Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Brain Behav Immun. 2013 Apr 26;32:173–179. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2013.04.008

Inflammatory Biomarkers and Emotional Approach Coping in Men with Prostate Cancer

Michael A Hoyt 1, Annette L Stanton 2,3,4, Julienne E Bower 2,3,4, KaMala S Thomas 5, Mark S Litwin 4,6,7, Elizabeth C Breen 3, Michael R Irwin 2,3,4
PMCID: PMC3706095  NIHMSID: NIHMS473011  PMID: 23624266

Abstract

Objective

Emotion-regulating coping is associated with improvements in psychological and physical health outcomes. Yet in the context of prostate cancer-related stressors, limited research has characterized associations of emotion-regulating coping processes (emotional expression, emotional processing) and inflammatory processes that are related to disease risk. This investigation examined the relation of Emotional Approach Coping (EAC) with markers of inflammation to test the hypothesis that higher EAC scores at study entry (T1) would be associated with lower proinflammatory markers four months later (T2), specifically sTNF-RII, CRP, and IL-6.

Methods

Forty-one men (M age=66.62 years; SD=9.62) who had undergone radical prostatectomy or radiation therapy for localized prostate cancer within two years completed questionnaires, including assessments of EAC, at T1, and provided blood samples for immune assessments at T2.

Results

When controlling for relevant biobehavioral controls, emotional processing predicted lower IL-6 (B=−.66, p<.01), sTNF-RII (B=−.43, p<.05), and CRP (B=−.43, p<.10), whereas emotional expression was significantly associated with higher levels of sTNF-RII (B=.55, p<.05). Associations of emotional expression and IL-6 (B=.38, p<.10), and CRP (B=.44, p<.10) approached significance. Probing interactions of emotional processing and expression (though only approaching significance) suggested that expression of emotion is associated with higher inflammation (CRP and sTNF-RII) only in the context of low emotional processing.

Conclusions

Attempts at emotion regulation via emotional processing appear to modulate inflammatory processes. Understanding, making meaning of, and working through emotional experience may be a promising target of intervention to reduce inflammation with potential effects on psychological and cancer outcomes in men with prostate cancer.

Keywords: emotional approach coping, emotional processing, emotional expression, proinflammatory cytokines, inflammation, prostate cancer


The experience of cancer diagnosis and treatment exposes individuals to numerous physical and emotional stressors and accounts for increased risk of emotional distress and depression. The first one to two years following radical prostatectomy or completion of radiation treatment for prostate cancer are markedly demanding and characterized by relatively rapid changes in physical functioning (Litwin et al., 2001; Stanford et al., 2000), increased risk for depression (Jayadevappa et al., 2011), sleep disturbance (Savard et al., 2005), worry and anxiety (Sharpley et al., 2008), and declining health-related quality of life (Gore et al., 2009). Emotional perturbations and chronic negative affective states associated with stressors can invoke potent negative effects on inflammatory and other cellular immune processes (Irwin, 2002; Kemeny, 2007; Reiche et al., 2004; Segerstrom and Miller, 2004; Steptoe et al., 2007), which can compound the burden associated with cancer and have a direct and indirect impact on mental and physical health outcomes (Grivennikov et al., 2010; Mantovani et al., 2008; see also Irwin and Cole, 2011 on behavior and inflammation). Coping through avoidance has typically been associated with suppression of cellular immune function, as seen in individuals with HIV and cancer (Fawzy et al., 1990; Futterman et al., 1996; Goodkin et al., 1992a; 1992b); however, effects of approach coping on immune parameters have rarely been assessed (see Goodkin et al., 1992a). Use of coping strategies that lead one to focus on and regulate difficult emotions (e.g., emotional approach coping) related to one’s cancer experience has potential for modifying the affective response, reducing inflammation (Master et al., 2009), and improving psychological (Hoyt, 2009) and physical (Hoyt et al., 2013) health outcomes in men with cancer.

Emotional approach coping (EAC) (Stanton et al., 1994) is comprised of two distinct but related emotion-regulating strategies: emotional processing and emotional expression. In the case of prostate cancer, emotional processing includes purposive attempts to acknowledge, explore, and understand one’s emotions related to prostate cancer; whereas emotional expression represents active verbal and non-verbal efforts to communicate or symbolize cancer-related emotional experiences (Stanton et al., 2000b). Research with breast cancer survivors has characterized a benefit of EAC on physical health and psychological adjustment, including increased vigor, fewer medical visits for cancer-related morbidities, and improved self-reported health (Stanton et al., 2000a; Stanton et al., 2002). However, research in men with mixed cancer types (Hoyt, 2009) found that greater use of emotional expression in response to cancer-related stressors was associated with lower levels of psychological distress, whereas emotional processing demonstrated a positive relationship with distress. In an expanded cohort of the current study, Hoyt et al. (2013) found an opposite pattern, with emotional processing associated with improved prostate-specific functioning, but no relationship with emotional expression. Such disparate associations might be better understood by distinguishing the interactions of expressing and processing cancer-related emotions. Expressing emotions without attempts to understand or make meaning from emotional experience might exacerbate affective dysregulation and promote rumination, persistent worry, or other maladaptive repetitive thought patterns.

Little work has attempted to identify biological mechanisms by which EAC operates, including markers of inflammation. In fact, only one study (Master et al., 2009) has examined inflammatory markers in association with dispositional use of EAC. In a sample of young adults undergoing an acute laboratory stressor, higher levels of EAC, particularly emotional processing, was associated with a less pronounced increase in soluble tumor necrosis factor-receptor type-II (sTNF-RII) in oral mucosal transudate (and interleukin-6, though non-significant after controlling for behavioral factors). Those who are less likely to focus on emotions as a means of coping may be at heightened risk for chronic elevations in inflammation and health-related consequences.

To examine whether coping with cancer-related stressors via emotion-regulatory coping processes was associated with markers of inflammation, this study tested the hypothesis that EAC processes would correlate with circulating levels of proinflammatory cytokines within a clinically relevant period following prostate cancer treatment. Prior work supports a benefit to psychological and physical outcomes of emotional expression (Hoyt, 2009) and emotional processing (Hoyt et al., 2013) in men with cancer. Thus, we hypothesized that EAC processes would predict lower inflammation levels during this time period. The interaction of EAC processes (emotional processing × emotional expression) was also examined. We expected that emotional expression in the presence of emotional processing would exhibit a more pronounced effect on biomarkers.

There is growing evidence that activation of the proinflammatory cytokine network underlie negative psychological states in cancer patients (Irwin & Miller, 2007; Lee et al., 2004; Miller et al., 2008; Musselman et al., 2001). We focus on three inflammatory markers, interleukin-6 (IL-6), C-reactive protein (CRP), and sTNF-RII, which can be reliably assessed in plasma and have been linked to behavioral and cancer-related outcomes in previous research with cancer survivors (Bower et al., 2011; Bower et al., 2009; Collado-Hidalgo et al., 2002; Orre et al., 2009). Evidence from non-cancer populations implicates these markers in emotional disturbance and depression (Dowlati et al., 2010; Howren et al., 2009; O’Brien et al., 2007), and elevations in IL-6 (Jehn et al., 2006; Musselman et al., 2001; Seruga et al., 2008) and TNF-a (Seruga et al., 2008) have been linked to depression in cancer patients. Notably, sTNF-RII reflects TNF-a activity (Spinas et al., 1992) and tends to be more reliably measured than TNF-a (Diez-Ruis et al., 1995). Experimentally induced immune activation with the release of sTNF-RII leads to increased emotional disturbance (Reichenberg et al., 2001) and in cancer samples has been connected to behavioral symptoms including fatigue (Bower et al., 2011). Because IL-6 induces CRP, this biomarker can provide a measure of the cumulative activity of IL-6 and systemic inflammation.

Method

Participants

Men who completed radical prostatectomy or radiation therapy for localized prostate cancer within the prior two years were recruited to take part in a larger study on “health-related quality of life after prostate cancer.” Forty-one participants were recruited via physician/clinic referrals (n = 2), community outreach and advertisement (n = 15), and from an institutional tumor registry database (n = 24). Participants were screened to exclude individuals not meeting entrance criteria (e.g., localized disease, time since treatment; English speaking) and those with any cognitive debilitating co-morbidity (e.g., dementia). Participants were also excluded for presence of medical conditions or medications that would likely confound immune evaluation (e.g., autoimmune disorder, inflammatory disease, uncontrolled thyroid disease, active infection, recent myocardial infarction); regular smoker (daily use); or heavy alcohol use (more than 14 drinks per week). Participants were re-screened at each study visit for acute infection, recent vigorous exercise, recent caffeine or cigarette use, and changes in medications, and rescheduled as appropriate.

Participants were English speaking men who ranged in age from 51 to 87 years (M = 66.62, SD = 9.62) and on average, completed treatment 15.63 months prior (SD = 7.53; Range = 2 to 24 months) to study completion. Clinical and socio-demographic variables are reported in Table 1. No men were on active hormonal therapy at the time of participation. Notably, an additional three men completed the initial assessment but not the T2 session (not reported on here). These men did not differ significantly from those who completed the T2 assessment on sociodemographic or cancer-related variables.

Table 1.

Participant Characteristics (N = 41)

Characteristic, Mean (SD) or %
Characteristic, Mean (SD) or %
Age, in years 66.62 Job Status:
(SD=9.62)     full-time employment 39.0%
Ethnicity:     part-time employment 9.8%
    White (non-Hispanic) 80.5%     retired 46.3%
    African American/Black 12.2%     unemployed/disability 4.9%
    Hispanic/Latino 4.9% Relationship Status:
    Other 2.4%     married/partnered 87.8%
Education:     widowed/divorced 7.3%
    high school 7.2%     single, never married 4.9%
    some post-high school 22.0% Treatment:
    2-year college degree 9.8%     prostatectomy/surgery 78.0%
    4-year college degree 22.0%     radiation therapy 22.0%
    advanced degree 39.0%     previous hormone therapy 4.9%
Annual Household Income: Months since diagnosis 22.24
    $15,000 or less 2.5% (SD=10.90)
    $15,001-$45,000 2.5% Months since treatment 15.63
    $45,001-$75,000 27.5% (SD=7.53)
    $75,001-$100,000 20.0% Gleason sum 5.7 (SD=1.39)
    $100,001 or more 47.5%

Procedures

After providing written informed consent, participants attended an individual session (T1) to complete questionnaire and interview assessments and returned for a follow-up session 4 months later (T2) where they provided blood samples, Their height and weight as well as other relevant biobehavioral variables were recorded at each session. All assessments were conducted in the morning (before 11am). Participants received $25 after each session. The institutional review board at the University of California, Los Angeles approved study procedures.

Measures

Emotional approach coping

Use of EAC processes in response to prostate cancer-related stressors were measured at T1 using Stanton and colleagues’ (Stanton et al., 1994; 2000a; 2000b) EAC scales, which consist of the 4-item Emotional Processing (e.g., ‘I take time to figure out what I’m really feeling’, “I delve into my feelings to get a thorough understanding of them”) and 4-item Emotional Expression (e.g., ‘I feel free to express my emotions’, “I let my feelings come out freely”) scales. Participants were instructed to complete items to reflect the degree to which they engaged in EAC in response to cancer problems and experiences on a 4-point scale (1 = I don’t do this at all; 4 = I do this a lot). As measured by the EAC scales, EAC processes may include verbal or nonverbal attempts at expression and intrapersonal or interpersonal coping efforts. Both EAC scales have been shown to demonstrate sound internal consistency and predictive validity [see Austenfeld and Stanton, 2004 for a review of the psychometric properties]. In the current study, Cronbach’s alphas were 0.82 for emotional processing and 0.90 for emotional expression.

Assessment of inflammatory markers

Blood samples for circulating inflammatory markers were collected by venipuncture into EDTA tubes, placed on ice, centrifuged for acquisition of plasma, and stored at −80 degrees Celsius for subsequent batch testing. Plasma levels of IL-6 were determined by high sensitivity enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA; R&D Systems, Minneapolis, MN) according to manufacturer’s protocols, with a lower limit of detection of 0.2 pg/mL sTNF-RII levels were determined by regular-sensitivity ELISA (R&D Systems, lower limit 234 pg/mL). CRP levels were determined by a high-sensitivity ELISA (Immundiagnostik, ALPCO Immunoassays, Salem, NH) according to the manufacturer’s protocol, but with an extended standard curve to a lower limit of detection of 0.2 mg/L. All samples were run in duplicate; intra-assay and inter-assay precision of all tests were less than or equal to 10%.

Clinical characteristics, sociodemographics, and biobehavioral factors

Participants reported their age, level of education, income, employment status, ethnicity, and other socio-demographic variables. This included information regarding health history, health behaviors, diagnosis and treatment factors, and level of prostate-specific functioning (as measured by The UCLA Prostate Cancer Index (UCLA-PCI) (Litwin et al, 1998).

Data Analyses

Descriptive statistics were computed for key study variables including sociodemographic and relevant clinical characteristics. Distributions of inflammatory markers were non-normal. Thus, all inflammatory markers were transformed prior to analyses using a natural log transformation; non-transformed values are shown on tables and figures for ease of interpretation. Zero-order correlations among predictor and dependent variables were also computed. Several covariates were identified a priori based on recommendations for treating biobehavioral factors in the assessment of circulating inflammatory markers (O’Connor et al., 2009). These included participants’ age, ethnicity (White non-Hispanic vs. ethnic minority), body mass index, and number of months since completing primary cancer treatment. Covariates were controlled in subsequent analyses.

Multiple linear regression was used to test study hypotheses. T2 Inflammatory markers were separately regressed on EAC measures at T1, controlling for identified variables. Control variables were entered in the first step, EAC processes were entered in step 2, and the interaction terms of emotional expression and emotional processing were entered in the final step. Moderator analyses allowed for examination of the possibility that emotional processing efforts are conditioned by emotional expression (and vice versa) as observed in previous research (Stanton et al., 2000a) and were conducted in accordance with procedures outlined by Aiken and West (1991).

Results

Sociodemographic and cancer-related variables are reported in Table 1. On average, participants were 66.62 years old (SD=9.62; range = 51–87 years) and had completed prostate cancer treatment 2–24 months prior to study participation. Descriptive statistics and correlations for key study variables are reported in Table 2. Mean values of emotional expression and emotional processing were slightly higher than those documented by Hoyt (2009) in men with mixed cancer types for emotional expression (M = 2.46, SD = .91) and emotional processing (M = 2.41, SD = .81), and slightly lower than those documented by Stanton et al. (2000a) in women with breast cancer, which were 2.95 (SD = .84) and 3.00 (SD = .80) for emotional expression and emotional processing, respectively.

Table 2.

Descriptive Statistics and Correlations for Key Variables

Descriptive Statistics
Correlations
Variable Mean SD Range 1 2 3 4 5
1. Emotional Processing 2.79 .80 1.00 – 4.00 - .64*** −.20 −.50** −.04
2. Emotional Expression 2.62 .81 1.00 – 4.00 - −.04 −.18 −.15
3. IL-6, pg/mLa 1.92 1.88 .48 – 11.10 - −.08
4. CRP, mg/L 2.85 4.24 .23 – 18.23
5. sTNF-RII, pg/mL 2754.43 812.15 1634.20 – 4817.10
a

Descriptive statistics for inflammatory markers show non-transformed values; all analyses were performed on natural-log transformed values.

p <.10;

*

p<.05;

**

p<.01;

***

p<.001

Men in the sample reported notable difficulty with sexual function (M = 40.18; range = 0 – 93) and psychological bother of sexual symptoms (M = 43.05; range = 0 −100); as well as with urinary function (M = 73.32; range = 6.60 – 100) and psychological bother of urinary symptoms (M = 53.05; range = 0 – 100). These reflect variation in prostate cancer treatment-related physical and psychological demands at levels similar or lower than samples of men treated for localized disease (Gore et al., 2009).

Relationships between Emotional Approach Coping and Inflammatory Markers

To test the hypothesis that men reporting greater levels of emotional expression and emotional processing would evidence lower levels of proinflammatory markers at T2, multiple regression analyses were conducted using EAC processes as predictors and inflammatory markers as dependent variables. Analyses included identified control variables for each outcome. Consistent with hypotheses, T1 emotional processing was significantly associated with lower circulating levels of IL-6 (B = −.66, p<.01) and sTNF-RII (B = −.43, p<.05) at T2. The relationship with CRP was in the same direction and approached significance (B = −.43, p<.10). However, contrary to hypotheses, T1 emotional expression was related to increased levels of sTNF-RII (B = .55, p<.05), and similar pattern approached significance for IL-6 (B = .38, p<.10) and CRP (B = .44, p<.10).

Interaction of Emotional Processing and Emotional Expression

To assess the possibility that emotional processing and emotional expression have combined effects, interaction terms were also included in analyses. Though only approaching significance, interactions were observed for effects on T2 sTNF-RII and CRP (see Table 3). Because these Interactive effects accounted for 7–8% of the variance and might help explain the unexpected influence of emotional expression on inflammatory markers, moderated effects were probed and plotted at 1 SD above and below the mean of emotional processing. At relatively low levels of emotional processing, high emotional expression was significantly associated with higher CRP (B = .73, p<.01) (see Figure 1a) and sTNF-RII (B = .81, p<.01) (see Figure 1b). However, this effect is buffered (no longer significant) in the presence of high levels of emotional processing in both the case of CRP (B = .13, ns) and sTNF-RII (B = .23, ns).

Table 3.

Predictors of Inflammatory Markers at Time 2

T2 Inflammatory Marker
IL-6 CRP sTNF-RII



Variablea ΔR2 B SE β ΔR2 B SE β ΔR2 B SE β
Block 1 .27* .17 .23
  Age .02 .01 .33* −02 .02 −.13 .01 .01 .28
  Ethnicityb .13 .27 .08 −.23 .53 −.08 −.17 .11 −.24
  Body Mass Index .04 .03 .23 .11 .06 .31 .01 .01 .18
  Time since treatment .01 .01 .16 .02 .02 .11 <.01 .01 −.01
  completion
Block 2 .17* .10 .15*
  Emotional Processing −.59 .19 −.66** −.69 .38 −.43 −.16 .06 −.43*
  Emotional Expression .31 .16% .38 .63 .32 .44 .18 .07 .55*
Block 3 .03 % .08 .07
  Emotional Processing X −.17 .14 −.18 −.52 .29 −.31 −.11 .06 −.27
  Emotional Expression
F(7, 35) = 3.48**; R2 = .47 F(7, 34) = 2.12; R2 = .35 F(7, 35) = 3.23*; R2=.45
a

Regression coefficients reflect values at the end of Block 3, with all variables entered into the equation.

b

0 = White (non-Hispanic); 1 = ethnic minority

p<.10;

*

p<.05;

**

p<.01;

***

p<.001

Figure 1. Emotional expression × emotional processing interaction on CRP and sTNF-RII.

Figure 1

Figure displays interaction effects for (a) T2 CRP and (b) T2 sTNF-RII. Analyses controlled for age, body mass index, ethnic status, and time since completion of primary cancer treatment (radiation therapy, surgery). Dashed line denotes nonsignificant simple slopes.

Discussion

Research has demonstrated that, under particular conditions, coping with stressful circumstances through emotional approach confers benefit to health and well-being (see Stanton, 2011), yet few studies have examined correlated physiological processes. The goals of this study were to examine relationships of the use of EAC processes (i.e., emotional expression, emotional processing) to regulate responses to emotion-laden stressors associated with prostate cancer and biological markers of inflammation (i.e., IL-6, sTNF-RII, CRP) during a period of known high demand for prostate cancer survivors. T1 EAC processes and their interaction accounted for 18- 20% of the variance in T2 inflammatory markers.

Hypotheses regarding longitudinal effects of emotional processing were generally supported. Emotional processing of prostate cancer-related stressors predicted lower levels of inflammation over time, providing support for the prospect that processing of emotions associated with prostate cancer-related stressors modulates changes in inflammatory responses and lending insight into the possibility that inflammatory mechanisms have the potential to contribute to the benefits of emotional processing on psychological and physical outcomes observed across studies. It may be that the process of “working through” or making meaning from difficult emotions provides adaptive regulation of emotion and greater likelihood of a declining trajectory of chronic systemic inflammation following treatment for prostate cancer, though examination of changes over time is required. Prior work on emotional approach has shown that emotional processing supports adaptive emotion regulation by fostering positive cognitive reappraisals (Creswell et al., 2007),improving interpersonal support (Lichtenthal et al., 2006), and promoting physiological habituation to emotional stressors through sustained exposure (Low et al., 2006, 2008), which may then influence inflammatory responses.]

In contrast to those for emotional processing, hypotheses regarding the links between emotional expression and inflammatory markers were not supported. The association of emotional expression with higher levels of inflammation was unexpected. However, examination of the interactions with emotional processing offers a possible explanation. Although the relevant interactions did not attain statistical significance at p < .05, they accounted for seven to eight percent of the variance in the inflammatory markers. Explorations of the interactions suggest that emotional expression is associated with higher levels of inflammation only when used in relative isolation of emotional processing. Expressing one’s emotions without efforts to understand them might promote emotion dysregulation by extending maintenance of negative affect (Rude et al., 2007), impairing cardiovascular habituation (Low et al., 2008), or preventing adaptive processing allowing one to maintain distance from emotional stressors and thus blocking attempts at cognitive change or active coping. Expressive writing or similar interventions that also prompt cognitive processing of emotions may be optimal in promoting affect regulation and associated downstream immune changes. In fact, avoidance of processing of HIV-related emotional content has been associated with a trend toward lower CD4+ percentages (Lugendorf et al., 1997) and cognitive processing after loss predicts less rapid declines in CD4+ cells in HIV positive men (Bower et al., 1996).

Future work might also explore the neural correlates of EAC that might be associated with upstream processes of the central nervous system affecting downstream immune regulation. For instance, Master et al. (2009) revealed that higher levels of emotional expression were associated with greater resting left-sided frontal electroencephalography asymmetry, an indicator of approach-oriented motivation and lower likelihood of negative affectivity in response to stressors. Such patterns of cerebral activation have been associated with increased immunocompetence including smaller decrements in natural killer cell activity following an emotional stressor (Davidson et al., 1999), and a larger antibody titer rise after influenza vaccination (Rosenkranz et al., 2003).

Several assumptions underlie the conceptual model under investigation in this study. One assumption is that emotion regulating coping processes have the potential to interrupt affective pathways to proinflammatory cytokine activity. However, it is also possible that cytokine signals communicate with the brain and ultimately influence emotional response (Miller et al., 2005). A second assumption is that heightened cytokine levels are maladaptive. The body’s attempt at tissue repair following surgical intervention and exposure to radiation involves up-regulation of proinflammatory cytokine responses (Barcellos-Hoff, 1998; Goldfarb et al., 2010; Stone et al., 2003) to promote tissue repair. Indeed, positive affect was associated with increased levels of proinflammatory cytokines in a study of men with prostate cancer undergoing radiation therapy (Sepah and Bower, 2009). However, all the men in this study had completed treatment, and persistently elevated proinflammatory markers have been associated with treatment-related toxicities following treatment for prostate cancer (Christiansen et al., 2007).

Study limitations should be noted. This study relied on a relatively small and relatively homogenous sample of primarily white, highly educated men with localized disease with limited statistical power. Examination of samples with diversity across race, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, age, cultural experience, and disease characteristics will extend this research. This study also utilized a somewhat arbitrary window of four months in which to detect changes in a sample of patients with varied durations since treatment completion. However, longitudinal studies of men treated for prostate cancer have reported detectable changes in prostate-specific functioning in as little as 3-month re-assessments within the 2-year period following treatment (Litwin et al., 2001; Namiki et al., 2008). Further, men reported being bothered by urinary and sexual (via responses on the UCLA-PCI) at notably higher proportions than reports in larger longitudinal population studies. Stanford et al. (2000) reported minor to severe bother related to urinary problems in 62.2% of men with localized prostate cancer (N = 1291), and minor to severe bother related to sexual problems in 86.0% of men at 24 months post-surgery. This compares to 92.7% and 73.2%, respectively, in the current study. Finally, the current study offers a preliminary examination of measures of inflammation utilizing a traditional ELISA approach to examine a limited set of biomarkers. Other approaches should be utilized in future research to explore the possibility of associations of coping with other inflammatory markers, including anti-inflammatory cytokines. Despite these limitations, this is the only study to examine emotion regulating coping processes on inflammation in men with prostate cancer and these results inform our limited understanding of the influence of these coping processes in this population. Results from this study are correlational, and although consistent with our conceptual framework, causal conclusions cannot be made.

The present study implicates emotional processing as an effective coping strategy by which regulation of emotion modulates inflammatory processes. Over time, coping through emotional processing might be protective against chronic inflammatory processes, which have been associated with a variety of adverse health outcomes, including overall and disease-related mortality (Shafique et al., 2012). Identifying the degree to which coping processes account for changes in immune regulation will contribute to the growing knowledge-base regarding psychosocial influences on cancer-related morbidity, progression, recurrence, and mortality (Lutgendorf and Sood, 2011). Studies that examine trajectories of dynamic relationships of coping and immune activity will make a significant contribution. Further, as psychosocial interventions continue to identify inflammatory processes as primary outcomes (Cohen et al., 2011), more precise identification of psychological and behavioral processes that underlie signaling of inflammatory biomarkers is increasingly critical. Prostate cancer often presents unique circumstances to patients and the degree to which these findings generalize to other populations and contexts warrants further investigation. Additional research is needed to characterize the effect on varied and prolonged immune responses, and most importantly, to prostate-specific outcomes.

Highlights.

Emotion regulating coping processes may be promising intervention targets to reduce inflammation and improve psychological and physical outcomes prostate cancer patients.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by funds from the Cousins Center for Psychoneuroimmunology of the University of California, Los Angeles, and the following support from NIH: R01-AG034588; R01-AG026364; R01-CA119159; R01-HL079955; R01 HL095799; P30-AG028748 and UCLA CTSI UL1TR000124.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Conflict of Interest Statement: All authors declare there are no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Aiken LS, West SG. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park: Sage Publications; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  2. Austenfeld JL, Stanton AL. Coping through emotional approach: A new look at emotion, coping, and health-related outcomes. J Pers. 2004;72:1335–1363. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00299.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barcellos-Hoff MH. How do tissues respond to damage at the cellular level? The role of cytokines in irradiated tissues. Rad Res. 1998;150:S109–S120. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bower JE, Ganz PA, Irwin MR, Kwan L, Breen EC, Cole S. Inflammation and behavioral symptoms after breast cancer treatment: Do fatigue, depression, and sleep disturbance share a common underlying mechanism? J Clin Oncol. 2011;29:3517–3522. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2011.36.1154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bower JE, Ganz PA, Tao ML, Hu W, Belin TR, Sepah S, Cole S, Aziz N. Inflammatory biomarkers and fatigue during radiation therapy for breast and prostate cancer. Clin Cancer Res. 2009;15:5534–5540. doi: 10.1158/1078-0432.CCR-08-2584. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bower JE, Kemeny ME, Taylor SE, Fahey JL. Cognitive processing, discovery of meaning, CD4 decline, and AIDS-related mortality among bereaved HIV-seropositive men. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1996;66:979–986. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.66.6.979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Christiansen H, Saile B, Hermann RM, Rave-Fränk M, Hille A, Schmidberger H, Hess CF, Ramadori G. Increase of hepcidin plasma and urine levels is associated with acute proctitis and changes in hemoglobin levels in primary radiotherapy for prostate cancer. J Cancer Res Clin Oncol. 2007;133:297–304. doi: 10.1007/s00432-006-0170-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cohen L, Parker PA, Vence L, Savary C, Kentor D, Pettaway C, Babaian R, Pisters L, Miles B, Wei Q, Wiltz L, Patel T, Radvanyi L. Presurgical stress management improves postoperative immune function in men with prostate cancer undergoing radical prostatectomy. Psychosom Med. 2011;73:218–225. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31820a1c26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Collado-Hidalgo A, Bower JE, Ganz PA, Cole SW, Irwin MR. Fatigue and proinflammatory cytokine activity in breast cancer survivors. Psychosom Med. 2002;64:604–611. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200207000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Creswell JD, Lam S, Stanton AL, Taylor SE, Bower JE, Sherman DK. Does self-affirmation, cognitive processing, or discovery of mean explain cancer-related health benefits of expressive writing? Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2007;33:238–250. doi: 10.1177/0146167206294412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Davidson RJ, Coe CC, Dolski I, Donzella B. Individual differences in prefrontal activation asymmetry predict natural killer cell activity at rest and in response to challenge. Brain Behav Immun. 1999;13:93–108. doi: 10.1006/brbi.1999.0557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Diez-Ruis A, Tilz GP, Zangerle R, Baier-Bitterlich G, Wachter H, Fuchs D. Soluble receptors for tumour necrosis factor in clinical laboratory diagnosis. Eur J Haematol. 1995;54:1–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0609.1995.tb01618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dowlati Y, Herrmann N, Swardfager W, Liu H, Sham L, Reim EK, Lanctôt KL. A meta-analysis of cytokines in major depression. Biologic Psychiatry. 2010;67:446–457. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fawzy FI, Kemeny ME, Fawzy NW, Elashoff R, Morton D, Cousins N, Fahey JL. A structured psychiatric intervention for cancer patients II: Changes over time in immunological measures. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47:729–735. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810200037005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Futterman AD, Wellisch DK, Zighelboim J, Luna-Raines M, Weiner H. Psychological and immunological reactions of family members to patients undergoing bone marrow transplantation. Psychosom Med. 1996;58:472–480. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199609000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Goldfarb M, Brower S, Schwaitzberg SD. Minimally invasive surgery and cancer: controversies part 1. Surg Endosc. 2010;24:304–334. doi: 10.1007/s00464-009-0583-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Goodkin K, Blaney NT, Feaster D, Fletcher MA, Baum MK, Mantero-Atienza E, Klimas NG, Millon C, Szapocznik J, Eisdorfer C. Active coping style is associated with natural killer cell cytotoxicity in asymptomatic HIV-1 seropositive homosexual men. J Psychosom Res. 1992a;36:635–650. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(92)90053-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Goodkin K, Fuchs I, Feaster D, Leeka MA, Rishel DD. Life stressors and coping style are associated with immune measures in HIV-1 infection – a preliminary report. Int J Psychiatry Med. 1992b;22:155–172. doi: 10.2190/BD46-F4JD-K8TW-RUFH. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gore JL, Kwan L, Lee SP, Reiter RE, Litwin MS. Survivorship beyond convalescence: 48-month quality-of-life outcomes after treatment for localized prostate cancer. JNCI. 2009;101:888–892. doi: 10.1093/jnci/djp114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Grivennikov SI, Greten FR, Karin M. Immunity, inflammation, and cancer. Cell. 2010;140:883–899. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2010.01.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hoyt MA. Gender role conflict and emotional approach coping in men with cancer. Psychol Health. 2009;24:1–15. doi: 10.1080/08870440802311330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hoyt MA, Stanton AL, Irwin MR. Thomas, K.S. Cancer-related masculine threat, emotional approach coping, and physical functioning following treatment for prostate cancer. Health Psychol. 2013;32:66–74. doi: 10.1037/a0030020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Howren MB, Lamkin DM, Suls J. Associations of depression with C-reactive protein, IL-1, and IL-6: a metaanalysis. Psychosom Med. 2009;71:171–186. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181907c1b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Irwin M. Psychoneuroimmunology of depression: Clinical implications. Brain Behav Immun. 2002;21:185–194. doi: 10.1006/brbi.2001.0654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Irwin MR, Cole SW. Reciprocal regulation of the neural and innate immune systems. Nat Rev Immunol. 2011;11:625–632. doi: 10.1038/nri3042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Irwin MR, Miller AH. Depressive disorder and immunity: 20 years of progress and discovery. Brain Behav Immun. 2007;21:374–383. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Jayadevappa R, Malkowicz S, Chhatre S, Johnson J, Gallo J. The burden of depression in prostate cancer. Psychooncology. 2011;21:1338–1345. doi: 10.1002/pon.2032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kemeny ME. Emotions and the immune system. In: Ader R, editor. Psychoneuroimmunology. 4th ed. vol 1. Burlington: Elsevier Academic Press; 2007. pp. 619–629. [Google Scholar]
  29. Lee BN, Dantzer R, Langley KE, Bennett GJ, Dougherty PM, Dunn AJ, Meyers CA, Miller AH, Payne R, Reuben JM, Wang XS, Cleeland CS. A cytokine-based neuroimmunologic mechanism of cancer-related symptoms. Neuroimmunomodulation. 2004;11:279–292. doi: 10.1159/000079408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lichtenthal WG, Cruess DG, Schuchter LM, Ming ME. Psychosocial factors related to the correspondence of recipient and provider perceptions of social support among patients diagnosed with or at risk for malignant melanoma. J Health Psychol. 2006;8:705–719. doi: 10.1177/13591053030086005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Litwin MS, Melmed GY, Nakazon T. Life after radical prostatectomy: longitudinal study. J Urol. 2001;166:587–592. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Low CA, Stanton AL, Bower J. Effects of acceptance-oriented versus evaluative emotional processing on heart rate recovery and habituation. Emotion. 2008;8:419–424. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Low CA, Stanton AL, Danoff-Burg S. Expressive disclosure and benefit finding among breast cancer patients: Mechanisms for positive health effects. Health Psychol. 2006;25:181–189. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.2.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lutgendorf SK, Antoni MH, Ironson G, Kilmas N, Fletcher MA, Scheiderman N. Cognitive processing style, mood, and immune function following HIV seropositivity notification. Cog Ther Res. 1997;21:157–184. [Google Scholar]
  35. Lutgendorf SK, Sood AK. Biobehavioral factors and cancer progression: Physiological pathways and mechanisms. Psychosom Med. 2011;73:724–730. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318235be76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mantovani A, Allavena P, Sica A, Balkwill F. Cancer-related inflammation. Nature. 2008;454:436–444. doi: 10.1038/nature07205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Master SL, Amodio DM, Stanton AL, Yee CM, Hilmert CJ, Taylor SE. Neurobiological correlates of coping through emotional approach. Brain Behav Immun. 2009;23:27–35. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2008.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Miller AH, Ancoli-Israel S, Bower JE, Capuron L, Irwin MR. Neuroendocrine-immune mechanisms of behavioral comorbidities in patients with cancer. J Clin Oncol. 2008;26:971–982. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2007.10.7805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Miller AH, Capuron L, Raison CL. Immunologic influences on emotion regulation. Clin Neurosci Res. 2005;4:325–333. [Google Scholar]
  40. Musselman DL, Miller AH, Porter MR, Manatunga A, Gao F, Penna S, Pearce BD, Landry J, Glover S, McDaniel JS, Nemeroff CB. Higher than normal plasma interleukin-6 concentrations in cancer patients with depression: preliminary findings. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:1252–1257. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Namiki S, Kwan L, Kagawa-Singer M, Tochigi T, Loritani N, Terai A, Arai Y, Litwin MS. Sexual function following radical prostatectomy: A prospective study of cultural differences between Japanese and American men. Prostate Cancer Prostatic Dis. 2008;11:298–302. doi: 10.1038/sj.pcan.4501013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. O'Conner MF, Bower JE, Cho HJ, Creswell JD, Dimitrov S, Hamby ME, Hoyt MA, Martin JL, Robles TF, Sloan EK, Thomas K, Irwin MR. To assess, to control, to exclude: Effects of biobehavioral factors on circulating inflammatory markers. Brain Behav Immun. 2009;23:887–897. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. O’Brien S, Scully P, Fitzgerald P, Scott LV, Dinan TG. Plasma cytokine profiles in depressed patients who fail to respond to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor therapy. J Psychiatric Res. 2007;41:326–331. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Orre IJ, Murison R, Dah AA, Ueland T, Aukrust P, Fosså SD. Levels of circulating interleukin-1 receptor antagonist and C-reactive protein in long-term survivors of testicular cancer with chronic cancer-related fatigue. Brain Behav Immun. 2009;23:868–874. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Reiche EM, Nunes SO, Morimoto HK. Stress, depression, the immune system, and cancer. Lancet Oncol. 2004;5:617–625. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(04)01597-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Reichenberg A, Yirmiya R, Schuld A, Kraus T, Haack MA, Morag A, Pollmächer T. Cytokine associated emotional and cognitive disturbances in humans. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:445–452. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.5.445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rosenkranz MA, Jackson DC, Dalton KM, Dolski I, Ryff CD, Singer BH, Muller D, Kalin NH, Davidson RJ. Affective style and in vivo immune response: Neurobehavioral mechanisms. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2003;100:11148–11152. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1534743100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Rude SS, Maestas KL, Neff K. Paying attention to distress: What’s wrong with rumination? Cog Emotion. 2007;21:843–864. [Google Scholar]
  49. Savard J, Simard S, Hervouet S, Ivers H, Lacombe L, Fradet Y. Insomnia in men treated with radical prostatectomy for prostate cancer. Psychooncology. 2005;14:147–156. doi: 10.1002/pon.830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Segerstrom SC, Miller GE. Psychological stress and the human immune system: A meta-analytic study of 30 years of inquiry. Psychol Bull. 2004;130:601–630. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sepah SC, Bower JE. Positive affect and inflammation during radiation treatment for breast and prostate cancer. Brain Behav Immun. 2009;23:1068–1072. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.06.149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Seruga B, Zhang H, Bernstein LJ, Tannock LF. Cytokines and their relationship to the symptoms and outcome of cancer. Nat Rev Cancer. 2008;8:887–899. doi: 10.1038/nrc2507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Shafique K, Proctor MJ, McMillan DC, Qureshi K, Leung H, Morrison DS. Systemic inflammation and survival of patients with prostate cancer: Evidence from the Glasgow Inflammation Outcome Study. Prostate Cancer Prostatic Dis. 2012;15:195–201. doi: 10.1038/pcan.2011.60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Sharpley C, Bitsika V, Christie D. Psychological distress among prostate cancer patients: Fact or fiction? Clin Med Oncol. 2008;2:563–572. doi: 10.4137/cmo.s955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Spinas GA, Keller U, Brockhaus M. Release of soluble receptors for tumor necrosis factor (TNF) in relation to circulating TNF during experimental endotoxinemia. J Clin Invest. 1992;90:533–536. doi: 10.1172/JCI115891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Stanford JL, Feng Z, Hamilton AS, Gilliland FD, Stephenson RA, Eley JW, Albertsen PC, Harlan LC, Potosky AL. Urinary and sexual function after radical prostatectomy for clinically localized prostate cancer. JAMA. 2000;283:354–360. doi: 10.1001/jama.283.3.354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Stanton AL. Regulating emotions during stressful experiences: The adaptive utility of coping through emotional approach. In: Folkman S, editor. The Oxford Handbook of Stress, Health, and Coping. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc; 2011. pp. 369–386. [Google Scholar]
  58. Stanton AL, Danoff-Burg S, Cameron CL, Bishop MM, Collins CA, Kirk SB, Twillman R. Emotionally expressive coping predicts psychological and physical adjustment to breast cancer. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000a;68:875–882. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Stanton AL, Danoff-Burg S, Cameron CL, Ellis AP. Coping through emotional approach: Problems of conceptualization and confounding. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;66:350–362. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.66.2.350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Stanton AL, Danoff-Burg S, Sworowski LA, Collins CA, Branstetter AD, Rodriguez-Hanley A, Kirk SB, Austenfeld JL. Randomized, controlled trial of written emotional expression and benefit finding in breast cancer patients. J Clin Oncol. 2002;20:4160–4168. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2002.08.521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Stanton AL, Kirk SB, Cameron CL, Danoff-Burg S. Coping through emotional approach: Scale construction and validation. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000b;78:1150–1169. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.6.1150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Steptoe A, Hamer M, Chida Y. The effects of acute psychological stress on circulating inflammatory factors in humans: A review and meta-analysis. Brain Behav Immun. 2007;21:901–912. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Stone HB, Coleman CN, Anscher MS, McBride WH. Effects of radiation on normal tissue: Consequences and mechanisms. Lancet Oncol. 2003;4:529–536. doi: 10.1016/s1470-2045(03)01191-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES