Abstract
Purpose
To develop perimetric stimuli which are resistant to the effects of peripheral defocus.
Methods
One eye each was tested on subjects free of eye disease. Experiment 1 assessed spatial frequency, testing 12 subjects at eccentricities from 2° to 7°, using blur levels from 0 D to 3 D for two (Gabor) stimuli (spatial standard deviation (SD) = 0.5°, spatial frequencies of 0.5 and 1.0 cpd). Experiment 2 assessed stimulus size, testing 12 subjects at eccentricities from 4° to 7°, using blur levels 0 D to 6 D, for two Gaussians with SDs of 0.5° and 0.25° and a 0.5 cpd Gabor with SD of 0.5°. Experiment 3 tested 13 subjects at eccentricities from fixation to 27°, using blur levels 0 D to 6 D, for Gabor stimuli at 56 locations; the spatial frequency ranged from 0.14 to 0.50 cpd with location, and SD was scaled accordingly.
Results
In experiment 1, blur by 3 D caused a small decline in log contrast sensitivity (CS) for the 0.5 cpd stimulus (mean ± SE = −0.09 ± 0.08 log unit) and a larger (t = 7.7, p <0.0001) decline for the 1.0 cpd stimulus (0.37 ± 0.13 log unit). In experiment 2, blur by 6 D caused minimal decline for the larger Gaussian, by −0.17 ± 0.16 log unit, and larger (t >4.5, p < 0.001) declines for the smaller Gaussian (−0.33 ± 0.16 log unit) and the Gabor (−0.36 ± 0.18 log unit). In experiment 3, blur by 6 D caused declines by 0.27 ± 0.05 log unit for eccentricities from 0° to 10°, by 0.20 ± 0.04 log unit for eccentricities from 10° to 20° and 0.13 ± 0.03 log unit for eccentricities from 20°–27°.
Conclusions
Experiments 1 & 2 allowed us to design stimuli for Experiment 3 that were resistant to effects of peripheral defocus.
Keywords: perimetry, visual fields, peripheral refractive error, perimetric stimuli
Automated perimetric testing is an integral part of 21st-century clinical practice, yet relies on stimuli developed 70 years ago. The most commonly used perimetric stimulus is a sharp-edged circular luminance increment with diameter of 26 minutes of arc, the “size III” stimulus developed by Goldmann1. For the central visual field (within 30° of fixation), sensitivity to this stimulus is reduced by modest amounts of optical defocus,2–7 so trial lenses are used to refract for the test distance. However, it is now well documented that there is the potential for significant shifts in the refraction of an eye at locations within 30° of fixation, with substantial variation in peripheral refraction across individuals.8 Defocus at 20° from fixation ranges from −2 D to +2D across individuals,8 and 2 D of defocus has been found to reduce sensitivity to size III by 0.3 log unit 2. Individual variations in peripheral defocus may be one reason that between-subject variability for size III perimetry increases with eccentricity9.
Effects of optical blur can be greatly reduced by use of grating stimuli at 0.25 cycle/degree, as in frequency-doubling perimetry, where normal between-subject variability does not increase with eccentricity10, 11. These grating stimuli are quite large, 100 deg2, with the same contrast across the entire stimulus, making them poorly suited for localizing visual field defects. By comparison, the conventional size III stimulus covers less than 0.2% of the area covered by these frequency-doubling stimuli. The goal of the current study was to use Gaussian windows to produce stimuli substantially smaller than 100 deg2 that retained much of the relative immunity to effects of blur of 0.25 cpd gratings.
Gaussian windows allow smaller and more localized stimuli than hard-edged gratings, and provide a central location at maximum contrast rather than the same contrast across the stimulus as in frequency-doubling perimetry and Pulsar perimetry12. The use of Gaussian-windowed sinusoids in perimetry has been termed “contrast sensitivity perimetry” (CSP)13. We have found that CSP with spatial frequencies from 0.38 to 1.0 cycles per degree (cpd) is as effective as size III perimetry, frequency-doubling perimetry and area of neuroretinal rim in assessing depth of defect14, 15. The current study assessed the relative depth of field for potential CSP stimuli with peak spatial frequencies from 0.1 to 1.0 cpd, in order to reduce the potential effects of peripheral defocus16. The first two experiments were performed in the macula, where blur conditions could be manipulated without concern for peripheral defocus. The first experiment varied spatial frequency at a fixed size and the second experiment varied size. These results were used to design CSP stimuli that should be similar to 0.25 cpd gratings in terms of resistance to effects of peripheral defocus. The third experiment tested this prediction.
Oriented grating stimuli are affected by astigmatic error, for which blur effect is maximum when the angle of the astigmatism is perpendicular to the orientation of the grating. The J0 astigmatism component17 approaches 1D at 30° eccentricity16, so oriented stimuli could have as much as 3D of peripheral defocus. Therefore this study aimed to develop stimuli for which a blur level of 3 D has only minor impact on perimetric sensitivity, and effect of blur does not increase with eccentricity.
METHODS
Expt 1 assessed effects of blur vs peak spatial frequency in the macula for a fixed size stimulus with no change in mean luminance. Expt 2 assessed effects of blur vs size in the macula with a change in mean luminance. Results of these two experiments were used to design stimuli that should be minimally affected by 3 D of optical blur, with small stimuli in the macula and larger (more blur-resistant) stimuli at more eccentric locations in the central visual field. This prediction was tested in Expt 3.
Participants
Expt 1 was conducted at Indiana University (IU) where 10 subjects were recruited out of the larger pool of control subjects in a longitudinal study of contrast sensitivity perimetry (CSP) at the IU School of Optometry. These subjects had previously participated in this perimetric research project and were experienced. Their ages ranged from 45 to 64 (mean ± SD, 54.5 ± 5.9 years).
Expts 2 & 3 were conducted at the State University of New York (SUNY) School of Optometry, where subjects were recruited from optometry students who had prior experience with perimetry. Experiment 2 recruited 12 people ages 24 to 26 years (25.1 ± 1.1 years), and Experiment 3 recruited people ages 23 to 27 years (24.5 ± 1.1 years). The only overlap between experiments is that two people who participated in Experiment 3 also participated in Experiment 2.
For all three experiments, subjects were recruited who were free of eye disease on a recent comprehensive evaluation within 2 years, including a detailed personal and family history, assessment of visual acuity, refraction, extra-ocular muscle function and binocular vision, as well as evaluation of anterior segment, screening visual fields and dilated fundus exam. Additional inclusion criteria were: best corrected visual acuity of 20/20 or better, spherical correction within −6 to + 2 D, cylinder correction < 3D, clear ocular media, and IOP < 22 mm Hg. Exclusion criteria were an ocular or systemic disease known to affect the visual field, a first-degree relative with glaucoma, usage of medications known to affect visual function. The preferred eye of each person was tested, and data collected from the left eyes were converted to right-eye format by multiplying x-values for all locations by −1. The other eye was occluded with a translucent white patch.
The research for this study adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the institutional review boards at Indiana University (Expt 1) and SUNY College of Optometry (Expts 2 & 3). Informed consent was obtained from each participant after explanation of the procedures and goals of the study, before testing began.
Equipment
Custom testing stations were built using cathode-ray-tube (CRT) displays driven by a 14-bit visual stimulus generator that allowed precise control of contrast (ViSaGe; Cambridge Research Systems, Ltd., Cambridge, UK). A photometer with calibration software (Opti-Cal; Cambridge Research Systems Ltd.) was used to measure luminance versus voltage values for each phosphor, calculate transfer functions, and produce red-green-blue (RGB) gamma correction look-up tables. The resolution of the monitor was 800 X 600 pixels, subtending 42°X 35° of visual angle at 40 cm (Expts 1 & 2) and 51°X 42° at 33 cm (Expt 3). A webcam allowed the operator to continuously monitor the subject’s fixation stability.
Expt 1 used a 21-in. monitor (Diamond Pro 2070SB; Mitsubishi Digital Electronics America Inc. Irvine, CA) with a frame rate of 140 Hz. Expts 2 & 3 used a 21-in. monitor (Radius PressView 21SR, Miro Displays, Inc., Germany) with a frame rate of 152 Hz.
For Experiments 1 & 2, testing was in the macula and a 40 cm test distance was used. Each subject was asked to place his or her head in a chin rest (UHCOTech HeadSpot) with the forehead against a bar so that the eye was 40 cm from a fixation target. Trial frames were used to refract the subject for the 40 cm test distance, and accuracy of refraction was checked by having the subject read the 20/20 line on a near chart placed on the monitor.
For Experiment 3, a 33 cm test distance was used to allow testing of more eccentric locations, using a custom-built motorized headrest and monitor stand that allowed better control of head position, and two adjustable headrests which allowed compensation for prominence of the brow. Custom 50 mm spherical lenses were held in place by magnets, and plain glass was used when no spherical correction was needed, so that the metal rim remained as a cue to head and eye position. The patient's head was placed in the motorized chinrest and positioned so that the patient's pupil was centered in the corrective lens (checked with webcam) that was centered on the fixation target. The custom lenses were in 0.5 D steps, and the appropriate lens was selected based on the spherical equivalent of the subject’s refraction plus 3 D for the 33 cm test distance. Appropriateness of refraction was checked by having the subject read the 20/20 line on a near chart placed on the monitor (equivalent to 20/24 at this distance).
Stimuli
Stimuli were two-dimensional Gaussian windows multiplied by horizontal sinusoidal gratings. Stimulus size was determined by the standard deviation of the Gaussian window, and peak spatial frequency was determined by the spatial frequency of the grating, in cycles per degree (cpd). Luminance profiles are shown in Figure 1.
Experiment 1 used stimuli in sine phase, so there was no change in mean luminance of the window. Two stimuli were used, with the same size Gaussian window (SD = 0.5°), and grating spatial frequencies of 0.5 and 1.0 cpd, presented at twelve locations along the diagonal meridians with eccentricities of 1.6°, 4.2°, 7.1°. Background luminance was 50 cd/m2.
Experiment 2 used two sizes for the Gaussian window, with SDs of 0.5° and 0.25°, with an increase in mean luminance of the stimuli. Two of the stimuli were Gaussian blobs (equivalent to Gabor in cosine phase at 0 cpd), an increase in mean luminance without oriented spatial content. The third was a Gabor in cosine phase with a peak spatial frequency of 0.5 cpd. The smaller Gaussian blob had similar width to the central bar in the 0.5 cpd stimulus. These stimuli were presented at eight locations along the diagonal meridians with eccentricities of 4.2° and 7.1°. Background luminance was 20 cd/m2 for the Gabor cosine and 10 cd/m2 for the Gaussian blobs
The third experiment used stimuli in sine phase, so there was no change in mean luminance of the window. Window size and spatial content varied with visual field location. Based on experiment 1, spatial frequencies did not exceed 0.5 cpd, and based on experiment 2, the smallest stimuli had windows with SD = 0.5°. Larger stimuli were used outside the macula to make contrast sensitivity relatively independent of stimulus location, using the method of Watson18 to magnify the stimulus, based on empirical magnification factors derived by our lab19. The product of SD times spatial frequency was held constant at 0.25; the stimuli had spatial frequencies from 0.14 to 0.5 cpd, and SDs from 0.5° to 1.8°. Luminance profiles for the stimuli are shown in Figure 2. Background luminance was 40 cd/m2.
Temporal presentation was a 200 msec rectangular pulse for Experiments 1 & 2, and three cycles of 5 Hz counterphase flicker for Experiment 3. In a separate experiment investigating test-retest variability, 5 Hz counterphase flicker was found to be the most stable.
Protocol
Each subject was refracted for the test distance and then had positive blur added. Experiment 1 used eight conditions: two stimuli times four blur levels from 0 D to 3 D in steps of 1 D. Experiment 2 used nine conditions: three stimuli times three blur levels from 0 to 6D in steps of 3 D. Experiment 3 used five conditions, one stimulus at five blur levels from 0 to 6 D in steps of 1.5 D.
For Experiments 1 & 2, log contrast sensitivities were averaged across all locations, to give an average macular log contrast sensitivity for that test. For Experiment 3, log contrast sensitivities were averaged for three rings: Ring 1 included 14 locations at 0 to 9.5° eccentricity, Ring 2 included 25 locations at 10.2° to 19.8° eccentricity, and Ring 3 included 18 locations at 20.0° to 27.5° eccentricity. Figure 2 indicates which stimuli were associated with each ring.
Threshold Algorithm
Contrast sensitivity across the central visual field was measured by having the subject fixate a target in the center of the display and click a button whenever a stimulus was seen. Stimuli were presented at the different visual field locations on a uniform gray background, in a darkened room.
In Experiments 1 & 2 a one-down, one-up staircase method with four reversals was used to determine the subject’s contrast threshold at each location. For each staircase a stimulus with 25% contrast was presented first. Contrast of the stimulus was decreased by 0.3 log units, if responded to, or else increased by 0.3 log units if not responded to, until a reversal occurred. The second reversal was obtained with the same 0.3 log unit steps. For the third and fourth reversals, 0.15 log unit steps were employed. The reciprocal of the “last seen” contrast was the specific measure of contrast sensitivity, the last contrast for which the subject responded was taken as an estimate of contrast threshold. For Experiment 3, the threshold algorithm was a ZEST algorithm,20 which terminated after 6 presentations21, and allowed twice as many locations to be tested in the same amount of time as the staircase. The inter-stimulus interval averaged 1700 msec for the staircase and 1200 msec for the ZEST, with a variable foreperiod.
Further detail on strategies for assessing and managing fixation loss, false positives, false negatives and other artifacts are described elsewhere19
RESULTS
Figure 3 shows mean log contrast sensitivity versus blur condition for the three experiments; error bars show ± 1 standard error of the mean (SEM). In each experiment, SEMs were low and ranged from 0.03 to 0.06 log unit. A blur effect was considered to be significant when the mean log CS declined by more than three times the SEM.
For Experiment 1 (solid symbols in middle of Figure 3) there were significant blur effects at 2 D and 3 D for the 1.0 cpd stimulus, but not for the 0.5 cpd stimulus. The 3 D blur condition caused a greater decline (t = 7.7, p <0.0001) in log contrast sensitivity for the 1 cpd stimulus (−0.37 log unit) than for the 0.5 cpd stimulus (−0.11 log unit).
For Experiment 2 (solid symbols, middle and bottom in Figure 3), for all three stimuli there were significant blur effects for the 6 D condition but not for the 3 D condition. Effects of the 6 D blur condition were similar for the smaller window with SD = 0.25° (−0.33 log unit) and for the 0.5 cpd cosine stimulus (−0.36 log unit). The larger Gaussian (SD = 0.5°) showed less of a decline (t >4.5, p < 0.001), by −0.17 log unit.
For Experiment 3 (open symbols, top in Figure 3), Ring 1 had significant blur effects for the 4 D (−0.17 log unit) and 6 D (−0.27 log unit) conditions, and Ring 2 had a significant effect for the 6 D (−0.20 log unit) condition; there were no significant blur effects for Ring 3. For the 3 D blur condition, all three rings had less than 0.08 log unit decline in sensitivity. For each blur level, the standard deviation was greatest for Ring 1.
DISCUSSION
These experiments explored window size and spatial frequency content to identify stimuli that should be minimally affected by peripheral defocus. For the central visual field, peripheral defocus can be as much as 3 D of blur for an oriented stimulus. Expt 1 found that 3 D of blur had little effect on contrast sensitivity at 0.5 cpd, and had a stronger impact at 1.0 cpd. Expt 2 found that use of even lower spatial frequencies reduced the blur effect at 6 D. This allowed design of CSP stimuli for which the effect of 3 D of blur on contrast sensitivity should be less than 0.1 log unit throughout the central visual field, as confirmed in Exp 3.
Peripheral defocus can be due to either hyperopic or myopic change in peripheral refractive error with eccentricity, and the blur conditions would have opposite effects for these two types of defocus. If in some subjects peripheral refractive error would be compounded by the positive blur of the test lenses and others the effect of the positive test lenses would be corrective then between-subject variability for Rings 2 & 3 would be smallest for Ring 1. However, at all blur levels the standard deviation was largest for Ring 1, which is further evidence that peripheral defocus has minimal effect for these stimuli.
These results are similar to those of a study of blur effects for frequency-doubling stimuli10, whose 3 D blur condition caused a decline in contrast sensitivity by 0.15 log unit at 0.5 cpd and only 0.05 log unit at 0.25 cpd. Our blur effects at 3 D were similar to theirs for 0.5 cpd: 0.11, 0.07, 0.08 log unit for Experiments 1, 2 ,3, respectively. They also found that between-subject variability did not increase with blur level, and described why this ruled out significant effects of peripheral refractive error. A study of blur effects for 0.25 cpd frequency-doubling stimuli on a clinical device found blur effects of ~0.05 log unit for a 3 D blur condition11. These studies used large stimuli, 100 deg2, while our stimuli had areas (at ± 3 SD) as small as 2 deg2. For Experiment 3, stimulus areas ranged from 7 deg2 to 90 deg2. The spatial scaling was intended to make contrast sensitivity constant across the central visual field, and results of Experiment 3 show that this was in the main accomplished, as mean contrast sensitivity for Rings 1 & 3 differed by only 0.01 log unit.
Previous studies have found that grating patches with peak spatial frequencies from 0.25 to 1.0 cpd can provide lower test-retest variability in glaucomatous visual field defects than conventional perimetry, while retaining good ability to assess defects.15, 22, 23 The current study finds that caution should be used with spatial frequencies greater than 0.5 cpd, because peripheral defocus could reduce contrast sensitivity. Recently, Harwerth et al.24 found that contrast sensitivity losses at 1–2 cpd preceded losses at 0.25–0.5 cpd in monkeys with experimental glaucoma, which provides a potential support for using 1–2 cpd in CSP. However, the results of the current study show that peripheral defocus may be a potential confounder for these spatial frequencies when used clinically.
To further investigate the potential effects of peripheral defocus, we used geometric optics in three steps of modeling to assess effects of blur. This modeling incorporates the effects of higher-order aberrations, in that the combined effect of all aberrations can be approximated with an equivalent blur circle, which in turn corresponds to spherical blur. The modeling was for spatial frequencies from 0 to 2 cpd, and pupil diameters from 2 to 8 mm. The first step used geometrical optics to calculate the modulation transfer function (MTF) for a given pupil diameter and blur level. The MTF characterizes the reduction of contrast at each spatial frequency for that pupil diameter and blur level (blur effects are greater for larger pupils).
Two examples of MTFs are shown in the upper left panel of Figure 4. The second step calculated changes in the spatial frequency spectrum of the stimulus by multiplying the spectrum by the absolute value of the MTF, as illustrated in the remaining panels. The spatial frequency spectrum of a stimulus is the Fourier transform of the stimulus, and shows how much contrast is present at each spatial frequency. Multiplying the spectrum times the MTF characterizes how blur affects the stimulus. The third step estimated how this change in the stimulus would be expected to affect a subject’s contrast sensitivity. The relative contributions of different spatial frequencies to detection was approximated with a lowpass filter derived from a study of contrast sensitivity using Gabor stimuli19. The blur effect was computed as the logarithm of the ratio of the mean of the filtered stimulus spectrum and the mean of the filtered stimulus spectrum reduced by the MTF.
Figure 5 shows the results of step three, the expected reduction in contrast sensitivity as a function of blur for a variety of pupil sizes, along with means from the three experiments. The thick curves are for the mean pupil diameter calculated using a standard model25 for pupil diameter incorporating age, mean luminance and display size for each test. We measured pupil size in a subset of our subjects and obtained results consistent with the standard model. All mean blur effects fell within 1 standard error of the thick curves, although the data tended to fall above the curve for 1–3 D and below the curve for 6 D.
Figure 6 applies this analysis to data in the literature about blur effects on perimetric sensitivity. The top two panels show blur effects for 0.25 and 0.5 cpd gratings from a study of frequency-doubling stimuli10, and the bottom panel shows blur effects from a study of size III perimetry2. The predicted blur effect for the standard pupil size is shown with a thick curve for each stimulus. Means were within 1 SE of the predicted blur effect for all conditions with grating stimuli, but for the size III data blur effects were greater than expected. The size III stimulus has substantial energy at spatial frequencies greater than 0.5 c/deg, so we removed the lowpass filter and even so the data were more than 1 SE below the predictions at 4–6 D. The ability of the modeling to generate reasonable predictions for most of the stimuli is encouraging, but further work is needed before it can be applied more broadly than the range 0.1 – 1.0 cpd for Gaussian-windowed gratings. The analyses in Figures 5 & 6 provide a basic guideline from geometrical optics: restricting spatial frequency to a maximum of 0.5 cpd provides means that 3 D of blur will cause no more than 0.1 log unit reduction in mean perimetric sensitivity. This is substantially less than the 0.4 log unit reduction reported for size III.
Experiments 1 & 2 studied effects of blur for variations in spatial frequency and size in the macula, and found the results to be consistent with predictions based on geometrical optics. This allowed us to design CSP stimuli that should be sufficiently resistant to blur throughout the central visual field that perimetric sensitivities will be relatively affected by peripheral refractive error, as confirmed in Experiment 3.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported by NIH Grants R01EY007716 (Swanson), 5P30EY019008 (Indiana University School of Optometry), and T35EY020481 (SUNY College of Optometry). Larry Thibos and Stephen Burns provided useful background for the modeling with geometrical optics.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
REFERENCES
- 1.Goldmann H. Fundamentals of exact perimetry. 1945. Optom Vis Sci. 1999;76:599–604. doi: 10.1097/00006324-199908000-00030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Heuer DK, Anderson DR, Feuer WJ, Gressel MG. The influence of refraction accuracy on automated perimetric threshold measurements. Ophthalmology. 1987;94:1550–1553. doi: 10.1016/s0161-6420(87)33248-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Anderson RS, McDowell DR, Ennis FA. Effect of localized defocus on detection thresholds for different sized targets in the fovea and periphery. Acta Ophthalmol Scand. 2001;79:60–63. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0420.2001.079001060.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Atchison DA. Effect of defocus on visual field measurement. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1987;7:259–265. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Aung T, Foster PJ, Seah SK, Chan SP, Lim WK, Wu HM, Lim AT, Lee LL, Chew SJ. Automated static perimetry: the influence of myopia and its method of correction. Ophthalmology. 2001;108:290–295. doi: 10.1016/s0161-6420(00)00497-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gaffney M. Refractive errors and automated perimetry: discussion and case studies. J Ophthalmic Nurs Technol. 1993;12:167–171. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Weinreb RN, Perlman JP. The effect of refractive correction on automated perimetric thresholds. Am J Ophthalmol. 1986;101:706–709. doi: 10.1016/0002-9394(86)90774-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Tabernero J, Ohlendorf A, Fischer MD, Bruckmann AR, Schiefer U, Schaeffel F. Peripheral refraction profiles in subjects with low foveal refractive errors. Optom Vis Sci. 2011;88:388–394. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0b013e31820bb0f5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Heijl A, Lindgren G, Olsson J. Normal variability of static perimetric threshold values across the central visual field. Arch Ophthalmol. 1987;105:1544–1549. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1987.01060110090039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Anderson AJ, Johnson CA. Frequency-doubling technology perimetry and optical defocus. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003;44:4147–4152. doi: 10.1167/iovs.02-1076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Artes PH, Nicolela MT, McCormick TA, LeBlanc RP, Chauhan BC. Effects of blur and repeated testing on sensitivity estimates with frequency doubling perimetry. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003;44:646–652. doi: 10.1167/iovs.02-0532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Salvetat ML, Zeppieri M, Parisi L, Johnson CA, Sampaolesi R, Brusini P. Learning effect and test-retest variability of pulsar perimetry. J Glaucoma. 2011 doi: 10.1097/IJG.0b013e318237bfe7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Harwerth RS, Crawford ML, Frishman LJ, Viswanathan S, Smith EL, 3rd, Carter-Dawson L. Visual field defects and neural losses from experimental glaucoma. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2002;21:91–125. doi: 10.1016/s1350-9462(01)00022-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sun H, Dul MW, Swanson WH. Linearity can account for the similarity among conventional, frequency-doubling, and gabor-based perimetric tests in the glaucomatous macula. Optom Vis Sci. 2006;83:455–465. doi: 10.1097/01.opx.0000225103.18087.5d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hot A, Dul MW, Swanson WH. Development and evaluation of a contrast sensitivity perimetry test for patients with glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008;49:3049–3057. doi: 10.1167/iovs.07-1205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Charman WN, Radhakrishnan H. Peripheral refraction and the development of refractive error: a review. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2010;30:321–338. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2010.00746.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Thibos LN, Wheeler W, Horner D. Power vectors: an application of Fourier analysis to the description and statistical analysis of refractive error. Optom Vis Sci. 1997;74:367–375. doi: 10.1097/00006324-199706000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Watson AB. Estimation of local spatial scale. J Opt Soc Am (A) 1987;4:1579–1582. doi: 10.1364/josaa.4.001579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Keltgen KM, Swanson WH. Estimation of spatial scale across the visual field using sinusoidal stimuli. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012;53:633–639. doi: 10.1167/iovs.10-6674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.King-Smith PE, Grigsby SS, Vingrys AJ, Benes SC, Supowit A. Efficient and unbiased modifications of the QUEST threshold method: theory, simulations, experimental evaluation and practical implementation. Vision Res. 1994;34:885–912. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90039-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Turpin A, McKendrick AM, Johnson CA, Vingrys AJ. Properties of perimetric threshold estimates from full threshold, ZEST, and SITA-like strategies, as determined by computer simulation. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003;44:4787–4795. doi: 10.1167/iovs.03-0023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Artes PH, Hutchison DM, Nicolela MT, LeBlanc RP, Chauhan BC. Threshold and variability properties of matrix frequency-doubling technology and standard automated perimetry in glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005;46:2451–2457. doi: 10.1167/iovs.05-0135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Chauhan BC, Johnson CA. Test-retest variability of frequency-doubling perimetry and conventional perimetry in glaucoma patients and normal subjects. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1999;40:648–656. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Harwerth RS, Wheat JL, Fredette MJ, Anderson DR. Linking structure and function in glaucoma. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2010;29:249–271. doi: 10.1016/j.preteyeres.2010.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Watson AB, Yellott JI. A unified formula for light-adapted pupil size. J Vis. 2012;12:12. doi: 10.1167/12.10.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]