Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Addict Dis. 2013;32(2):206–216. doi: 10.1080/10550887.2013.795471

Relationship of Age to Impulsivity and Decision-Making: A Baseline Secondary Analysis of a Behavioral Treatment Study in Stimulant Use Disorders

Raj K Kalapatapu 1,2, Daniel F Lewis 3, Sophia Vinogradov 1,2, Steven L Batki 1,2, Theresa Winhusen 3
PMCID: PMC3703837  NIHMSID: NIHMS473015  PMID: 23815427

Abstract

Since stimulant use disorders (SUDs) remain prevalent across the lifespan, cognition is an important area of clinical care and research focus among aging adults with SUDs. This secondary analysis of a National Institute on Drug Abuse Clinical Trials Network study suggests that decision-making, verbal learning/memory, executive function and set shifting are important cognitive domains to screen clinically and treat in aging adults with SUDs. Some suggestions are made on how clinical treatment providers can practically use these results. An important direction for future research is the development of cognitively remediating treatments for impaired cognitive domains in aging adults with SUDs.

Keywords: cocaine, methamphetamine, aging, addiction, neurocognitive

Introduction

Stimulant use disorders (SUDs) remain a prevalent public health problem across the lifespan13. One important research area in the treatment of SUDs is cognition47, since cognitive changes are associated with clinical outcomes such as abstinence and treatment completion8, 9. The cognitive domains typically involved in SUDs include attention, memory, executive function1013, impulsivity14, 15 and decision-making1618. As SUDs affect individuals across the lifespan and as evidence shows that SUDs remain prevalent even as adults grow older1, 1922, knowledge of age-specific cognitive differences in individuals with SUDs might help inform the development of age-specific cognitive treatments.

Impulsivity and decision-making are important cognitive domains affected by aging2328. Some literature shows that impulsivity and decision-making improve with increasing age, such as in the context of borderline personality disorder29, 30. Other literature shows poorer impulsivity and decision-making with increasing age24, 27. Because of brain abnormalities in a middle-aged adult with a SUD that are not typically found in a healthy middle-aged adult31, 32, changes in cognitive domains in a middle-aged adult with a SUD may be greater than the changes in cognitive domains expected in a healthy middle-aged adult. Thus, additional literature on the interaction between age and impulsivity and between age and decision-making might help develop age-specific treatments targeting these important domains in individuals with SUDs.

There is limited research exploring the relationship between age and the domains of attention, memory, and executive function20, 33 in SUDs. However, there is even more limited research on the relationship between age and impulsivity and between age and decision-making in SUDs. One study of pathological gamblers, a classic disorder of impulsivity, found 18.2% of the males and 14.8% of the females in the older sub-group to report ever using cocaine34. In another study of participants who reported using methamphetamine at least twice in the past 2 months, participants in the high impulsivity group were younger than those in the low impulsivity group35.

A recently published behavioral treatment study (n = 183) of individuals with SUDs, which included a baseline neurocognitive battery containing impulsivity and decision-making measures36, can help add to the literature on the relationship between age and impulsivity and between age and decision-making in SUDs. Exploring this relationship can help theoretically understand how the aging process, in the context of concurrent SUDs, affects the underlying brain regions responsible for complex cognitive domains like impulsivity and decision-making (e.g., orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex)18, 37. Exploring this relationship can also help clinically develop cognitively remediating treatments (pharmacological and/or non-pharmacological) specific to affected cognitive domains4, 6, 7 in aging adults with SUDs. Thus, the aim of this secondary analysis was to assess the association of age on baseline measures of impulsivity and decision-making, among other neurocognitive measures. We hypothesized that increasing age would be associated with greater impulsivity and poorer decision-making.

Methods

Study Setting and Measures

Full details of the parent study 38 and the ancillary study 36 used for this analysis are described elsewhere. Briefly, the National Institute on Drug Abuse Clinical Trials Network (NIDA CTN) ancillary study enrolled 183 adult participants from 6 substance abuse community treatment programs nationwide. Participants had a current diagnosis of stimulant abuse or dependence based on the DSM-IV Checklist39, endorsed methamphetamine or cocaine as their primary drug of choice, were seeking outpatient substance use disorder treatment, used stimulants in the prior 60 days, and were medically and psychiatrically stable enough for participation based on medical history and the Addiction Severity Index-Lite interview40. Participants were randomized to Stimulant Abuser Groups to Engage in 12-Step (STAGE-12)38 or treatment as usual (TAU). All participants provided informed consent, and the study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the participating sites.

Demographic variables included age, sex, race, ethncity, education, marital status, and employment status. Substance use variables included substance use disorder diagnosis, age of onset of illicit stimulant use, years of illicit stimulant use, years of non-stimulant use, and route of illicit stimulant use. The Patient Health Questionnaire [PHQ41] was used to assess for depression, panic, and non-panic anxiety disorders.

The baseline neurocognitive measures assessed: impulsivity and decision-making [Barratt Impulsiveness Scale version-11 (BIS-11), Frontal Systems Behavioral Scale (FrSBe), Comalli-Kaplan version of the Stroop Color Word Task (C-K Stroop), Iowa Gambling Task (IGT)], verbal learning/memory [Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT)], executive function and set shifting [Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST)].

The self-report BIS-11 is designed to assess the personality/behavioral construct of impulsiveness in three sub-domains: attention, motor, non-planning42. A total score and a score for each sub-domain were calculated; higher scores indicate greater impulsiveness. The self-report version of the FrSBe is a brief, valid, and reliable assessment of three areas of functioning associated with the pre-frontal cortex: apathy, disinhibition, and executive dysfunction43. An overall T-score and a T-score for each area of functioning were calculated; higher scores indicate poorer functioning. The C-K Stroop is an experimenter-administered measure that assesses impulsivity and response inhibition44, 45. Interference errors, Interference time (in seconds), and a Derived Interference score (Interference minus color naming) were calculated; higher scores indicate poorer performance.

The IGT is a computerized gambling exercise that simulates real-life decision making via selection of 100 cards from four decks in 5 trial blocks46. A Net Total T-score reflects a summary score, and each Net 1–5 T-score reflects a block of 20 cards; higher scores indicate the participant is more often choosing advantageous decks. Selection of cards 1–40 may be categorized as decision-making under ambiguity, and selection of cards 41–100 may be categorized as decision-making under risk47, 48.

The RAVLT is an experimenter-administered measure of verbal learning and memory49. Scores for Learning (sum of Trials I to V of List A), Trial B (free recall of Interference list B), and Trial VI (free recall of List A after Interference List B) were calculated; higher scores indicate better performance. The WCST is a computerized test of executive function and set shifting50. T-scores for perseverative, nonperseverative and total errors, as well as perseverative responses, were calculated; higher scores indicate better performance.

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics were conducted on the demographic, clinical and neurocognitive data. The Spearman’s rho, a non-parametric test, assessed correlation between age and each neurocognitive measure. We analyzed all overall and sub-domain scores from all neurocognitive measures, because age can potentially affect various cognitive domains5153. After controlling for sex, years of education, race (Caucasian or African-American), ethnicity (Hispanic or Non-Hispanic), years of illicit stimulant use, route of illicit stimulant use, and substance use disorder diagnosis as indicated by the Corrected Akaike Information Criteria (AIC-C)54, 55, the Wald statistic assessed the contribution of age in each regression analysis. We controlled for sex, as hormonal factors may mediate sex differences in impulsivity56. We also controlled for other demographic features (years of education, race, ethnicity), since such factors can influence performance on neurocognitive measures57. Since this was an exploratory secondary analysis, p-values < 0.05 were considered significant. All analyses were conducted using SAS version 9.3 (Cary, NC).

Results

Table 1 presents baseline demographic and clinical data. Table 2 presents baseline neurocognitive data, and Table 3 presents the contribution of age to the neurocognitive data. The age range of the sample was 19–60, primarily being late 30’s. Most participants were female, high school educated, unmarried, and unemployed. Most participants had a primary cocaine use disorder diagnosis and used smoking as their primary route of illicit stimulant use. Most participants also had a secondary alcohol use disorder diagnosis and smoked cigarettes. The mean values for the neurocognitive data in Table 2 are consistent with previous research (see Discussion).

Table 1.

Baseline demographic and clinical data.

Demographic & Clinical Data (n = 183) Mean (S.D.) or %
Age 38.6 (9.3), range 19 – 60, median 38.4
Age group 19–30 n = 47
Age group 31–40 n = 58
Age group 41–50 n = 66
Age group 51–60 n = 12
Male 31.7%
Racea Caucasian 43.4%
African-American 46.2%
Other/Mixed 10.4%
Hispanic 5.5%b
Years of Education 12.0 (1.6)
Currently married 22.7%c
Employed in last 30 days 23%
Age of 1st illicit stimulant use 20.8 (6.1)
Years of illicit stimulant use 12.2 (7.6)
Years of non-stimulant use 14.8 (10.5)
Smoking as primary route of illicit stimulant use 72.4%c
Stimulant use disorder diagnosisb Methamphetamine 25.8%
Cocaine 68.7%
Both 5.5%
Secondary substance use disorder diagnosis Alcohol 60.7%
Marijuana 38.3%
Opiate 18%
Benzodiazepine 8.2%
Currently smoking cigarettes 79.2%
PHQd Depression 27.3%e
PHQ Panic disorder 17.8%f
PHQ Non-panic anxiety disorder 17.2%f
a

n = 182 due to missing data

b

n = 182 due to missing data

c

n = 181 due to missing data

d

PHQ = Patient Health Questionnaire

e

n = 176 due to missing data

f

n = 180 due to missing data

Table 2.

Baseline neurocognitive data.

Neurocognitive measure (n = 183) Mean (S.D.)
BIS-11 Attention impulsiveness total score 17.7 (3.5)
Motor impulsiveness total score 25.2 (4.3)
Non-planning impulsiveness total score 24.5 (4.6)
Total score 67.4 (8.9)
FrSBe Current – Apathy T-score 76.9 (12.9)
Current – Disinhibition T-score 77.5 (17.5)
Current – Executive dysfunction T-score 73.0 (13.7)
Current – Total T-score 79.4 (15.7)
C-K Stroop Interference errors 1.9 (3.8)
Interference time 118.5 (27.4)
Derived interference score 54.0 (22.4)
IGT Net Total T-score 45.0 (9.6)
Net 1 T-score 52.9 (11.5)
Net 2 T-score 47.6 (9.7)
Net 3 T-score 44.5 (11.4)
Net 4 T-score 43.4 (11.6)
Net 5 T-score 42.7 (12.5)
RAVLT Learning 45.1 (9.6)
Trial B 5.2 (1.6)
Trial VI 8.7 (3.0)
WCST Perseverative errors T-score 47.2 (13.7)
Nonperseverative errors T-score 42.3 (12.1)
Total errors T-score 43.5 (12.7)
Perseverative responses T-score 48.2 (14.4)

Table 3.

Baseline adjusted neurocognitive dataa – association with age.

Neurocognitive measure Parameter estimate with 95% confidence interval Type III Wald Chi-Squareb p-value Spearman’s rhoc
BIS-11 Attention impulsiveness total score 0.04 (−0.03, 0.12) 1.14 0.28 −0.17
Motor impulsiveness total score −0.07 (−0.15, −0.006) 3.33 0.07 −0.24
Non-planning impulsiveness total score 0.03 (−0.05, −0.12) 0.65 0.42 −0.01
Total score −0.07 (−0.24, 0.10) 0.63 0.43 −0.17
FrSBe Current – Apathy T-score 0.22 (−0.04, 0.48) 2.75 0.10 0.20
Current – Disinhibition T-score 0.27 (−0.10, 0.65) 2.10 0.15 0.03
Current – Executive dysfunction T-score 0.28 (0.02, 0.54) 4.56 0.03 0.30
Current – Total T-score 0.37 (0.05, 0.68) 5.28 0.02 0.27
C-K Stroop Interference errors 0.05 (−0.01, 0.11) 2.36 0.12 0.21
Interference time 0.41 (−0.21, 1.03) 1.71 0.19 0.25
Derived interference score 0.19 (−0.33, 0.71) 0.52 0.47 0.20
IGT Net Total T-score 0.14 (−0.06, 0.34) 1.88 0.17 0.06
Net 1 T-score −0.29 (−0.52, −0.07) 6.57 0.01 −0.23
Net 2 T-score 0.09 (−0.10, 0.29) 0.88 0.35 0.03
Net 3 T-score 0.12 (−0.12, 0.35) 0.99 0.32 0.06
Net 4 T-score 0.24 (0.01, 0.48) 4.20 0.04 0.15
Net 5 T-score 0.25 (−0.002, 0.49) 3.85 0.049 0.16
RAVLT Learning −0.22 (−0.39, −0.05) 6.66 0.01 −0.33
Trial B 0.004 (−0.02, 0.03) 0.10 0.75 −0.13
Trial VI −0.08 (−0.14, −0.02) 6.40 0.01 −0.35
WCST Perseverative errors T-score −0.43 (−0.71, −0.15) 8.99 0.003 −0.37
Nonperseverative errors T-score −0.49 (−0.75, −0.23) 13.79 0.0002 −0.44
Total errors T-score −0.46 (−0.73, −0.18) 10.83 0.001 −0.42
Perseverative responses T-score −0.45 (−0.75, −0.15) 8.90 0.003 −0.38
a

Controlling for sex, years of education, race (Caucasian or African-American), ethnicity (Hispanic or Non-Hispanic), years of illicit stimulant use, route of illicit stimulant use, and substance use disorder diagnosis as indicated by the Corrected Akaike Information Criteria.

b

Degrees of freedom for all results = 1

c

Correlation between age and the respective neurocognitive measure. Calculated independent of regression.

BIS-11

Age was not significantly associated with any of the BIS-11 scores.

FrSBe

Age positively correlated with and was significantly associated with the current Executive dysfunction T-score (ρ = 0.30, p = 0.03) and the current Total T-score (ρ = 0.27, p = 0.02). Age was not significantly associated with the other FrSBe scores.

C-K Stroop

Age was not significantly associated with any of the C-K Stroop scores.

IGT

Age negatively correlated with and was significantly associated with the Net 1 T-score (ρ = −0.23, p = 0.01), and positively correlated with and was significantly associated with the Net 4 T-score (ρ = 0.15, p = 0.04) and Net 5 T-score (ρ = 0.16, p = 0.049). Age was not significantly associated with the other IGT scores.

RAVLT

Age negatively correlated with and was significantly associated with Trial VI (ρ = −0.35, p = 0.01) and Learning (ρ = −0.33, p = 0.01). Age was not significantly associated with Trial B.

WCST

Age negatively correlated with and was significantly associated with Perseverative errors T-score (ρ = −0.37, p = 0.003), Nonperseverative errors T-score (ρ = −0.44, p = 0.0002), Total errors T-score (ρ = −0.42, p = 0.001), and Perseverative responses T-score (ρ = −0.38, p = 0.003).

Discussion

The present analysis evaluated the association of age with impulsivity, decision-making, and other neurocognitive measures in cocaine- and/or methamphetamine-dependent patients. The results revealed that age was significantly associated with some of the scores on the FrSBe (positive correlation with executive dysfunction and total), IGT (Net 1 negative correlation, Net 4 and Net 5 positive correlation), and RAVLT (negative correlation with Trial VI and Learning). Age negatively correlated with and was significantly associated with all of the WCST scores. Age was not significantly associated with any of the BIS-11 and C-K Stroop scores. Thus, our hypotheses were partially supported.

Impulsivity and decision-making are complex cognitive domains that are not unidimensional42, 58. The neurocognitive battery used in this study measured these domains using four different measures (BIS-11, FrSBe, C-K Stroop, IGT), and the significant association with age to only some scores may be due to specific sub-domains or underlying neural pathways tapped by these measures. Also, in the presence of SUDs, the aging process may be differentially affecting brain regions, such as the orbitofrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate cortex, which might explain the significant association with age to only some scores; such brain regions are vulnerable to the aging process59, 60. However, since no neuroimaging was conducted in this study, it is hard to speculate too much about what underlying neural correlates explain the results. It is also important to note that the range of mean/S.D. values for each neurocognitive measure (Table 2) are similar to the range of mean/S.D. values found in previous research in those with SUDs and/or are worse than controls: BIS-1161, 62, FrSBe36, 63, C-K Stroop44, IGT64, RAVLT65, 66, and WCST67.

Studies comparing older and younger adults on the IGT have found conflicting results68. Our finding of age not correlating with the overall score is consistent with literature in healthy aging adults69. Our mixed findings on the Net 1 score versus the Net 4 and Net 5 scores may be reflecting the complex relationship between age and SUDs interacting with a change in learning and decision-making while completing the IGT70, 71. However, other literature has found differences of aging affecting decisions under ambiguity but not decisions under risk72. A future four-group study (older adults with SUDs, younger adults with SUDs, healthy older adults, healthy younger adults) using the IGT can help tease out this complex relationship.

The finding of age positively correlating with executive dysfunction on the FrSBe may be related to a loss of white matter structural integrity in the prefrontal cortex, which is found in both aging73 and SUDs74, 75. We were surprised to find age not correlating with any of the BIS-11 scores, as age and SUDs can affect the three sub-domains captured by the BIS-11. But, this finding is consistent with previous research on the BIS-1142. We were also surprised to find age not correlating with any of the C-K Stroop scores, but the sample’s age range may not have been large enough to see age-related differences on this measure. The domains tapped by the RAVLT and WCST are consistent with both aging literature5153, where verbal learning/memory and executive function are affected by aging.

This analysis has several strengths. First, we had a fairly large sample size for this analysis, as we benefited from data collected from 6 substance abuse community treatment programs nationwide. Second, the non-pure sample of participants allows these findings to be generalized to clinical samples, where participants don’t necessarily use one substance alone. Third, we were able to analyze four different measures of impulsivity and decision-making. Fourth, we controlled for sex, years of education, race, and ethnicity as indicated by the AIC-C, especially after a recent review76 warned about appropriate interpretations when examining neurocognitive data in SUDs. Finally, since age and years of substance use can be naturally positively associated and since years of illicit stimulant use can significantly affect cognition6, 7, we controlled for years of illicit stimulant use as indicated by the AIC-C in each regression analysis. As we still found significant results even after controlling for chronicity of illicit stimulant use, this further supports our position that a unique aging effect may be explaining these results over and above what is cognitively expected from chronicity of illicit stimulant use alone.

This analysis also has several limitations. First, the primary study was not specifically designed to assess the aims of this post-hoc analysis. Second, the amount of illicit stimulant use was not quantified in grams or dollar-value, which may have interacted with age to affect cognition. Third, the age span may not have been enough to see further age-related cognitive differences, since there were no individuals above age 60 in the sample. Fourth, a comprehensive neurocognitive battery was not conducted, which might also include an assessment of premorbid intelligence, attention, processing speed, and attentional bias. These untested cognitive domains may have been impaired and unknowingly influenced the tested cognitive domains. Finally, since this sample does not consist of participants with pure SUDs, the comorbid substance abuse and depressive/anxiety disorders (Table 2) may have contributed to inconsistent findings across impulsivity and decision-making.

Clinical treatment providers might consider practically using these results in a few ways when clinically managing an aging adult with an SUD. First, a neurocognitive assessment might be considered as a standard at an initial visit and at periodic follow-up visits (e.g., every 1 or 2 years). Previous research shows that even recognizing and screening for cognitive impairment in addiction populations is poor overall7781. Next, providers may consider adding adjunctive treatments in a patient’s treatment plan to directly remediate the impaired cognitive domains (e.g., decision-making, executive function). Examples of adjunctive cognitively remediating treatments that have shown efficacy in addiction and other populations include pharmacological interventions47 (e.g., cholinesterase inhibitors, nicotinic agonists) and non-pharmacological interventions8287 (e.g., computerized software, physical exercise). Finally, by capitalizing on unimpaired cognitive domains, treatment providers may modify their treatment approach to compensate for impaired cognitive domains. For example, instead of totally relying on verbal modalities, a provider may also consider using a visual modality (e.g., pictures, white board, computer screen) to convey information and compensate for a patient’s impairment in verbal learning/memory. Such compensatory cognitive techniques have shown efficacy in other medical8890 and psychiatric9194 populations.

Conclusions

This analysis suggests that decision-making, verbal learning/memory, executive function and set shifting are important cognitive domains to screen clinically and potentially treat in adults with SUDs who continue to abuse stimulants as they grow older. Future directions include potentially conducting pre/post neuroimaging of frontal cortical regions in aging adults with SUDs, correlating neuroimaging findings with neurocognitive measures in aging adults with SUDs, and developing cognitively remediating treatments (pharmacological and/or non-pharmacological) specific to affected cognitive domains (e.g., decision-making, verbal learning/memory, executive function, set shifting) in aging adults with SUDs.

Acknowledgments

The National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) contributed to the development of study design and initial protocol. Analysis, interpretation, manuscript preparation, and decision to submit the manuscript for publication was the sole responsibility of the authors. The publications committee of the NIDA Clinical Trials Network approved the final manuscript. The study in this manuscript was funded by the NIDA grant U10DA013732 (PI: Winhusen). Dr. Kalapatapu was funded by an Advanced Fellowship Program in Psychiatric Research/Neurosciences through the Veterans Affairs’ Office of Academic Affiliations.

Source of Funding:

The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the manuscript. The National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) contributed to the development of study design and initial protocol. Analysis, interpretation, manuscript preparation, and decision to submit the manuscript for publication was the sole responsibility of the authors. The publications committee of the NIDA Clinical Trials Network approved the final manuscript. The study in this manuscript was funded by the NIDA grant U10DA013732 (PI: Winhusen). Dr. Kalapatapu was funded by an Advanced Fellowship Program in Psychiatric Research/Neurosciences through the Veterans Affairs’ Office of Academic Affiliations. Dr. Vinogradov is a paid consultant in Brain Plasticity, Inc., a company with a commercial interest in cognitive training software. Dr. Vinogradov is also a consultant to Amgen, Genentech, and Hoffman-LaRoche. The authors are unaware of any other affiliations, funding, or financial holdings that might be perceived as affecting the objectivity of this manuscript.

Footnotes

Contributors: Dr. Kalapatapu completed the background literature search. Mr. Lewis completed the statistical analyses. Dr. Kalapatapu wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

References

  • 1.Arndt S, Clayton R, Schultz SK. Trends in substance abuse treatment 1998–2008: increasing older adult first-time admissions for illicit drugs. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;19:704–711. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31820d942b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.NIDA. [Last accessed on 3/10/2013.];Cocaine: Abuse and Addiction. Research Report Series: National Institute on Drug Abuse. 2009 http://www.drugabuse.gov/sites/default/files/rrcocaine.pdf.
  • 3.Wu LT, Blazer DG. Illicit and nonmedical drug use among older adults: a review. J Aging Health. 2011;23:481–504. doi: 10.1177/0898264310386224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Vocci FJ. Cognitive remediation in the treatment of stimulant abuse disorders: a research agenda. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2008;16:484–497. doi: 10.1037/a0014101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Sofuoglu M. Cognitive enhancement as a pharmacotherapy target for stimulant addiction. Addiction. 2010;105:38–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02791.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Sofuoglu M, Devito EE, Waters AJ, Carroll KM. Cognitive enhancement as a treatment for drug addictions. Neuropharmacology. 2013;64:452–463. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.06.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Brady KT, Gray KM, Tolliver BK. Cognitive enhancers in the treatment of substance use disorders: clinical evidence. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2011;99:285–294. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2011.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Aharonovich E, Amrhein PC, Bisaga A, Nunes EV, Hasin DS. Cognition, commitment language, and behavioral change among cocaine-dependent patients. Psychol Addict Behav. 2008;22:557–562. doi: 10.1037/a0012971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Aharonovich E, Hasin DS, Brooks AC, Liu X, Bisaga A, Nunes EV. Cognitive deficits predict low treatment retention in cocaine dependent patients. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2006;81:313–322. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Jovanovski D, Erb S, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive deficits in cocaine users: a quantitative review of the evidence. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2005;27:189–204. doi: 10.1080/13803390490515694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Simon SL, Dean AC, Cordova X, Monterosso JR, London ED. Methamphetamine dependence and neuropsychological functioning: evaluating change during early abstinence. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2010;71:335–344. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2010.71.335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lundqvist T. Imaging cognitive deficits in drug abuse. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2010;3:247–275. doi: 10.1007/7854_2009_26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Nordahl TE, Salo R, Leamon M. Neuropsychological effects of chronic methamphetamine use on neurotransmitters and cognition: a review. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2003;15:317–325. doi: 10.1176/jnp.15.3.317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Tziortzis D, Mahoney JJ, 3rd, Kalechstein AD, Newton TF, De la Garza R., 2nd The relationship between impulsivity and craving in cocaine- and methamphetamine-dependent volunteers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2011;98:196–202. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.12.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Moeller FG, Dougherty DM, Barratt ES, Schmitz JM, Swann AC, Grabowski J. The impact of impulsivity on cocaine use and retention in treatment. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2001;21:193–198. doi: 10.1016/s0740-5472(01)00202-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Cunha PJ, Bechara A, de Andrade AG, Nicastri S. Decision-making deficits linked to real-life social dysfunction in crack cocaine-dependent individuals. Am J Addict. 2011;20:78–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2010.00097.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fernandez-Serrano MJ, Perez-Garcia M, Schmidt Rio-Valle J, Verdejo-Garcia A. Neuropsychological consequences of alcohol and drug abuse on different components of executive functions. J Psychopharmacol. 2010;24:1317–1332. doi: 10.1177/0269881109349841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lucantonio F, Stalnaker TA, Shaham Y, Niv Y, Schoenbaum G. The impact of orbitofrontal dysfunction on cocaine addiction. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:358–366. doi: 10.1038/nn.3014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Weiss LM, Petry NM. Interaction effects of age and contingency management treatments in cocaine-dependent outpatients. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2011;19:173–181. doi: 10.1037/a0023031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kalapatapu RK, Campbell A, Aharonovich E, Hu MC, Levin FR, Nunes EV. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Middle-Aged versus Younger Adults Enrolled in a Clinical Trial of a Web-Delivered Psychosocial Treatment for Substance Use Disorders. Journal of Addiction Medicine. 2013;7:66–72. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0b013e31827e2d04. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Oslin DW. Evidence-based treatment of geriatric substance abuse. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2005;28:897–911. ix. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2005.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ersche KD, Jones PS, Williams GB, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET. Cocaine dependence: a fast-track for brain ageing? Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18:134–135. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lynch DJ. Future time perspective and impulsivity in old age. J Genet Psychol. 1971;118:245–252. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Mani TM, Bedwell JS, Miller LS. Age-related decrements in performance on a brief continuous performance test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;20:575–586. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2004.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Deakin J, Aitken M, Robbins T, Sahakian BJ. Risk taking during decision-making in normal volunteers changes with age. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:590–598. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704104104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Green L, Myerson J, Lichtman D, Rosen S, Fry A. Temporal discounting in choice between delayed rewards: the role of age and income. Psychol Aging. 1996;11:79–84. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.11.1.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Isella V, Mapelli C, Morielli N, Pelati O, Franceschi M, Appollonio IM. Age-related quantitative and qualitative changes in decision making ability. Behav Neurol. 2008;19:59–63. doi: 10.1155/2008/893727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Marschner A, Mell T, Wartenburger I, Villringer A, Reischies FM, Heekeren HR. Reward-based decision-making and aging. Brain Res Bull. 2005;67:382–390. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Snyder S, Pitts WM, Jr, Gustin Q. Absence of borderline personality disorder in later years. Am J Psychiatry. 1983;140:271–272. doi: 10.1176/ajp.140.2.271b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Stevenson J, Meares R, Comerford A. Diminished impulsivity in older patients with borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:165–166. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bartzokis G, Beckson M, Lu PH, Edwards N, Bridge P, Mintz J. Brain maturation may be arrested in chronic cocaine addicts. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:605–611. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01315-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bartzokis G, Goldstein IB, Hance DB, Beckson M, Shapiro D, Lu PH, Edwards N, Mintz J, Bridge P. The incidence of T2-weighted MR imaging signal abnormalities in the brain of cocaine-dependent patients is age-related and region-specific. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1999;20:1628–1635. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kalapatapu RK, Vadhan NP, Rubin E, Bedi G, Cheng WY, Sullivan MA, Foltin RW. A pilot study of neurocognitive function in older and younger cocaine abusers and controls. Am J Addict. 2011;20:228–239. doi: 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2011.00128.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Petry NM. A comparison of young, middle-aged, and older adult treatment-seeking pathological gamblers. Gerontologist. 2002;42:92–99. doi: 10.1093/geront/42.1.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Semple SJ, Zians J, Grant I, Patterson TL. Impulsivity and methamphetamine use. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2005;29:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Winhusen TM, Somoza EC, Lewis DF, Kropp FB, Horigian VE, Adinoff B. Frontal systems deficits in stimulant-dependent patients: Evidence of pre-illness dysfunction and relationship to treatment response. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;127:94–100. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.06.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Crews FT, Boettiger CA. Impulsivity, frontal lobes and risk for addiction. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2009;93:237–247. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.04.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Donovan DM, Daley DC, Brigham GS, Hodgkins CC, Perl HI, Garrett SB, Doyle SR, Floyd AS, Knox PC, Botero C, Kelly TM, Killeen TK, Hayes C, Kau’ibaumhofer N, Seamans C, Zammarelli L. Stimulant abuser groups to engage in 12-step: a multisite trial in the National Institute on Drug Abuse Clinical Trials Network. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2013;44:103–114. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2012.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hudziak JJ, Helzer JE, Wetzel MW, Kessel KB, McGee B, Janca A, Przybeck T. The use of the DSM-III-R Checklist for initial diagnostic assessments. Compr Psychiatry. 1993;34:375–383. doi: 10.1016/0010-440x(93)90061-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.McLellan AT, Kushner H, Metzger D, Peters R, Smith I, Grissom G, Pettinati H, Argeriou M. The Fifth Edition of the Addiction Severity Index. J Subst Abuse Treat. 1992;9:199–213. doi: 10.1016/0740-5472(92)90062-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB. Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: the PHQ primary care study. Primary Care Evaluation of Mental Disorders. Patient Health Questionnaire. JAMA. 1999;282:1737–1744. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.18.1737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Stanford MS, Mathias CW, Dougherty DM, Lake SL, Anderson NE, Patton JH. Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: An update and review. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009;47:385–395. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Grace J, Malloy PF. Frontal Systems Behavioral Scale (FrSBe): Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Streeter CC, Terhune DB, Whitfield TH, Gruber S, Sarid-Segal O, Silveri MM, Tzilos G, Afshar M, Rouse ED, Tian H, Renshaw PF, Ciraulo DA, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Performance on the Stroop predicts treatment compliance in cocaine-dependent individuals. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:827–836. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Winhusen T, Lewis D, Adinoff B, Brigham G, Kropp F, Donovan DM, Seamans CL, Hodgkins CC, Dicenzo JC, Botero CL, Jones DR, Somoza E. Impulsivity is associated with treatment non-completion in cocaine- and methamphetamine-dependent patients but differs in nature as a function of stimulant-dependence diagnosis. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2012.12.005. [Epub ahead of print 1/12/2013]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1994;50:7–15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Toplak ME, Sorge GB, Benoit A, West RF, Stanovich KE. Decision-making and cognitive abilities: A review of associations between Iowa Gambling Task performance, executive functions, and intelligence. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30:562–581. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Brand M, Recknor EC, Grabenhorst F, Bechara A. Decisions under ambiguity and decisions under risk: correlations with executive functions and comparisons of two different gambling tasks with implicit and explicit rules. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007;29:86–99. doi: 10.1080/13803390500507196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Schmidt M. Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test: A Handbook. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtiss G. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual, Revised and Expanded. Orlando, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Keys BA, White DA. Exploring the relationship between age, executive abilities, and psychomotor speed. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6:76–82. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700611098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Buckner RL. Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron. 2004;44:195–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Grady C. The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13:491–505. doi: 10.1038/nrn3256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Hook EB, Regal RR. Validity of methods for model selection, weighting for model uncertainty, and small sample adjustment in capture-recapture estimation. Am J Epidemiol. 1997;145:1138–1144. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Kletting P, Glatting G. Model selection for time-activity curves: the corrected Akaike information criterion and the F-test. Z Med Phys. 2009;19:200–206. doi: 10.1016/j.zemedi.2009.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Perry JL, Carroll ME. The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;200:1–26. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1173-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D. Neuropsychological Assessment. 5. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Arce E, Santisteban C. Impulsivity: a review. Psicothema. 2006;18:213–220. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Resnick SM, Lamar M, Driscoll I. Vulnerability of the orbitofrontal cortex to age-associated structural and functional brain changes. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1121:562–575. doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Pardo JV, Lee JT, Sheikh SA, Surerus-Johnson C, Shah H, Munch KR, Carlis JV, Lewis SM, Kuskowski MA, Dysken MW. Where the brain grows old: decline in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal function with normal aging. Neuroimage. 2007;35:1231–1237. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.12.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Lane SD, Moeller FG, Steinberg JL, Buzby M, Kosten TR. Performance of cocaine dependent individuals and controls on a response inhibition task with varying levels of difficulty. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2007;33:717–726. doi: 10.1080/00952990701522724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Bond AJ, Verheyden SL, Wingrove J, Curran HV. Angry cognitive bias, trait aggression and impulsivity in substance users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;171:331–339. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1585-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Verdejo-Garcia A, Bechara A, Recknor EC, Perez-Garcia M. Executive dysfunction in substance dependent individuals during drug use and abstinence: an examination of the behavioral, cognitive and emotional correlates of addiction. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006;12:405–415. doi: 10.1017/s1355617706060486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Hammers DB, Suhr JA. Neuropsychological, impulsive personality, and cerebral oxygenation correlates of undergraduate polysubstance use. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2010;32:599–609. doi: 10.1080/13803390903379599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Hoffman WF, Moore M, Templin R, McFarland B, Hitzemann RJ, Mitchell SH. Neuropsychological function and delay discounting in methamphetamine-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;188:162–170. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0494-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Fox HC, Jackson ED, Sinha R. Elevated cortisol and learning and memory deficits in cocaine dependent individuals: relationship to relapse outcomes. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:1198–1207. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Rosselli M, Ardila A, Lubomski M, Murray S, King K. Personality profile and neuropsychological test performance in chronic cocaine-abusers. Int J Neurosci. 2001;110:55–72. doi: 10.3109/00207450108994221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Buelow MT, Suhr JA. Construct validity of the Iowa Gambling Task. Neuropsychol Rev. 2009;19:102–114. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9083-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Carvalho JC, Cardoso Cde O, Shneider-Bakos D, Kristensen CH, Fonseca RP. The effect of age on decision making according to the Iowa gambling task. Span J Psychol. 2012;15:480–486. doi: 10.5209/rev_sjop.2012.v15.n2.38858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Verdejo-Garcia A, Benbrook A, Funderburk F, David P, Cadet JL, Bolla KI. The differential relationship between cocaine use and marijuana use on decision-making performance over repeat testing with the Iowa Gambling Task. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;90:2–11. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science. 1997;275:1293–1295. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Zamarian L, Sinz H, Bonatti E, Gamboz N, Delazer M. Normal aging affects decisions under ambiguity, but not decisions under risk. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:645–657. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.5.645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Caserta MT, Bannon Y, Fernandez F, Giunta B, Schoenberg MR, Tan J. Normal brain aging clinical, immunological, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging features. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2009;84:1–19. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7742(09)00401-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Kim YT, Song HJ, Seo JH, Lee JJ, Lee J, Kwon DH, Yoo DS, Lee HJ, Suh KJ, Chang Y. The differences in neural network activity between methamphetamine abusers and healthy subjects performing an emotion-matching task: functional MRI study. NMR Biomed. 2011;24:1392–1400. doi: 10.1002/nbm.1702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Paulus MP, Hozack NE, Zauscher BE, Frank L, Brown GG, Braff DL, Schuckit MA. Behavioral and functional neuroimaging evidence for prefrontal dysfunction in methamphetamine-dependent subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:53–63. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00334-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Hart CL, Marvin CB, Silver R, Smith EE. Is cognitive functioning impaired in methamphetamine users? A critical review. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:586–608. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Fals-Stewart W. Ability to counselors to detect cognitive impairment among substance-abusing patients: an examination of diagnostic efficiency. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1997;5:39–50. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.5.1.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Schrimsher GW, Parker JD, Burke RS. Relation between cognitive testing performance and pattern of substance use in males at treatment entry. Clin Neuropsychol. 2007;21:498–510. doi: 10.1080/13803390600674441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Horner MD, Harvey RT, Denier CA. Self-report and objective measures of cognitive deficit in patients entering substance abuse treatment. Psychiatry Res. 1999;86:155–161. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(99)00031-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Shelton MD, Parsons OA. Alcoholics’ self-assessment of their neuropsychological functioning in everyday life. J Clin Psychol. 1987;43:395–403. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198705)43:3<395::aid-jclp2270430314>3.0.co;2-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Copersino ML, Fals-Stewart W, Fitzmaurice G, Schretlen DJ, Sokoloff J, Weiss RD. Rapid cognitive screening of patients with substance use disorders. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2009;17:337–344. doi: 10.1037/a0017260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Vinogradov S, Fisher M, de Villers-Sidani E. Cognitive training for impaired neural systems in neuropsychiatric illness. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:43–76. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Bickel WK, Yi R, Landes RD, Hill PF, Baxter C. Remember the future: working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69:260–265. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Fals-Stewart W, Lam WK. Computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation for the treatment of patients with substance use disorders: a randomized clinical trial. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2010;18:87–98. doi: 10.1037/a0018058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Subramaniam K, Vinogradov S. Cognitive training for psychiatric disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38:242–243. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Hindin SB, Zelinski EM. Extended practice and aerobic exercise interventions benefit untrained cognitive outcomes in older adults: a meta-analysis. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60:136–141. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03761.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Vance DE, Keltner NL, McGuinness T, Umlauf MG, Yuan YY. The future of cognitive remediation training in older adults. J Neurosci Nurs. 2010;42:255–64. doi: 10.1097/jnn.0b013e3181ecb003. quiz 265–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Shum D, Fleming J, Gill H, Gullo MJ, Strong J. A randomized controlled trial of prospective memory rehabilitation in adults with traumatic brain injury. J Rehabil Med. 2011;43:216–223. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Winkens I, Van Heugten CM, Wade DT, Habets EJ, Fasotti L. Efficacy of time pressure management in stroke patients with slowed information processing: a randomized controlled trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2009;90:1672–1679. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2009.04.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Engelberts NH, Klein M, Ader HJ, Heimans JJ, Trenite DG, van der Ploeg HM. The effectiveness of cognitive rehabilitation for attention deficits in focal seizures: a randomized controlled study. Epilepsia. 2002;43:587–595. doi: 10.1046/j.1528-1157.2002.29401.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Twamley EW, Vella L, Burton CZ, Heaton RK, Jeste DV. Compensatory cognitive training for psychosis: effects in a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Psychiatry. 2012;73:1212–1219. doi: 10.4088/JCP.12m07686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Solanto MV, Marks DJ, Wasserstein J, Mitchell K, Abikoff H, Alvir JM, Kofman MD. Efficacy of meta-cognitive therapy for adult ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167:958–968. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09081123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Londos E, Boschian K, Linden A, Persson C, Minthon L, Lexell J. Effects of a goal-oriented rehabilitation program in mild cognitive impairment: a pilot study. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2008;23:177–183. doi: 10.1177/1533317507312622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Velligan DI, Bow-Thomas CC, Huntzinger C, Ritch J, Ledbetter N, Prihoda TJ, Miller AL. Randomized controlled trial of the use of compensatory strategies to enhance adaptive functioning in outpatients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:1317–1323. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.8.1317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES