. Development and Psychopathology 5: 135–151. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Ogilvie JM, Stewart AL, Chan RCK, Shum DHK (2011) Neuropsychological measures of executive function and antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis. Criminology 49: 1063–1107. [Google Scholar]
- 10. Raine A, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M, et al. (2005) Neurocognitive impairments in boys on the life-course persistent antisocial path. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 114: 38–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Séguin JR, Sylvers P, Lilienfeld S (2007) The neuropsychology of violence. In: Flannery DJ, Vazsonyi AT, Waldman ID, editors. The Cambridge handbook of violent behavior and aggression. New York: Cambridge University Press. 187–214. [Google Scholar]
- 12. Ogloff JRP, Wong S, Greenwood A (1990) Treating criminal psychopaths in a therapeutic community program. Behavioral Sciences and the Law 8: 181–190. [Google Scholar]
- 13. Olver ME, Wong S (2011) Predictors of sex offender treatment dropout: Psychopathy, sex offender risk, and responsivity implications. Psychology, Crime and Law 17: 457–471. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hare RD (2003) Manual for the Revised Psychopathy Checklist. Toronto, ON, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. [Google Scholar]
- 15. Hare RD, Hart SD, Harpur TJ (1991) Psychopathy and the DSM-IV criteria for antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 100: 391–398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hare RD (1991) The Hare Psychopathy Checklist - Revised. Toronto, Ontario: Multi-Health Systems. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Wong S (1985) Criminal and institutional behaviours of psychopaths. Ottawa, Ontario: Ministry of the Solicitor General of Canada. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Porter S, Porter S (2007) Psychopathy and violent crime. In: Hervé H, Yuille JC, editors. The Psychopath: Theory, Research, and Practice. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. 287–300. [Google Scholar]
- 19. Hare RD, McPherson LM (1984) Violent and aggressive behavior by criminal psychopaths. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 7: 35–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Kosson DS, Lorenz AR, Newman JP (2006) Effects of comorbid psychopathy on criminal offending and emotion processing in male offenders with antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 115: 798–806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Dolan M, Fullam R (2006) Face affect recognition deficits in personality-disordered offenders: association with psychopathy. Psychological Medicine 36: 1563–1569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Verona E, Sprague J, Sadeh N (2012) Inhibitory control and negative emotional processing in psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 121: 498–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Gregory S, Ffytche D, Simmons A, Kumari V, Howard M, et al. (2012) The antisocial brain: Psychopathy matters: A structural MRI investigation of antisocial male violent offenders. Archives of General Psychiatry 69: 962–972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Birbaumer N, Veit R, Lotze M, Erb M, Hermann C, et al. (2005) Deficient fear conditioning in psychopathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry 62: 799–805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Frick PJ, Viding E (2009) Antisocial behavior from a developmental psychopathology perspective. Development and Psychopathology 21: 1111–1131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Dolan MC, Fullam RS (2009) Psychopathy and functional magnetic resonance imaging blood oxygenation level-dependent responses to emotional faces in violent patients with schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 66: 570–577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Jones AP, Laurens KR, Herba CM, Barker GJ, Viding E (2009) Amygdala hypoactivity to fearful faces in boys with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. American Journal of Psychiatry: appi.ajp.2008.07071050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Marsh AA, Finger EC, Mitchell DG, Reid ME, Sims C, et al. (2008) Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 165: 712–720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Blair RJR (2007) The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex in morality and psychopathy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11: 387–392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Barker ED, Séguin JR, White HR, Bates ME, Lacourse E, et al. (2007) Developmental trajectories of male physical violence and theft: relations to neurocognitive performance. Archives of General Psychiatry 64: 592–599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Giancola PR, Mezzich AC, Tarter RE (1998) Executive cognitive functioning, temperament, and antisocial behavior in conduct-disordered adolescent females. J Abnorm Psychol 107: 629–641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Rubia K (2011) “Cool” inferior frontostriatal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder versus “hot” ventromedial orbitofrontal-limbic dysfunction in conduct disorder: A review. Biological Psychiatry 69: e69–e87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Zelazo PD, Cunningham WA (2007) Executive function: Mechanisms underlying emotion regulation In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press. 135–138. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kelly AMC, Scheres A, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Castellanos FC (2007) Functional neuroimaging of reward and motivational pathways in ADHD In: Bellgrove MA, Fitzgerald M, Gill M, editors. The handbook of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. 209–235. [Google Scholar]
- 35. Roiser JP, Cannon DM, Gandhi SK, Tavares JT, Erickson K, et al. (2009) Hot and cold cognition in unmedicated depressed subjects with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders 11: 178–189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Blair RJR, Mitchell DGV, Leonard A, Budhani S, Peschardt K, et al. (2004) Passive avoidance learning in individuals with psychopathy: Modulation by reward but not by punishment. Personality and Individual Differences 37: 1179–1192. [Google Scholar]
- 37. Newman JP, Kosson DS (1986) Passive avoidance learning in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 95: 252–256. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Ishikawa SS, Raine A, Lencz T, Bihrle S, Lacasse L (2001) Autonomic stress reactivity and executive functions in successful and unsuccessful criminal psychopaths from the community. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 110: 423–432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Barkataki I, Kumari V, Das M, Hill M, Morris R, et al. (2005) A neuropsychological investigation into violence and mental illness. Schizophrenia Research 74: 1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Dolan M (2012) The neuropsychology of prefrontal function in antisocial personality disordered offenders with varying degrees of psychopathy. Psychological Medicine 42: 1715–1725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Crowell TA, Kieffer KM, Kugeares S, Vanderploeg RD (2003) Executive and nonexecutive neuropsychological functioning in antisocial personality disorder. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology 16: 100–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Deckel A, Hesselbrock V, Bauer L (1996) Antisocial personality disorder, childhood delinquency, and frontal brain functioning: EEG and neuropsychological findings. Journal of Clinical Psychology 52: 639–650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Gillen R, Hesselbrock V (1992) Cognitive functioning, ASP, and family history of alcoholism in young men at risk for alcoholism. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 16: 206–214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Swann AC, Lijffijt M, Lane SD, Steinberg JL, Moeller FG (2009) Trait impulsivity and response inhibition in antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research 43: 1057–1063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Hoffman JJ, Hall RW, Bartsch TW (1987) On the relative importance of “psychopathic” personality and alcoholism on neuropsychological measures of frontal lobe dysfunction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 96: 158–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Mazas CA, Finn PR, Steinmetz JE (2000) Decision-making biases, antisocial personality, and early-onset alcoholism. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 24: 1036–1040. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Blair KS, Newman C, Mitchell DGV, Richell R, Leonard A, et al. (2006) Differentiating among prefrontal substrates in psychopathy: neuropsychological test findings. Neuropsychology 20: 153–165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Hare RD (1984) Performance of psychopaths on cognitive tasks related to frontal lobe function. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 93: 133–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Hart SD, Forth AE, Hare RD (1990) Performance of criminal psychopaths on selected neuropsychological tests. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 99: 374–379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Lapierre D, Braun CM, Hodgins S (1995) Ventral frontal deficits in psychopathy: Neuropsychological test findings. Neuropsychologia 33: 139–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Mitchell DGV, Colledge E, Leonard A, Blair RJR (2002) Risky decisions and response reversal: Is there evidence of orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in psychopathic individuals? Neuropsychologia 40: 2013–2022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Mitchell DGV, Avny SB, Blair RJR (2006) Divergent patterns of aggressive and neurocognitive characteristics in acquired versus developmental psychopathy. Neurocase 12: 164–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Smith SS, Arnett PA, Newman JP (1992) Neuropsychological differentiation of psychopathic and nonpsychopathic criminal offenders. Personality and Individual Differences 13: 1233–1243. [Google Scholar]
- 54. Budhani S, Richell RA, Blair RJR (2006) Impaired reversal but intact acquisition: probabilistic response reversal deficits in adult individuals with psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 115: 552–558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Newman JP, Patterson CM, Kosson DS (1987) Response perseveration in psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 96: 145–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Sutker PB, Allain AN Jr (1987) Cognitive abstraction, shifting, and control: clinical sample comparisons of psychopaths and nonpsychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 96: 73–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Dolan M, Park I (2002) The neuropsychology of antisocial personality disorder. Psychological Medicine 32: 417–427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Gorenstein EE (1982) Frontal lobe functions in psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 91: 368–379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Hart SD, Cox DN, Hare RD (1995) The Hare Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version (PCL: SV). Toronto: Mutli-Health Systems. [Google Scholar]
- 60. Ross EH, Hoaken PNS (2010) Correctional remediation meets neuropsychological rehabilitation: How brain injury and schizophrenia research can improve offender programming. Criminal Justice and Behavior 37: 656–677. [Google Scholar]
- 61. Andrews DA, Bonta J (2010) Rehabilitating criminal justice policy and practice. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law 16: 39–55. [Google Scholar]
- 62. Duggan C (2008) Review. Why are programmes for offenders with personality disorder not informed by the relevant scientific findings? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 363: 2599–2612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Wechsler D (1997) Weschsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- 64.First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW (1995) Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders - Patient Edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0). New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute. [Google Scholar]
- 65. Cooke DJ, Michie C (1999) Psychopathy across cultures: North America and Scotland compared. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 108: 58–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Hare RD, Neumann CS (2005) Structural models of psychopathy. Current Psychiatry Reports 7: 57–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW (2004) Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 68. Rogers RD, Everitt BJ, Baldacchino A, Blackshaw AJ, Swainson R, et al. (1999) Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: Evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 20: 322–339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Howell DC (2007) Fundamental statistics for the behavioral sciences Wadsworth Publishing Co Inc. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Cardinal RN, Aitken MRF (2006) ANOVA for the behavioural sciences researcher. New Jersey, USA.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
- 71. Miller LA, Collins RL, Kent TA (2008) Language and the modulation of impulsive aggression. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 20: 261–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Séguin JR, Pihl RO, Harden PW, Tremblay RE, Boulerice B (1995) Cognitive and neuropsychological characteristics of physically aggressive boys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 104: 614–624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Stevens MC, Kaplan RF, Hesselbrock VM (2003) Executive-cognitive functioning in the development of antisocial personality disorder. Addictive Behaviors 28: 285–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Mitchell DGV, Fine C, Richell RA, Newman C, Lumsden J, et al. (2006) Instrumental learning and relearning in individuals with psychopathy and in patients with lesions involving the amygdala or orbitofrontal cortex. Neuropsychology 20: 280–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Rahman S, Sahakian BJ, Hodges JR, Rogers RD, Robbins TW (1999) Specific cognitive deficits in mild frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain 122: 1469–1493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Manes F, Sahakian B, Clark L, Rogers R, Antoun N, et al. (2002) Decision-making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. Brain 125: 624–639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Dunn BD, Dalgleish T, Lawrence AD (2006) The somatic marker hypothesis: A critical evaluation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30: 239–271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Newman JP, Kosson DS, Patterson CM (1992) Delay of gratification in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 101: 630–636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Craig MC, Catani M, Deeley Q, Latham R, Daly E, et al. (2009) Altered connections on the road to psychopathy. Molecular Psychiatry 14: 946–953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Motzkin JC, Newman JP, Kiehl KA, Koenigs M (2011) Reduced prefrontal connectivity in psychopathy. The Journal of Neuroscience 31: 17348–17357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Sundram F, Deeley Q, Sarkar S, Daly E, Latham R, et al. (2012) White matter microstructural abnormalities in the frontal lobe of adults with antisocial personality disorder. Cortex 48: 216–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Koenigs M, Kruepke M, Newman JP (2010) Economic decision-making in psychopathy: A comparison with ventromedial prefrontal lesion patients. Neuropsychologia 48: 2198–2204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Kessler RC, Nelson CB, McGonagle KA, Edlund MJ, Frank RG, et al. (1996) The epidemiology of co-occurring addictive and mental disorders: Implications for prevention and service utilization. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 66: 17–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.