Abstract
We examined Genotype × Environment (G × E) interactions between children’s genotypes (the serotonin transporter linked promoter region [5-HTTLPR] gene) and maternal responsive care observed at 15, 25, 38, and 52 months on three aspects of children’s competence at 67 months: academic skills and school engagement, social functioning with peers, and moral internalization that encompassed prosocial moral cognition and the moral self. Academic and social competence outcomes were reported by both parents, and moral internalization was observed in children’s narratives elicited by hypothetical stories and in a puppet interview. Analyses revealed robust G × E interactions, such that children’s genotype moderated the effects of maternal responsive care on all aspects of children’s competence. Among children with a short 5-HTTLPR allele (ss/sl), those whose mothers were more responsive were significantly more competent than those whose mothers were less responsive. Responsiveness had no effect for children with two long alleles (ll). For academic and social competence, the G × E interactions resembled the diathesis–stress model: ss/sl children of unresponsive mothers had particularly unfavorable outcomes, but ss/sl children of responsive mothers had no worse outcomes than ll children. For moral internalization, the G × E interaction reflected the differential susceptibility model: whereas ss/sl children of unresponsive mothers again had particularly unfavorable outcomes, ss/sl children of responsive mothers had significantly better outcomes than ll children.
An integration of biological and environmental constructs “from neurons to neighborhoods” (Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000) has been broadly accepted as the most fruitful approach to development (Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Born-stein, 2000). Research on Genotype × Environment (G × E) in teractions has elucidated origins of multiple aspects of adaptive and maladaptive development in human and animal species (Caspi et al., 2003; Champoux et al., 2002; Suomi, 2004, 2006). A G × E interaction occurs when environmental experi ence moderates the effect of a person’s genotype on physical or mental health outcomes, or when a genotype moderates an environmental effect (Moffit, Caspi, & Rutter, 2005; Rutter, Moffitt, & Caspi, 2006).
Many studies of G × E interactions have focused on a poly morphism in the serotonin transporter linked promoter region (5-HTTLPR). The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism has two common alleles: short (s) and long (l). The short allele has been linked to reduced 5-HTT transcription, lower 5-HTT protein levels, and diminished serotonin reuptake compared to individuals with the long allele. Dysfunctions in the serotonergic system have been broadly implicated in regulation of mood, attention, executive skills, and various forms of psychopathology, including aggression, risk-taking, alcohol use, as well as depression or anxiety (Auerbach, Faroy, Ebstein, Kahana, & Levine, 2001; Barr et al., 2004; Champoux et al., 2002; Hariri et al., 2005; Herrmann et al., 2007; Lesch et al., 1996; Lucki, 1998; Posner, Rothbart, & Sheese, 2007; Propper & Moore, 2006; Sourbrie, 1986; Suomi, 2004; van Goozen, Fairchild, Snoek, & Harold, 2007).
Human and animal studies have increasingly documented substantial G × E interactions between the genetic risk associ ated with 5-HTTLPR polymorphism (having a short allele) and environmental or experiential factors. Those effects have been typically interpreted as consistent with diathesis–stress or dual-risk model: individuals who carry a short allele develop a host of problems if they also experience adverse or suboptimal environments and stressful influences but have no worse outcomes than ll homozygotes if they experience favorable environments (Barry, Kochanska, & Philibert, 2008; Caspi et al., 2003; Champoux et al., 2002; Fox et al., 2005; Kaufman et al., 2006; Kochanska, Philibert, & Barry, 2009; Suomi, 2004, 2006). Furthermore, ll homozygotes are typically less affected by environmental variation (Belsky, Baker-mans-Kranenburg, & van Ijzendoorn, 2007; Belsky & Pluess, 2009a, 2009b). That research has been predominantly concerned with negative outcomes, for example, depression, anxiety, or substance use, and it has focused especially on the detrimental combination of the biological vulnerability and stressful or adverse environment, using the diathesis–stress model, prevalent in psychopathology studies. A beneficial environment, or one devoid of adversity, has been seen as a protective factor that may offset the risk conferred by biology.
Recently, however, particularly in developmental psychology, researchers have considerably broadened their interests in the interplay of genotypes and environment, and they have redirected their attention to a wider range of outcomes and environments than those typically studied. A broader approach that addresses both maladaptive and adaptive outcomes, including psychopathology, personality, social cognition and behavior, social competence, or emotion regulation, is clearly ascending in research on 5-HTTLPR polymorphism (Canli & Lesch, 2007; Lesch, 2007). That shift in the approach has coincided with and fueled newly emerging questions about possible models of G × E effects that may not conform to the diathesis–stress models.
Belsky and colleagues (Belsky, 1997; Belsky & Pluess, 2009a, 2009b; Belsky et al., 2007; Belsky, Hsieh, & Crnic, 1998), as well as Boyce and Ellis (2005) have proposed, and persuasively argued, that the diathesis–stress approach does not adequately describe another important form of G × E interaction: differential susceptibility. In this latter model, individuals are seen as differing in plasticity or malleability rather than in vulnerability or risk proneness. Certain genetic polymorphisms, including 5-HTTLPR, are seen not as “vulnerability factors,” but rather as “plasticity factors.” Children with such genotypes (or with other biological traits often seen as conferring “high risk”) are more malleable or susceptible than others to both negative and positive environmental influences. When subjected to adverse, stressful, and suboptimal environment, such children do have much worse outcomes than children who do not have the biological vulnerability. However, when provided with supportive, optimal, and enriching experiences, such children may actually do better than children who do not have “high-risk” biological profiles. Consequently, differential susceptibility model may be seen as one that subsumes two effects: a G × E interaction that occurs in the range of the poor environment (resembling the traditional diathesis–stress effect, where children with certain genotypes have worse outcomes than their peers without such genotypes) and an interaction that occurs in the range of beneficial environment (where children with those same genotypes actually have better outcomes than their peers).
In the G × E literature genotype can be seen as the main causal factor and environment as a moderator of the effect of the genotype, or environment can be seen as the main causal factor and genotype as a moderator (Moffitt et al., 2005). The new broader approach to G × E interactions in de velopmental psychology has reinvigorated the ecological tradition by refocusing research interests on the environment. Belsky and Pluess (2009a, 2009b) strongly urged scholars to study both adverse and suboptimal developmental circumstances, and positive, beneficial, supportive circumstances. Consequently, developmental scholars tend to adopt the perspective where the child’s experiences and the childrearing environment are considered the main causal factors influencing child outcomes, and the child’s genotype is typically conceptualized as a moderator of those effects. We have adopted such a view in the current article. We study children’s experiences in their childrearing early environment as differentially predicting developmental outcomes for children with two different 5-HTTLPR genotypes, carriers of a short allele (ss/sl), and homozygotic on the long allele (ll).
Despite the growing and consistent body of evidence on G × E interactions in development, very few studies have ex amined G × E interactions in social–emotional development using longitudinal designs, a multitrait multimethod approach to the assessment of outcomes, and robust behavioral observations to measure environmental influence. We present a multi-trait multimethod longitudinal study whose main goal was to examine, using a combination of research methodologies, interactions between the 5-HTTLPR genotype and the childrearing environment in the development of children’s competencies assessed at kindergarten age (5.5 years). That age represents a particularly important transition to expanded ecological contexts beyond the family. Although many children begin to participate in various out-of-family environments at earlier ages, the kindergarten age introduces a relatively uniform set of new daily expectations and demands. Those new developmental tasks include school and academic performance, and functioning in a peer group (Rimm-Kaufman & Pianta, 2000).
Competence is a broad construct that describes how effectively a child meets his or her salient developmental tasks. Typically, in childhood, those tasks encompass academic achievement and school functioning, social functioning in the peer group, and effective internalization of rules and values (Masten et al., 1995). Consequently, we aimed to assess all those domains: academic skills and school engagement, social functioning with peers (ability to get along with peers, social acceptance, prosociality, and absence of aggression, social isolation, and victimization), and aspects of moral internalization (prosocial, moral cognition and a positive view of self in terms of meeting moral conduct standards). The measures of academic, school, and social competence were a combination of well-established mothers’ and fathers’ reports, whereas the measures of internalization included children’s narratives produced in standard laboratory paradigms and children’s self-reports elicited in a puppet interview.
The early childrearing environment was conceptualized as a history of maternal responsive care, assessed repeatedly across the first 4 years of life (at 15, 25, 38, and 52 months). To assure sufficient variability in behaviors and emotions of the mother and the child, at each time, the dyads were observed in lengthy, naturalistic yet scripted, contexts in their homes and in the laboratory (cumulatively close to 4 hr per dyad). Multiple types of contexts were included, such as free time, chores, multiple demands, play, or meals. Consequently, the sampling of those diverse situations, varying in their psychological potentials, assured sufficient variability in behavior and emotion of the mother and the child.
Consistent with the existing animal and human research, we expected that variations in maternal responsiveness would predict future competence for children who carry the short allele (ss/sl genotypes), with children of more responsive mothers being more competent than children of less responsive mothers. The effects of maternal responsiveness were expected to be significantly weaker or absent for children who carry two long alleles (ll genotype).
An additional important goal was to examine the specific forms of the G × E interactions: diathesis–stress or differential susceptibility (Belsky & Pluess, 2009a, 2009b; Boyce & Ellis, 2005). Given how recently this issue has been introduced to the field, this direction of analyses was exploratory.
To date, most conclusions about the form of the G × E interaction have not been based on a formal testing, but rather, on a subjective impression of the shape of the effect. In contrast, we have implemented a new formal approach to the testing of interactions that involves the analysis of regions of significance (Aiken & West, 1991; Hayes & Matthes, 2009; Preacher, Curran, & Bauer, 2006). Discussing ways to identify regions of significance, Hayes and Matthes (2009) stated: “Although this method has been around for decades, it is rarely used, to our knowledge, probably due to a lack of researchers’ familiarity with the method and its lack of implementation in popular data analysis programs . …” (p. 925). They further provided a useful and simple computational method to conduct such analyses.1
Generally, this strategy entails graphing the interaction effects beyond the traditional range from ±1 SD below and above the mean of the independent variable (in this case, the childrearing environment, measured as maternal responsiveness) to the broader range of ±2 SD below and above the mean. This considerably increases the chances of pinpointing interaction effects that occur beyond the traditional ±1 SD range (even though some interactions may occur at even lower or higher values that ±2 SD). The regression lines based on the predicted values extrapolate from the empirical observed values obtained in the actual sample. Such graphs allow for marking the upper and lower bounds of the regions of significance, that is, the specific values of the independent variable (maternal responsiveness) below which and above which the regression lines for the two studied groups (children with two different genotypes, ss/sl and ll) differ significantly in terms of a specific outcome (a given aspect of competence or behavioral problems).
Expanding graphs beyond the traditional ±1 SD increases the chances of identifying G × E interaction that occurs under either very poor or very beneficial environmental conditions (i.e., particularly low or particularly high maternal responsiveness). Imaginably, for some outcomes, children with ss/sl genotypes will show impairments already when maternal care is just below average, but for some other outcomes, they will show impairments only when maternal care is very poor. Likewise, for some outcomes for those children, maternal care that is just above average may be sufficient to serve as a buffer, but maternal care that is particularly responsive may lead to especially good outcomes (as in differential susceptibility model).
In summary, this approach allows us to make significant strides in thinking about G × E interactions because it provides answers to the following types of questions. For example, if a G × E interaction conforms to the traditional diathesis–stress model, how “good” does the environment (i.e., how responsive the mother) needs to be to offset the potential risk conferred by the child’s ss/sl genotype? In what range of maternal responsiveness do children with ss/sl genotypes show significantly lower competence than their peers without the short allele? Likewise, if a G × E interaction effect conforms to the dif ferential susceptibility model, how responsive does the mother need to be to help her child with ss/sl genotype become more competent than his or her less vulnerable peers?
In addition, this method illustrates what inferences could likely be drawn about the shape of G × E interactions if the current sample size and variation in the independent variable (maternal responsiveness) were increased to include values beyond the currently observed range, for example, by including extremely unresponsive and/or extremely responsive mothers. This is particularly important from the conceptual point of view. Often, a G × E interaction “appears” to conform to di athesis–stress model just because the given sample includes enough children from adverse environments, but not enough children from especially beneficial circumstances. If more children from particularly advantageous environments were included, the data might well show the differential susceptibility effect (Belsky & Pluess, 2009b). We believe that in light of the tremendous interest in, and the current debate about the form of G × E interactions, implementing this approach is most timely.
Method
Participants and design
Two-parent families of infants, volunteers for a longitudinal study, ranged in education from high school (24% of mothers, 30% of fathers) to postcollege (21%, 20%) and ranged in annual income from under $20,000 (8%) to over $70,000 (34%). Ninety-one percent of mothers and 83% of fathers were White, 3% and 8% Hispanic, 1% and 3% African American, 1% and 3% Asian, 1% and 0% Pacific Islander, and 3% and 3% other non-White. In 20% of families, at least one parent was non-White.
Multiple lengthy home and laboratory sessions, all conducted by female experimenters, were videotaped for later coding. Here, we report data collected when children were 15 months (N = 101, 51 girls), 25 months (N 100, 50 girls), 38 months (N = 100, 50 girls), 52 months (N = 99, 49 girls), and 67 months (N = 92, 45 girls). Most analyses in this report are for the subset of children whose parents consented to the genetic testing at 52 months. The independent variable, maternal responsiveness, was observed in lengthy interactions at 15, 25, 38, and 52 months. Children’s outcomes were assessed at 67 months, using parents’ reports and behavioral observations of children’s behavior in laboratory paradigms. The moderator variable, the child’s genotype, was assessed at 52 months.
Independent teams coded different behavioral measures, using at least 15% to 20% of cases for reliability. Coders realigned periodically to prevent drift. The measures were aggregated at multiple levels to produce robust constructs (Rushton, Brainerd, & Pressley, 1983).
Mothers’ responsiveness at 15, 25, 38, and 52 months
Observed contexts
Mothers and children were observed at each assessment in multiple naturalistic, yet carefully scripted and standardized diverse contexts: daily chores, meal preparation and cleanup, snack, play, leisure, routine care, mother “busy,” etc. The cumulative observed times ranged from 45 min at younger ages to 75 min at older ages (approximate total of 230 min for each mother–child dyad across all assessments).
Coding, reliability, and data aggregation
The coding of responsiveness was adapted from Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall (1978). For each context (e.g., play, snack), the mother was given a score that integrated Ainsworth’s original scales of sensitivity-insensitivity, acceptance–rejection, and cooperation–interference, from 1 (highly unresponsive) to 7 (highly responsive). Nine different coders were trained to code data up to 52 months; six coded only one time of assessment; not a single coder remained involved across all assessments or coded home and lab visits for the same mother. Intercoder reliability for those judgments (αs) ranged from 0.90 to 0.98, and κ values ranged from 0.60 to 0.82.
The scores for all contexts at the same assessment cohered: Cronbach αs ranged from 0.68 to 0.84. At each assessment, the scores were then aggregated across all contexts. Those aggregated scores cohered across longitudinal assessments (rs = .35–.56, ps, < .001) and were standardized and aggregated into an overall maternal responsiveness score from 15 to 52 months (Cronbach α 0.79, M = −0.02, SD = 0.82, range = −2.51–1.46).
Children’s outcomes: Measures of competence at 67 months
The measures of children’s competence at 67 months included school competence, social competence (both derived from mothers’ and fathers’ ratings in MacArthur Health Behavior Questionnaire [HBQ]; Essex et al., 2002) and moral internalization (prosocial, moral cognition, and moral self, assessed in observational paradigms in the laboratory). Depending on the HBQ scale, items were rated from 1 to 3, 1 to 4, or 1 to 7 to capture how well a certain item applied to or described the child. Below, the Cronbach α for mothers is reported first and for fathers second for each scale; interparent correlation is reported last. Finally, the α value for the combined final score (mother and father) is reported.
School competence
We selected three HBQ scales to capture children’s school competence: math skills, four items, and reading skills, four items (for the combined eight items, αs = 0.92 and 0.90; inter-parent correlation = .60, p < .001), and school engagement, eight items (0.89, 0.87; 0.56, p < .001). The scales’ scores were standardized and aggregated for each parent into an overall measure of child school competence; mothers’ and fathers’ scores correlated, r (87) = .61, p < .001. Consequently, mothers’ and fathers’ ratings were averaged into a new composite of school competence score (M = −0.01, SD 0.68, α = 0.75). There was no gender difference, t (90) < 1.
Social competence
Two HBQ scales were selected to assess children’s successful peer functioning: peer acceptance, 8 items (0.86, 0.87; 0.30, p < .005), and prosociality, 20 items (0.88, 0.91; 0.30, p < .005). Their scores were standardized and aggregated for each parent into an overall measure of child social competence; mothers’ and fathers’ scores correlated, r (88) = .36, p < .001, and were averaged into one score (M = 0.01, SD = 0.67). There was no gender difference, t (90) = 1.55.
Four HBQ scales captured children’s social problems: peer victimization, three items (0.54, 0.61; 0.19, p < .10), overt aggression, four items (0.64, 0.55; 0.24, p < .025), peer isolation, six items (0.78, 0.76; 0.47, p < .001), and relational aggression, six items (0.81, 0.76; 0.21, p < .05). Mothers’ and fathers’ scores correlated, r (88) .33, p < .0025, and were averaged into one score (M = 0.00, SD = 0.55). There was no gender effect, t (90) < 1.
The scores of successful functioning and social problems correlated, r (92) = −.55, p < .001. Consequently, we created a new composite of social competence, by averaging across mothers’ and fathers’ ratings (reversing the ratings of social problems; M = 0.00, SD = 0.52, α 0.76). There was no gender effect, t (90) < 1.
Moral internalization
Prosocial, moral cognition
Paradigm
During the laboratory sessions, the experimenter administered a battery of seven stories, each accompanied by pictorial vignettes. Four stories involved hypothetical moral dilemmas. Each presented a conflict between the interests of the protagonist and those of others (e.g., deciding whether to run to a birthday party or to help another child find lost dog; use remaining paint to finish one’s own picture or let another child use it). Originally based on Eisenberg-Berg and Hand (1979), the stories have been rewritten during subsequent adaptations, but they all retained the core feature: a salient and inevitable conflict (either the protagonist or another child can benefit, but not both). All protagonists matched the child’s gender. The experimenter asked what the child would do if he or she were the protagonist and why, then challenged the child’s response by pointing out a prosocial concern in the case of a selfish choice, or a self-concern in the case of a prosocial choice, and asked the child to make the final decision.
Three remaining stories did not involve a conflict; instead, each described a (different) protagonist committing a transgression (e.g., cheating in a game, taking a toy from another child). Those were adapted from Thompson and Hoffman (1980) and our earlier work (Kochanska, Aksan, & Nichols, 2003). The child was then asked how he or she would feel if he or she were the protagonist, and why. The child then was asked to indicate, verbally and using pictorial depictions, the intensity of the feelings.
Coding
In the first set of four stories, we assessed the child’s prosocial solutions to each dilemma, coded for each story as 0 (absent), 1 (chosen as the first choice but changed when challenged), 2 (second or changed choice that remained final), or 3 (first choice, unchanged when challenged, and final). Reliability (κ) was 0.83. In the second set of three stories, we assessed the wrongdoer’s presence of bad feelings after the transgression and their intensity; κ values ranged from 0.95 to 1.00. For each of the seven stories, we also coded the presence of empathic rationales given by the child (e.g., “would feel bad because she was hurt,” “would share so the other would not be sad”; “it would make her happy”), as 0 (absent), 1 (present once), or 2 (present more than once). Kappa values ranged from 0.74 to 0.89.
Data aggregation
The scores for prosocial decisions were summed across the four pertinent stories (M = 7.28, SD = 3.43). The scores for bad feeling after transgressions were summed across the three pertinent stories (M = 17.45, SD = 7.03), and the scores for empathic rationales were = summed across all seven stories (M = 1.00, SD = 1.30). Those scores were intercorrelated, rs ranging from .20 (p = .06) to .28 (p < .01, average r = .23), so they were standardized and aggregated into an overall moral cognition score (M = 0.00, SD = 0.70). There was no gender difference, t (88), <1.
Moral self
Paradigm
The measure of the child’s moral self was derived from a puppet interview that had been originally adapted from Eder’s (1990) assessment of young children’s selves. The interview was administered during the laboratory sessions. We had adapted it to assess the dimensions of “moral self” and used it successfully in another longitudinal study (Kochanska, 2002a). The experimenter used two puppets to anchor the opposite ends of each of 31 items. The items all pertained to dimensions of early conscience (e.g., internalization of rules, guilt, empathy, apology, etc.). The experimenter presented each item as a very brief scenario, with one puppet representing one option and the other puppet representing the opposite option. The experimenter used equally “self-righteous” voices to speak for the puppets and varied the high and low end across the puppets. For example, one puppet would say, “When I break something, I try to hide it so no one finds out,” and the other one would say “When I break something, I tell someone about it right away.” The experimenter then asked the child, “What about you? Do you try to hide something that you broke or do you tell someone about it right away? Typically, children quickly “caught on” to the rhythm of the interview, and began to point to one of the puppets without the need for further prompting,
Coding
The child’s response to each item was coded as 0 if the child chose the puppet that anchored the low end, as 2 if he or she chose the puppet that anchored the high end, and as 1 if he or she hesitated or endorsed both (e.g., “I am sometimes like him and sometimes like him”). All 31 items were then added into a composite of the child’s moral self (Cronbach α = 0.65, M = 48.09, SD = 7.59). There was no gender effect, t (88) = 1.48.
Composite of moral internalization
The two scores, prosocial, moral cognition, and moral self were correlated, r (90) = .31, p < .005. Consequently, they were aggregated (following the standardization of the latter) into a composite of moral internalization (M = 0.00, SD = 0.69). There was no gender effect, t (88) = 1.14, ns.
Genotype measures: 5-HTTLPR status at 52 months
Mothers of 89 children consented to this assessment. There were no significant differences, on any variable examined here, between the families that did and did not consent. Child DNA was obtained using buccal swabs and genotype at the 5-HTTLPR was determined for each sample (Barry et al., 2008; Bradley, Dodelzon, Sandhu, & Philibert, 2005; Philibert et al., 2007); 88 samples were successfully genotyped. There were 13 ss homozygotes (3 girls, 10 boys), 47 sl heterozygotes (23 girls, 24 boys), and 28 ll homozygotes (18 girls, 10 boys). Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium testing was nonsignificant (p < .66). The difference in gender distribution across the genotypes was not significant (χ2 = 3.35, df = 1, p < .10). Because of the small number of ss children, and following past research (Hariri et al., 2005), children with ss and sl genotypes were combined into one group of children with either two copies or one copy of the short allele (ss/sl). The two subgroups (ss and sl) did not differ significantly on any of the outcome measures.
Results
The analyses were straightforward. For each of the three outcomes (school competence, social competence, and moral internalization) we conducted a hierarchical multiple regression. Because there were no significant gender differences for any of the outcomes, child gender was not covaried. In each regression, at Step 1, the two main effects were entered: the effect of environmental influence (the overall maternal responsiveness score from 15 to 52 months) and child genotype (5-HTTLPR status, ss/sl vs. ll). At Step 2, their interaction, G × E, was added. Table 1 presents the results of the hierarchical multiple regressions.
Table 1. Mothers’ responsiveness at 15, 25, 38, and 52 months, children’s 5-HTTLPR status, and their interaction as predictors of children’s competencies and problems at 67 months.
School Competence |
Social Competence |
Moral Internalization |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | F | Beta | F | Beta | F | Beta |
Step 1 | ||||||
Maternal responsiveness | 6.17** | 0.26 | 8.01*** | 0.30 | 6.72** | 0.28 |
5-HTTLPR status | 4.10* | 0.21 | 2.52 | 0.17 | <1 | −0.08 |
Step 2 | ||||||
Maternal responsiveness | 12 42**** | 0.47 | 12.40**** | 0.48 | 16.35**** | 0.55 |
5-HTTLPR status | 4.73* | 0.22 | 2.87† | 0.17 | <1 | −0.08 |
5-HTTLPR Status × Maternal Responsiveness | 5.91** | −0.33 | 4.17* | −0.28 | 9.23*** | −0.41 |
Note: For school competence, after Step 1, R2 = .12, F (2, 80) = 5.20***; after Step 2, R2 = .18, F (3,79) = 5.65***. For social competence, after Step 1, R2 = .12, F (2, 80) = 5.33***; after Step 2, R2 = .16, F(3,79) = 5.08***. For moral internalization, after Step 1, R2 = .09, F(2,78) = 3.67*; after Step 2, R2 = .18, F (3, 77) = 5.78***. 5-HTTLPR, serotonin transporter linked promoter region.
p < .10.
p < .05.
p < .025.
p < .01.
p < .001.
In addition, for each outcome where the G × E interaction was significant, we examined, using the aforementioned “regions of significance” approach, whether the interaction effect conformed more to the diathesis–stress model or the differential susceptibility model (Belsky & Pluess, 2009b).
School competence
Both main effects and the interaction effect were significant in the final equation. To probe the interaction effect, we estimated the simple slopes for children with the ss/sl and ll genotypes (Aiken & West, 1991), and the regions of significance where the outcomes for the ss/sl and ll children were significantly different (Aiken & West, 1991; Hayes & Matthes, 2009; Preacher et al., 2006). Figure 1 presents the results.
High and very high maternal responsiveness were represented by the scores 1 SD and 2 SD above the mean, respectively. Likewise, low and very low responsiveness were represented by 1 SD and 2 SD below the mean, respectively (recall that the final score was the mean of standardized scores at each of four assessments that ranged from −2.51 to 1.46).
The interaction effect qualified the main effects. The simple slope for ss/sl children was significant (b = 0.40, SE = 0.11, p < .0001), but for those with the ll genotypes it was not (b = −0.02, SE = 0.13, ns). The lower and upper bounds of regions of significance were 0.07 and 4.31, respectively. This indicates that two regression lines were significantly different for all possible points when the score of maternal responsiveness was lower than 0.07 or higher than 4.31. The shaded area of Figure 1 represents the region of significance within ±2 SD of the maternal responsiveness mean score.
In the abstract, the regression line for the predicted scores suggests that those ss/sl children would have showed higher school competence if they had been exposed to extremely high maternal responsiveness. That value, however, although calculable (4.31), was well beyond the observed range (recall that maximum was 1.46), and higher than 2 SD. Consequently, based on the empirical observed values of the maternal responsiveness, we can only infer that children with the ss/sl genotypes showed significantly lower school competence scores if their mothers’ responsiveness was lower than 0.07, resembling the diathesis–stress model. Note also that ss/sl children of mothers whose responsiveness was higher than 0.07 (thus, approximately above the mean) were no less competent at school than their ll peers.
Social competence
There was a robust main effect of maternal responsiveness: children of more responsive mothers were more socially competent (accepted by peers, likely to behave prosocially, having few social problems). That effect, however, was qualified by the significant G × E interaction. We estimated the simple slopes for children with ss/sl and ll genotypes (Aiken & West, 1991). Figure 2 presents the results.
The simple slope for children with the ss/ss genotypes was significant (b = 0.31, SE = 0.09, p < .001), but for those with the ll genotypes it was not (b = 0.03, SE = 0.10, ns). The lower and upper bounds of regions of significance were −0.13 and 31.41, respectively. This indicates that two regression lines were significantly different for all possible points when the score of maternal responsiveness was lower than −0.13 or higher than 31.41. The shaded area of Figure 2 represents the region of significance within ±2 SD of the maternal responsiveness mean score.
In the abstract, the regression line for the predicted scores suggests that those ss/sl children would have showed higher school competence if they had been exposed to extremely high maternal responsiveness. However, again, that value, although calculable (31.41), was well beyond the observed range and 2 SD, and consequently, based on the empirical observed values of the maternal responsiveness, we can only infer that children with the ss/sl genotypes showed significantly lower school competence scores if their mothers’ responsiveness was lower than −0.13, resembling the diathesis–stress model. Note also that ss/sl children of mothers whose responsiveness was higher than −0.13 were no less competent at school than their ll peers.
Moral internalization
In the final equation, maternal responsiveness remained significant, along with the significant interaction effect of Child Genotype × Maternal Responsiveness, G × E, that qualified the main effect. To probe the interaction effect, we estimated the simple slopes for children with ss/sl and ll genotypes (Aiken & West, 1991).
The simple slope for the children with the ss/sl genotypes was significant (b = 0.63, SE = 0.18, p < .001), but for those with the ll genotypes it was not (b 0.14, SE = 0.21, ns). The lower and upper bounds of the regions of significance were −1.17 and 0.41, respectively; thus, the two regression lines were significantly different for all possible points when the score of maternal responsiveness was lower than −1.17 or higher than 0.41. The shaded areas of Figure 3 represent the regions of significance within ±2 SD of the maternal responsiveness mean score. Because both values (−1.17 and 0.41) were within the observed range of maternal responsiveness, we can draw empirical inferences about both the effects of low maternal responsiveness and high responsiveness.
It appears that when exposed to low maternal responsiveness (< −1.17, ~1.5 SD below the mean), the ss/sl children scored significantly lower on the moral internalization measure than did the ll children, resembling the traditional diathesis–stress model. When reared by more responsive mothers, however (whose responsiveness scores were >0.41, ~0.5 SD above the mean), the ss/sl children scored significantly higher than the ll children. Thus, the entire picture of G × E effects for moral internalization is consistent with the differential susceptibility model.
Discussion
This longitudinal study, using extensive observational, molecular genetic, and informants’ measures, informs the ongoing debate on G × E interactions in development. We embrace the recent broadening approach to G × E interactions that expands the inquiry to include positive developmental outcomes (competence) and positive, beneficial environments (Canli & Lesch, 2007; Lesch, 2007). We further address the emerging intriguing issue of the form of the interactions: the traditional diathesis–stress model versus differential susceptibility (Belsky & Pluess, 2009a, 2009b).
This article elucidates the role of maternal responsiveness as an environmental mechanism that may not only merely buffer children from risks conferred by their genotypes (Rutter, 2009) but may also occasionally foster and enhance competencies in children with genetic vulnerabilities consistent with the differential susceptibility model. The relatively new statistical approach to the G × E effects allows us to begin to make tentative judgments about the range of the environmental variation (maternal responsiveness across the first 4 years of life) in which the diathesis–stress or differential susceptibility models emerged for the various aspects of child competence.
We examined children’s competence at the age of a salient and uniform transition to expanded ecologies (Rimm-Kaufman & Pianta, 2000). Following Masten at al. (1995), we conceptualized competence broadly as encompassing effective and engaged school functioning, successful social functioning in peer contexts, and moral internalization that included moral cognition and a view of self as moral. Both moral cognition and the moral self have attracted strong renewed interest (Hardy & Carlo, 2005; Lapsley & Narvaez, 2004a, 2004b; Nucci, 2004; Thompson, Meyer, & McGinley, 2006). An earlier study, using the same puppet interview strategy to assess 5.5-year-olds’ moral selves, revealed that children’s self-views on moral dimensions were internally consistent and meaningfully linked to moral conduct (Kochanska, 2002a).
Those developmental outcomes were assessed using a combination of a multifaceted, well-established measure of child functioning as reported by two informants, children’s narratives produced in response to standard stories, and their self-descriptive responses to a puppet interview. Consequently, our outcome measures provide a relatively broad, multimethod multitrait assessment of social–emotional development that compares favorably with the extant research on G × E interactions that has typically focused on single outcomes.
The expected significant G × E interactions were found for all three aspects of children’s competence. The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism or genotype moderated the links between maternal responsiveness (environment) and children’s school competence, social competence, and moral internalization. Differences in maternal responsiveness were significantly associated with those outcomes for children who had a short allele, ss or sl. Variation in maternal responsiveness was unrelated to future competence for children homozygotic for the long allele, ll.
The new analytic strategy of testing regions of significance (Aiken & West, 1991; Hayes & Matthes, 2009; Preacher et al., 2006) adopted in this study provided a formal way of testing the vigorously debated issue of the shape of G × E interactions. What have we learned from this application?
Two effects clearly resembled the diathesis–stress (genetic vulnerability, or dual-risk) model (Belsky et al., 2007; Belsky & Pluess, 2009a, 2009b). Children traditionally considered more biologically vulnerable (ss/sl), when exposed to poor maternal responsiveness, had lower school competence and lower social competence than those who were less biologically vulnerable. Those presumably more vulnerable children, when exposed to favorable environments or responsive care (already at the point of the mean of maternal responsiveness), fared equally well as their less vulnerable peers. However, even given favorable conditions, they did not fare better than children with two long alleles (ll).
Despite the differences in the studied populations, age of children, the measures of parenting and outcomes, and designs, our findings dovetail with a recent intervention study with a large African American sample of mothers and their preadoles-cent and adolescent children (Brody, Beach, Philibert, Chen, Lei, et al., 2009; Brody, Beach, Philibert, Chen, & McBride Murry, 2009). The intervention (Strong African American Families) aimed to reduce youths’ risky behaviors by targeting multiple aspects of mothers’ parenting (nurturance, communication, monitoring, and control) and youths’ adaptive strategies. Mother-reported supportive parenting served to offset the risk for an increase over time in substance use in children with ss/ sl 5-HTTLPR genotypes. Furthermore, the effects of the intervention were consistent with the diathesis–stress model: the intervention significantly reduced risky behaviors in children with ss/sl genotypes but not in children with ll genotypes. Youth with ss/sl genotypes who received the intervention did as well, but not better, than youth with ll genotypes. The intervention offset the considerable risk conferred by ss/sl genotype (also documented in the study).
Our third G × E effect, for children’s moral internalization, conformed to the differential susceptibility model (Belsky & Pluess, 2009a, 2009b). That G × E interaction incorporated the effects in both the lower range of maternal responsiveness (traditional diathesis–stress) and in its upper range. Thus, taken together, this phenomenon embodied a complete differential susceptibility model. Children with ss/sl genotypes fared less well than their ll peers if their mothers were unresponsive; those children, however, fared better than their biologically invulnerable peers when they had a history of responsive care. Notably, the “plasticity” effect (ss/sl children doing better than ll children when given responsive care) emerged for children of mothers whose responsiveness exceeded approximately half of standard deviation above the mean. Consequently, we can conclude that for some aspects of competence, even a relatively modest improvement in environmental influences, in this case the quality of the mother-child relationship, may be sufficient not only to offset the putative risk but also to enhance children’s developmental outcomes.
How can we interpret the differences in the form of G × E interactions obtained for school and social competence versus the interaction obtained for moral internalization? One frankly tentative interpretation involves a possibility that the outcomes in the three areas of functioning call for different proportions of children’s inner regulatory resources. It is possible that school and social competence engage, to a significant degree, attentional, intellectual, and executive capacities that relatively robustly regress on the child’s genotype. Thus, a certain geno-type (here, ss/sl) might introduce a constraint in terms of the child’s upper achievable level of school and social compe tence. Consequently, although maternal optimal care could effectively offset such constraints, in that the child would perform no worse than his or her peers with “low-risk” genotypes, it may not be sufficient for the child to significantly outperform those peers.
In contrast, moral internalization may regress to a lesser extent on cognitive resources and to a greater extent on the quality of the child’s emotional and relational early experiences (Thompson et al., 2006). For example, the parent-child mutually responsive orientation during the first years of life has been implicated as a powerful factor in emerging conscience and internalization of family values (Kochanska, 2002b). Consequently, maternal highly responsive care may have the potential of significantly fostering the child’s moral internalization, such that even children with presumed biological vulnerabilities would outperform their less vulnerable peers. Such a possibility, however, is frankly exploratory and needs to be tested in future studies.
This study has several limitations. In particular, the most serious and most obvious limitation is the small size of the sample. Although our sample is comparable to some recent studies of the interaction between 5-HTTLPR polymorphism and environment (e.g., Fox et al., 2005; Gilissen, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, & Linting, 2008), a larger sample would allow for a separate examination of ss and sl children, which may elucidate better the studied processes. Thus, until the effects are replicated with a larger sample, considerable caution needs to be exercised while drawing inferences from the current study.
Another limitation is the normative and relatively homogeneous nature of the sample. The effects are likely to be stronger in children and families at a higher risk, for example, families where parental responsiveness is particularly impaired (Kaufman et al., 2006). Furthermore, although 20% of the families had at least one non-White parent, the ethnic range was relatively limited.
The above limitations constrain the variation in the studied constructs. The analytic strategy implemented in this study further highlights the importance of that variation, and particularly the need for samples with broad ranges of variation in the studied dimensions of the environment. As our analyses show, the larger the environmental variation, the better the chances that both lower and upper bounds of the regions of significance will fall within the range of the empirically observed values, allowing us to describe precisely the form of the potential G × E effects. Toward that goal, researchers studying G × E interactions in development should collect robust measures of environmental influences by sampling lengthy and multiple observational contexts, they should use instruments that can capture well variability of those influences (e.g., multiple and sensitive coding systems), and they should recruit large and diverse samples where variation in environments is likely to be substantial. It will also be important to include measures of both environmental adversity and environmental advantage (Belsky & Pluess, 2009b). Such a strategy would increase the likelihood that G × E interactions emerging at both ends of the environmental spectrum would be detected.
In the context of this and other recent research, it may be worthwhile to rethink our traditional labeling of ss/sl and ll genotypes as “high risk” and “low risk,” respectively, derived from and related to the concept of diathesis. Just like the concept of diathesis, those labels are better suited to the study of maladaptive outcomes, such as depression, substance use, aggression, and other aspects of psychopathology, and adverse environments. However, with developmental inquiry broadening the G × E focus to include positive, competent out comes and beneficial environments, and findings demonstrating that under some conditions so-called “high-risk genotypes” may be associated with superior outcomes, new labels (e.g., plasticity or malleability; Belsky & Pluess, 2009a, 2009b) may be more appropriate.
Finally, shared genes may influence both maternal responsiveness and children’s competence. Future designs should aim at collecting molecular genetic data from parents and children.
Research that integrates molecular genetic information with rich behavioral measures of the environment and developmental outcomes and employs longitudinal designs is only beginning to flourish. That research embodies the goals of developmental psychopathology, because it allows for mapping divergent trajectories for children with similar biological profiles but differing experiences. In particular, bridging social relationships and molecular genetics holds promise for progress in understanding the complex nature of adaptive and mal-adaptive social and emotional development, and progress in research on risk, resilience, competence, intervention, and prevention.
Acknowledgments
This research was funded by the grants from the NIMH (RO1 MH63096 and KO2 MH01446), a Stuit Professorship (to G.K.), and Grant RO1 DA015789 (to R.A.P.). We thank Nazan Aksan, Lea Boldt, Jennifer Carlson, Amanda Hollatz, Jamie Koenig, Douglas Long, Michael McPartland, Sara Penney, Theresa Prisco, Sarah Stellern, and Jarilyn Woodard for help with data collection and coding; Dianna Edwards for help with genotyping; and the participants in the Family Study for their commitment to this research. The Mac-Arthur Health Behavior Questionnaire was made available free of charge by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Psychopathology and Development (David J. Kupfer, Network Chair).
Footnotes
Preacher et al. (2006) provide a program (http://www.people.ku.edu/preacher/interact/mlr2.htm) to obtain confidence bands. Analyses using regions of significance and confidence bands led to equivalent findings (available from the second author). Note that the typical application of Preacher et al. (2006) is for cases where the independent variable and the moderator are continuous. They also discuss an option for cases with a continuous independent variable and a dichotomous moderator (as in this article). Then, the specific values for the independent variable are identified, below and above which the slopes of the two dichotomous groups are significantly different.
References
- Aiken LS, West SG. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Sage; Newbury, CA: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Auerbach JG, Faroy M, Ebstein R, Kahana M, Levine J. The association of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) and the serotonin transporter promotor gene (5-HTTLPR) with temperament in 12-month-old infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2001;6:777–783. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr CS, Newman TK, Lindell S, Shannon C, Champoux M, Lesch KP, et al. Interaction between serotonin transporter gene variation and rearing condition in alcohol preference and consumption in female primates. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61:1146–1152. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.11.1146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barry RA, Kochanska G, Philibert RA. G × E interactions in the organization of attachment: Mothers’ responsiveness as a moderator of children’s genotypes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;49:1313–1320. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01935.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J. Variation in susceptibility to rearing influences: An evolutionary argument. Psychological Inquiry. 1997;8:182–186. [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH. For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16:300–304. [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Hsieh KH, Crnic K. Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys’ externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rearing experience? Development and Psychopathology. 1998;10:301–319. doi: 10.1017/s095457949800162x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin. 2009a;135:885–908. doi: 10.1037/a0017376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Pluess M. The nature (and nurture?) of plasticity in early human development. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2009b;4:345–351. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01136.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyce WT, Ellis BJ. Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary- developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:271–301. doi: 10.1017/s0954579405050145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley SL, Dodelzon K, Sandhu HK, Philibert RA. The relationship of serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms and haplotypes to mRNA transcription. American Journal of Medical Genetics: Neuro-psychiatric Genetics. 2005;136B:58–61. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brody GH, Beach SRH, Philibert RA, Chen YF, Lei MK, McBride Murry V, et al. Parenting moderates a genetic vulnerability factor in longitudinal increases in youths’ substance use. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2009a;77:1–11. doi: 10.1037/a0012996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brody GH, Beach SRH, Philibert RA, Chen YF, McBride Murry V. Prevention effects moderate the association of 5-HTTLPR and youth risk behavior initiation: Gene × Environment hypotheses tested via a randomized prevention design. Child Development. 2009b;80:645–661. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01288.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canli T, Lesch KP. Long story short: The serotonin transporter in emotion regulation and social cognition. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:1103–1109. doi: 10.1038/nn1964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, et al. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301:386–389. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Champoux M, Bennett A, Shannon C, Higley JD, Lesch KP, Suomi SJ. Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism, differential early rearing, and behavior in rhesus monkey neonates. Molecular Psychiatry. 2002;7:1058–1063. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins WA, Maccoby EE, Steinberg L, Hetherington EM, Born-stein MH. Contemporary research on parenting: The case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist. 2000;55:218–232. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eder R. Uncovering young children’s psychological selves. Child Development. 1990;61:849–863. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg-Berg N, Hand M. The relationship of preschoolers’ reasoning about prosocial moral conflicts to prosocial behavior. Child Development. 1979;50:356–363. [Google Scholar]
- Essex MJ, Boyce WT, Goldstein LH, Armstrong JM, Kraemer HC, Kupfer DJ. The confluence of mental, physical, social and academic difficulties in middle childhood. II: Developing the MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41:588–603. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200205000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox NA, Nichols KE, Henderson HA, Rubin K, Schmidt L, Hamer D, et al. Evidence for a gene-environment interaction in predicting behavioral inhibition in middle childhood. Psychological Science. 2005;16:921–926. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01637.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilissen R, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van JIzendoorn MH, Linting M. Electrodermal reactivity during the Trier Social Stress Test for children: Interaction between the serotonin transporter polymorphism and children’s attachment representation. Developmental Psychobiology. 2008;50:615–625. doi: 10.1002/dev.20314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hardy SA, Carlo G. Identity as a source of moral motivation. Human Development. 2005;48:232–256. [Google Scholar]
- Hariri AR, Drabant EM, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, Mattay VS, Egan MF, et al. A susceptibility gene for affective disorders and the response of the human amygdala. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:146–152. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.2.146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes AF, Matthes J. Computational procedures for probing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations. Behavior Research Methods. 2009;41:924–936. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.3.924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrmann MJ, Huter T, Muller F, Muhlberger A, Pauli P, Reif A, et al. Additive effects of serotonin transporter and tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene variation on emotional processing. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17:1160–1163. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman J, Yang B, Douglas-Palumberi H, Grasso D, Lipschitz D, Houshyar S, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor-5-HTTLPR gene interactions and environmental modifiers of depression in children. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;59:673–680. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kochanska G. Committed compliance, moral self, and internalization: A mediational model. Developmental Psychology. 2002a;38:339–351. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.3.339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kochanska G. Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: A context for the early development of conscience. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2002b;11:191–195. [Google Scholar]
- Kochanska G, Aksan N, Nichols KE. Maternal power assertion in discipline and moral discourse contexts: Commonalities, differences, and implications for children’s moral conduct and cognition. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39:949–963. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.6.949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kochanska G, Philibert RA, Barry RA. Interplay of genes and early mother-child relationship in the development of self-regulation from toddler to preschool age. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50:1331–1338. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02050.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lapsley DK, Narvaez D, editors. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 2004a. Moral development, self, and identity. [Google Scholar]
- Lapsley DK, Narvaez D. A social-cognitive approach to the moral personality. In: Lapsley DK, Narvaez D, editors. Moral development, self, and identity. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 2004b. pp. 189–212. [Google Scholar]
- Lesch KP. Linking emotion to the social brain: The role of the serotonin transporter in human social behaviour. EMBO Reports. 2007;8:S24–S29. doi: 10.1038/sj.embor.7401008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, et al. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science. 1996;274:1527–1531. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5292.1527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucki I. The spectrum of behaviors influenced by serotonin. Biological Psychiatry. 1998;44:151–162. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00139-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masten AS, Coatsworth JD, Neemann J, Gest SD, Tellegen A, Garmezy N. The structure and coherence of competence from childhood through adolescence. Child Development. 1995;66:1635–1659. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M. Strategy for investigating interactions between measured genes and measured environments. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:473–481. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nucci L. Reflections on the moral self construct. In: Lapsley DK, Narvaez D, editors. Moral development self, and identity. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 2004. pp. 111–132. [Google Scholar]
- Philibert RA, Madan A, Anderson A, Cadoret R, Packer H, Sandhu HK. Regulation of serotonin transporter mRNA levels by an upstream CpG island. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2007;144:101–105. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posner MI, Rothbart MK, Sheese BE. Attention genes. Developmental Science. 2007;10:24–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00559.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preacher KJ, Curran PJ, Bauer DJ. Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics. 2006;31:437–448. [Google Scholar]
- Propper C, Moore GA. The influence of parenting on infant emotionality: A multi-level psychobiological perspective. Developmental Review. 2006;26:427–460. [Google Scholar]
- Rimm-Kaufman SE, Pianta RC. An ecological perspective on the transition to kindergarten: A theoretical framework to guide empirical research. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2000;21:491–511. [Google Scholar]
- Rushton JP, Brainerd CJ, Pressley M. Behavioral development and construct validity: The principle of aggregation. Psychological Bulletin. 1983;94:18–38. [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M. Understanding and testing risk mechanisms for mental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50:44–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01976.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M, Moffitt TE, Caspi A. Gene-environment interplay and psychopathology: Multiple varieties but real effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:226–261. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01557.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shonkoff JP, Phillips DA. From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. National Academy Press; Washington, DC: 2000. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sourbrie P. Reconciling the role of central serotonin neurons in human and animal behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1986;9:319–335. [Google Scholar]
- Suomi SJ. How gene-environment interactions shape biobehavioral development: Lessons from studies with rhesus monkeys. Research in Human Development. 2004;1:205–222. [Google Scholar]
- Suomi SJ. Risk, resilience, and Gene × Environment interactions in rhesus monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2006;1094:52–62. doi: 10.1196/annals.1376.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson RA, Hoffman ML. Empathy and the development of guilt in children. Developmental Psychology. 1980;16:155–156. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson RA, Meyer S, McGinley M. Understanding values in relationship: The development of conscience. In: Killen M, Smetana J, editors. Handbook of moral development. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 2006. pp. 267–297. [Google Scholar]
- van Goozen SHM, Fairchild G, Snoek H, Harold GT. The evidence for a neurobiological model of childhood antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133:149–182. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]