Abstract
Objective
To examine the association of perceived stress with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) markers of subclinical cerebrovascular disease in an elderly cohort.
Design
Cross -sectional study
Setting
Community based cohort in Chicago, IL
Participants
571 adults (57% female; 58.1% African American; 41.9% non-Hispanic white; mean [SD] age=79.8 [5.9] years) from the Chicago Health and Aging Project (CHAP), an epidemiologic study of aging, completed questionnaires on perceived stress, medical history, and demographics as part of an in-home assessment, and 5 years later underwent a clinical neurological examination and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of the brain.
Outcome Measures
Volumetric MRI assessments of white matter hyperintensity volume (WMHV), total brain volume (TBV), and cerebral infarction.
Results
Stress was measured with 6 items from the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS); item responses, ranging from never (0) to often (3), were summed to create an overall stress score, (mean (SD) = 4.9 (3.3), range 0–18). Most participants had some evidence of vascular disease on MRI, with 153 (26.8%) having infarctions. In separate linear and logistic regression models adjusted for age, sex, education, race and time between stress assessment and MRI, each 1-point increase in PSS score was associated with significantly lower TBV (coefficient= −0.111; SE=0.049; t[563]=−2.28; P=0.023) and 7% greater odds of infarction (odds ratio=1.07; 95% CI=1.01, 1.13; Wald χ2[1]=4.90; P=0.027). PSS scores were unrelated to WMHV. Results were unchanged with further adjustment for smoking, body mass index, physical activity, history of heart disease, stroke, diabetes, and hypertension, depressive symptoms and dementia.
Conclusions
Greater perceived stress was significantly and independently associated with cerebral infarction and lower brain volume assessed 5 years later in this elderly cohort.
Keywords: MR measures, perceived stress, biracial population sample
INTRODUCTION
A growing body of research shows that various indicators of stress, including job strain, chronic severe stress, and poor stress-coping capability, are associated with excess risk of incident stroke and stroke-related mortality.1–5 These studies add to the existing literature regarding the influences of psychosocial factors on cardiovascular disease (CVD), which clearly documents the important contributions of chronic psychological stress to CVD morbidity, mortality and other CVD-related health outcomes.1,6–10 A number of studies have examined measures of stress in relation to prevalence and progression of subclinical atherosclerosis or other subclinical forms of CVD. However, few previous studies have investigated stress in relation to subclinical indicators of cerebrovascular disease as revealed by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Understanding the impact of stress earlier in the disease process may further understanding of disease progression and of the mechanisms by which chronic stress can contribute to increased stroke risk.
We used data from more than 500 participants in the Chicago Health and Aging Project to examine the association between perceived stress and subclinical cerebrovascular disease measured on average five years later. We hypothesized that higher levels of perceived stress would be associated with greater subclinical cerebrovascular disease, as measured by MRI and manifested as greater white matter hyperintensity volume (WMHV), lower total brain volume (TBV) and increased risk of cerebral infarction. We further hypothesized that these associations would be independent of known vascular risk factors and conditions.
METHODS
Study Design
The Chicago Health and Aging Project (CHAP) is a longitudinal population-based study of common chronic health problems among older adults, with a focus on dementia and cognitive decline. CHAP study design and population characteristics have been previously reported.11,12 Briefly, a complete census of three adjacent community areas in south Chicago, IL was completed between 1993–97. All residents identified via the census who were age 65 years or older were invited to participate; 78.9% of eligible persons (N=6,158) agreed and provided informed consent. This is the CHAP Original Cohort. The study population reflects the race/ethnicity makeup of the community areas at the time of the census, predominantly African American and non-Hispanic white (<1% reported another race category or Hispanic ethnicity). Five data collection cycles have occurred, with data obtained, on average, every 3 years; i.e., 1993–97 (cycle 1), 1997–99 (cycle 2), 2000–02 (cycle 3), 2003–05 (cycle 4), and 2006–08 (cycle 5). Beginning with data collection cycle 3, residents from the CHAP community areas who had since turned 65 years old and who were identified through the previous community census or commercially available lists were enrolled into CHAP. These are the CHAP study Successive Cohorts and they follow the same three-year interview cycles and complete the same measures as the CHAP Original Cohort. For analyses, data from both cohorts are combined.
Procedures
Each CHAP data collection cycle has: (1) an in-home population interview, with brief tests of physical function, psychosocial variables, and cognitive function; and (2) a clinical evaluation of a stratified random sample (about one-sixth) of subjects at each cycle that includes neuropsychological testing, a neurological examination, medical history, laboratory testing, and expert clinical assessment for dementia. Starting with cycle 3 and continuing with subsequent cycles, those completing the clinical evaluation were invited to complete a neurological imaging evaluation (MRI). Clinical evaluations usually take place in the subjects’ homes and are conducted by a team of examiners led by a senior neurologist (N.T.A). Structured neurological examinations and medical histories are performed by specially trained nurse clinicians. The diagnosis of dementia required the senior neurologist’s assessment of loss of cognitive function and impairment in two or more areas during cognitive performance testing. The diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease used the criteria of the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke-Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS-ADRDA Criteria),13 except that subjects that met these criteria and had another condition that impaired cognition were retained (that is, enrolled in the present study). Vascular dementia diagnosis followed the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke and Association Internationale pour la Recherché et l’Enseignement (NINDS-AIREN) criteria.14
Study Sample
As previously reported 15, of the 1260 persons who completed the clinical evaluation as part of cycle 3 and cycle 4, 663 (52.6%) participated in the MRI evaluation and thus were eligible for inclusion in the present analysis. Persons without MRI data (n=597) were older (mean age, 81.5 ± 6.5 vs. 80.1 ± 5.9 years; t[1211]=4.1, P<0.001), less educated (mean years of education, 12.4 ± 3.5 vs. 12.9 ± 3.7, t[1258]=−2.3, P=0.02), and more likely to be women (396/775 women vs. 201/485 men, χ2[1]=11.15, P=0.008) compared to those who had complete MRI data. The final study sample was limited to 571 subjects who had complete data on the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), which was obtained at the CHAP study visit at cycle 2, as well as complete data on the MRI measures, which were obtained at either cycle 3 or 4. Due to missing values on covariates, the N for analyses ranged from 557 to 571. The mean (SD) for time between stress assessment and MRI was 5.2 (1.8) years. Signed informed consent was obtained from each subject, and the Institutional Review Board of Rush University Medical Center approved the study.
MRI Evaluation
The methods for MR image acquisition and assessment of TBV, WHMV, and degree of cerebral infarction have been previously described.15–17 The same MRI methods were used for this study. White matter hyperintensity volume was calculated as a proportion of total cranial volume (to account for variations in head size) and log-transformed (natural log) to achieve a normal distribution (skew, −0.21). Total brain volume was computed as the ratio of total parenchymal volume to total cranial volume and had an approximately normal distribution (skew, −0.10). The presence or absence of cerebral infarction was determined manually by the operator, based on the lesion’s size and imaging characteristics. The image analysis system allowed for superimposition of the fluid-attenuation inversion recovery (FLAIR) image, proton-density image, and T2-weighted image at 3 times magnified view, to assist in interpreting lesion characteristics. Signal void seen on T2-weighted images was interpreted as being indicative of a vessel. Lesions 3 mm or larger were considered to be cerebral infarctions. Inter-rater reliability for the MRI measures has been previously published,18,19 and intra- and inter-rater reliabilities for this study were consistently above 0.90. The frequency of cerebral infarction had a skewed distribution; therefore, for analyses, we created a dichotomous variable (“yes” or “no”) to indicate the presence of an infarction. MRI scoring was completed by a neurologist (C.D.) who was blind to the data on reported levels of stress from CHAP participants.
Assessment of Stress
Stress was assessed by the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) as part of the in-home CHAP interview. The PSS assesses the degree to which the respondent appraises situations in the previous month to be stressful and is considered an indicator of the global level of stress experienced by a person.20,21 The most frequently used version of the PSS includes 10 items; due to considerations regarding participant burden and the wide range of assessments completed as part of the CHAP in-home interview, six of the 10 PSS items were used, as follows: (1) In the last month, how often have you been upset because of something that happened unexpectedly? (2) In the last month, how often have you felt that you were unable to control the important things in your life? (3) In the last month, how often have you felt that confident about your ability to handle your personal problems? (4) In the last month, how often have you felt that things weren’t going your way? (5) In the last month, how often have you felt that you were on top of things? and (6) In the last month, how often have you felt difficulties were piling up so high that you could not overcome them? PSS item responses typically are on a 5- point scale ranging from never (0) to very often (4). However, to streamline responses to a number of questionnaire items in the CHAP in home interview, we modified the response options slightly, creating a 4-point scale, with categories that ranged from never (0) to often (3). Scores for positively worded items were reverse-coded and responses to all items were summed (range 0–18) to create an overall score, where higher scores indicate greater stress. These scores had an approximately normal distribution and required no transformation. The PSS is well validated and has been used widely in epidemiologic studies.20,21 The 6 PSS items show good reliability (Cronbach’s coefficient alpha, 0.75) in CHAP. A 4-item version of the PSS, which includes items 2, 3, 4, and 6 above, also has been published;21 the correlation between the 6 items used in CHAP and the 4-item scale is > 0.95.
Assessment of covariates
Several questions assessing self-reporting of vascular risk factors were asked during the CHAP in-home interviews, including a history of smoking, heart disease, stroke, diabetes, and use of antihypertensive medications. Smoking status was classified on the basis of questions about whether the patient currently smoked, smoked in the past, or had never smoked, and was coded according to these questions as “ever smoked” or “never smoked”. Heart disease was ascertained by the question, “Have you ever been told by a nurse or physician that you have had a MI, or experienced angina?” and coded as yes or no. Diabetes was identified by (a) self-reported history of diagnosis of diabetes, or (b) by medication to treat diabetes, as determined by direct inspection of prescriptions and of prescription medication containers. History of hypertension was identified by (a) self-reported history of diagnosis of hypertension, (b) measured blood pressure at the CHAP visit ≥ 140/90 mmHg based on the average of 2 seating measurements after at least a 5-minute rest, or (c) by use of antihypertensive medications ascertained by direct inspection of prescription medication containers. BMI was calculated as weight in kilograms divided by meters squared; height and weight were measured using standard protocols appropriate for elderly adults 22. Physical activity was assessed via self-report using questions from the Established Populations for Epidemiologic Studies of the Elderly (EPESE) project 23. Self-reported history for the diagnosis of stroke was obtained with the question, “Have you ever been told by a doctor, or nurse that you had a stroke?” and was rated as “yes” or “no.” Depressive symptoms were assessed with a 10-item form of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), which was developed for use with older cohorts.24 The items inquire about depressive symptoms during the past week, and the dichotomous (“yes” or “no”) responses are coded in a manner in which higher scores indicate greater depressive symptomatology (range=0–10). The CES-D is widely used in epidemiologic studies of older persons, and the reliability of this version of the CES-D has been established.24 For all participants, data on vascular risk factors and the CES-D were obtained coincident with the data collection cycle when perceived stress was measured.
Data analysis
Descriptive statistics were calculated for the demographic characteristics of our sample. Correlations (r) and t-tests (t) were used to examine the relationship between perceived stress and socio-demographic characteristics of the sample. The association of the perceived stress measure with each MRI measure was examined in a series of separate linear (for TBV and WMHV) and logistic (for cerebral infarctions) regression models. Model 1 included covariates for age, sex, education, race and time from the ascertainment of perceived stress to MRI (years). Model 2 additionally adjusted for five vascular risk factors, including history of diabetes, heart disease, stroke, hypertension, and smoking. Model 3 further adjusted for depressive symptoms and a dementia diagnosis. Analyses were first conducted with perceived stress modeled continuously; subsequently, analyses were repeated to evaluate whether a threshold effect for stress existed by modeling the PSS scores in approximate tertiles from low (reference) to high levels of stress. Interactions were tested between perceived stress and age, sex, education, and race. In addition, sensitivity analyses were conducted that excluded persons with a self-reported history of stroke at any CHAP assessment prior to the MRI. Model assumptions about linearity, normality, independence, and homoscedasticity of errors were assessed graphically and analytically and were adequately met. Analyses were performed using SAS/STAT software version 9.2 (SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina, United States). Results with a P < 0.05 are reported as significant unless otherwise specified.
RESULTS
Participant characteristics
Table 1 presents the demographic and neurologic imaging characteristics for the sample overall, and also by each categorical level of perceived stress. The mean PSS score in our sample was 4.9 (SD=3.3). Perceived stress was unrelated to age (r[569]=0.017, P=0.68), educational attainment (r[569]=−0.078, P=0.061), or sex (t[569]=0.77, P=0.44). African Americans reported higher perceived stress levels than did non-Hispanic whites [mean = 5.4 for African Americans versus 4.1 for non-Hispanic whites, t[569]=−4.59, P<0.0001].
Table 1.
All Participants (N=571) | Low Stress (N=159) | Moderate Stress (N=238) | High Stress (N=174) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N (%) | Mean (SD) | N (%) | Mean (SD) | N (%) | Mean (SD) | N (%) | Mean (SD) | |
Age | 79.8 (5.9) | 79.8 (5.9) | 79.0 (5.8) | 80.4 (6.1) | ||||
Sex | ||||||||
Male | 245 (42.9) | 73 (46.0) | 100 (42.0) | 72 (41.4) | ||||
Female | 326 (57.1) | 86 (54.1) | 138 (58.0) | 102 (58.6) | ||||
Race | ||||||||
Black | 332 (58.1) | 78 (49.1) | 127 (53.4) | 127 (73) | ||||
White | 239 (41.9) | 81 (50.9) | 111 (46.6) | 47 (27) | ||||
Education (years) | 12.9 (3.7) | 13.4 (3.6) | 13.1 (3.7) | 12.4 (3.7) | ||||
Smoking | ||||||||
Ever | 286 (50.1) | 79 (50.3) | 118(49.8) | 86 (50.3) | ||||
Never | 285 (49.9) | 78 (49.7) | 119 (50.2) | 85 (49.7) | ||||
Body Mass Index (kg/m2) | 27.0 (5.1) | 27.2 (4.7) | 26.9 (5.4) | 26.8 (5.1) | ||||
Physical Activity a | 2.5 (4.2) | 2.6(3.9) | 2.9 (4.7) | 2.0 (3.4) | ||||
Chronic Conditions b | ||||||||
Diabetes | 51 (9.0) | 10 (6.4) | 25 (10.6) | 16 (9.4) | ||||
Heart Disease | 100 (17.7) | 33 (21.0) | 42 (17.7) | 25 (14.6) | ||||
Stroke | 65 (11.5) | 15 (9.6) | 27 (11.4) | 23 (13.5) | ||||
Hypertension | 461 (81.6) | 117 (74.5) | 173 (73) | 129 (75.4) | ||||
Dementia | 81 (14.2) | 21 (13.2) | 29 (12.3) | 31 (17.8) | ||||
Depressive symptoms c | 1.3 (1.8) | 0.8 (1.3) | 1.1 (1.6) | 2.1 (2.1) | ||||
Perceived Stress d | 4.9 (3.3) | 1.1 (0.9) | 4.4 (1.1) | 9.1 (1.9) | ||||
MRI measures e | ||||||||
Total brain volume (%) | 74.3 (4.6) | 74.6 (4.5) | 74.6 (4.8) | 73.7 (4.4) | ||||
WMHV (%) | −5.13 (1.09) | −5.18 (1.04) | −5.18 (1.08) | −5.0 (1.12) | ||||
Infarcts (yes/no) | 153 (26.8) | 32 (20.1) | 64 (26.9) | 57 (32.8) |
Note. Due to small numbers of missing values on smoking, blood pressure, chronic conditions, and depressive symptoms, Ns for these variables ranged from 558 to 565.
Physical activity measured by self-report using questions from the Established Populations for Epidemiologic Studies of the Elderly. Chronic conditions (except dementia) defined by self-report of a physician diagnosis of each condition; diabetes further defined by use of insulin or other medications for diabetes; hypertension also included measured blood pressure of 140/90 mmHg or higher and/or use of anti-hypertensive medications; dementia defined by the loss of cognitive function and impairment in two or more areas during cognitive performance testing.
Depressive symptoms measured by 10-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; scores ranged from 0–10.
Stress categories based on approximate tertiles of the distribution of perceived stress scores.
Total brain volume calculated as (total parenchymal volume/total cranial volume) and white matter hyperintensity volume (WMHV) calculated as natural log(white matter hyperintensity volume/total cranial volume). Infarcts defined as presence/absence of an infarction at least 3 mm in size.
Stress and MRI Outcomes
Table 2 presents findings from our primary analyses that evaluated the association of perceived stress with TBV, with results from the models with PSS modeled continuously shown in the upper half of the table and results from the models with PSS modeled categorically shown in the lower half of the table. Controlling for age, sex, race, education and time from stress ascertainment to MRI (Model 1), each 1-point higher PSS score was associated with significantly lower TBV, assessed, on average, 5 years later. This association was unchanged with further adjustment for vascular risk factors (Model 2) and depressive symptoms and dementia (Model 3). Subsequent categorical analysis using approximate tertiles of stress revealed a graded association between stress level and subsequent TBV. In the fully adjusted model (Model 3), participants who had the highest levels of stress had nearly 1.2% lower TBV relative to those with low stress. Although persons with medium stress levels had approximately 0.5% lower TBV than those with low stress, this difference was not significant.
Table 2.
Results of linear regression models with perceived stress modeled continuously. | |||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 (N=571) | Model 2 (N=559) | Model 3 (N=557) | |||||||||||||
Regression coefficient | S.E. | t[563] | P-value | Adjusted R2 | Regression coefficient | S.E. | t[544] | P-value | Adjusted R2 | Regression coefficient | S.E. | t[540] | P-value | Adjusted R2 | |
Perceived Stress | −0.111 | 0.049 | −2.28 | 0.023 | 0.317 | −0.105 | 0.049 | −2.14 | 0.032 | 0.329 | −0.134 | 0.05 | −2.69 | 0.0074 | 0.354 |
Results of linear regression models with perceived stress modeled categorically | |||||||||||||||
Model 1 (N=571) | Model 2 (N=559) | Model 3 (N=557) | |||||||||||||
Regression coefficient | S.E. | t[562] | P-value | Adjusted R2 | Regression coefficient | S.E. | t[543] | P-value | Adjusted R2 | Regression coefficient | S.E. | t[539] | P-value | Adjusted R2 | |
Low Stress | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- |
Mod. Stress | −0.449 | 0.392 | −1.15 | 0.252 | −0.438 | 0.389 | −1.13 | 0.261 | −0.532 | 0.383 | −1.39 | 0.165 | |||
High Stress | −0.965 | 0.429 | −2.25 | 0.025 | 0.316 | −0.920 | 0.427 | −2.15 | 0.032 | 0.328 | −1.178 | 0.432 | −2.73 | 0.0066 | 0.353 |
Note.
Total brain volume calculated as total parenchymal volume/total cranial volume. Values shown are from regression models adjusted for covariates, as follows: Model 1 included age, sex, race, education, time between stress assessment and MRI. Model 2 included Model 1 covariates and vascular risk factors (risk factors included history of smoking, heart disease, stroke, diabetes, and history of hypertension). Model 3 included Model 2 covariates and depressive symptoms and diagnosis of dementia. S.E. = standard error; in each model shown, df were calculated as N – number of parameters in the model.
Table 3 shows the results for the stress analyses in relation to infarctions as shown on MR images, with the findings from the logistic regression models with PSS modeled continuously shown in the upper half of the table and findings with PSS modeled in approximate tertiles shown in the lower half of the table. Controlling for age, sex, education, and time from stress ascertainment to MRI (Model 1), each 1-point higher PSS score was associated with a 7% greater odds of having an infarction. The inclusion of vascular risk factors (Model 2), dementia, and depression (Model 3) did not modify any observed associations. With PSS modeled categorically, we observed a graded association between stress and odds of occurrence of a cerebral infarction. As shown in the bottom half of Table 3, compared to those with low stress levels, participants who experienced high levels of stress had more than twice the odds of cerebral infarction, which was significant in all models. For the moderate stress group, the OR for infarcts was approximately 1.5 but this did not differ significantly from the low stress group.
Table 3.
Results of logistic regression models with perceived stress modeled continuously. | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | ||||||||||
Odds Ratio | 95% CI | Wald χ2 | P-value | Odds Ratio | 95% CI | Wald χ2 | P-value | Odds Ratio | 95% CI | Wald χ2 | P-value | |
Perceived Stress | 1.07 | 1.01–1.13 | 4.90 | 0.027 | 1.07 | 1.00–1.13 | 4.17 | 0.041 | 1.06 | 0.99–1.12 | 3.16 | 0.076 |
Results of logistic regression models with perceived stress modeled categorically. | ||||||||||||
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | ||||||||||
Odds Ratio | 95% CI | Wald χ2 | P-value | Odds Ratio | 95% CI | Wald χ2 | P-value | Odds Ratio | 95% CI | Wald χ2 | P-value | |
Low Stress | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | --- | -- |
Mod. Stress | 1.52 | 0.93–2.49 | 2.82 | 0.093 | 1.50 | 0.90–2.49 | 2.41 | 0.120 | 1.49 | 0.90–2.48 | 2.38 | 0.123 |
High Stress | 2.07 | 1.23–3.50 | 7.49 | 0.006 | 3.51 | 1.17–3.82 | 6.42 | 0.011 | 1.94 | 1.11–3.40 | 5.41 | 0.020 |
Note: N=571 in Model 1; due to missing data on covariates, N= 559 in Model 2 and N= 557 in Model 3. Infarcts modeled as presence/absence of lesions at least 3 mm in size. Model 1 included covariates for age, sex, race, education, time between stress assessment and MRI. Model 2 included the covariates from Model 1 and vascular risk factors (history of smoking, heart disease, stroke, diabetes, and history of hypertension). Model 3 included all covariates from Model 2 and depressive symptoms and diagnosis of dementia. In each model shown, df = 1 for each variable in the model.
Stress scores were unrelated to WMHV (coefficient=0.019; SE=0.013; t[563]=1.46; P =0.14), which was unchanged after additional covariate adjustment (not shown). Because little is known about the relationship of stress to outcomes as shown on MR images, we also examined whether the observed associations varied in demographically defined subgroups. No interactions between PSS score and age, sex, education, or race (each tested as a two-way interaction in separate models) were noted (data not shown).
Sensitivity Analyses
In subsequent analyses that excluded 65 persons with a history of stroke and 6 additional persons whose stroke history was unknown, we observed similar though somewhat weaker associations for both total brain volume and infarcts. In a risk factor-adjusted model, perceived stress modeled continuously was associated with lower brain volume but the association was marginally significant (coefficient=−0.091; SE=0.052; t[479]=−1.76; P=0.079). However, the most stressed group (top tertile) still showed significantly lower brain volume relative to the least stressed group (coefficient=−0.897; SE=0.45; t[478]=−2.0; P=0.047). For infarcts, a graded association with stress levels was evident. The odds ratios were 1.31 (95% CI=0.75, 2.26; Wald χ 2[1]=0.93; P=0.33) for the moderate stress group and 1.82 (95% CI=1.01, 3.27; Wald χ2[1]=3.93; P<0.05) for the high stress group, adjusting for demographic characteristics and vascular risk factors.
DISCUSSION
In this study of more than 500 elderly individuals from a population sample of African Americans and non-Hispanic whites, we found that greater perceived stress was significantly and independently associated with TBV and MRI infarcts, but not with WMHV, measured five years later. The association with TBV and infarcts measures remained robust after controlling for vascular risk factors, depressive symptoms, or dementia status. Furthermore, no differences were noted in the association of stress with cerebral infarction as shown on MRI and with TBV by socio-demographic subgroup. This suggests that perceived stress may contribute to subclinical vascular findings on MR images in a diverse population of older adults.
We are not aware of any previous population-based studies that directly examined the relationship of perceived stress measures to multiple subclinical MRI markers. Our results, however, are consistent with smaller clinical studies that have examined the association of MR imaging markers to other psychosocial factors. Indeed, a number of studies have linked decreased hippocampal volume to depressive episodes25–27 and psychiatric conditions.28–30 Other studies examining late-life depression, bipolar disorder, anxiety, and post-traumatic stress disorder have demonstrated an association between these conditions and smaller amygdala volumes.31–34 Depression also is recognized as an important risk factor for stroke35 and is related to subclinial cerebrovascular disease,30 yet the effects of stress on TBV and infarcts were independent of depressive symptoms in our analyses. Our findings suggest that perceived stress may reflect additional important psychological characteristics that are negatively and independently associated with subclinical MRI markers in old age.
The present study could not evaluate whether reported stress contributed to changes in brain volume, greater white matter hyperintensities or increased infarctions over time. However, our data on stress were collected on average over 5 years earlier than the MRI data and at least 3 years earlier for 90% of participants. Our findings thus show that perceptions of stress at an earlier point in time are related to future MRI markers of subclinical disease in this elderly cohort. It is possible that MRI abnormalities can influence perceptions of stress and to the extent any abnormalities were present at the earlier cycles of data collection when PSS scores were obtained this could be affecting our results. A small subset of CHAP participants have completed more than 1 MRI and as additional data become available, we will be able to assess whether stress relates to progression of subclinical cerebrovascular disease.
Although the relationship between stress and MRI markers in the general population remains poorly understood, the association between stress and health and disease has been well characterized. Some studies have shown that stress is related to outcomes that involve the cardiovascular and metabolic systems9,36 and the immunologic and inflammatory systems31 and that these contribute to morbidity and mortality from stroke.1–5, One possible mechanism suggests that stress can cause a surge in blood pressure that, in turn, may cause a cardiac event. Alternatively, sustained stress may accelerate atherosclerosis, increasing the risk of myocardial infarction or stroke. Another mechanism is related to the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis37 and the effects of glucocorticoids on the brain. Stress is thought to increase the activity of the HPA axis and the levels of these hormones, thereby causing structural and functional damage to the brain (i.e., reduction in hippocampus volumes)34. Some studies have implicated inflammatory and immunological processes as potential mediators between stress and health;33 others have examined genetic factors (e.g., allelic frequencies or polymorphisms) that may interact with chronic stress exposures to influence health,38–40 while others have focused on pathways related to socioeconomic, behavioral, and lifestyle variables that may provide links between psychosocial factors and health.41,42 Stress has also been postulated to influence the appearance of subclinical MRI markers on neuro-imaging through its association with underlying cardiovascular disease.43,44 In this community-based sample, however, adjusting for cardiovascular risk factors did not affect the association between stress and MRI infarcts and total brain volume, suggesting that these risk factors may not be an important mediator of this relationship.
In the present study, stress was associated with TBV and infarcts, but not with WMHV. Our findings are somewhat consistent with two prior studies that have examined stress or distress and brain volumes, including white matter hyperintensities. In a small study of 48 healthy postmenopausal women, Gianaros and colleagues45 reported that women with higher perceived stress scores over a 20-year period had decreased gray matter volume in both the right orbitofrontal cortex and right hippocampus, relative to women with low stress levels, though stress was not associated with total grey matter volume. No associations were noted between stress and white matter hyperintensities (graded by severity) in that study. A second study showed that psychological distress measured in midlife was related to atrophy in specific gray matter regions later in life in a population-based cohort of women 46. However, we were unable to ascertain regional brain volumes in the present study so we cannot specify whether hippocampal volumes or volumes of other brain regions were specifically related to stress levels in our older community-based cohort. Future research should address this issue.
Both our study and that of Gianaros et al45 failed to find an association between stress and WMHV, which is in contrast to the findings of Johansson et al46, who identified a link between distress at midlife and later odds of white matter lesions. While replication is needed in future studies, that two studies found no relation is interesting to the extent that WMHV has been thought to be a robust marker for clinical vascular disease, with prominent manifestations in the brain.39,40 The correlation between stress and WMHV in our study may not have reached statistical significance because a significant proportion of our sample was obtaining medical treatment with respect to cardiovascular risk factors (i.e., 74.1% of the sample was taking antihypertensive medications, while 25.9% of the sample had no history of hypertension, and less than 10% of the sample had a history of diabetes). Whether this or other factors play a role in the relation of stress to MRI markers remains to be determined in other studies, which would need to replicate and expand on the findings reported here. Data on biomarkers for stress may help clarify these findings.
The strengths of our study are the inclusion of both African American and non-Hispanic white participants, the use of volumetric brain MRI techniques, and the availability of both medical and psychosocial data. This study also has important limitations. These analyses utilized a single self-reported measure of perceived stress, which may have weakened our ability to detect levels of perceived stress. We also did not include laboratory stress biomarker data, which would have provided us with a better understanding of the potential mechanisms linking stress to subclinical cerebrovascular disease. Despite this, however, we did find associations of stress with two of our MRI measures. Our MRI data does not distinguish volumes for brain regions so we could not analyze the association of stress with hippocampal volume, for example, which other research suggests may be particularly influenced by stress.47,48 Finally, although our measure of stress was obtained earlier in time than the measures of subclinical cerebrovascular disease, a lack of baseline MRI data precludes our ability to examine stress in relation to change in MRI indicators of subclinical cerebrovascular disease and does not allow us to rule out whether subclinical vascular disease was present at baseline. Longitudinal analyses will provide increased insight into the relationship of stress to subclinical MRI markers of cerebral infarction. Overall, our findings suggest that perceived stress may have a separate and distinct role in the brain, affecting the occurrence of subsequent MRI markers in an apparently healthy population.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) grants HL084209, AG11101, and ES010902. Dr. Clark was supported by grant 1UL1RR033183 from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR) of the NIH to the University of Minnesota Clinical and Translational Science Institute (CTSI). The University of Minnesota CTSI is part of a national Clinical and Translational Science Award consortium created to accelerate laboratory discoveries into treatments for patients. Contents of this manuscript are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the CTSI or the NIH.
We are indebted to the hundreds of participants in the CHAP study. The authors also with to thank the study coordinators, Jennifer Tarpey and Colleen Plunkett; data and analytic programmers, George Dombrowski, MS, and the faculty and staff of the Rush Institute for Healthy Aging.
Footnotes
DISCLOSURES:
Dr. Clark received research support from NIH grants R01 HL084209 and 1UL1RR033183.
Mr. Beck received research support from NIH grants R01 AG11101 and R01 HL084209.
Dr. Aggarwal has received honoraria for serving as a consultant for Pfizer from 2009–2010, and is funded by grants from NIH: R01 AG022018, P30 AG010161 [Clinical Core Co -Leader], R01 AG011101, R01 AG009966, R01 HL084209, R01 AG032247; U01 AG010483 [Site Principal Investigator] and the Alzheimer’s Association: IIRG-06-27429.
Dr. DeCarli serves as a Editor in Chief for Alzheimer’s Disease and Associated Dementias--An International Journal and is a Consultant to Advanir, Takeda and Bayer Corporations.
Dr. Everson-Rose was Principal Investigator and received research support from NIH grant R01 HL084209 and is an Associate Editor and on the Editorial Board of Psychosomatic Medicine.
Dr. Mendes de Leon served as Associate Editor of the Journals of Gerontology Social Sciences and serves on the editorial boards of Psychosomatic Medicine, the Journal of Aging & Health, the International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, and the Archives of Internal Medicine and receives/has received research support from the NIH [Principal Investigator or Co – Investigator] grants R01 AG021972, R01 HL084209, R01 AG11101, R01 ES010902, R01 AG032247 and R01 AG022018.
Dr. Evans received honoraria for serving on the Data Monitoring Committee of a trial for Eli Lily and Company from 2007 to 2008. He is funded [Principal Investigator or Co-Investigator] by NIH grants AG11101, AG036650, AG09966, AG030146, AG10161, AG021972, ES10902, NR009543, HL084209, and AG12505l.
Responsibilities | NTA | CJC | TLB | CMdeL | CDeC | DAE | SER |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Draft/revise manuscript | x | x | x | x | x | x | |
Study concept/Design | x | x | x | ||||
Analysis or Interpretation of data | x | x | x | x | x | x | x |
Contribution of patients/tools | x | x | x | x | x | ||
Acquisition of data | x | x | x | x | x | ||
Statistical analysis | x | x | x | x | x | ||
Study | |||||||
Coordination | x | ||||||
Obtaining funding | x | x | |||||
Other | x | x |
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Reference List
- 1.Olin JT, Dagerman KS, Fox LS, Bowers B, Schneider LS. Increasing ethnic minority participation in Alzheimer disease research. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2002;16 (Suppl 2):S82–S85. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200200002-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Harmsen P, Rosengren A, Tsipogianni A, Wilhelmsen L. Risk factors for stroke in middle-aged men in Goteborg, Sweden. Stroke. 1990;21(2):223–229. doi: 10.1161/01.str.21.2.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Andre-Petersson L, Engstrom G, Hagberg B, Janzon L, Steen G. Adaptive behavior in stressful situations and stroke incidence in hypertensive men: results from prospective cohort study “men born in 1914” in Malmo, Sweden. Stroke. 2001;32(8):1712–1720. doi: 10.1161/01.str.32.8.1712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Truelsen T, Nielsen N, Boysen G, Gronbaek M. Self-reported stress and risk of stroke: the Copenhagen City Heart Study. Stroke. 2003;34(4):856–862. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000062345.80774.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Tsutsumi A, Kayaba K, Kario K, Ishikawa S. Prospective study on occupational stress and risk of stroke. Arch Intern Med. 2009;169(1):56–61. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2008.503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Everson-Rose SA, Lewis TT. Psychosocial factors and cardiovascular diseases. Annu Rev Public Health. 2005;26:469–500. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lee S, Colditz G, Berkman L, Kawachi I. Caregiving to children and grandchildren and risk of coronary heart disease in women. Am J Public Health. 2003;93(11):1939–1944. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.11.1939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lee S, Colditz GA, Berkman LF, Kawachi I. Caregiving and risk of coronary heart disease in U.S. women: a prospective study. Am J Prev Med. 2003;24(2):113–119. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(02)00582-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bosma H, Peter R, Siegrist J, Marmot M. Two alternative job stress models and the risk of coronary heart disease. Am J Public Health. 1998;88(1):68–74. doi: 10.2105/ajph.88.1.68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Siegrist J, Peter R, Motz W, Strauer BE. The role of hypertension, left ventricular hypertrophy and psychosocial risks in cardiovascular disease: prospective evidence from blue-collar men. Eur Heart J. 1992;13 (Suppl D):89–95. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/13.suppl_d.89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bienias JL, Beckett LA, Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Evans DA. Design of the Chicago Health and Aging Project (CHAP) J Alzheimers Dis. 2003;5(5):349–355. doi: 10.3233/jad-2003-5501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Evans DA, Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Morris MC, Scherr PA, et al. Incidence of Alzheimer disease in a biracial urban community: relation to apolipoprotein E allele status. Arch Neurol. 2003;60(2):185–189. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.2.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology. 1984;34(7):939–944. doi: 10.1212/wnl.34.7.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Roman GC, Tatemichi TK, Erkinjuntti T, Cummings JL, Masdeu JC, Garcia JH, et al. Vascular dementia: diagnostic criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology. 1993;43(2):250–260. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.2.250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Aggarwal NT, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, De Jager PL, Bennett DA, Evans DA, et al. The association of magnetic resonance imaging measures with cognitive function in a biracial population sample. Arch Neurol. 2010;67(4):475–482. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.DeCarli C, Fletcher E, Ramey V, Harvey D, Jagust WJ. Anatomical mapping of white matter hyperintensities (WMH): exploring the relationships between periventricular WMH, deep WMH, and total WMH burden. Stroke. 2005;36(1):50–55. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000150668.58689.f2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.DeCarli C, Maisog J, Murphy DG. Method for quantification of brain, ventricular, and subarachnoid CSF volumes for MRI images. Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography. 1992;16(2):274–284. doi: 10.1097/00004728-199203000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.DeCarli C, Massaro J, Harvey D, Hald J, Tullberg M, Au R, et al. Measures of brain morphology and infarction in the framingham heart study: establishing what is normal. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26(4):491–510. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.DeCarli C, Murphy DG, Gillette JA, Haxby JV, Teichberg D, Schapiro MB, et al. Lack of age-related differences in temporal lobe volume of very healthy adults. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1994;15(4):689–696. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24(4):385–396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cohen S, Williamson G. Perceived stress in a probability sample of the U.S. In: Spacapam S, Oskamp S, editors. The social psychology of health: Claremont symposium on Applied Socail Psychology. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Chumlea WC, Roche AF, Mukherjee D. Some anthropometric indices of body composition for elderly adults. J Gerontol. 1986;41(1):36–39. doi: 10.1093/geronj/41.1.36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.McPhillips JB, Pellettera KM, Barrett-Connor E, Wingard DL, Criqui MH. Exercise patterns in a population of older adults. Am J Prev Med. 1989;5(2):65–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kohout FJ, Bermkamn LF, Evans DA, Cornoni-Huntley J. Two shorter forms of the CES-D Depression Symptoms Index. J Aging Hlth. 1993;5(5):179–193. doi: 10.1177/089826439300500202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.MacQueen GM, Campbell S, McEwen BS, Macdonald K, Amano S, Joffe RT, et al. Course of illness, hippocampal function, and hippocampal volume in major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(3):1387–1392. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0337481100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sheline YI, Wang PW, Gado MH, Csernansky JG, Vannier MW. Hippocampal atrophy in recurrent major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93(9):3908–3913. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.9.3908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Sheline YI, Sanghavi M, Mintun MA, Gado MH. Depression duration but not age predicts hippocampal volume loss in medically healthy women with recurrent major depression. J Neurosci. 1999;19(12):5034–5043. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-12-05034.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Stein MB, Simmons AN, Feinstein JS, Paulus MP. Increased amygdala and insula activation during emotion processing in anxiety-prone subjects. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(2):318–327. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.2.318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Phan KL, Fitzgerald DA, Nathan PJ, Tancer ME. Association between amygdala hyperactivity to harsh faces and severity of social anxiety in generalized social phobia. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59(5):424–429. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Wendell CR, Hosey MM, Lefkowitz DM, Katzel LI, Siegel EL, Rosenberger WF, et al. Depressive symptoms are associated with subclinical cerebrovascular disease among healthy older women, not men. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(10):940–947. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181d57a2b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Rauch SL, Whalen PJ, Shin LM, McInerney SC, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, et al. Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47(9):769–776. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00828-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Shin LM, Orr SP, Carson MA, Rauch SL, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow in the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex during traumatic imagery in male and female Vietnam veterans with PTSD. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(2):168–176. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.2.168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Campbell S, MacQueen G. An update on regional brain volume differences associated with mood disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2006;19(1):25–33. doi: 10.1097/01.yco.0000194371.47685.f2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Burke J, McQuoid DR, Payne ME, Steffens DC, Krishnan RR, Taylor WD. Amygdala volume in late-life depression: relationship with age of onset. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;19(9):771–776. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e318211069a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Pan A, Sun Q, Okereke OI, Rexrode KM, Hu FB. Depression and risk of stroke morbidity and mortality: a meta-analysis and systematic review. JAMA. 2011;306(11):1241–1249. doi: 10.1001/jama.2011.1282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Everson SA, Lynch JW, Chesney MA, Kaplan GA, Goldberg DE, Shade SB, et al. Interaction of workplace demands and cardiovascular reactivity in progression of carotid atherosclerosis: population based study. BMJ. 1997;314(7080):553–558. doi: 10.1136/bmj.314.7080.553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.McEwen BS, Biron CA, Brunson KW, Bulloch K, Chambers WH, Dhabhar FS, et al. The role of adrenocorticoids as modulators of immune function in health and disease: neural, endocrine and immune interactions. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1997;23(1–2):79–133. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(96)00012-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Brummett BH, Kuhn CM, Boyle SH, Babyak MA, Siegler IC, Williams RB. Cortisol responses to emotional stress in men: Association with a functional polymorphism in the 5HTR2C gene. Biol Psychol. 2012;89(1):94–98. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.09.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Brummett BH, Siegler IC, Ashley-Koch A, Williams RB. Effects of 5HTTLPR on cardiovascular response to an emotional stressor. Psychosom Med. 2011;73(4):318–322. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182118c16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.O’Hara R, Marcus P, Thompson WK, Flournoy J, Vahia I, Lin X, et al. 5-HTTLPR Short Allele, Resilience, and Successful Aging in Older Adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012 doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31823e2d03. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Steptoe A, Marmot M. Burden of psychosocial adversity and vulnerability in middle age: associations with biobehavioral risk factors and quality of life. Psychosom Med. 2003;65(6):1029–1037. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000097347.57237.2d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Gallo LC, Bogart LM, Vranceanu AM, Matthews KA. Socioeconomic status, resources, psychological experiences, and emotional responses: a test of the reserve capacity model. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;88(2):386–399. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Seshadri S, Wolf PA, Beiser A, Elias MF, Au R, Kase CS, et al. Stroke risk profile, brain volume, and cognitive function: the Framingham Offspring Study. Neurology. 2004;63(9):1591–1599. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000142968.22691.70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Solfrizzi V, Panza F, Colacicco AM, D’Introno A, Capurso C, Torres F, et al. Vascular risk factors, incidence of MCI, and rates of progression to dementia. Neurology. 2004;63(10):1882–1891. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000144281.38555.e3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Gianaros PJ, Jennings JR, Sheu LK, Greer PJ, Kuller LH, Matthews KA. Prospective reports of chronic life stress predict decreased grey matter volume in the hippocampus. Neuroimage. 2007;35(2):795–803. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Johansson L, Skoog I, Gustafson DR, Olesen PJ, Waern M, Bengtsson C, et al. Midlife psychological distress associated with late-life brain atrophy and white matter lesions: a 32-year population study of women. Psychosom Med. 2012;74(2):120–125. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318246eb10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.McEwen BS. The brain is the central organ of stress and adaptation. Neuroimage. 2009;47(3):911–913. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Sapolsky RM. Stress hormones: good and bad. Neurobiol Dis. 2000;7(5):540–542. doi: 10.1006/nbdi.2000.0350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]