Skip to main content
American Journal of Public Health logoLink to American Journal of Public Health
. 2013 May;103(5):777–780. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.301056

Mental Illness Stigma, Help Seeking, and Public Health Programs

Claire Henderson 1,, Sara Evans-Lacko 1, Graham Thornicroft 1
PMCID: PMC3698814  PMID: 23488489

Abstract

Globally, more than 70% of people with mental illness receive no treatment from health care staff. Evidence suggests that factors increasing the likelihood of treatment avoidance or delay before presenting for care include (1) lack of knowledge to identify features of mental illnesses, (2) ignorance about how to access treatment, (3) prejudice against people who have mental illness, and (4) expectation of discrimination against people diagnosed with mental illness. In this article, we reviewed the evidence on whether large-scale anti-stigma campaigns could lead to increased levels of help seeking.


INCREASING EVIDENCE SUGgests that significantly greater barriers exist to receipt of mental health care in comparison with physical health care. Worldwide, more than 70% of young people and adults with mental illness do not receive any mental health treatment from health care staff.1 The difference between true prevalence and treated prevalence can be called the treatment gap.2 This article describes the roles that stigma and discrimination contribute to the treatment gap3,4 and assesses the evidence that public health approaches to stigma and discrimination can facilitate access to mental health care. We present new data from the evaluation of Time to Change, England’s largest ever program to reduce mental illness stigma and discrimination.5

DISCRIMINATION, STIGMA, AND MENTAL HEALTH CARE ACCESS

The relationship between stigma and discrimination and access to care is multifaceted; stigma and discrimination can impede access at institutional (legislation, funding, and availability of services),6–8 community (public attitudes and behaviors),9 and individual levels.10a Descriptive studies and epidemiological surveys suggest potent factors that increase the likelihood of treatment avoidance, delays to care, and discontinuation of service use include (1) lack of knowledge about the features and treatability of mental illnesses, (2) ignorance about how to access assessment and treatment, (3) prejudice against people who have mental illness, and (4) expectations of discrimination against people who have a diagnosis of mental illness.

Addressing public stigma might reduce experienced and anticipated stigma among services users and facilitate help seeking and engagement with mental health care. For example, individual service users living in countries with higher rates of help seeking and treatment utilization, in addition to better perceived access to information about how to deal with mental health problems and less stigmatizing attitudes, tended to have lower rates of self-stigma and perceived discrimination.10b Globally, however, stigmatizing attitudes persist among the public and have been shown to be prevalent11–13 and associated with a reluctance to seek help.14–16 Specifically, beliefs about effectiveness of treatment and services at the start of treatment have been shown to influence subsequent treatment behavior.17–19 This is significant because currently individuals often only access services once they have already experienced significant impairment, clinical symptoms, and stigma, and these effects may be difficult to reverse.

Stigma and discrimination and their influence on access to care may vary based on experience of mental distress or other sociodemographic factors. For instance, psychotic disorders are highly stigmatizing, and people with psychosis are more likely to be perceived as violent and unpredictable relative to people with other mental health problems. This can lead to high levels of experienced and anticipated discrimination in health care settings.20,21 Moreover, substance abuse is consistently associated with high rates of public stigma and institutional discrimination that may discourage individuals with substance abuse problems from getting health care; these individuals fear poor treatment by health care providers or trouble with the authorities.22 Multiple stigma among specific subpopulations may also exacerbate barriers to care. Different ethnic groups may have different histories and experiences with the health care system, and therefore, certain barriers may be more prevalent among individuals of different ethnic groups.23–25 For example, negative experiences of coercion in mental health care may be more prevalent among ethnic minorities.26 As a result, it has been suggested that future research should investigate subgroups and potential interactions between subgroups and on help-seeking attitudes and behavior.

IMPACT OF PUBLIC HEALTH PROGRAMS ON HELP SEEKING

Because of the complex multifaceted nature of stigma and discrimination and the subsequent barriers associated with accessing care, the solutions for reducing stigma and discrimination and facilitating access to care will need to be equally diverse.27 In the United Kingdom, there are related but separate national programs to reduce stigma and discrimination in Scotland, England, and Wales. Each of these anti-stigma programs consists of multiple components aimed at specific target groups (e.g., the media, young people) and at the general public, and operates at multiple levels (i.e., national social marketing campaigns and regional activities, such as those based on support from stakeholders), and at the level of small community groups funded to carry out local anti-discrimination work. Similar programs are also running in New Zealand (Like Minds Like Mine), Canada (Opening Minds), and Denmark (One of Us). No data are available regarding any increase in access to mental health care over the course of these programs, although it should be noted that an increase was observed over the course of a smaller scale mental health awareness program carried out in Nigeria.28 The lack of a control group makes it difficult to interpret the extent of any change as being the result of such programs,29 especially if there are contemporaneous policy and service developments. In Australia, however, there was variation among states and territories in the utilization of the depression program Beyondblue, allowing comparison of knowledge and attitudes toward treatment of depression to be compared across these areas.29 Although these data suggested a positive impact of Beyondblue on attitudes toward help seeking and treatment, no data from Australia are available on whether help seeking itself increased.

In England, the Time to Change program began in 2007, and the social marketing campaign started in January 2009.5 The second phase of Time to Change began in October 2011, and will run until March 2015. The evaluation of Time to Change is carried out by the United Kingdom's Institute of Psychiatry at King’s College London. Again, the lack of a control group did not allow us to determine whether help seeking increased as a result of Time to Change. However, questions about intended help seeking were included before the start of Time to Change in the Department of Health Attitudes to Mental Illness Survey, a nationally representative survey which has been ongoing since 1994.30 This survey thus provides a tool to evaluate the Time to Change campaign.

Using data from the survey, we found that mental health knowledge predicted intentions to seek help for a mental illness and to disclose such an illness to family and friends, which underlines the importance of mental health literacy.31 This applied to two types of knowledge measured by the Mental Health Knowledge Schedule.32 The first was knowledge that might influence subsequent mental health-related attitudes and behaviors. This type of knowledge was found to predict help seeking and disclosure more strongly than either attitude factor present in this survey. The second was whether major psychiatric disorders (depression, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder) were considered mental illnesses, which was associated with help-seeking intentions from a primary care physician.33

Attitudes toward mental illness showed a more mixed pattern with respect to help seeking and disclosure intentions. A factor analysis of the shortened version of the Community Attitudes Toward the Mentally Ill scale,34 used in the Department of Health Attitudes to Mental Illness Survey, suggested that intentions to seek help for a mental health problem were associated with attitudes of tolerance and support for community care, but not with stigmatizing attitudes of prejudice and exclusion. These findings suggested that the presence of strong positive attitudes might be more relevant to help seeking and disclosure than the absence of negative attitudes.

The preceding findings suggested that if social marketing campaigns were effective at improving knowledge and positive attitudes, they would result in increased intentions toward help seeking. However, it was also possible that awareness of the campaign affected help-seeking intentions through some other mechanism. For the 2012 Attitudes To Mental Illness Survey, we included questions to assess awareness of the Time to Change social marketing campaign so that we could directly examine the relationship between campaign awareness and intended help seeking and disclosure to friends or family. Table 1 describes the prevalence of intended help seeking by sample characteristics. Prevalence of intended help seeking ranged from 79% to 89% regardless of sociodemographic characteristics, campaign awareness, or familiarity with mental health problems through knowing someone.

TABLE 1—

Prevalence of Intended Help Seeking by Sample Characteristics: England, Department of Health Attitudes to Mental Illness Survey, 2012

Characteristic Intended Help Seeking, Unweighted No. (Weighted %) No Intended Help Seeking, Unweighted No. (Weighted %)
Campaign awareness
 Yes 423 (84.4) 74 (15.6)
 No 1008 (81.9) 212 (18.1)
Gender
 Female 790 (85.3) 134 (14.7)
 Male 641 (79.9) 152 (20.1)
Age, y
 16–24 206 (80.3) 52 (19.7)
 25–34 225 (75.7) 65 (72.3)
 35–44 236 (81.3) 54 (18.7)
 45–54 206 (85.2) 32 (14.8)
 55–64 235 (86.5) 33 (13.5)
 65–74 181 (89.0) 22 (11.0)
 ≥ 75 142 (83.9) 28 (16.1)
Ethnicity
 Asian 128 (78.8) 32 (21.2)
 Black 55 (81.4) 12 (18.6)
 Other 25 (80.0) 6 (20.0)
 White 1215 (83.2) 234 (16.8)
Socioeconomic statusa
 AB = highest income 249 (85.3) 43 (15.7)
 C1 = higher middle income 368 (79.6) 88 (20.4)
 C2 = lower middle income 315 (85.2) 53 (14.8)
 DE = lowest income 499 (82.9) 102 (17.1)
Familiarity with mental health problems
 Self 98 (89.0) 13 (11.0)
 Other 781 (83.1) 145 (16.9)
 None 530 (81.8) 115 (18.2)

Note. The sample size was n = 1717. Regarding the table title, the exact question wording was: “If you felt that you had a mental health problem, how likely would you be to go to your general physician for help?”

a

Categories used are those maintained by the UK Market Research Society and based on the National Readership Survey’s Social Grades. The classes are based on the chief income earner's occupation:

A = upper middle class: higher managerial, administrative or professional

B = middle class: intermediate managerial, administrative or professional

C1 = lower middle class: supervisory or clerical and junior managerial, administrative or professional

C2 = skilled working class: skilled manual workers

D = working class: semi- and unskilled manual workers

E = those at the lowest levels of subsistence: Casual or lowest grade workers, pensioners and others who depend on the welfare state for their income.

Table 2 shows the results of multivariable logistic regression that examined the relationship between campaign awareness and help seeking and disclosure, controlling for sociodemographic characteristics and familiarity with mental health problems. We found no relationship between campaign awareness and intended help seeking. For disclosure to family and friends, the unadjusted results suggested a marginally negative relationship; however, there was no relationship after adjustment. It was possible that those who were uncomfortable with discussing a mental health problem with friends and family were more likely to remember the campaign, which in 2012 emphasized the need to be more open in discussing mental health problems (It’s Time to Talk). For both items, we found positive relationships with being female; for the help-seeking item, we also found a negative relationship for the age category 25 to 34 years, which included some of Time to Change’s campaign target group of those aged 25 to 45 years with middle incomes.

TABLE 2—

Multivariable Logistic Regression of Predictors of Intended Help Seeking From Primary Care and Disclosure to Family or Friends: United Kingdom, Department of Health Attitudes to Mental Illness Survey, 2012

Help Seeking From Primary Care
Disclosure to Family or Friends
Predictors Unadjusted OR (95% CI) Adjusted OR (95% CI) Unadjusted OR (95% CI) Adjusted OR (95% CI)
Campaign awareness
 Yes 1.15 (0.84, 1.56) 1.11 (0.81, 1.52) 0.78 (0.60, 1.01) 0.88 (0.67, 1.16)
 No (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
Gender
 Female 1.56* (1.19, 2.05) 1.53* (1.16, 2.03) 0.91 (0.72, 1.15) 0.99 (0.78, 1.25)
 Male (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
Age, y
 16–24 0.72 (0.42, 1.23) 0.76 (0.43, 1.32) 1.32 (0.83, 2.08) 1.17 (0.73, 1.87)
 25–34 0.54* (0.32, 0.91) 0.56* (0.33, 0.95) 1.17 (0.74, 1.84) 1.10 (0.68, 1.76)
 35–44 0.75 (0.44, 1.29) 0.75 (0.44, 1.30) 0.86 (0.54, 1.38) 0.86 (0.53, 1.40)
 45–54 1.04 (0.57, 1.89) 1.03 (0.56, 1.88) 0.66 (0.40, 1.10) 0.70 (0.42, 1.18)
 55–64 1.12 (0.63, 2.02) 1.15 (0.64, 2.05) 0.92 (0.57, 1.47) 0.98 (0.60, 1.58)
 65–74 1.35 (0.72, 2.53) 1.34 (0.71, 2.53) 1.03 (0.63, 1.69) 1.06 (0.65, 1.75)
 ≥ 75 (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
Ethnicity
 Asian 0.74 (0.47, 1.15) 1.01 (0.61, 1.66) 2.53* (1.78, 3.61) 2.29* (1.55, 3.37)
 Black 1.03 (0.50, 2.10) 1.16 (0.56, 2.43) 1.10 (0.61, 1.98) 1.06 (0.57, 1.94)
 Other 0.79 (0.30, 2.08) 0.89 (0.32, 2.46) 0.30 (0.07, 1.26) 0.31 (0.07, 1.32)
 White (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
Socioeconomic status
 AB = highest income 1.10 (0.72, 1.66) 1.12 (0.74, 1.71) 0.81 (0.57, 1.13) 0.83 (0.58, 1.19)
 C1 = higher middle income 0.78 (0.56, 1.09) 0.85 (0.60, 1.18) 0.87 (0.65, 1.17) 0.84 (0.62, 1.13)
 C2 = lower middle income 1.13 (0.77, 1.65) 1.24 (0.84, 1.82) 0.86 (0.63, 1.17) 0.85 (0.62, 1.17)
 DE = lowest income (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
Familiarity with mental health problems
 Self 1.80 (0.95. 3.41) 1.68 (0.86, 3.30) 0.26* (0.13, 0.48) 0.33* (0.17, 0.64)
 Other 1.10 (0.83, 1.45) 0.97 (0.70, 1.35) 0.77 (0.60, 0.97) 0.99 (0.76 1.29)
 None (Ref) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00

Note. CI = confidence interval; OR = odds ratio. Regarding the table title, the exact question wording was: “If you felt that you had a mental health problem, how likely would you be to go to your general physician for help?”

*P ≤ .05.

Thus far, we considered initial help seeking; however, examination of the relationship between anti-stigma programs and help seeking should investigate initial and subsequent actions. Negative experiences with mental health professionals perceived to be discriminatory and discrimination experienced at the hands of others because of having a mental illness might deter individuals from seeking treatment. Therefore, it is hoped that programs such as Time to Change will lead to reductions in unfair treatment by both health professionals and others. Interim data from the Viewpoint survey35 suggested that between 2008 and 2009, after the Time to Change social marketing campaign began in January 2009, the overall level of discrimination fell. This was accounted for by reduced discrimination from a number of sources, including friends, family, dates, neighbors, employers, and education professionals. However, there was no reduction in reports of discrimination from either mental health professionals or physical health care professionals. This suggested that even if Time to Change were to increase initial treatment seeking, that is, if public knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors improved, a lack of reduction in the risk of negative experiences with health professionals would continue to deter people from seeking further help.

Acknowledgments

Data collection for this article was funded by the Big Lottery Fund; Comic Relief and SHiFT (Shifting attitudes to mental illness), UK Government Department of Health, through their funding of the Time to Change program. G. Thornicroft and C. Henderson were funded in relation to a National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Programme Grant for Applied Research awarded to the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, and G. Thornicroft was funded in relation to the NIHR Specialist Mental Health Biomedical Research Centre at the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London and the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust. C. Henderson was also funded by a grant from Guy’s and St Thomas Charity, a grant from the Maudsley Charity, and a NIHR Programme Grant for Applied Research awarded to Camden and Islington NHS Foundation Trust. G. Thornicroft received grants for stigma-related research in the past 5 years from Lundbeck UK, and from the National Institute for Health Research, and has acted as a consultant to the UK Office of the Chief Scientist.

We thank Sue Baker, Maggie Gibbons, and Paul Farmer, from Mind, Paul Corry and Mark Davies from Rethink Mental Illness, and Gillian Taylor from TNS BMRB, for their collaboration.

References

  • 1.Thornicroft G. Most people with mental illness are not treated. Lancet. 2007;370(9590):807–808 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Dua T, Barbui C, Clark Net al. Evidence-based guidelines for mental, neurological, and substance use disorders in low- and middle-income countries: summary of WHO recommendations. PLoS Med. 2011;8(11):e1001122 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Patel V, Koschorke M, Prince M. Closing the Treatment Gap for Mental Disorders. Routledge Handbook of Global Public Health. London, UK: Taylor & Francis; 2011:385–393 [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Thornicroft G. Physical health disparities and mental illness: the scandal of premature mortality. Br J Psychiatry. 2011;199(6):441–442 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Henderson C, Thornicroft G. Stigma and discrimination in mental illness: Time to Change. Lancet. 2009;373(9679):1928–1930 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Corrigan PW, Watson AC. Factors that explain how policy makers distribute resources to mental health services. Psychiatr Serv. 2003;54(4):501–507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Corrigan PW, Watson AC, Warpinski AC, Gracia G. Stigmatizing attitudes about mental illness and allocation of resources to mental health services. Community Ment Health J. 2004;40(4):297–307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Corrigan PW, Markowitz FE, Watson AC. Structural levels of mental illness stigma and discrimination. Schizophr Bull. 2004;30(3):481–491 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Evans-Lacko SE, Baum N, Danis M, Biddle A, Goold S. Laypersons’ choices and deliberations for mental health coverage. Adm Policy Ment Health. 2012;39(3):158–169 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10a.Rüsch N, Corrigan PW, Wassel Aet al. Self-stigma, group identification, perceived legitimacy of discrimination and mental health service use. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195(6):551–552 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10b.Evans-Lacko S, Brohan E, Mojtabai RThornicroft G Association between public views of mental illness and self-stigma among individuals with mental illness in 14 European countries. Psychol Med. 2012;42(8):1741–1752 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Angermeyer MC, Dietrich S. Public beliefs about and attitudes towards people with mental illness: a review of population studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2006;113(3):163–179 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Angermeyer MC, Holzinger A, Matschinger H. Mental health literacy and attitude towards people with mental illness: a trend analysis based on population surveys in the eastern part of Germany. Eur Psychiatry. 2009;24(4):225–232 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Pescosolido BA, Martin JK, Long JS, Medina TR, Phelan JC, Link BG. “A disease like any other”? A decade of change in public reactions to schizophrenia, depression, and alcohol dependence. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(11):1321–1330 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Barney LJ, Griffiths KM, Jorm AF, Christensen H. Stigma about depression and its impact on help-seeking intentions. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2006;40(1):51–54 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Thornicroft G. Stigma and discrimination limit access to mental health care. Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc. 2008;17(1):14–19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wrigley S, Jackson H, Judd F, Komiti A. Role of stigma and attitudes toward help-seeking from a general practitioner for mental health problems in a rural town. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2005;39(6):514–521 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Sirey JA, Bruce ML, Alexopoulos GSet al. Perceived stigma as a predictor of treatment discontinuation in young and older outpatients with depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(3):479–481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.DosReis S, Mychailyszyn M, Evans-Lacko S, Riley A, Myers M. The meaning of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder medication to parents' initiation and continuity of treatment for their child. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2009;19(4):377–383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Sirey JA, Bruce ML, Alexopoulos GS, Perlick DA, Friedman SJ, Meyers BS. Stigma as a barrier to recovery: perceived stigma and patient-rated severity of illness as predictors of antidepressant drug adherence. Psychiatr Serv. 2001;52(12):1615–1620 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Thornicroft G, Brohan E, Rose D, Sartorius N, Leese M. Global pattern of experienced and anticipated discrimination against people with schizophrenia: a cross-sectional survey. Lancet. 2009;373(9661):408–415 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hamilton S, Pinfold V, Rose Det al. The effect of disclosure of mental illness by interviewers on reports of discrimination experienced by service users: a randomized study. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2011;23(1):47–54 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ahern J, Stuber J, Galea S. Stigma, discrimination and the health of illicit drug users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;88(2–3):188–196 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Leaf PJ, Livingston MM, Tischler GL, Weissman MM, Holzer CE, III, Myers JK. Contact with health professionals for the treatment of psychiatric and emotional problems. Med Care. 1985;23(12):1322–1337 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Shefer G, Rose D, Nellums L, Thornicroft G, Henderson C, Evans-Lacko C. ‘Our community is the worst’: the influence of cultural beliefs on stigma, relationships with family, and help seeking in three ethnic communities in London. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 2012; [e-pub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Knifton L, Gervais M, Newbigging Ket al. Community conversation: addressing mental health stigma with ethnic minority communities. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2010;45(4):497–504 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Bindman J, Reid Y, Szmukler G, Tiller J, Thornicroft G, Leese M. Perceived coercion at admission to psychiatric hospital and engagement with follow-up–a cohort study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2005;40(2):160–166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Clement S, Brohan E, Jeffery D, Henderson C, Hatch SL, Thornicroft G. Development and psychometric properties the Barriers to Access to Care Evaluation scale (BACE) related to people with mental ill health. BMC Psychiatry. 2012;12:36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Eaton J, Agomoh AO. Developing mental health services in Nigeria: the impact of a community-based mental health awareness programme. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2008;43(7):552–558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jorm AF, Christensen H, Griffiths KM. The impact of beyondblue: the national depression initiative on the Australian public’s recognition of depression and beliefs about treatments. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2005;39(4):248–254 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mehta N, Kassam A, Leese M, Butler G, Thornicroft G. Public attitudes towards people with mental illness in England and Scotland, 1994-2003. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;194(3):278–284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Rüsch N, Evans-Lacko S, Henderson C, Flach C, Thornicroft G. Public knowledge and attitudes as predictors of help-seeking and disclosure in mental illness. Psychiatr Serv. 2011;62(6):675–678 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Evans-Lacko S, Little K, Meltzer Het al. Development and psychometric properties of the Mental Health Knowledge Schedule. Can J Psychiatry. 2010;55(7):440–448 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rüsch N, Evans-Lacko S, Thornicroft G. What is a mental illness? Public views and their effects on attitudes and disclosure. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2012;46(7):641–650 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Taylor SM, Dear MJ. Scaling community attitudes toward the mentally ill. Schizophr Bull. 1981;7(2):225–240 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Henderson C, Corker E, Lewis-Holmes Eet al. Reducing mental health related stigma and discrimination in England: one year outcomes of the Time to Change Programme for service user-rated experiences of discrimination. Psychiatr Serv. 2012;63(5):451–457 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from American Journal of Public Health are provided here courtesy of American Public Health Association

RESOURCES