Abstract
Research investigating how people remember the distance of paths they walk has shown two apparently conflicting effects of experience during encoding on subsequent distance judgments. By the feature accumulation effect discrete path features such as turns, houses or other landmarks cause an increase in remembered distance. By the distractor effect performance of a concurrent task during path encoding causes a decrease in remembered distance. This study asks: What are the conditions that determine whether the feature accumulation or the distractor effect dominates distortions of space? In two experiments, blindfolded participants were guided along two legs of a right triangle while reciting nonsense syllables. On some trials, one of the two legs contained features: horizontally mounted car antennas (gates) that bent out of the way as participants walked past. At the end of the second leg participants either indicated the remembered path leg lengths using their hands in a ratio estimation task, or attempted to walk, unguided, straight back to the beginning. In addition to response mode, visual access to the paths and time between encoding and response were manipulated to determine if these factors affected feature accumulation or distractor effects. Path legs with added features were remembered as shorter than those without, but this result was only significant in the haptic response mode data. This finding suggests that when people form spatial memory representations with the intention of navigating in room-scale spaces, interfering with information accumulation substantially distorts spatial memory.
Knowledge of distances helps humans navigate, remain oriented, plan routes and give directions. Attempts to measure mental representations of distance produce varying results depending in large part on how the distances are encoded and recalled (for reviews see Montello, 1997; 2009). Previous studies have shown that across a range of circumstances, paths with more features (e.g., turns, intersections, landmarks) tend to be remembered as longer than paths with fewer features. In contrast, two studies by Glasauer and colleagues (Glasauer, Schneider, Grasso & Ivanenko, 2007; Glasauer et al., 2009) have shown that paths walked while counting backward by sevens are perceived as shorter than paths walked without a concurrent task. The current study examines the effects of path features on remembered distance for paths walked with a concurrent task. By using a conjunction of the conditions that give rise to the expanding and shrinking of distance representations, we aim to understand the mechanisms responsible for these contrasting, systematic distortions in spatial memory.
Features Make Paths Seem Longer
The bulk of the relevant research shows that paths associated with more features are remembered as longer than paths associated with fewer features (e.g., Allen, 1981; Allen & Kirasic, 1985; Briggs, 1973; Jansen-Osmann & Berendt, 2005). We will refer to this as the feature accumulation effect (Montello, 1997). For example, Sadalla and Staplin (1980A) required participants to follow experimental paths of masking tape, 8.53 m long, while wearing a blinder that restricted vision to approximately .91 m ahead. At the end of each experimental path, participants walked a short (1.52 m) reference path, and then drew the relative length of the experimental path on a piece of paper that showed a representation of the reference path (ratio estimation). Distance estimates were longer for paths that contained more intersections (other, crossing masking tape paths). Sadalla and colleagues also showed that paths on which the masking tape intersections were labeled with high frequency names were judged to be longer than equivalent length paths marked with low frequency names (Sadalla & Staplin, 1980B; Sadalla, Staplin & Burroughs, 1979). These data suggested that the observed feature accumulation effect was driven by participants’ greater ability to recall the high frequency names, which resulted in the association of the high frequency name paths with more information. The authors interpreted these results as consistent with an information storage account of the feature accumulation effect; remembered route length scales with the amount of information, spatial or otherwise, associated with that route in memory.
Hutcheson and Wedell (2009) provide a theoretical explanation of the feature accumulation effect, based on dual memory systems, that refines the information storage account. They showed that the feature accumulation effect increases when a concurrent task is included during path encoding or when a distractor task is included during a delay period between encoding and responding. The authors note that this increase in the feature accumulation effect with decreasing memory fidelity contrasts with the results of Sadalla and Staplin (1980B). Sadalla and Staplin showed that the feature accumulation effect was greater when more information about the route (names that labeled the intersections) was available in memory. Hutcheson and Wedell explain this discrepancy in terms of a dual memory systems account. This view distinguishes between fine-grained, metric spatial memory which is more precise but harder to encode/maintain, and categorical or non-metric spatial memory which is less accurate but more robust (Huttenlocher, Hedges & Duncan, 1991). According to this account, the feature accumulation effect results from the influence of non-metric memory codes, which increases when more fragile metric spatial codes are less available. Sadalla and Staplin’s name frequency manipulation served to increase the relative availability of non-metric codes (how many names are associated with a given path), and thus increased the feature accumulation effect. Hutcheson and Wedell employed manipulations (concurrent tasks and filled delays) that served to decrease the relative availability of metric spatial codes, which also increased the feature accumulation effect (also see Jansen-Osmann & Wiedenbauer, 2006). Thus, the dual memory systems account suggests that the feature accumulation effect depends not only on the quantity of information associated with a route, but on the quality of that information as well.
The feature accumulation effect has been demonstrated in several studies comparing remembered distances for paths containing a greater and smaller number of turns. For example, Sadalla and Magel (1980, Ex. 1) required participants to first walk two 60.96 m experimental paths, one containing two turns, the other containing seven turns, and then to walk a straight 30.48 m reference path. The remembered distances of the experimental paths were then indicated by drawing a vertical hash mark on a horizontal response line that had the length of the reference path already marked on it (ratio estimation). The seven-turn paths were remembered as significantly longer than the two-turn paths. This variety of feature accumulation effect, termed the angularity effect, has been replicated using different numbers of turns, different behavioral measures of path length (e.g., reproduction of walking distances) (Sadalla & Magel, 1980, Ex. 2 & 3), and in virtual reality (Hutcheson & Wedell, 2009; Jansen-Osmann & Berendt, 2002).
Consistent with a dual memory systems account of feature accumulation effects, the most consistent moderator of the angularity effect appears to be the degree to which memory for the routes is taxed during either encoding or maintenance (e.g., Hutcheson & Wedell, 2009). Because metric representations are less durable, conditions that challenge memory systems are likely to result in relative reliance on non-metric memory codes. For example, Jansen-Osmann and Wiedenbauer (2006) observed an angularity effect when distance estimates were made after walking all paths (two turn, seven turn, reference), but no effect when estimates were made after each path. As participants walk and encode additional path lengths, the relative availability of fine-grained, metric spatial information decreases, and the influence of non-metric information (number of turns) emerges. Likewise, angularity effects may be elusive in prospective distance judgments because foreknowledge of the upcoming spatial task results in particular effort directed towards encoding and maintaining metric information.
Another perspective that has been proposed to explain feature accumulation effects focuses on the role of features as segmenters of associated routes. Route segmentation accounts suggest that path features break routes up into sub-sections, and that routes associated with more sections are remembered as being longer (Allen, 1981; Allen & Kirasic, 1985; Jansen-Osmann & Berendt, 2005; Montello, 1997). The tendency for human memory systems to segment space into manageable chunks is well documented (e.g., Gobet, et al, 2001; Sargent, Dopkins, Philbeck & Chichka, 2010; Wang & Brockmole, 2003). In addition, there is evidence that distances between locations in different chunks are exaggerated in spatial memory relative to distances between locations in common chunks (Allen & Kirasic, 1985; McNamara, Hardy & Hirtle, 1989, for a discussion of possible mechanisms see Holyoak & Mah, 1982; Sadalla & Magel, 1980; Zacks, Speer, Swallow, Braver & Reynolds, 2007). Unlike the information storage and dual memory systems accounts, the route segmentation account predicts that the feature accumulation effect depends on the ability of features to segment the route. Thus, features that are homogenous and continuous throughout a route should be less likely to produce the feature accumulation effect than features that cause a route to be segmented into chunks.
Concurrent Tasks Make Paths Seem Shorter
Evidence that concurrent tasks can actually cause a decrease in distance estimates for associated paths comes from studies of self-motion reproduction (Glasauer et al., 2007; Glasauer et al., 2009). In the most relevant experiments, blindfolded participants walked along outbound paths, between 9.14 and 18.29 m in length, and then reproduced the paths either by continuing on in the same direction as the outbound path or by turning around and walking back to the origin. When participants were required to perform a concurrent mental task (counting backwards by sevens) during only the outbound path, subsequent reproductions were shorter. Conversely, when participants were required to perform the concurrent task during only the reproductions, those reproductions were longer. In other words, performing a concurrent task while walking a path led to a shorter estimate of that path’s distance. Glasauer and colleagues suggested that counting backwards by sevens distracted participants from the accumulation of temporal or distance cues (or both) resulting in shorter distance representations. Following Glasauer, we will call this the distractor effect. An analogous mechanism has been proposed by researchers studying temporal duration judgments. The attentional allocation model posits that if more attention is devoted to the perception of passing time, then subsequent judgments of duration will be longer because increased attentional resources facilitate the accumulation of temporal cues1 (e.g, Brown, 1997; Brown & West, 1990; Hicks, Miller, Gaes, & Bierman, 1977; Zakay, 1989).
While counting backward by sevens might not be a path feature per se, it might still result in a path being associated with a greater amount of information. So why did Glasauer et al. (2007, 2009) find that concurrent tasks consistently decrease remembered distances? Likewise, why do the various path features described in literature reporting the feature accumulation effect show no evidence of distracting participants from the accumulation of distance cues? One possibility is that in the paradigm used by Glasauer and colleagues, the concurrent task was simply more distracting than the path features that lead to the feature accumulation effect. Another possibility is that the feature accumulation effect depends on the segmentation of paths into multiple discrete parts, and concurrent tasks do not segment paths as well as features do. The contrasting effects might also have resulted from procedural differences, for example, in the degree of visual access at encoding and in the method of assessing distance knowledge. The current study evaluates these possibilities.
Current Study
In order to understand the conditions under which the distractor and feature accumulation effects obtain, we examined four potentially critical differences between the paradigms that have found feature accumulation effects and those that have found distractor effects. First, in studies showing the feature accumulation effect, the features were punctate and environmental. That is, the features were of limited spatial and temporal extent relative to the entire path, and they were part of the physical environment. These landmark characteristics are not shared by the concurrent tasks that caused the distractor effect. The current study tested for the distractor effect using punctate environmental features. One possibility is that the perception of these features requires less mental effort than counting backwards by sevens, and thus is less distracting. The punctate environmental features used in this study (gates) were linked to changes in a concurrent task (recitation of a nonsense syllable); a new syllable was given at each gate. So in addition to being generally distracting throughout the paths, this concurrent task demanded increased attention precisely at time points when punctate environmental features also demanded attention. Thus, if taxing attentional resources by distraction underlies the distractor effect, the inclusion of this linked concurrent task should increase the relative probability that punctate environmental features will cause the distractor effect, rather than the feature accumulation effect.
However, requiring a change in the concurrent task at each punctate environmental feature may also serve to increase the salience or memorability of those features, and thus increase the likelihood of the feature accumulation effect (Hutcheson & Wedell, 2009; Sadalla & Staplin, 1980B). Furthermore, the association of each path segment with different concurrent task features (syllables) might distinguish those segments in memory. This also might be expected to increase the likelihood of the feature accumulation effect. It has been suggested that the feature accumulation effect arises because features segment associated routes into discrete parts, and routes remembered as having more parts are remembered as being longer (e.g., Allen & Kirasic, 1985; Jansen-Osmann & Berendt, 2005). The concurrent tasks used by Glasauer and colleagues may fail to make paths seem longer because they do not segment those paths. We tested this possibility by looking at the effect of punctate features on remembered path length.
A second difference between paradigms showing distractor and feature accumulation effects is that demonstrations of the feature accumulation effect involve at least some visual access to the environment whereas the distractor effect has been observed only in blindfolded participants. One might expect path features to be considerably less distracting when paths are encoded with visual input. For example, vision provides a preview of upcoming features so that they may be less surprising, and thus less distracting. Experiment 2 asks whether the distractor effect can be obtained even when visual information is available at encoding.
Third, the feature accumulation effect increases when a filled delay occurs between encoding and responding (Hutcheson & Wedell, 2009), but the distractor effect has been observed only when encoding immediately precedes responding. There is support, reviewed above, for the idea that the feature accumulation effect arises from the influence of non-metric spatial information on distance estimation (e.g., paths with more segments seem longer, regardless of the metric properties of the segments). Relative reliance on non-metric spatial properties increases as the availability of less durable metric information decreases. Therefore, inserting a filled delay between encoding and response may serve to increase the relative influence of the feature accumulation effect. Experiment 2 tested this possibility as well.
Fourth, whereas the feature accumulation effect has been observed using a number of response modes, including psychophysical scaling techniques such as ratio estimation and action-based methods such as distance reproduction (Sadalla & Magel, 1980), the distractor effect has only been observed in reproduction tasks. Glasauer and colleagues suggest that the distractor effect, which is well established in time perception tasks, influences the representations of time used for movement reproduction. It has also been suggested that movement reproduction tasks depend not on static distance estimates, but rather on the storage of velocity profiles that can be recreated at response (Berthoz, Israel, Georges-Francois, Grasso & Tsuzuku, 1995). If the distractor effect operates strictly by influencing the temporal aspects of an experienced (to be reproduced) velocity profile, then we would expect no distractor effect in responses that do not depend on a stored velocity profile. We tested this possibility by looking for the distractor effect in ratio estimation and triangle completion tasks, neither of which can be performed by reproducing movements or durations.
In sum, this study asked four questions about the effects of distraction on spatial encoding. Can we find evidence that features distract from the accumulation of distance cues when 1) those features are physical and punctate (as opposed to continuous), 2) participants have visual access to the environment, 3) there is a filled delay between encoding and responding, and when 4) distance estimates are made by ratio estimation and triangle completion? We used a paradigm in which participants walked short “L” shaped paths (L paths) with limited or no vision, and then either estimated the lengths of the path legs using their hands or walked, unguided, back to the beginning of the path (triangle completion).
Experiment 1
Experiment 1 asked if continuous, non-physical distractors and action-based responding are necessary conditions for the distractor effect by looking for the effect with punctate environmental features in ratio estimation responding. Blindfolded participants walked a series of L paths while reciting a nonsense syllable and at the end of each L path either used their hands to estimate the lengths of the two path legs by ratio estimation (explicit, haptic response mode) or performed a triangle completion task (action-based, walking response mode). Analyses tested for the distractor effect using discrete path features and two different response modes.
Methods
Participants
Twenty-five undergraduates (16 females) participated in this study in exchange for course credit. Their mean age was 20 years (range: 18 – 28).
Apparatus
The experiment took place inside a large room (8.23 m × 15.54 m) in which two identical L paths were set up. Starting and stopping points for leg 1 and leg 2, respectively, were marked on the paths for the three levels of path leg length manipulation (leg 1/leg 2): long (6.10 m)/long (6.10 m), short (4.88 m)/long (6.10 m), and long (6.10 m/short (4.27 m). These are illustrated in Figure 1. The short version of leg 2 was slightly shorter than the short version of leg 1 in order to discourage participants from representing the path lengths non-metrically, dichotomously, as either long or short. Participants alternated between the two L paths on successive trials to avoid error feedback on walking trials. The path features were gates through which blindfolded participants were led; they were made from flexible car antennas mounted horizontally on movable stands. The antennas bent out of the way on participant’s torsos as they walked past. For trials on which the L paths contained features, the gates were either on leg 1 or leg 2, never both. Legs with features always contained two gates. The gate locations on the paths, by leg length condition, are shown in Figure 1. Hearing protectors provided 29 dB of noise reduction and blindfolds were of the soft light blocking variety designed for sleeping. Experimenters led participants using a dowel .61 m long and 2.54 cm in diameter. The experimenter held the middle of the dowel horizontally, at chest level, in front of the participants who grabbed the ends of the dowel, one in each hand.
Procedure
Participants were met outside the experimental room, where they were briefed and gave consent to participate. After receiving instructions, they were fitted with a blindfold and hearing protectors and led into the experimental room. Participants were told that they would be led along paths of varying lengths, and sometimes they would be asked to show the lengths of the paths using their hands, other times they would be asked to walk back to the start of the paths on their own. Participants were also instructed that as they were led along the various experimental paths they would recite, out loud, a nonsense syllable (e.g., val, jub, niv, ver) which would be called out by an experimenter. Different nonsense syllables were randomly assigned to the different L path conditions. Participants were instructed not to synchronize the syllables with their steps, and generally not to count anything throughout the experiment in order to discourage the use of counting as a basis for subsequent distance estimates. Also, participants were warned that they would occasionally pass through a gate, and that a new nonsense syllable would be given at each gate, and at the 90° turns on the L paths. So path features were not only the gates but also contemporaneous changes in the nonsense syllable being recited.
First, participants were led along a straight comparator path (12.19 m). At the end of this path their hands were placed on a yard stick mounted horizontally on a stand at waist level, with a rubber band marking a distance 68.58 cm from the left end of the stick. They were told that the distance from the end of the stick to the rubber band represented the length of the comparator path, and that they would be asked to estimate with their fingers the lengths of other paths on the measuring stick relative to the comparator path. The comparator path was then walked a second and final time to provide a better sense of the comparator distance.
Then, two practice trials walking the L paths were completed, one haptic and one walking response, to make sure participants were comfortable with the tasks. For haptic response trials, participants indicated on the yardstick, separately, the lengths of the first and second legs of the L path they had just walked, relative to the comparator path. For walking response trials, participants were instructed to turn left and walk straight back to the origin. The stopping place was then recorded using reel measuring tapes as the distance from each of two anchor points. Each anchor point was a bolt protruding through a piece of sheet metal that was taped to the floor, one on each side of the experimental room. The ends of the reel tape measures were hooked over the bolts. Response times were recorded in seconds. Times to walk leg 1, leg 2, and to complete the response were all recorded separately for each trial. The experimenter guiding participants along the L paths (the same experimenter guided all participants) tried to achieve a plateau shaped velocity profile that might be considered typical for walking with vision. Maximum velocity was also typical for walking with vision but was determined, to some extent, by what individual participants were comfortable with. Experimental sessions lasted approximately 60 minutes.
Design
A within-subjects design was used: 2 (response mode: haptic vs. walking) × 3 (features: gates on leg 1, leg 2, or no gates) × 3 (leg length: long/long, long/short, short/long)2. Each subject completed 30 trials. One trial was run for each of the 18 conditions except the long/long conditions; these were run 3 times each in order to examine effects of response mode and features on response variability. Order of the trial conditions was randomized separately for each participant.
Results
For haptic responses, the raw data were the leg lengths indicated by participants on the yard stick. For walking responses, the raw data were the angles through which participants rotated, counter-clockwise, at the ends of the L paths as indicated by the stopping locations of participants on unguided paths (observed rotation angle in Fig. 2).
We first present results for the haptic responses (see Fig. 3). A three-way, 2 (features, no features) × 2 (long [6.1 m], short [4.3 – 4.9 m]) × 2 (leg 1, leg 2), ANOVA showed a main effect of features: paths with features were remembered as being shorter than those without, F(1,24) = 4.33, p = .048. A main effect of leg length was also observed, F(2,24) = 63.40, p < .001. No other main effects or interactions reached significance. The effect of leg length shows that our behavioral measures were sensitive to actual differences in leg length. The effect of features indicates that paths with gates and syllable changes were remembered as being shorter than those without. Thus the distractor effect, previously observed for continuous (concurrent task) distractors in motor reproduction response data, was demonstrated using discrete, physical distractors in ratio estimation response data.
In the walking response mode, independent data for each individual path leg were not available. Therefore, walking data are presented by L path condition, rather than by L path leg condition (see Fig. 4). Greater observed rotation values indicate greater remembered length for leg 2 relative to leg 1 (See Fig. 2). A two-way, 3 (features on leg 1, no features, features on leg 2) × 3 (short/long, long/long, long/short) ANOVA showed a main effect of relative leg length, F(2,48) = 12.69, p < .001. No other effects reached significance. As with the haptic data, the walking response data showed sensitivity to actual differences in leg length; participants rotated more as the relative length of leg 2 to leg 1 increased. However, there was no significant main effect of features in the walking data, nor did any simple main effects of features at any of the levels of leg length reach significance.
Figure 4 shows a general tendency to under-rotate. Although participants were explicitly told that the turn between legs 1 and 2 was always 90°, rotation error may have been influenced by misperception of this turn or by bias in the response turn. Previous work using triangle completion tasks has shown that people show rotation bias towards pre-established, internal default values such that larger turns tend to be underestimated and smaller turns tend to be overestimated (Fujita, Klatzky, Loomis & Golledge, 1993; Klatzky, Beall, Loomis, Golledge & Philbeck, 1999; Loomis et al., 1993). In the current experiment, because the turn between the two path legs was always 90°, and because this is a canonical turn size, it is possible that 90° was a common default value. Given that the correct response rotation angles were all greater than 90°, the observed under-rotation may reflect a bias toward the canonical 90° angle. In any case we do not suggest that rotation error reflects an unbiased measure of remembered leg lengths, but rather, assume that any bias is independent of the effect of features, our variable of primary interest.
Finally, we examined the time taken to walk the paths. Repeated measures ANOVAs showed a significant main effect of features on walk time, F(1,24) = 7.71, p = .01 (see Table 1). Despite efforts to keep velocity across all paths consistent, paths with features took slightly longer than paths without. Possible confounding of the effect of features with the effect of path duration is of little concern in the haptic response data because paths with features, though associated with slightly longer durations, were remembered as shorter in length. To test whether controlling for walk time might reveal a significant effect of features in the walking response data, for each subject we first regressed rotation angle (see Fig. 2) on the ratio of walk times for the two path legs (Leg 1 time/Leg 2 time). Then we repeated the ANOVA described above using the residuals from these regressions instead of rotation angle as the dependent variable. The effect of features still did not approach significance. The walk time data also showed that time from stopping at end of leg 2 to completion of responses was greater for haptic than for walking responses, and time to complete walking responses was slightly longer for trials on which either path leg contained features, t[24] = 2.10, p = .047 (see Table 2).
Table 1.
Leg Length: | 6.10 m | 4.88 m | 4.27 m | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Features: | Gates | No Gates | Gates | No Gates | Gates | No Gates |
Experiment 1 | 8.17 (.17) | 7.81 (.16) | 6.78 (.19) | 6.67 (.14) | 6.36 (.20) | 6.14 (.15) |
Experiment 2 | 6.91 (.14) | 6.73 (.14) | 5.81 (.15) | 5.83 (.14) | 5.33 (.14) | 5.03 (.11) |
Table 2.
Response Mode: | Haptic | Walking | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Features: | Gates | No Gates | Gates | No Gates |
Experiment 1 | 19.38 (.76) | 19.34 (.97) | 17.73 (.67) | 17.00 (.60) |
Experiment 2 | 42.19 (1.61) | 41.93 (1.80) | 42.64 (1.18) | 42.14 (1.29) |
Discussion
In experiment 1 we found a distractor effect in the ratio estimation haptic task, but not in the triangle completion walking task. In the haptic data the distractor effect emerged even under conditions that might have been expected to produce the opposite result. First, punctate features caused paths to be remembered as shorter. This suggests that distractors need not be continuous throughout the paths in order to distract from the accumulation of distance cues. Even discrete features that segment paths into multiple parts can lead to shortening of remembered path lengths. Second, the distractor effect was found in a ratio estimation task in which distance representations must be rescaled and indicated haptically. This suggests that if the distractor effect operates by warping temporal representations, as suggested by Glasauer et al. (2007, 2009), these representations are shared between systems that guide motor reproduction and ratio estimation.
By the same logic, the absence of a distractor effect in the walking response mode data might indicate that warped temporal representations do not play as large a role in triangle completion tasks. Motor reproduction may be performed by reproducing stored temporal profiles (e.g, Berthoz, et al. 1995) and ratio estimation may be performed by simply reporting remembered path durations in spatial units. However, triangle completion tasks are more likely to depend on the construction of flexible environmental representations, which may be less influenced by temporal biases. We revisit the null result in the walking response data in the discussion of Experiment 2.
Experiment 2
Experiment 2 asked if either visual deprivation or immediate responding are necessary conditions for the distractor effect. Previously, the distractor effect has been observed only in blindfolded participants, responding immediately after path encoding (Glasauer et al., 2007; 2009). Either or both of these procedural features might be critical for punctate features to cause the distractor effect. For example, one might expect path features to be less distracting when participants can see them coming. Also, filled delays have been shown to increase the feature accumulation effect (Hutcheson & Wedell, 2009). Experiment 2 attempted to replicate the distractor effect using multiple response modes (as in Experiment 1), visual access to the paths at encoding, and a filled delay between path encoding and responding.
Methods
Participants
Twenty-five Washington University undergraduates (12 females) participated for course credit. One participant did not complete the experimental session due to mild disorientation and dizziness. The mean age of the group was 19 years (range: 18 – 26 years).
Materials
The following changes were made to the materials used in Experiment 1: The hearing protectors and blindfold were replaced with earplugs and a welder’s helmet with blackout curtain attached to both the front and back of the helmet. The flap of curtain on the front of the helmet could be taped up, allowing participants to see under the welding visor approximately 1.22 m in front of them and .61 m to either side. The paths were marked by colored string running along the floor that participants could see and follow when the flap was up.
The Brooks (1968) letter task was used as a distractor task to be performed between path encoding and response. This visual imagery task requires participants to picture a given block letter and trace clockwise around the outside of the image in the mind’s eye, classifying each corner as being on the extreme top or bottom of the letter, for which a verbal “yes” response is given, or anywhere else, for which a verbal “no” response is given. Hutcheson and Wedell (2009) used a Shepard-Metzler mental rotation task as a distractor during the delay between encoding and response. We used the Brooks letter task because it is a spatial mental imagery task that can be administered to blindfolded participants.
Procedure
The following changes were made to the procedure from Experiment 1. Before entering the experimental room, participants were instructed that they would be allowed to see about four feet in front of them while being led along the L shaped paths, but otherwise, vision would be occluded throughout the experiment. Also, participants practiced performing the distractor (Brooks letter) task. At the beginning of each L shaped path, the flap on the helmet was raised so that participants had limited vision as they were led along the L paths. At the end of each L path, before haptic or walking responses were given, the flap on the helmet was lowered, and participants performed the distractor task for 30 s on three letters (10 s each) as they were called out by an experimenter. Experimental sessions lasted approximately 90 minutes.
Results
Data were analyzed as in Experiment 1. Results for the haptic response data were similar to those observed in Experiment 1 (See Fig. 5). Repeated measures ANOVA showed main effects of features, F(1,23) = 10.03, p = .004, and leg length, F(2,23) = 50.85, p < .001. No other effects reached significance. Again, participants showed sensitivity to actual path length, and more importantly, paths with features were remembered as being shorter than those without.
Results for the walking response mode were also similar to Experiment 1. Repeated measures ANOVA showed a main effect of relative leg length, F(2,46) = 18.22, p < .001, but no other effects reached significance (see Fig. 6). The walking response data again showed sensitivity to actual differences in leg length but no effect of features. Figure 6 shows the tendency, observed in Experiment 1, to under-rotate, except when the correct rotation was smallest, when the second leg was short. This pattern is consistent with previous triangle completion studies showing that participants under rotate when larger turns are in order and over rotate for smaller turns (Fujita, Klatzky, Loomis & Golledge, 1993; Klatzky, Beall, Loomis, Golledge & Philbeck, 1999; Loomis et al., 1993).
Tables 1 and 2 show that the pattern of temporal data for Experiment 2 is similar to that for Experiment 1. Repeated measures ANOVAs showed a significant main effect of features on walk time, F(1,23) = 8.67, p = .007; paths with features took slightly longer than paths without (see Table 1). We re-ran the analysis of walking response mode data controlling for walk time, as described in Experiment 1. Again, this analysis failed to reveal a significant effect of features. No significant differences were seen in response times (see Table 2). Walk times were markedly shorter in these data than in Experiment 1, probably reflecting a tendency amongst participants to walk faster when allowed some visual access to the environment. Compared to Expeirment 1, response times are much longer and more variable in Experiment 2 (see Table 2) because they include the filled delay.
Discussion
The results of Experiment 2 again show a distractor effect, but only in the ratio estimation task. Even when distances are encoded with vision and there is a filled delay between encoding and response, the presence of additional path features at encoding results in a reduction in remembered path lengths as measured by ratio estimation. The conditions under which the distractor effect obtains are further extended here to conditions previously associated with the contrasting feature accumulation effect.
Walking response mode data again suggest that the mechanisms by which the distractor effect occurs play a smaller (or no) role in triangle completion. However, we hesitate to read too much into this null result. Although no significant effect of features was observed in the walking response data, figures 4 and 6 indicate an overall trend in favor of the distractor effect. Given the high degree of inter-individual variability observed in path integration tasks (e.g., Wolbers, Wiener, Mallot & Buchel, 2007), it may be that we did not have enough power to show a significant effect in noisy data.
General Discussion
As indicated by a ratio estimation task, walked paths associated with more features were remembered as being shorter than paths associated with fewer features. This result contrasts with previous results showing that path features cause an increase in remembered path length (the feature accumulation effect), and extends results from a pair of studies (Glasauer et al., 2007, 2009) in which a concurrent task caused a shortening of perceived path lengths (the distractor effect). Observation of the distractor effect in the current paradigm suggests that the effect is more general than previously indicated. The distractor effect was observed here using punctate environmental (landmark-type) distractors, vision at encoding, a filled delay between encoding and response, and a ratio estimation response mode. We consider what the extension of the distractor effect to this particular set of conditions can tell us about the underlying mechanisms.
The current demonstration of the distractor effect using punctate environmental features speaks to the relative strength of the effect. Unlike the continuous concurrent tasks used by Glasauer et al., the features used in the current study did not demand attention throughout the entire path. However, distraction from the accumulation of temporal or distance cues for even relatively small portions of a path affected remembered path length. In addition, unlike continuous concurrent tasks, punctate path features divide paths into discrete segments, which we might expect to produce an increase in remembered length (Allen, 1981; Allen & Kirasic, 1985; Jansen-Osmann & Berendt, 2005; Montello, 1997). However, any route segmentation effects that might have been at play in the current study were overpowered by the distractor effect.
Experiment 2 shows that the distractor effect is not limited to the relatively uncommon task of encoding traversed distances without vision. Glasauer and colleagues suggested that the distractor effect is caused by distraction from the accumulation of temporal cues, and the consequent association of routes with shorter durations. When more perceptual distance cues are available during the encoding of walked distances (e.g., from optic flow) the relative influence of temporal information on subsequent distance estimates ought to be reduced (Montello, 2009, but see Mossio, Vida & Berthoz, 2008). Also, participants in this experiment could see the features coming and prepare for the new nonsense syllables, which should result in the features being easier to process and less distracting. Therefore, the results of Experiment 2 are somewhat surprising, and further indicate the potential strength of the distractor effect.
Why was a feature accumulation effect seen in other similar studies whereas the distractor effect was observed in this study? Hutcheson and Wedell (2009) also used a concurrent task (mental arithmetic) during path encoding, and a filled delay between encoding and responding. In contrast to the current study, Hutcheson and Wedell found that both these manipulations caused an increase in the feature accumulation effect. This contrast may stem from one or more methodological differences between the studies. For example, Hutcheson and Wedell presented the paths in desktop virtual reality environments. Recall that Hutcheson and Wedell suggested that the feature accumulation effect results from relative reliance on categorical as opposed to metric representations. The additional idiothetic motion information sources associated with walking (e.g., proprioception, kinesthesis, efference copy) may have resulted in relatively robust metric representations in the current study (e.g., Chance, Gaunet, Beall, & Loomis, 1998; Klatzky, Loomis, Beall, Chance, & Golledge, 1998). The paths used in the current study were shorter on average (12.2 – 10.4 m vs. 12.2 – 24.4 m) which also may have favored metric spatial coding. Finally, the association of path features in our study with changes in the concurrent task may have made the features more distracting. Future work should continue to test which of these differences may be critical.
The current results are also striking in light of a similar study by Sadalla and Staplin (1980A) showing a feature accumulation effect. In both studies, participants’ vision of the upcoming path was limited to a few feet, they followed paths marked on the floor that were approximately 6.10 m long, and crossed by visible, linear features, and distance estimates were made using ratio estimation. For Sadalla and Staplin, features increased remembered path length; for us, features shortened remembered path length. One potentially important difference that sets the current study apart from both Sadalla and Staplin’s and Hutcheson and Wedell’s (2009) is that the current experiments used both ratio estimation and triangle completion (action-based) response modes. Participants never knew, trial by trial, whether they would need to use the distance information they were encoding along the paths to support explicit symbolic responding (ratio estimation), or to navigate back to the beginning of the L path. In none of the studies showing the feature accumulation effect did participants encode path lengths with the intention of using that information to support navigation oriented motor responses (e.g., walking).
Thus, in the current study, and in those of Glasauer et al. (2007, 2009), the anticipation by participants of (potential) upcoming motor responses may have resulted in the engagement of different or additional processes during path encoding, compared to studies showing the feature accumulation effect. For example, there is support for the idea that the anticipation of upcoming movement trajectories results in forward modeling (Wolpert & Ghahramani, 2000; Philbeck, Klatzky, Behrmann, Loomis, & Goodridge, 2001). Modeling movements before they occur improves spatial updating because, as the movement is executed, incoming self-motion signals can be compared against model predictions, providing an additional source of feedback. During path encoding, processes such as forward modeling, that are distinct to situations in which specific upcoming motor responses are expected, may result in the formation of spatial representations that are less susceptible to the type of heuristic, non-metric information thought to cause feature accumulation effects. As a result, even when these representations were used to support ratio estimation responses in the current study the distraction effect dominated. This suggests that one’s intention during encoding to use distance information for action may be an important factor in determining whether the feature accumulation or the distractor effect emerges in a given situation.
If we accept Glasauer et al.’s framing of the distractor effect in terms of path integration processes, and the related suggestion that the distractor effect influences kinesthetic, action-based spatial knowledge, the current results are surprising for another reason. It has been suggested that path integration and perceptual updating processes are relatively automatic (Gallistel, 1990; Farrell & Robertson, 2000; Loomis et al., 1999; Waller, Montello, Richardson & Hegarty, 2002). Survival for humans and other animals would be difficult if the ability to remain oriented with regard to immediate surroundings placed large demands on attentional resources. In the current study, distraction of attention away from spatial updating processes had a measureable effect on remembered distance (see also, Takei, Grasso, Amorim & Berthoz, 1997; Yardley et al., 2002). Conditions in the current study (partial vision, concurrent task) may have caused what is normally an automatic process to require some attention. Future studies are needed to determine whether the distractor effect disappears under less challenging spatial updating conditions.
Finally, our discussion of the current results has been framed in terms of path features causing distraction from the accumulation of distance or temporal cues. However, we may consider explanations that do not invoke the distractor effect. Montello (2009) suggests that heuristics such as counting one’s steps play a large role in how people prospectively encode walked distances, especially when perceptual information is limited. Step counting was minimized in the current study by the syllable recitation task. In addition, participants were instructed not to count syllables and not to synchronize syllables with steps, in order to discourage any explicit counting strategies. However, the concurrent task may have had an implicit influence on perceived distance. Paths without features were associated with longer periods of homogeneous syllable recitation, which may have influenced distance representations. In essence, an implicit and coarse form of syllable counting may have occurred wherein the counter was re-set (to zero) at syllable changes. Future work will investigate this possibility.
In sum, the current results broaden the relevance of the distractor effect for spatial cognition. The bulk of the evidence that distraction shortens perceived extent comes from research into temporal processing (e.g., Brown, 1997). Therefore, the current demonstration of the distractor effect in the spatial realm supports suggestions that temporal and spatial processing share common cognitive and neural architecture.
Acknowledgments
Thanks to Owen Footer and Jacob Marks for invaluable help collecting and analyzing these data. This research was supported by NIH grants R01 AG031150, principle investigator Jeffrey Zacks, and R01 EY021771, principle investigator John Philbeck.
Footnotes
Because the effect of distraction on duration perception is well documented, distorted temporal representations are likely to play a role in how distraction affects distance perception. However, neither current data, nor those of Glasauer and colleagues specifically indicate that time perception is involved in the observed effects of distraction on distance knowledge.
For ease of interpretation and because the leg length manipulation was not of primary interest, in the statistical analyses response data were collapsed across the two shorter leg lengths (4.27 m and 4.88 m) to create two categorical levels of this variable, short and long.
Correct responses based on relative length of comparator path (12.19 m) and corresponding haptic response distance (68.58 cm) are 29.6, 23.7 and 20.74 cm for the 6.10, 4.88 and 4.27 m paths, respectively. Thus, in absolute terms, paths lengths were grossly over-estimated in the haptic response data. However, ratio estimation ability per se was not a focus in the current study. For example, participants only underwent two trials in which the relationship between walked and haptic response distances was demonstrated at the outset of the experimental session. Therefore, we do not discuss this result.
References
- Allen GL. A developmental perspective on the effects of “subdividing” macrospatial experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory. 1981;7:120–132. [Google Scholar]
- Allen GL, Kirasic KC. Effects of the cognitive organization of route knowledge on judgments of macrospatial distance. Memory & Cognition. 1985;13:218–227. doi: 10.3758/bf03197684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berthoz A, Israel I, Georges-Francois P, Grasso R, Tsuzuku T. Spatial memory of body linear displacement: What is being stored? Science. 1995;269(5220):95–98. doi: 10.1126/science.7604286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briggs R. Urban cognitive distance. In: Downs R, Stea D, editors. Image and environment: Cognitive mapping and spatial behavior. New Brunswick, NJ: Aldine; 1973. pp. 361–388. [Google Scholar]
- Brooks LR. Spatial and verbal components of the act of recall. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Canadian Review of Psychology. 1968;22(5):349–368. [Google Scholar]
- Brown SW. Attentional resources in timing: Interference effects in concurrent temporal and nontemporal working memory tasks. Perception & Psychophysics. 1997;59(7):1118–1140. doi: 10.3758/bf03205526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown SW, West AN. Multiple timing and the allocation of attention. Acta Psychologica. 1990;75(2):103–121. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(90)90081-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chance SS, Gaunet F, Beall AC, Loomis JM. Locomotion mode affects the updating of objects encountered during travel: The contribution of vestibular and proprioceptive inputs to path integration. Presence. 1998;7(2):168–178. [Google Scholar]
- Farrell MJ, Robertson IH. The automatic updating of egocentric spatial relationships and its impairment due to right posterior cortical lesions. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38:585–595. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00123-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujita N, Klatzky RL, Loomis JM, Golledge RG. The encoding-error model of pathway completion without vision. Geographical Analysis. 1993;25(4):295–314. [Google Scholar]
- Gallistel CR. The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Glasauer S, Schneider E, Grasso R, Ivanenko YP. Space-time relativity in self-motion reproduction. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;97(1):451–461. doi: 10.1152/jn.01243.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glasauer S, Stein A, Gunther AL, Flanagin VL, Jahn K, Brandt T. The effect of dual tasks in locomotor path integration. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2009;1164(1):201–205. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.03862.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gobet F, Lane PCR, Croker S, Cheng PCH, Jones G, Olivier I, Pine JM. Chunking memories in human learning. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2001;5:236–243. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01662-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herman JF, Norton LM, Klein CA. Children’s distance estimates in a large-scale environment: A search for the route angularity effect. Environment & Behavior. 1986;18(4):533–558. [Google Scholar]
- Hicks RE, Miller GW, Gaes G, Bierman K. Concurrent processing demands and the experience of time-in-passing. The American Journal of Psychology. 1977;90(3):431–446. [Google Scholar]
- Holyoak KJ, Mah WA. Cognitive reference points in judgments of symbolic magnitudes. Cognitive Psychology. 1982;14:328–352. [Google Scholar]
- Hutcheson AT, Wedell DH. Moderating the route angularity effect in a virtual environment: Support for a dual memory representation. Memory & Cognition. 2009;37(4):514–521. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.4.514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huttenlocher J, Hedges LV, Duncan S. Categories and particulars: Prototype effects in estimating spatial location. Psychological Review. 1991;98(3):352–376. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.98.3.352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jansen-Osmann P, Berendt B. Investigating distance knowledge using virtual environments. Environment & Behavior. 2002;34(2):178–193. [Google Scholar]
- Jansen-Osmann P, Berendt B. What makes a route appear longer? An experimental perspective on features, route segmentation and distance knowledge. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2005;58A(8):1390–1414. doi: 10.1080/02724980443000638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jansen-Osmann P, Wiedenbauer G. Distance cognition in virtual environmental space: Further investigations to clarify the route-angularity effect. Psychological Research (Psychologische Forschung) 2006;70(1):43–51. doi: 10.1007/s00426-004-0183-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klatzky RL, Beall AC, Loomis JM, Golledge RG, Philbeck JW. Human navigation ability: Tests of the encoding-error model of path integration. Spatial Cognition and Computation. 1999;1(1):31–65. [Google Scholar]
- Klatzky RL, Loomis JM, Beall AC, Chance SS, Golledge RG. Spatial updating of self-position and orientation during real, imagined, and virtual locomotion. Physiological Science. 1998;9(4):293–298. [Google Scholar]
- Loomis JM, Klatzky RL, Golledge RG, Philbeck JW. Human navigation by path integration. In: Golledge RG, editor. Wayfinding behavior: Cognitive mapping and other spatial processes. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins UP; 1999. pp. 125–151. [Google Scholar]
- Loomis JM, Klatzky RL, Golledge RG, Cicinelli JG, Pellegrino JW, Fry P. Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: Assessment of path integration ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1993;122(1):73–91. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.122.1.73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNamara TP, Hardy JK, Hirtle SC. Subjective hierarchies in spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1989;15(2):211–227. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.15.2.211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montello DR. The perception and cognition of environmental distance: Direct sources of information. In: Hirtle SC, Frank AU, editors. Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 1997. pp. 297–311. Lecture Notes in Computer Science 1329. [Google Scholar]
- Montello DR. A conceptual model of the cognitive processing of environmental distance information. In: Hornsby KS, Claramunt C, Denis M, Ligozat G, editors. Spatial information theory; Proceedings of COSIT ‘09; Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag; 2009. pp. 1–17. Lecture Notes in Computer Science 5756. [Google Scholar]
- Mossio M, Vidal M, Berthoz A. Traveled distances: New insights into the role of optic flow. Vision Research. 2008;48(2):289–303. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.11.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Philbeck JW, Klatzky RK, Behrmann M, Loomis JM, Goodridge J. Active control facilitates non-visual navigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2001;27:141–153. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadalla EK, Magel SG. The perception of traversed distance. Environment & Behavior. 1980;12(1):65–79. [Google Scholar]
- Sadalla EK, Staplin LJ. The perception of traversed distance: Intersections. Environment & Behavior. 1980A;12(2):167–182. [Google Scholar]
- Sadalla EK, Staplin LJ. An information storage model for distance cognition. Environment & Behavior. 1980B;12(2):183–193. [Google Scholar]
- Sadalla EK, Staplin LJ, Burroughs WJ. Retrieval processes in distance cognition. Memory & Cognition. 1979;7(4):291–296. [Google Scholar]
- Sargent J, Dopkins S, Philbeck J, Chichka D. Chunking in spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2010;36(3):576–589. doi: 10.1037/a0017528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takei Y, Grasso R, Amorim MA, Berthoz A. Circular trajectory formation during blind locomotion: A test for path integration and motor memory. Experimental Brain Research. 1997;115(2):361–368. doi: 10.1007/pl00005705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waller D, Montello DR, Richardson AE, Hegarty M. Orientation specificity and spatial updating of memories for layouts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2002;28(6):1051–1063. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.6.1051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh V. A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7(11):483–488. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang RF, Brockmole JR. Simultaneous spatial updating in nested environments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2003;10:981–986. doi: 10.3758/bf03196562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolbers T, Hegarty M. What determines our navigational abilities? Trends in Cognitive Neurosciences. 2010;14(3):138–146. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolbers T, Wiener JM, Mallot HA, Buchel C. Differential recruitment of the hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, and the human motion complex during path integration in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(35):9408–9416. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2146-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z. Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3:1212–1217. doi: 10.1038/81497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yardley L, Papo D, Bronstein A, Gresty M, Gardner M, Lavie N, Luxon L. Attentional demands of continuously monitoring orientation using vestibular information. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40(4):373–383. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00113-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zacks JM, Speer NK, Swallow KM, Braver TS, Reynolds JR. Event perception: A mind-brain perspective. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133(2):273–293. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zakay D. Subjective time and attentional resource allocation: An integrated model of time estimation. In: Levin I, Zakay D, editors. Time and Human Cognition. Amsterdam: North-Holland; 1989. pp. 365–397. [Google Scholar]