Abstract
Objective
To assess the cross-sectional relationship of glycemic control to memory impairment and executive dysfunction in older adults with diabetes treated at an urban primary care center.
Participants and Methods
As part of a primary care-based cognitive screening program, we identified adults age 65 or older with a diagnosis of diabetes. Glycosylated hemoglobin level (HbA1c) was used to define diabetes as controlled (HbA1c <7) or inadequately controlled(HbA1c ≥ 7). Episodic memory was measured by quartile of free recall scores on the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test. Executive function was measured using an ordinal composite score derived from animal fluency and months backward. These were the main predictors of diabetic control.
Results
The 169 participants with diabetes had a median age of 74. The sample was 38% African American and 42% Latino. One hundred four (61%) had inadequately controlled diabetes. Memory impairment and executive dysfunction were independent predictors of diabetic control after adjusting for age and education. Binary logistic regression models indicated that the odds of inadequately controlled diabetes was higher for patients in the worst quartile of memory functioning compared to patients in higher quartiles of memory functioning (odds ratio = 6.4; 95% confidence interval: 2.3, 17.6). Any level of executive dysfunction increased the odds of inadequately controlled diabetes compared to patients in the best quintile of executive functioning (odds ratio =3.6; 95% confidence interval: 1.58, 8.35).
Conclusions
Memory impairment and executive dysfunction were associated with inadequately controlled diabetes. Though causal inferences are not robust in a cross-sectional study, we suggest that cognitive dysfunction may interfere with diabetes management and that inadequate diabetic control may contribute to cognitive dysfunction.
Keywords: diabetes, dementia, cognitive impairment, African American, Latino
Introduction
Both diabetes and cognitive impairment are common problems in older adults,1,2–4 and these problems disproportionally affect African Americans and Latinos.4,5 Type 2 diabetes is a risk factor for cognitive decline and dementia.6 Cognitive impairment, in turn, may make optimal glycemic control more difficult, because diabetic self-management activities are cognitively demanding.7 Recent studies have shown that cognitive impairment is associated with poor glycemic control.8–10 In the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), respondents in the lowest quartile of their cognitive scale had significantly higher glycosylated hemoglobin (HbA1c) levels than those in the highest quartile.9 These findings are compatible with the hypothesis that cognitive dysfunction may interfere with diabetic control,6 or that poor diabetic control contributes to cognitive dysfunction.11
Most studies of cognitive function and glycemic control included relatively small numbers of African American and Latino patients, who are at elevated risk for diabetes. In this study, a largely African American and Hispanic cohort of patients with diabetes 65 years and older was recruited from patients making routinely scheduled visits to an urban primary care clinic. Consistent with earlier work, we anticipated that cognitive impairment would predict inadequate glycemic control. We focused separately on memory and executive functioning because impairment of either could interfere with self-management activities. Glycemic control was measured by HbA1c. We expected that both executive dysfunction and memory impairment would predict inadequately controlled diabetes.
Methods
Clinical Setting
Older patients with diabetes were identified from 350 participants in an ongoing memory screening project being conducted in the Adult Primary Care Clinic of the Jacobi Medical Center, which serves a diverse patient population in the Bronx, NY. The project was approved by the Institutional Review Boards at the Albert Einstein College of Medicine and Jacobi Medical Center. The 20-minute screening battery was coordinated with each patient’s regularly scheduled clinic appointment.
There were 169 patients with diagnosed diabetes (ICD-9 codes of 249, 250, or 790). Patients were 65 or older, fluent in English or Spanish, and had adequate vision and hearing. We included the 2 patients with dementia ICD-9 codes because there were most certainly patients with unrecognized dementia in the cohort.12 The HbA1c level closest in time to the screening date was used to define diabetes as controlled (HbA1c<7) or inadequately controlled (HbA1c ≥ 7).13
One hundred four (61%) of the patients met the definition for inadequately controlled diabetes. The majority of patients were African American (44%) or Latino (49%), and most were women (74%). Patients with inadequately controlled diabetes tended to be younger than patients with good control (72.6 vs 74.5, P = .06), did not differ in education (9.4 vs 8.6, P = .18), and had had diabetes longer than patients with good control (7.3 vs 5.4, P = .01).
Episodic Memory
Episodic memory was assessed with the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test,14 which identifies very mild dementia,15 predicts future dementia and Alzheimer’s disease (AD),16–18 and distinguishes AD from non-AD dementias.15,19,20 Unlike other memory tests, it includes a study phase that controls attention and cognitive processing to identify memory impairment that is not secondary to other cognitive deficits. Sixteen pictures are presented 4 at a time; participants identify each picture by naming it (eg, grapes) after its cue is presented (fruit). Immediate cued recall of the 4 pictures is tested before the next set is studied. There are 3 test trials consisting of free recall followed by cued recall of items not retrieved in free recall. Four strata were defined by quartiles for the sum of free recall among the 350 participants in the screening project.
Executive Function
Executive function was measured by a composite score derived from animal fluency and months backwards among the 350 participants in the screening project. In animal fluency, patients have 60 seconds to generate the names of animals, a task that requires rapid self-guided retrieval from semantic memory. Reciting the months backwards requires reversing the normal order and keeping track of one’s place. A composite score was constructed with 5 levels, as shown at the bottom of Table 1.
Table 1.
Level of Executive Dysfunction and Relative Odds of Inadequate Diabetic Control: Results from Multiple Logistic Regression Adjusting for Age and Years of Education
Executive function impairment | Odds ratio vs. reference unimpaired | Lower 95% Confidence Level | Upper 95% Confidence Level | P |
---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 3.59 | 1.41 | 9.15 | .007 |
2 | 3.10 | 1.16 | 8.31 | .024 |
3 | 5.26 | 1.49 | 18.55 | .01 |
4 | 5.25 | 0.84 | 32.82 | .076 |
Key to executive function impairment levels (0=unimpaired, reference group):
0= Animal Naming ≥ 15 and no errors on Months Backwards (unimpaired)
1 = Animal Naming 10–14 and no errors in Months Backwards, OR Animal Naming ≥15 and uncorrected errors in Months Backwards
2 = Animal Naming ≤ 9 and no errors in Months Backwards, OR Animal Naming 10–14 and uncorrected errors in Months Backwards, OR Animal Naming ≥ 15 and unable to complete Months Backwards
3 = Animal Naming ≤ 9 and uncorrected error(s) in Months Backwards, OR Animal Naming 10–14 and unable to complete Months Backwards
4 = Animal Naming ≤ 9 and unable to complete Months Backwards
Statistical Analyses
The relationship between glycemic control and cognition was assessed with a series of binary logistic regression models adjusted for age and education, which could confound the relationships of interest. The main predictors were the episodic memory and executive function measures as defined above.
Results
Impaired executive function was a strong predictor of inadequate glycemic control (odds ratio [OR] 3.64, 95% confidence interval [CI] 1.58, 8.35). Lack of glycemic control increased with greater executive function impairment (Table 1). Memory impairment was also a strong predictor of glycemic control (OR 6.39, 95% CI 2.32–17.6). Persons in the lowest quartile of memory function were at increased risk of inadequate control, whereas persons in the second and third quartiles were not (Table 2).
Table 2.
Level of Memory Impairment and Relative Odds of Inadequate Diabetic Control: Results from Multiple Logistic Regression Adjusting for Age and Years of Education
Free Recall Score | Odds Ratio vs. Reference Unimpaired (34+) | Lower 95% Confidence Level | Upper 95% Confidence Level | P |
---|---|---|---|---|
34+ (ref.) | — | — | — | — |
29–33 | 1.86 | 0.71 | 4.87 | .207 |
24–28 | 1.17 | 0.44 | 3.11 | .752 |
Under 24 | 8.24 | 2.56 | 26.60 | <.001 |
Entering executive dysfunction and memory impairment in the same model did not substantially reduce their predictive values compared to the separate models, nor did they interact (P = .38). Being in the bottom quartile of memory function increased the risk of inadequately controlled diabetes (OR 5.16, 95% CI 1.84, 14.45) as did being other than in the top quintile of executive function (OR 2.56, 95% CI 1.07, 6.13]). Each additional year of diagnosis increased the risk of inadequate control by 29% (OR 1.29, 95% CI 1.12,1.50]). There was a trend for younger patients with diabetes and patients with diabetes who had more education to have inadequately controlled diabetes (both, P = .07).
Discussion
These results confirm our hypothesis that both memory impairment and executive dysfunction would predict inadequate glycemic control in a racially diverse sample of older adults with type 2 diabetes. Patients with any level of executive dysfunction were 3.6 times more likely to have inadequately controlled diabetes (HgA1c ≥ 7) than patients with no executive dysfunction. Patients with more severe executive dysfunction were at greater risk of inadequate control than patients with milder executive dysfunction, suggesting a dose-dependent relationship. These results highlight the need for executive function assessment in primary care settings because of the impact that even the lowest levels of impairment may have on diabetic control.
Patients in the lowest quartile of memory function were 6.4 times more likely to have inadequately controlled diabetes than patients with better memory. The memory impairment displayed by these patients is at the level of patients with very mild dementia.15 Memory impairment and executive dysfunction were independent predictors of glycemic control and did not interact. Each year of the diagnosis increased the risk of uncontrolled diabetes by 29%.
The results extend our understanding in 2 ways. First, they extend the previous results linking cognitive impairment and inadequate glucose control to a cohort of African American and Latino primary care patients. Second, in the HRS,9 cognition was indicated by a single score summarizing cognitive functioning. Because a composite score was used, identifying which aspects of cognition influenced glycemic control was not possible.
Though directionality cannot be established from these cross-sectional studies, we suggest that cognitive dysfunction may interfere with diabetes management and that inadequate diabetic control may contribute to cognitive decline, thereby producing a feed-forward mechanism. Recognizing cognitive impairment would create opportunities to develop management strategies that do not depend on the cognitive status of the patient with diabetes. Unfortunately, cognitive impairment and dementia may go unrecognized in primary care settings,12,21,22 which may result in missed opportunities to improve the management of diabetes in patients with cognitive impairment. As a first step to improving adherence, we developed and validated several tools for identifying patients with cognitive impairment and dementia in primary care settings,23–25 and we implemented them here. The second step may be to involve a family member or friend in the patients’ diabetes care. In the HRS study, high levels of family support amerliorated the association between cognitive impairment and worse glycemic control.9 Activating a family to be part of the care team is already an established element of successful collaborative care models for chronic illness.26,27 Family involvement may be particularly important for cognitively impaired older adults who may be unable to manage their diabetes care effectively without assistance.
This study has several limitations. First, the cross-sectional design does not permit robust inferences about the causal pathways between cognition and diabetic control. A bidirectional relationship is plausible. Second, we did not have sufficient power to examine other factors that might influence diabetic control (eg, racial/ethnic membership, medication regimen). Finally, we decided against removing 2 patients with diagnosed dementia because some patients may have a clinical dementia that was not reflected in ICD-9 codes. Excluding these 2 patients did not affect any of our conclusions. If underdiagnosed dementia is more common in patients with diabetes than in the rest of the sample, this finding could explain some of the observed association between glycemic control and memory and executive functioning. We are not aware of evidence for differential misdiagnosis by diabetes status, which makes confounding unlikely. A test of this explanation awaits the completion of follow-up to ascertain each participant’s dementia status.
Acknowledgments
The Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test with Immediate Recall is copyrighted by the Albert Einstein College of Medicine, and 1 author (EG) receives a small percentage of any royalties on the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test with Immediate Recall when it is used for commercial purposes. The authors thank Radisel Paulino for her assistance in patient screening and Franklin Duckett for his assistance is obtaining patient medical data.
Funding
Research was supported in part by funding from the National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities, P60MD000514.
Biographies
Ellen Grober, PhD, is Clinical Professor of Neurology and Neuropsychology at the Albert Einstein College of Medicine and Montefiore Medical Center in the Bronx, NY.
Charles B. Hall, PhD, is Professor of Epidemiology and Population Health and Professor of Neurology at the Albert Einstein College of Medicine in the Bronx, NY.
Steven R. Hahn, MD, is Professor of Clinical Medicine and Instructor in Psychiatry at Albert Einstein College of Medicine and Director of the Primary Care Track of the Residency Program in Internal Medicine at Jacobi Medical Center in the Bronx, NY.
Richard B. Lipton, MD, is Professor and Vice Chair of Neurology at the Albert Einstein College of Medicine and Director of the Einstein Aging Study in the Bronx, NY.
Footnotes
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
References
- 1.Alzheimer’s Association. 2009 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 2009;5:234–270. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2009.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Brookmeyer R. Forecasting the global prevalence and burden of Alzheimer’s Disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2007;3:S168. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.04.381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cowie CC, Rust KF, Byrd-Holt DD, et al. Prevalence of Diabetes and High Risk for Diabetes Using A1C Criteria in the U.S. Population in 1988–2006. Diabetes Care. 2010;33:562–568. doi: 10.2337/dc09-1524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Division of Diabetes Translation. [Accessed March 30, 2011];National Diabetes Fact Sheet. 2011 http://www.cdc.gov/diabetes/pubs/pdf/ndfs_2011.pdf.
- 5.Alzheimer’s Association. 2010 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 2010;6:158. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2010.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Strachan MW. RD Lawrence Lecture 2010. The brain as a target organ in Type 2 diabetes: exploring the links with cognitive impairment and dementia. Diabet Med. 2011;28:141. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-5491.2010.03199.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Crandall J. Type 2 diabetes: geriatric considerations. In: Muller-Wielnad G, editor. Type 2 Diabetes: Principles and Practice. 2. 2008. pp. 479–488. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Munshi M, Grande L, Hayes M, et al. Cognitive dysfunction is associated with poor diabetes control in older adults. Diabetes Care. 2006;29:1794–1799. doi: 10.2337/dc06-0506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Okura T, Heisler M, Langa KM. Association between cognitive function and social support with glycemic control in adults with diabetes mellitus. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009;57:1816. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2009.02431.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cukierman-Yaffe T, Gerstein HC, Williamson JD, et al. Relationship between baseline glycemic control and cognitive function in individuals with type 2 diabetes and other cardiovascular risk factors. Diabetes Care. 2009;32:221–226. doi: 10.2337/dc08-1153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Whitmer RA, Karter AJ, Yaffe K, Quesenberry CP, Selby JV. Hypoglycemic episodes and risk of dementia in older patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus. JAMA. 2009;301:1565–1572. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Callahan CM, Hendrie HC, Tierney WM. Documentation and evaluation of cognitive impairment in elderly primary care patients. Ann Intern Med. 1995;122:422–429. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-122-6-199503150-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.California Healthcare Foundation/American Geriatrics Society Panel in Improving Care for Elders with Diabetes C. Guidelines for improving the care of the older persons with diabetes mellitus. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51:S265. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.51.5s.1.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Grober E, Buschke H. Genuine memory deficits in dementia. DevNeuropsychol. 1987;3:13–36. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Grober E, Sanders A, Hall C, Lipton RB. Free and cued selective reminding identifies very mild dementia in primary care. Alzheimer Dis AssocDisord. 2010;24:284–290. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181cfc78b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Grober E, Lipton RB, Hall C, Crystal H. Memory impairment on free and cued selective reminding predicts dementia. Neurology. 2000;54:827–832. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.4.827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Grober E, Hall C, Lipton RB, Zonderman A, Resnick S, Kawas C. Memory impairment, executive dysfunction, and intellectual decline. J Intl Neuropsychol Assoc. 2008;14:266–278. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Sarazin M, Berr C, De Rotrou J, et al. Amnestic syndrome of the medial temporal type identifies prodromal AD: a longitudinal study. Neurology. 2007;69:1859–1867. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000279336.36610.f7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Pillon B, Deweer B, Agid Y, Dubois B. Explicit memory in Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s diseases. Arch Neurol. 1993;50:374–379. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1993.00540040036010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Traykov L, Baudic S, Raoux N, et al. Patterns of memory impairment and perseverative behavior discriminate early Alzheimer’s disease from subcortical vascular dementia. J Neurol Sci. 2005:229–230. 75–79. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2004.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Knopman DS, Donohue JA, Gutterman EM. Patterns of care in the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease: impediments to timely diagnosis. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48:300–304. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2000.tb02650.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Valcour VG, Masaki KH, Curb D, Blanchette PL. The detection of dementia in the primary care setting. Arch Intern Med. 2000;160:2964–2968. doi: 10.1001/archinte.160.19.2964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Grober E, Hall C, McGinn M, et al. Neuropsychological strategies for detecting early dementia. J Intl Neuropsychol Soc. 2008;14:130–142. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Grober E, Hall C, Lipton RB, Teresi J. Primary care screen for early dementia. J Am GeriatrSoc. 2008;56:206–213. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01553.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Grober E, Ocepek-Welikson K, Teresi J. The Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test: evidence of psychometric adequacy. PsycholSci Q. 2009;51:266–282. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bodenheimer T, Lorig K, Holman H, Grumbach K. Patient self-management of chronic disease in primary care. JAMA. 2002;288:2469–2475. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.19.2469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Von Korff M, Gruman J, Schaefer J, Curry SJ, Wagner EH. Collaborative management of chronic illness. Ann Intern Med. 1997;127:1097–1102. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-127-12-199712150-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]