Skip to main content
AJNR: American Journal of Neuroradiology logoLink to AJNR: American Journal of Neuroradiology
. 2012 Oct;33(9):1797–1803. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A3048

The Contributions of MRI-Based Measures of Gray Matter, White Matter Hyperintensity, and White Matter Integrity to Late-Life Cognition

J He a,, VSS Wong b, E Fletcher a, P Maillard a, DY Lee a,d, A-M Iosif c, B Singh a, O Martinez a, AE Roach a, SN Lockhart a, L Beckett c, D Mungas b, ST Farias b, O Carmichael a,b, C DeCarli a,b
PMCID: PMC3694809  NIHMSID: NIHMS486091  PMID: 22538073

Abstract

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE:

GM volume, WMH volume, and FA are each associated with cognition; however, few studies have detected whether these 3 different types of MR imaging measurements exert independent or additive effects on cognitive performance. To detect their extent of contribution to cognitive performance, we explored the independent and additive contributions of GM atrophy, white matter injury, and white matter integrity to cognition in elderly patients.

MATERIALS AND METHODS:

Two hundred and 9 elderly patients participated in the study: 97 were CN adults, 65 had MCI, and 47 had dementia. We measured GM on T1-weighted MR imaging, WMH on FLAIR, and FA on DTI, along with psychometrically matched measures of 4 domains of cognitive performance, including semantic memory, episodic memory, executive function, and spatial abilities.

RESULTS:

As expected, patients with dementia performed significantly more poorly in all 4 cognitive domains, whereas patients with MCI performed generally less poorly than dementia patients, though considerable overlap in performance was present across groups. GM, FA, and WMH each differed significantly between diagnostic groups and were associated with cognitive measures. In multivariate models that included all 3 MR imaging measures (GM, WMH, and FA), GM volume was the strongest determinant of cognitive performance.

CONCLUSIONS:

These results strongly suggest that MR imaging measures of GM are more closely associated with cognitive function than WM measures across a broad range of cognitive and functional impairment.


MR imaging–based brain volumetric measurements are widely used in clinical studies of aging, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia, particularly Alzheimer disease. Multiple reports confirm that global GM loss is significantly associated with advancing age and the onset and progression of AD.1 The extent of GM loss in AD is associated with cognitive performance.1,2 In addition, increased WMH burden on FLAIR MR imaging has been observed in both MCI3,4 and clinical AD,3,5 and is associated with diminished episodic memory and executive function.6,7 More recently, studies of white matter microstructural integrity using FA derived from DTI8,9 have shown that FA in the temporal lobe, posterior cingulum bundle, and fornix is reduced in dementia10,11 and MCI,12 while FA in the corpus callosum may be reduced in CN elders.13 Reduced FA in these tracts is also associated with poor memory performance.14

Prior studies, however, have generally failed to assess whether different types of MR imaging measurements exert independent or additive effects on cognitive performance. Addressing this issue could have important implications for understanding the biologic underpinnings of age-associated cognitive decline, MCI, and dementia. In particular, because GM loss probably reflects injury to the neuronal soma and dendritic arborizations, whereas WMH and FA reflect injury to axonal tracts, and both aspects of brain structure are critical to higher-order cognition, each of the MR imaging measurements has the potential to be independently associated with loss of cognitive abilities. However, it is also possible that 1 of these forms of brain injury has a greater impact on brain function and thus has the greatest biologic impact.

In this study, we measured GM, WMH, and FA in a diverse population of 209 community-dwelling elders and assessed their independent strengths of association with psychometrically matched measures of semantic memory, episodic memory, executive function, and spatial ability. We combined these 3 MR imaging measures into unified models to examine how strongly individual biologic substrates, as measured by MR imaging, are associated with cognitive function.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited through both memory clinic referrals and community outreach. Participants included patients with complaints of cognitive problems and cognitively normal controls. To be included in the study, participants had to be older than 60 years. Exclusion criteria were limited to unstable major medical illness, major primary psychiatric disorder (history of schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, or recurrent major depression), and substance abuse or dependence in the last 5 years. Each of the participants signed informed consent approved by the University of California, Davis, institutional review board. The sample consisted of 209 participants. Most participants (n = 138; 66.0%) were recruited from the community using protocols designed to enhance demographic diversity,15 whereas 71 participants (34.0%) were recruited from memory clinics.

Clinical Evaluation

Each participant received a multidisciplinary clinical evaluation through the University of California, Davis, Alzheimer Disease Center, which included a detailed medical history, physical examination, and neurologic examination. All participants received a standardized neuropsychological test battery that was distinct from the outcome measures in our analyses.

Each participant was diagnosed at a consensus conference by a clinical team. Ninety-seven CN patients, 65 diagnosed with MCI and 47 diagnosed with dementia, were included in this study. Among the patients with dementia, 79.5% were diagnosed with probable AD, 11.4% were diagnosed with AD mixed with cerebrovascular dementia, 4.5% were diagnosed with Lewy body dementia or mixed Lewy body dementia and AD, 2.3% were diagnosed with cerebrovascular dementia, and 2.3% were diagnosed with frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Dementia was diagnosed using the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition-revised criteria16 for dementia, modified to exclude the requirement of memory impairment. Underlying etiology was determined according to standardized criteria and methods. MCI was diagnosed using a modified version of the Petersen criteria, which did not require cognitive complaints.17,18 Participants were considered cognitively normal if they had no clinically significant cognitive impairment.

MR Imaging Acquisition

Brain imaging was obtained at the University of California, Davis, Imaging Research Center (Sacramento, California) on a 1.5T Signa Horizon LX Echospeed system (GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, Wisconsin) and an Eclipse machine (Philips Healthcare, Andover, Massachusetts) at the Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Northern California Health Care System (Martinez, California).19,20 Brain volume measures, including GM and TCV, were obtained from a T1-weighted fast-spoiled gradient recalled echo with TE = 2.9 ms, TR = 9 ms, and a flip angle = 15°. Voxel size was 0.977 × 0.977 × 1.5 mm; WMH was determined from FLAIR imaging with TE = 144 ms, TR = 11,000 ms, TI = 2250 ms, and a voxel size of 0.859 × 0.859 × 3 mm; FA was determined from DTI with TE = 90.4 ms, TR = 8000 ms, flip angle = 90°, and a voxel size of 1.875 × 1.875 × 5 mm. A rigorous protocol ensured the validity of the MR imaging measures across differing scanners and analysts. ICCs between new analysts, all previously trained analysts, and the neurologist were required to be above 0.95 and 0.95 for TCV and WMH. Within-subject between-scanner agreement in TCV and WMH was strong (ICCs = 0.96, and 0.89). Within-subject agreement on the same scanner was strong (ICCs = 0.97 and 0.99).21

Four-Tissue Image Segmentation

Segmentation of GM, WM, and CSF was performed in native space T1-weighted images by an in-house computer program using a Bayesian maximum likelihood expectation-maximization algorithm.22 Tissue probabilities at each voxel were based on a combination of Gaussian intensity distributions and a Markov random field component for modeling tissue configurations within voxel neighborhoods.23 Two in-house enhancements included 1) automatic initialization via a high-dimensional image warp in which tissue probability maps in a template space were fitted to the native T1 images, and 2) edge detection to encourage homogeneous tissue labels within homogeneous image regions. The segmentation of WMH was determined from subject FLAIR images using a semiautomated approach, as described previously.21 Briefly, nonbrain elements were manually removed from FLAIR images by operator-guided tracing of the dura mater in the cranial vault. The resulting corrected image was modeled as a mixture of 2 Gaussian probability functions, with the segmentation threshold determined at the minimum probability between these 2 distributions, followed by a single Gaussian distribution fitted to the image data using an a priori threshold of 3.5 SD in pixel intensity above the mean to identify WMH. Intrarater and interrater reliability of these methods are high and have been published previously.24

Tissue Volumes and FA within ROIs

Linear alignment, followed by high-dimensional image warping, was performed to register native T1-weighted images to a MDT, as described previously.25 These steps were then reversed to map hand-traced ROIs from the space of the MDT back into native space. Global GM and WMH volumes were calculated by counting voxels within those labels throughout the brain. ICC between hand-traced brain volumes and this automatic method are greater than 0.90.

A WM probability map in the MDT space was created by labeling the WM voxels in each T1-weighted scan,26 transforming the resulting WM masks to MDT space, and averaging the masks across the population. Thresholding this WM average map provided a binary WM mask in MDT space.

An average-young-adult FA map was also created in MDT space, as previously described,8 to provide normative FA values for comparison with the elderly participants in the present study. This map was made by transforming the FA images of 15 healthy young adults to MDT space (mean age = 24.1 ± 3.1 years, 60.0% male) and taking the FA average at each voxel.

FA Ratio Values

FA values were indexed against normative values to account for inherent variability in FA that is caused by the intrinsic organization of WM tracts.8 The WM mask described here was used to remove non-WM voxels from the individual FA images, and FA at each voxel was divided by the corresponding entry in the young mean FA map to express FA as a percentage of the FA value that would be expected at that voxel in a healthy young person. In the resulting FA ratio map, a voxel with a value lower than 1 indicates that the subject exhibits FA that is reduced compared with the young group independent of intrinsic local WM organization. Finally, after applying WM tract region of interest masks27 to each individual FA ratio map, we computed the means of FA ratio values within fornix, CC, cingulum, SLF-FP, and UNC ROIs for each subject (Fig 1).

Fig 1.

Fig 1.

Flow chart of FA normalization and masking of a region of interest (μ indicates mean). At the first step, non-WM voxels from FA images are removed by applying each individual normal-appearing WM mask. The FA at each voxel is then divided by the corresponding voxel from the young mean FA map to express FA as a percentage of the FA value. Finally, after applying a region of interest mask (in this case, the CC) to the FA ratio map, we compute the means of FA ratio values within the ROI. This process was repeated for each of the ROIs used in this analysis.

Outcome Measures

Neuropsychological Measures.

Spanish and English Neuropsychological Assessment Scales were used to measure 4 specific domains of cognitive functioning.28,29 A subset of Spanish and English Neuropsychological Assessment Scales were combined using methods from item response theory to create composite measures: The semantic memory measure was based on object naming and picture association tests, the episodic memory measure was based on word list learning tests, and the spatial ability measure was based on pattern recognition and spatial localization tests. A composite executive function score was created from a set of fluency and working memory measures that have been previously developed using the same methods as the original Spanish and English Neuropsychological Assessment Scales.28

Statistical Analyses.

WMH and GM volumes were divided by intracranial volume to avoid confounding effects of head size. WMH volume was also log transformed to better approximate a normal distribution for analysis, as previously described.19

Analyses of variance were used to detect diagnostic group differences in demographics, cognitive performance, and MR imaging measures. Tukey-Kramer adjustment was used for post hoc analyses. P values smaller than .05 were considered statistically significant. Multiple regression models were used to examine the associations between MR imaging measures and cognitive performance, while adjusting for the potential effects of sex, education, and recruitment source. Subject age was not used as a covariate in these adjusted models because previous studies indicate that controlling for age may attenuate the associations between MR imaging variables and cognitive function in this sample.21,28 Because there were 4 outcomes, we used Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons and set the α level to .05/4. P values smaller than this α level were considered significant. All statistical analyses were performed in JMP 8 (SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina).

Results

Diagnostic Group Differences

This study included 209 participants (average age 74.5 ± 7.8 years). Sample sizes, demographic characteristics, composite cognitive scores, and MR imaging measures are summarized by diagnostic group in Table 1. Age (P = .13) and education (P = .09) were not significantly different among the 3 diagnostic groups.

Table 1:

Participant demographic characteristics, cognitive function, and MRI measures across diagnostic groups

CNn = 97 MCIn = 65 Dementedn = 47 P Value
Demographic characteristics
    Gender (M/F) 33 / 64 32 / 33 19 / 28 P = .15
African American 13.4% 13.8% 8.5%
    Hispanic 38.1% 29.2% 25.5%
    White 41.2% 47.7% 61.7%
    Age (years) 74.1 ± 7.5 73.6 ± 7.2 76.5 ± 9.2 P = .13
    Education (years) 12.0 ± 5.0 13.0 ± 5.4 11.0 ± 4.3 P = .09
    RS (clinic/community) 14 / 83 30 / 35 27 / 20 P < .001
Cognitive performance
    MMSE 27.9 ± 2.3a 25.7 ± 3.7b 21.1 ± 5.2c P < .001
    Semantic .16 ± .88a −.18 ± .90a −.71 ± .95b P < .001
    Episodic .05 ± .86a −.86 ± .63b −1.56 ± .61c P < .001
    Executive −.08 ± .69a −.49 ± .59b −.91 ± .80c P < .001
    Spatial .02 ± .86a −.23 ± .97a −.73 ± .97b P < .001
Brain MRI measures
    GM .47 ± .03a .45 ± .03b .43 ± .03c P < .001
    WMH .007 ± .008a .009 ± .009a,b .012 ± .016b P = .03
    FA-CC .90 ± .06a .88 ± .06a .85 ± .10b P = .003
    FA-Cingulum .90 ± .08a .88 ± .09ab .85 ± .15b P = .01
    FA-Fornix .78 ± .09a .74 ± .07b .71 ± .09b P < .001
    FA-SLF-FP .89 ± .06 .87 ± .08 .87 ± .09 P = .22
    FA-UNC .91 ± .07a .90 ± .08ab .88 ± .08b P = .03

Note:—Continuous variables (MRI measures, cognition scores, education years, and age) are presented as mean and SD; ethnicity data are presented as percentages across cognitive groups.

Group differences determined by simple ANOVA; group differences of character variables determined by χ square. FA, ratio of young adult FA value; GM, percentage of total cranial volume; MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination.

a,b,c

Means with different superscripts indicate significant group differences after Tukey-Kramer adjustments, where P <.05 was considered statistically significant.

Cognitive Performance

Cognitive performance differed significantly across diagnostic groups (all P < .001) (Table 1 and Fig 2), though significant overlap in performance between groups was found. Patients with dementia, as expected, had significantly poorer cognitive performance than both CN (all P < .001) and MCI participants (semantic, P = .02; spatial, P = .04; episodic, P < .001; executive, P = .01). MCI participants also scored significantly worse than CN participants in episodic memory and executive function (all P < .001) but not in spatial ability (P = .25) or semantic memory (P = .08).

Fig 2.

Fig 2.

Differences between clinical diagnostic groups in MR imaging measures and cognitive function.

Brain MR Imaging Measures

Among MR imaging measures, the GM volume differences between diagnostic groups were most pronounced (CN/Demented and MCI/Demented, P < .001; CN/MCI, P = .008). Global WMH volume was significantly higher in the group with dementia compared with controls (P = .04). FA-CC (P = .002), FA-Cingulum (P = .01), FA-Fornix (P = .002), and FA-UNC (P = .02) were each significantly lower in the group with dementia compared with the CN group; FA-CC was significantly lower in dementia compared with MCI (P = .05); and FA-Fornix was significantly lower in MCI compared with the CN group (P = .01). However, FA-SLF-FP did not differ significantly across diagnostic groups (Table 1 and Fig 2).

Individual MR Imaging Measures and Cognitive Performance

Diagnostic groups showed considerable heterogeneity and overlap in both cognitive performance and MR imaging measures (Fig 2). Therefore, regression models controlling for sex, education, and recruitment source were used to assess the associations between continuous cognitive and MR imaging measures independent of clinical diagnosis. Models including individual MR imaging predictors suggested that poor performance in each cognitive domain was associated with smaller GM volume (semantic, b = 6.14, P = .001; and episodic, b = 10.87; executive, b = 6.95; spatial, b = 7.65; all P < .001). Poorer episodic memory performance was associated with greater WMH volume (b = −.16, P = .006). Lower FA-Cingulum was associated with poor episodic memory (b = 1.80, P = .004) and executive function (b = 1.32, P = .005), and lower FA-fornix was associated with poor episodic memory (b = 2.78, P < .001), executive function (b = 1.71, P = .003), and spatial ability (b = 2.10, P = .005). Associations between FA-SLF-FP or FA-UNC and cognitive measures were not statistically significant (Table 2).

Table 2:

Results of the individual linear regression models predicting cognition for each individual MRI measure

Semantic Episodic Executive Spatial
GM 6.14 (1.90)P = .001* 10.87 (1.92) P < .001* 6.95 (1.46)P < .001* 7.65 (2.07)P < .001*
WMH −.06 (0.05)P = .27 −.16 (.06)P = .0068 −.09 (.04)P = .04 −.08 (.06)P = .17
FA-CC .91 (.86)P = .30 1.82 (.92)P = .05 1.24 (.68)P = .07 1.70 (.93)P = .07
FA-Cingulum .11 (.58)P = .85 1.80 (.62)P = .004* 1.32 (.46)P = .005* 1.28 (.61)P = .04
FA-Fornix .83 (.70)P = .24 2.78 (.75)P < .001* 1.71 (.57)P < .003* 2.10 (.74)P < .005*
FA-SLF .27 (.82)P = .74 .67 (.89)P = .45 .69 (.67)P = .30 1.18 (.88)P = .18
FA-UNC 1.45 (.80)P = .07 1.90 (.86)P = .03 1.49 (.64)P = .02 1.98 (.85)P = .02

Note:—Estimated from separate linear regression models that controlled for gender, education, and recruitment source. The α level was set at .01 to account for the 4 outcome measures (.05/4 = .0125). FA indicates ratio of young adults fractional anistropy value; GM, percentage of total cranial volume; WMH, white matter hyperintensities (log-transformed percentage of total cranial volume).

*

Indicates P values not larger than the α level which was set at .0125.

Correlations among MR Imaging Measures

Before including multiple MR imaging predictors into multiple regression models of cognitive performance, we used Pearson correlations to assess whether colinearities among the MR imaging measures could influence their strengths of association with the cognitive outcomes. No strong associations among the MR imaging measures were detected (GM/WMH, r = .20; GM/FA-Fornix, r = 0.33; GM/FA-Cingulum, r = .26; GM/FA-CC, r = .25; WMH/FA-Fornix, r = −.31; WMH/FA-Cingulum, r = −.26; WMH/ FA-CC, r = −.37).

Multiple MR Imaging Measures and Cognitive Performance

Because GM, WMH, FA-Cingulum, and FA-Fornix were each significantly associated with cognitive performance, all 3 measures (GM, WMH, and FA) were added into multiple regression models predicting each cognitive outcome with different models for each FA measure: FA-Fornix and FA-Cingulum. These models also included sex, education, and recruitment source as additional covariates (Table 3). In the models with GM, WMH, and FA-Fornix, smaller GM was still associated with poorer semantic memory (b = 5.89, P = .004), episodic memory (b = 9.13, P < .001), executive function (b = 5.95, P < .001), and spatial ability (b = 6.39, P = .004), while none of the associations between these cognitive measures and FA-Fornix or WMH were statistically significant. Similarly, in the model with GM, WMH, and FA-Cingulum, reduced GM was associated with poorer cognitive performance (semantic, b = 6.26, P = .002; episodic, b = 9.50, P < .001; executive, b = 6.0, P < .001; spatial, b = 6.86, P = .002), while WMH and FA-Cingulum were not significantly associated with any cognitive measures.

Table 3:

Results of the multiple linear regression models predicting cognition, including MRI measures as simultaneous predictors

Semantic Episodic Executive Spatial
GM 5.89 (2.03)P = .004* 9.13 (2.03)P < .001* 5.95 (1.55) P < .001* 6.39 (2.21) P = .004*
WMH −.02 (.06)P = .77 −.06 (.06)P = .29 −.03 (.04)P = .51 −.004 (.06)P = .96
FA-Fornix .13 (.75)P = .86 1.47 (.77)P = .06 .90 (.59)P = .13 1.39 (.79)P = .06
GM 6.26 (2.0) P = .002* 9.50 (2.02)P < .001* 6.0 (1.54) P < .001* 6.86 (2.19) P = .002*
WMH −.03 (.05)P = .64 −.07 (.06)P = .18 −.03 (.04)P = .44 −.01 (.06)P = .80
FA-Cingulum −.42 (.59)P = .48 .79 (.62)P = .21 .72 (.47)P = .13 .77 (.63)P = .22

Note:—FA indicates ratio of young adults fractional anistropy value; GM, gray matter (percentage of total cranial volume); WMH, log transformed percentage of total cranial volume.

*

Indicates P values not larger than the α level which was set at .0125.

Discussion

MR imaging measures of GM, WMH volumes, and FA are each independently associated with cognition.3032 However, few studies have examined whether these 3 different types of MR imaging measurements exert independent or additive effects on cognitive performance. While GM atrophy, white matter integrity, and white matter injury were each individually associated with cognitive performance in our study, only GM atrophy was associated with cognition in a multivariate model that included all 3 measures. Thus, while longitudinal measurement of the 3 brain injury measures and cognition are needed to fully understand their coevolution over time, our cross-sectional findings support the view that GM atrophy may be the most relevant MR imaging measurement of cognitive ability. We speculate that GM atrophy is more tightly tied to cognitive performance because it most specifically represents neuronal injury, and neuronal injury appears to be the most important biologic process associated with cognition. We do not wish, however, to imply that injury to white matter is not important. In fact, multiple studies show that neurobehavioral deficits arise both from cortical lesions and injury to fiber tracts that link cortical and subcortical regions into distributed networks.33 However, WMH and reduced FA represent injury to fiber tracts that is probably incomplete (ie, injured but not transected axons), thereby impeding, but not preventing, neuronal transmission via those tracts. Furthermore, because any pair of regions may be connected directly or indirectly via multiple fiber pathways,33,34 injury to any individual pathway may alter or make inefficient, but not completely prevent, communication between functional cortical systems. Conversely, GM atrophy probably reflects injury to neuronal soma, dendrites, and, most importantly, synapses, leading to primary impairments in the nodes of the neural networks. The importance of GM atrophy in our study is further strengthened by the fact that most of the cognitively impaired participants exhibited clinical phenotypes suggesting the presence of AD pathology, which results in specific injury to pyramidal cell neurons in cortical layers III and IV responsible for cortical-cortical connectivity.35,36 Again, these results should not be interpreted as suggesting that white matter integrity has no direct bearing on late-life cognition; indeed, WMHs have been shown to impact cognitive function in the absence of severe GM atrophy,37 and white matter injury may in fact exacerbate the gray matter atrophy that seems to drive cognitive losses.3,8,32 However, in a population spanning a range of both GM atrophy and WM injury, our data suggest that GM atrophy may represent the final common pathway to cognitive decline.

The veracity of our findings is bolstered by a number of additional observations. First, echoing previous studies, we found significant univariate associations between GM, WM measures, and cognitive performance,30 particularly for performance in the executive and episodic memory domains.6 In addition, we found significant differences in cognitive performance across all the cognitive domains comparing CN patients with ones with MCI and MCI with dementia, similar to prior studies.18,38 Reductions in GM and WM across syndromes have been similarly shown.33,3942 We believe similarities between our study and prior observations support the primary finding of differing strengths of association between cognition and GM, WMH, and FA.

Our study is not without limitations, however. For example, this analysis is entirely cross-sectional and therefore cannot address causality or temporal evolution. It is possible, for example, that white matter lesions may have independent effects on cognition early in the course of brain injury when clinical impairment is absent or only mild37,43,44 but are overwhelmed in the setting of cortical brain injury through the AD process, as suggested in pathologic studies.45 In addition, this study included participants with cognitive decline of various etiologies, though most were AD associated. This may limit our ability to investigate relations between AD-related brain injury and cognition, but the more diverse cohort would have been expected to increase the effects of WMH and FA, because many of the non-AD subjects would be expected to exhibit vascular disease. In future studies, we may restrict analysis to only patients with amnestic MCI and AD to further study the specific MR imaging changes and cognition decline associated with AD.

Conclusions

These results strongly suggest that MR imaging measures of GM associate more closely with cognitive function than white matter measures do across a broad range of cognitive and functional impairment. We believe our study is among the first to comprehensively assess the relationship between various MR imaging measures of gray and white matter injury and cognitive performance. From our results, we conclude the GM injury is most important to cognitive performance among a group of older participants with various degrees of cognitive impairment. Future research will evaluate possible effects of regional differences in GM volume on longitudinal cognition and risk for transitions from normal cognitive ability to cognitive impairment.

ABBREVIATIONS:

AD

Alzheimer Disease

CC

corpus callosum

CN

cognitively normal

FA

fractional anisotropy

GM

gray matter

ICC

intraclass correlation coefficient

MCI

mild cognitive impairment

MDT

minimum deformation template

RS

recruitment source

SLF-FP

superior longitudinal fasciculus, frontal part

TCV

total cranial volume

UNC

uncinate fasciculus

WMH

white matter hyperintensity

Footnotes

Disclosures: Laurel Beckett—RELATED: Grant: NIH,* Comments: My research in cognitive aging is supported by the UC Davis Alzheimer's Disease Center and by the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (UC Davis subcontract for biostatistics). Sarah Tomaszewski Farias—RELATED: Grant: NIH.* Charles DeCarli—RELATED: Grant: NIH;* Support for Tavel to Meetings for the Study or Other Purposes: NIH;* UNRELATED: Grants/Grants Pending: NIH;* Payment for Lectures (including service on speakers bureaus): Northwestern University, UC Irvine, Comments: Numerous invited speaking engagements; Patents (planned, pending, or issued): 1991 – U.S. Patent Number 5262945, Method for Quantification of Brain Volumes from Magnetic Resonance Images;* Payment for Development of Educational Presentations: AAN educational courses. (*Money paid to institution.)

This research was supported by NIH grants: P30 AG10129, R01 AG010220, R01 AG 031563, R01 AG021028, and K01 AG 030514.

References

  • 1. Mouton PR, Martin LL, Calhoun ME, et al. Cognitive decline strongly correlates with cortical atrophy in Alzheimer's dementia. Neurobiol Aging 1998; 19: 371– 77 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Kopelman MD, Lasserson D, Kingsley D, et al. Structural MRI volumetric analysis in patients with organic amnesia, 2: correlations with anterograde memory and executive tests in 40 patients. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001; 71: 23– 28 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Yoshita M, Fletcher E, Harvey D, et al. Extent and distribution of white matter hyperintensities in normal aging, MCI, and AD. Neurology 2006; 67: 2192– 98 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. DeCarli C, Miller BL, Swan GE, et al. Cerebrovascular and brain morphologic correlates of mild cognitive impairment in the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Twin Study. Arch Neurol 2001; 58: 643– 47 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Scheltens P, Barkhof F, Valk J, et al. White matter lesions on magnetic resonance imagings in clinically diagnosed Alzheimer's disease. Evidence for heterogeneity. Brain 1992; 115: 735– 48 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Parks CM, Iosif A.-M, Farias S, et al. Executive function mediates effects of white matter hyperintensities on episodic memory. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49: 2817– 24 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Petkov CI, Wu CC, Eberling JL, et al. Correlates of memory function in community-dwelling elderly: the importance of white matter hyperintensities. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004; 10: 371– 81 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Lee DY, Fletcher E, Martinez O, et al. Regional pattern of white matter microstructural changes in normal aging, MCI, and AD. Neurology 2009; 73: 1722– 28 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Assaf Y, Pasternak O. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI)-based white matter mapping in brain research: a review. J Mol Neurosci 2008; 34: 51– 61 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Ringman JM, O'Neill J, Geschwind D, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging in preclinical and presymptomatic carriers of familial Alzheimer's disease mutations. Brain 2007; 130: 1767– 76 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Takahashi S, Yonezawa H, Takahashi J, et al. Selective reduction of diffusion anisotropy in white matter of Alzheimer disease brains measured by 3.0 Tesla magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci Lett 2002; 332: 45– 48 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Fellgiebel A, Müller MJ, Wille P, et al. Color-coded diffusion-tensor-imaging of posterior cingulate fiber tracts in mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging 2005; 26: 1193– 98 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV, Hedehus M, et al. Age-related decline in brain white matter anisotropy measured with spatially corrected echo-planar diffusion tensor imaging. Magn Reson Med 2000; 44: 259– 68 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Chang MC, Kim SH, Kim OL, et al. The relation between fornix injury and memory impairment in patients with diffuse axonal injury: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Neurorehabilitation 2010; 26: 347– 53 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Hinton L, Carter K, Reed BR, et al. Recruitment of a community-based cohort for research on diversity and risk of dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2010; 24: 234– 41 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000 [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med 2004; 256: 183– 94 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, et al. Mild cognitive impairment—Clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 1999; 56: 303– 08 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. DeCarli C, Reed BR, Jagust W, et al. Brain behavior relationships among African Americans, Whites, and Hispanics. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2008; 22: 382– 91 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. He J, Farias S, Martinez O, et al. Differences in brain volume, hippocampal volume, cerebrovascular risk factors, and apolipoprotein E4 among mild cognitive impairment subtypes. Arch Neurol 2009; 66: 1393– 99 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Carmichael O, Mungas D, Beckett L, et al. MRI predictors of cognitive change in a diverse and carefully characterized elderly population. Neurobiol Aging 2012; 33: 83– 95 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Dempster AP, Laird NM, Rubin DB. Maximum likelihood from incomplete data via EM algorithm. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol 1977; 39: 1– 38 [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Rajapakse JC, Giedd JN, Rapoport JL. Statistical approach to segmentation of single-channel cerebral MR images. IEEE Trans Med Imag 1997; 16: 176– 86 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. DeCarli C, Massaro J, Harvey D, et al. Measures of brain morphology and infarction in the Framingham heart study: establishing what is normal. Neurobiol Aging 2005; 26: 491– 510 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. DeCarli C, Fletcher E, Ramey V, et al. Anatomical mapping of white matter hyperintensities (WMH): exploring the relationships between periventricular WMH, deep WMH, and total WMH burden. Stroke 2005; 36: 50– 55 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Rajapakse JC, DeCarli C, McLaughlin A, et al. Cerebral magnetic resonance image segmentation using data fusion. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1996; 20: 206– 18 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Zhang Y, Zhang J, Oishi K, et al. Atlas-guided tract reconstruction for automated and comprehensive examination of the white matter anatomy. NeuroImage 2010; 52: 1289– 301 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Mungas D, Reed BR, Farias ST, et al. Age and education effects on relationships of cognitive test scores with brain structure in demographically diverse older persons. Psychol Aging 2009; 24: 116– 28 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Mungas D, Reed BR, Crane PK, et al. Spanish and English Neuropsychological Assessment Scales (SENAS): further development and psychometric characteristics. Psychol Assess 2004; 16: 347– 59 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Mueller SG, Mack WJ, Mungas D, et al. Influences of lobar gray matter and white matter lesion load on cognition and mood. Psychiatry Res 2010; 181: 90– 96 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Cardenas VA, Chao LL, Studholme C, et al. Brain atrophy associated with baseline and longitudinal measures of cognition. Neurobiol Aging 2011; 32: 572– 80 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Lee DY, Fletcher E, Martinez O, et al. Vascular and degenerative processes differentially affect regional interhemispheric connections in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer disease. Stroke 2010; 41: 1791– 97 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Schmahmann JD, Smith EE, Eichler FS, et al. Cerebral white matter: neuroanatomy, clinical neurology, and neurobehavioral correlates. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008; 1142: 266– 309 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Schmanhmann JD, Pandya DN. Cerebral white matter—historical evolution of facts and notions concerning the organization of the fiber pathways of the brain. J Hist Neurosci 2007; 16: 237– 67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Morrison JH, Hof PR. Life and death of neurons in the aging cerebral cortex. Int Rev Neurobiol 2007; 81: 41– 57 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Thangavel R, Sahu SK, Van Hoesen GW, et al. Loss of nonphosphorylated neurofilament immunoreactivity in temporal cortical areas in Alzheimer's disease. Neuroscience 2009; 160: 427– 33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Mayda AB, Westphal A, Carter CS, et al. Late life cognitive control deficits are accentuated by white matter disease burden. Brain 2011; 134: 1673– 83 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Petersen RC, Morris JC. Mild cognitive impairment as a clinical entity and treatment target. Arch Neurol 2005; 62: 1160– 63 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Fox NC, Warrington EK, Rossor MN. Serial magnetic resonance imaging of cerebral atrophy in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Lancet 1999; 353: 2125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Foundas AL, Leonard CM, Mahoney SM, et al. Atrophy of the hippocampus, parietal cortex, and insula in Alzheimer's disease: a volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol and Behav Neurol 1997; 10: 81– 89 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Barber R, Scheltens F, Gholkar A, et al. White matter lesions on magnetic resonance imaging in dementia with Lewy bodies, Alzheimer's disease, vascular dementia, and normal aging. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999; 67: 66– 72 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Yoshita M, Fletcher E, Ortega M, et al. White matter hyperintensities and fractional anisotoropy are associated with corpus callosum atrophy in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2007; 68: A62 [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Nordahl CW, Ranganath C, Yonelinas AP, et al. Different mechanisms of episodic memory failure in mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 2005; 43: 1688– 97 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Nordahl CW, Ranganath C, Yonelinas AP, et al. White matter changes compromise prefrontal cortex function in healthy elderly individuals. J Cogn Neurosci 2006; 18: 418– 29 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Chui HC, Zarow C, Mack WJ, et al. Cognitive impact of subcortical vascular and Alzheimer's disease pathology. Ann Neurol 2006; 60: 677– 87 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from AJNR: American Journal of Neuroradiology are provided here courtesy of American Society of Neuroradiology

RESOURCES