Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Jul 11.
Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychiatry. 2007 Aug 20;63(1):72–79. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.031

Genetic Variation in COMT: Effects on Working Memory in Schizophrenic Patients, Their Siblings, & Healthy Controls

Catherine M Diaz-Asper 1, Terry E Goldberg 1, Bhaskar S Kolachana 1, Richard E Straub 1, Michael F Egan 1, Daniel R Weinberger 1
PMCID: PMC3708610  NIHMSID: NIHMS36160  PMID: 17707347

Abstract

Background

COMT val108/158met (rs4680) is thought to affect dopamine regulated prefrontal cortical activity during working memory (WM) tasks, and to weakly increase risk for developing schizophrenia. Recently, other single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) across the gene have emerged as additional risk factors for schizophrenia: namely rs737865, rs165599, and rs2097603. In a large sample, we examined whether these SNPs affect WM.

Method

Schizophrenic probands (n=325), their nonpsychotic siblings (n=359), and normal control subjects (n=330) completed tests of WM function. Data were analyzed with a series of mixed model ANOVAs.

Results

Val homozygotes performed most poorly on all conditions of the N-BACK, irrespective of diagnosis. Additionally, there was a trend towards a disease-only val108/158met effect on a test of attentional set-shifting; val homozygote probands performed most poorly. Significant or near-significant effects of rs737865 were found on all conditions of the N-BACK, with G homozygotes performing worst. There also was a disease-only COMT rs737865 effect on the 0-BACK. None of the other SNPs showed main effects by themselves. A haplotype constructed from promoter and val108/158met SNPs showed main effects on WM parameters, consistent with inverted U models of dopamine signaling.

Conclusions

We extended earlier findings of a val108/158met effect on WM function, and suggest that combinations of alleles within COMT may modulate the val108/158met effect in a nonlinear manner.

Keywords: Schizophrenia, Catechol-O-Methyltransferase, COMT, Working Memory, Attention, Prefrontal Cognition

INTRODUCTION

Cognitive dysfunction is widely accepted as a cardinal feature of schizophrenia, with deficits in attention, working memory and executive function commonly reported [1, 2]. One gene that affects neuronal functions involved in working memory, and may be a potential susceptibility gene to develop the disorder, is catechol-o-omethyltransferase (COMT), a methylation enzyme that converts dopamine to inactive 3-methoxytyramine [35]. The COMT gene, mapped to chromosome 22q11, contains a functional polymorphism, val108/158met (rs4680) that results in two common variants of the enzyme (val and met), and exerts a significant effect on enzyme activity (val being associated with higher activity). This polymorphism appears to affect dopamine regulated prefrontal cortical activity during working memory tasks [613]. For example, we have previously reported that COMT val108/158met genotype predicts performance on two different tests of working memory/executive function (the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and N-BACK), with the high activity val allele, which presumably allows less dopamine to become available at the synapse, being associated with poorest performance [7, 9]. These effects were observed independent of psychiatric diagnosis.

The relationship between COMT genotype and prefrontal cognitive function, together with demonstrated prefrontal functional abnormalities in schizophrenia [14, 15], implicates COMT genotype as a plausible risk factor to develop the disorder. In fact, inheritance of one or two val alleles may slightly elevate the risk of developing schizophrenia [3, 7, 1621]. Recently, other single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) across the COMT gene have emerged as possible alleles that increase risk for schizophrenia, including rs737865 (located in intron 1), rs165599 (located in the 3’ flanking region), and rs2097603 (in the P2 promoter region) [19, 2224].

The aim of the present study was to re-examine the previously reported relationship between the COMT val108/158met genotype and working memory function in an expanded test battery, and also to explore potential cognitive effects of three other COMT SNPs. Based on our previous work, we predicted that carriers of the COMT val allele would perform more poorly than met allele carriers on tests of working memory/executive function, notably updating [9] and distraction control [25]. We further predicted that COMT val108/158met genotype would affect basic attention or target detection functions on the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) in this larger sample, based on several lines of evidence, including animal research ([2628], ADHD [29], and cognitive enhancement [30].

Shifman and colleagues reported that the G–G genotype at rs737865 and at rs165599 were most strongly associated with risk to develop schizophrenia in their large Ashkenazi Jewish sample, (with slightly different effects based on gender) [19]. Therefore, we sought similar findings with rs737865 and rs165599 as with val108/158met; namely that the “risk” alleles of the two COMT SNPs (i.e., G–G) would be associated with deficits on tests sensitive to updating and attentional function. No clear risk allele has emerged for the P2 promoter (rs2097603), although Li and colleagues reported a nonsignificant preferential transmission of the A allele in 198 Chinese schizophrenic family trios [31]. This finding is consistent with evidence in lymphocytes and in brain that the A allele is associated with greater COMT activity, an effect analogous to the val allele [16]. We therefore predicted that the A allele would be most strongly associated with poor working memory function in our sample.

Finally, based on recent data from our group that haplotypic combinations of functional loci within the COMT gene can result in non-linear effects on prefrontal function, consistent with other evidence of an inverted U shaped dopamine-response function within prefrontal cortex [12, 32, 33], we examined the impact of COMT P2 promoter/val108/158met diplotypes on prefrontal cognitive function.

METHOD

Subjects

1014 subjects participated in this study, recruited from local and national sources as volunteers for the Clinical Brain Disorders Branch/NIMH sibling study, as has been previously described [7, 34]. In brief, all participants provided written informed consent of an IRB-approved protocol. Most families had two eligible full siblings (at least one of whom met DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, depressed type). All subjects were between 18 and 60 years of age, had a premorbid IQ above 70, and were able to provide informed consent. Subjects with significant medical problems, history of head trauma with loss of consciousness for longer than 5 minutes, or alcohol or drug abuse within the last 6 months were excluded. Given that COMT allele frequencies differ across some ethnic groups [24, 35], only Caucasian subjects were included in these analyses. Of the 1014 subjects, 250 were included in a previous study of COMT effects on cognition [9].

Each schizophrenic proband had at least one full sibling included in the study. No twins were included. Siblings with a history of schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, or schizophrenia spectrum disorder were excluded. All subjects were medically screened and interviewed by a research psychiatrist using the Structured Clinical Interview [36]. Demographic information for the sample is presented in Table 1. Diagnosis for schizophrenia was confirmed by a second board-certified psychiatrist. All schizophrenic patients were clinically stable and receiving antipsychotic medications (predominantly second generation) at the time of testing.

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics of the Groups

Characteristic Probands (n=325) Siblings (n=359) Controls (n=330)
Age (years)1 36.43 (9.21) 37.02 (9.81) 34.29 (10.35)
Sex (% male)2 77 40 47
Current IQ34 93.05 (12.06) 106.15 (10.42) 108.23 (8.99)
Premorbid IQ56 102.54 (11.31) 107.45 (9.93) 108.96 (8.67)
1

Control subjects were significantly younger than probands and siblings, who did not differ from each other

2

Significantly more males comprised the proband group

3

As measured by a 4-subtest short-form of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Revised

4

Probands’ current IQ was significantly lower than siblings and controls. There was a trend towards siblings’ IQ being lower than controls

5

As measured by the Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised, Reading subtest, standard score

6

Probands’ premorbid IQ was significantly lower than siblings, whose IQ was lower than controls

Procedure

Subjects were tested on a wide range of neuropsychological tests assessing various cognitive functions, including attention and concentration, language and visuospatial function, working memory and executive function, and semantic and episodic memory. Here, we report only the results of tests of intellectual function (WAIS-R) and select tests assessing attention and working memory/executive function (Table 2). Since our previous reports of a relationship between COMT val108/158met and working memory function [7, 9], we have included an additional test of executive function in our test battery (ID/ED), specific stages of which have been shown to be mediated by prefrontal dopaminergic function [27] [37] (Table 2).

Table 2.

Tests Used in the Current Study

Test Description Dependent Variable
N-BACK A number between 1 and 4 is presented every 1.8
seconds for a 200 msec duration, at set locations at
the points of a diamond-shaped box. Subjects are
instructed to recall the stimulus seen “N”
previously. In the 0-BACK condition, subjects
press the corresponding number onscreen (i.e., no
memory load per se). In the 1-BACK condition,
subjects do not respond to the first number that
appears, but when the second number appears,
press the first number, and so on. Subjects
complete six trials each of the 0, 1, and 2-BACK
conditions.
Mean percent correct
per condition
The total N available for the N-BACK was 620.
WCST1 In this computerized task, subjects view four key
cards at the top of the screen and must match a card
at the bottom of the screen to one of those above.
Each card contains stimuli that are characterized by
three different stimulus dimensions. The subject is
told to match each new card to one of the four key
cards, and is given feedback by the computer as to
whether each sort is correct or incorrect. After 10
consecutive correct sorts, the sorting rule changes
without the subject’s knowledge. The task ends
when all cards have been sorted, or when all
categories are attained. The maximum number of
cards is 128.
Percent perseverative
errors (t-score), and
number of categories
achieved
The total N available for the WCST was 880.
CPT2 In the Vigilance portion of the test, subjects view a
continuous sequence of single digits (presented at
the rate of one per second) on a screen and must
respond to an infrequently occurring target
sequence (1 followed by 9). The Distractibility
portion is similar, except that other digits appear
simultaneously flanking the stimuli that the subject
is told to pay attention to. Each portion of the test
lasts six minutes.
d` (takes into
account both
omission and
commission errors)
for Vigilance and
Distractibility.
The total N available for the CPT was 877.
WAIS-R3 All subjects completed a short form of the
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-
R;(Wechsler 1981)), consisting of Arithmetic,
Similarities, Picture Completion, and Digit Symbol
subtests.
IQ
The total N available for the WAIS-R was 896.
ID/ED4 Subjects learn a series of discriminations in which
one stimulus dimension (e.g., line contour) is
relevant and the other (e.g., object shape) is not.
On each trial, the two test stimuli appear randomly
in two of four squares positioned towards the
perimeter of the screen. The subject must
determine which is the correct stimulus by touching
one of the two patterns. After indicating their
choice, subjects receive visual and auditory
feedback. The test comprises 9 stages (subtests of
increasing difficulty), presented in a fixed order.
For each subtest, continuation to the next trial is
dependant on a criterion of 6 consecutive correct
discriminations being reached. If the criterion is
not reach at the 50th trial of a subtest, the test is
discontinued. Three stages were examined in the
current study: At the third (compound
discrimination, C_D) stage, additional stimuli are
superimposed on the original two line drawings
presented, (i.e., a second (and irrelevant) stimulus
dimension is introduced). Correct feedback is
contingent upon responding to the originally-
correct “line” stimulus. The sixth (intradimensional
shift, IDS) stage comprises new exemplars of “line”
and “shape.” Correct feedback is contingent upon
continuing to respond to the “line” stimuli. At the
eighth (extradimensional shift, EDS) stage, new
exemplars from each dimension are again
introduced, but correct feedback relies on selecting
one of the two “shape” stimuli.
Number of errors at
stages 3 (C_D), 6
(IDS), and 8 (EDS)
The total N available for ID/ED was 171.
1

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [49]

2

Continuous Performance Test [50]

3

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised [51]

4

Intradimensional-Extradimensional set shifting [52]

COMT genotyping

Blood samples were collected from all subjects, and DNA was extracted using standard methods. COMT val108/158met genotype was determined using the Taqman allelic discrimination assay, as previously described [38]. In addition to the COMT val108/158met polymorphism, SNPs rs737865 and rs165599 [19] and the promoter SNP rs2097603 [22] were genotyped using the same technique. All SNPs were in Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium for each group. D', a measure of linkage disequilibrium, was >.93 for rs2097603 and rs737865 for all groups; was between .44 and .49 for rs737865 and rs4680 for all groups; was between .47 and .53 for rs2097603 and rs4680 for all groups; and was between .65 and .79 for rs4680 and rs16559 for all groups.

Data Analyses

Parametric statistical analyses were conducted to determine if COMT genotype and diagnostic group differences were present in our working memory/executive function phenotypes (N-BACK, CPT, WCST, ID/ED). All subjects were included in the analyses, but family membership was treated as a random factor, and diagnosis, genotype, and gender were treated as fixed effects in a mixed model (SAS Mixed-Model ANOVA [39]). The dependent measures were treated as continuous variables. Although some studies have reported linkage disequilibrium between COMT val108/158met and rs737865 and rs165599, we did not find evidence of this in our sample, so chose to examine these SNPs independent of val108/158met.

We also conducted a series of similar mixed model analyses to assess the effects of a COMT haplotype on prefrontal cognition. In our sample, (and earlier reports, e.g. [23]) the rs737865 SNP is in strong linkage disequilibrium with the P2 promoter, rs2097603. We chose to examine the P2 promoter/val108/158met haplotype, which encompasses this linkage disequilibrium, based on evidence that rs209760 and val108/158met are functional variants in the gene (Bray 2003). To minimize phase uncertainty in diplotype assignment, we excluded all individuals who were heterozygous at both loci and then examined genotype interactions with ANOVAs. Haplotypes were determined using SNPHAP. Based on the putative inverted U-shaped curve model [12, 32], we predicted a G-G/val-val diplotype would result in superior prefrontal cognitive test performance compared with an A-A/val-val diplotype, presumably due to underavailability of dopamine at the synapse in the latter group, and an A-A/met-met diplotype would result in superior test performance compared with a G-G/met-met diplotype, as here overavailability of dopamine might impair information processing in the latter group.

For the ID/ED task, we analyzed subjects’ performance “non-conditionally,” which assigns subjects who failed a stage the maximal error score for all subsequent stages. Mean errors at three stages of the task were analyzed; the C_D stage, the intradimensional (IDS) shifting stage, and the extradimensional (EDS) stage (see Table 2).

To control for multiple comparisons within our data, we adopted a stringent Bonferroni corrected p-value of .001, based on 11 comparisons across 4 SNPs for a COMT main effect. (The haplotype analyses were exploratory so excluded from this calculation). We chose to report all alpha values between the uncorrected, conventional level of .05 and our corrected value of .001, as trends.

RESULTS

As shown in Table 1, age, gender ratios, and performance on estimates of both premorbid and current intellectual function differed significantly among the groups.

Table 3 summarizes all significant and trend-level COMT main effects and interactions for the four SNPs.

Table 3.

Significant and Near-Significant (Italics) Results for a COMT Effect* on Working Memory/Executive Function, by COMT Genotype

Genotype Test Name Diagnosis Main
Effect
COMT Main
Effect
Gender Main
Effect
COMT/Diagnosis
Interaction
COMT/Gender
Interaction
3-Way
Interaction
COMT
val158met
(rs4680)
0-BACK F(2,573)=5.94
p=.003
F(2,573)=3.60
p=.028
-- -- -- --
1-BACK F(2,574)=41.38,
p<.0001
F(2,574)=6.93,
p=.001
-- -- -- --
2-BACK F(2,573)=63.84,
p<0001
F(2,573)=5.93
p=.003
F(1,573)=5.31
p=.022
-- -- --
CPT
Vigilance
F(2,817)=65.56,
p<.0001
-- -- -- F(2,817)=3.04
p=.048
--
CPT
Distractibility
F(2,812)=100.46,
p<.0001
-- -- F(4,812)=3.36
p=.010
-- --
WCST
Perseverative
Errors
F(2,819)=39.13,
p<0001
-- -- -- F(2,819)=6.90,
p=.001
--
WCST
Categories
F(2,827)=101.45,
p<.0001
-- -- -- F(2,827)=3.90,
p=.021
--
IDED C_D F(2,149)=8.38,
p=.0004
-- -- F(4,149)=2.86
p=.026
-- --
IDED IDS F(2,149)=7.17,
p=.001
-- -- F(4,149)=2.46
p=.048
-- --
WAIS-R IQ F(2,835)=113.94,
p<.0001

-- --
F(2,835)=3.75
p=.024
--
--
rs737865 0-BACK F(2,432)=26.83,
p<0001
F(2,432)=8.01,
p=0004
-- F(4,432)=5.80,
p<0001

--
1-BACK F(2,432)=36.21,
p<.0001
F(2,432)=4.19
p=.0158
-- -- -- --
2-BACK F(2,432)=40.93,
p<0001
F(2,432)=4.13
p=.0167
-- -- -- --
CPT
Vigilance
F(2,643)=61.27,
p<.0001
-- -- F(4,643)=3.41,
p=.009
-- --
CPT
Distractibility
F(2,637)=75.67,
p<.0001
-- F(1,637)=4.31
p=.0384
F(4,637)=3.76
p=.0049
-- F(6,637)=3.25
p=.0038
rs165599 CPT
Distractibility
F(2,628)=62.47,
p<.0001
-- F(1,628)=4.84,
p=.0281
F(4,628)=2.55,
p=.0385
-- --
rs2097603 CPT
Vigilance
F(2,792)=50.10,
p<.0001
-- -- F(4,792)=2.95,
p=.019
-- --
CPT
Distractibility
F(2,789)=76.62,
p<0001
-- -- F(4,789)=3.12,
p=.015
-- --
*

Main effects of diagnosis only, while present for numerous other tests for rs737865, rs165599, and rs2097603 are not reported here.

COMT val108/158met (rs4680)

N-BACK

In a series of mixed-model ANOVAs in which diagnostic group, COMT genotype, and gender served as main effects, COMT val108/158met genotype had a significant effect on 1-BACK accuracy, and a near significant effect on 0-BACK and 2-BACK accuracy. The val-val group performed more poorly than the val-met and met-met groups, who did not differ from each other. This pattern was present irrespective of diagnostic group (proband, sibling, or control). The effect sizes (Cohen d) of the val-val, met-met genotype contrasts across all N-BACK conditions were at the low end of the moderate range: 0.20 for the 0-BACK, 0.31 for the 1-BACK and 0.29 for the 2-BACK. Across all N-BACK conditions, there was a significant or near-significant main effect of diagnosis and of COMT genotype, but no interaction between the two (see Table 3). Planned contrasts revealed that the probands performed significantly (or near significantly) more poorly than their siblings and the healthy controls, who performed better than the other groups.

When we restricted our analyses to those individuals who did not participate in our prior study (Goldberg 2003), we found that the results remained significant, and demonstrated that COMT genotype had a significant impact on both 1-BACK and 2-BACK performances (F(2,491)=6.76, p=.001 and F(2,490)=6.74, p=.001 respectively), suggesting a positive replication.

WCST

There was no significant main effect of COMT val108/158met genotype on perseverative errors (t-score) or categories achieved on the WCST. However, there was a main effect of diagnostic group, with probands performing significantly more poorly than siblings and controls, who did not differ from each other.

CPT

There was no significant main effect of COMT val108/158met genotype on Vigilance or Distractibility performance on the CPT. However, there was a significant main effect of diagnostic group for both variables, with probands performing significantly more poorly than their siblings and the healthy controls, who did not differ from each other. There also was a trend towards a COMT/gender interaction for the Vigilance portion (F(2,817)=3.04, p=.048), with males with two copies of the val allele performing more poorly overall. A near significant interaction between COMT val108/156met genotype and diagnosis on Distractibility performance also was observed (F(4,812)=3.36, p=.010). Probands with two copies of the val allele performed most poorly overall.

ID/ED

There was no significant main effect of COMT val108/158met genotype on any of the three conditions of the ID/ED task (C_D errors, IDS errors, and EDS errors). However, across all conditions, there was a main effect of diagnosis, and a near significant interaction between diagnosis and genotype for C_D (F(4,149)=2.86, p=.026) and IDS errors (F(4,149)=2.46, p=.048). Probands with two copies of the val allele performed most poorly, at both the IDS and C_D stages. The lack of a main effect of COMT val108/158met genotype may have been due to ceiling effects in the sibling and control groups. Planned contrasts revealed that the probands committed significantly more errors than the healthy controls, and near-significantly more than their siblings. Siblings and controls did not differ from each other.

IQ

There was no significant main effect of COMT val108/158met genotype on estimated IQ, as measured by the WAIS-R. However, unsurprisingly, there was a main effect of diagnostic group, with probands performing significantly more poorly than siblings and controls, and a trend towards siblings performing more poorly than controls.

Other COMT SNPs

Main effects of diagnosis and gender will not be further discussed in the SNPs listed below, as results differed only trivially due to small fluctuations in sample size. Evidence of strong linkage disequilibrium between rs4680 and two of the other COMT SNPs (rs737865 and rs165599) was not present in our sample. Therefore, the following analyses were not conditioned against val108/158met homozygote backgrounds.

COMT rs737865 (5’ region)

As shown in Table 3, significant and near-significant main effects and COMT/diagnosis interactions for rs737865 were restricted to the N-BACK and CPT. Specifically, there was a significant main effect of COMT on the 0-BACK, with the G-G genotype group performing more poorly than both the A–A and A–G genotype groups, who did not differ from each other. A similar pattern was seen at the trend level for a main effect of COMT on the 1-BACK and 2-BACK, with the G–G genotype group performing most poorly overall. The effect sizes (Cohen d) of the A–A, G–G contrasts across all N-BACK conditions were at the low end of the moderate range: 0.38 for the 0-BACK, 0.25 for the 1-BACK and 0.27 for the 2-BACK. The significant COMT/diagnosis interaction on the 0-BACK was characterized by G–G probands performing most poorly overall. There were no main effects of COMT seen on the CPT, but near significant COMT/diagnosis interactions were found for both Vigilance (F(4,643)=3.41, p=.009) and Distractibility (F(4,637)=3.76, p=.0049). Both interactions were characterized by G–G probands performing most poorly overall. Of note, this SNP also is in tight linkage disequilibrium with rs2097603, which shows asymmetric allele sharing across these loci [23, 38]. Thus, we cannot disambiguate in our sample the possible effects of linkage disequilibrium across these loci.

COMT rs165599 (3’ region)

There were no significant main effects of rs165599 on any of the measures, although there was a trend towards a COMT/diagnosis interaction on the Distractibility portion of the CPT (F(4,628)=2.55, p=.039). This interaction was characterized by G–G probands performing most poorly overall, and G–G controls performing the best.

COMT rs2097603 (P2 promoter region)

Rs2097603 showed no significant main effects on any of the measures, but trends towards COMT/diagnosis interactions on both the Vigilance (F(4,792)=2.95, p=.019) and Distractibility (F(4,789)=3.12, p=.015) portions of the CPT were seen. Both of these interactions were characterized by A–A probands performing most poorly overall.

Diplotype Analyses

We next examined the effects of COMT P2 promoter/val108/158met diplotypes on cognition. (G-G/val-val individuals (n=1 in the probands, n=5 in the whole sample) were excluded because of the very small cell size. (All other cell sizes were greater than 22)). A significant three-way interaction between diplotypes and diagnosis on CPT Vigilance was observed (F(8,704)=3.30, p=.001), with G-G/met-met homozygotes and A-A/val-val homozygotes performing more poorly than other diplotypes, most strikingly in the schizophrenic group. There were no significant interaction effects (i.e., diplotype effects) across the whole sample for all other tasks, though a near significant three-way interaction between diplotypes and diagnosis on the 1-BACK task (F(6,361)=2.39, p=.03) similar to that noted above, was seen. Thus, the significant P2 promoter/val108/158met/diagnosis interaction was characterized by both low COMT expression met homozygotes (putatively related to very high dopaminergic tone) and high expression val homozygotes (putatively related to very low dopaminergic tone) performing most poorly overall, with the profile most evident in the probands. (However, we note the presence of ceiling effects in the control and sibling groups on these tasks).

DISCUSSION

COMT val108/158met (rs4680)

Our current findings support and extend our earlier report of a COMT val108/158met genotype effect on working memory function [9]. Consistent with our prediction, individuals with two copies of the val allele performed most poorly on the N-BACK, irrespective of psychiatric diagnosis. Met homozygotes demonstrated the best performance on the task, and heterozygotes performed intermediate between the other two groups. Once again, we failed to see a COMT/diagnosis interaction on any condition of the N-BACK, in the face of a significant COMT effect, which suggests that normal controls, siblings, and probands were more or less equally affected by val108/158met. The fact that the three diagnostic groups were affected in a near linear fashion in this, and our earlier studies [7, 9], supports the notion of an additive genetic model in which allele loading influences working memory in a similar fashion, irrespective of an individual’s specific clinical background.

In contrast with our previous findings, we found a near-significant val108/158met genotype effect on 0-BACK performance in the current study. Additionally, as predicted, we found a near significant COMT/diagnosis interaction on performance on the Distractibility portion of the CPT, which we did not find previously with our smaller sample [9]. Both the 0-BACK and the CPT make demands on attention, vigilance and encoding in the service of target detection. In a related study of a novel attentional control task dependent primarily on cingulate cortex function, Blasi et al [25] recently showed an effect of COMT val108/158met genotype on task performance and on cingulate activation, broadly consistent with our results here. Certainly, there is evidence supporting a role for dopamine in attention and target detection tasks (e.g. [29]). Interestingly, Bilder et al [6] suggested something similar when they examined COMT val108/158met genotype effects on cognition in schizophrenia, reporting that met homozygotes outperformed heterozygotes and val homozygotes on a composite measure of attention and processing speed. Furthermore, Crofts et al. [27] reported increased susceptibility to distraction from task-irrelevant stimuli in marmosets with catecholamine-depleted frontal lobes, supporting the contention that COMT val108/158met genotype could influence performance on the CPT, and Eisenberg and colleagues [40] reported that the val allele was strongly associated with false alarms on a version of the CPT.

We also found a near-significant COMT/diagnosis interaction on two of the three ID/ED attentional set shifting measures examined. Probands with two copies of the high activity val allele committed substantially more errors at the C_D stage than probands with one or no copies of the allele. Distracting and irrelevant stimuli are introduced at the C_D stage, and subjects must ignore stimuli from the novel dimension and continue to respond to the previously-rewarded stimulus. Our results are consistent with both the animal literature [27] and work from our group [41]. There also was a trend towards a COMT/diagnosis interaction at the IDS stage, which suggests probands with two copies of the val allele were significantly less able to discern the conceptual category within which they were responding. A deficit in ID shifting represents an impairment in the ability to generalize a discrimination learned for a particular set of exemplars to another set from the same category in the face of newly salient distractors. These results raise the more general possibility that val108/158met genotype affects both updating or rule learning during interference laden contexts (ID/ED, N-BACK) and attentional control (CPT, 0-BACK). We suspect that the failure to find an overall effect of genotype relates to ceiling effects in the performance of the control subjects on this task.

Although we found a significant group effect at the EDS stage of the ID/ED task, we failed to see a COMT/diagnosis interaction, which was unexpected, given that ED shifting is the core component of the WCST, and is the basis for achieving novel sorting categories [41]. We, and others, have previously reported a significant COMT val108/158met effect on WCST performance [7, 11] (but also see [42]), so we expected to replicate this result in the current study. However, we failed to see a significant COMT effect on the WCST in our sample, nor for EDS, the stage from the ID/ED most comparable to the WCST. These findings, using a substantially larger sample than in our previous study, argue that the WCST may not be the ideal task to examine COMT effects on working memory function. Other studies have also reported negative findings for COMT and the WCST [42, 43], suggesting that the N-BACK and other attentional control tasks (e.g., CPT, C_D, IDS) may be more sensitive measures to detect the specific cognitive operations influenced by val108/158met genotype. Significant findings from previous studies might have more to do with the characteristics of their smaller, less representative samples, than to an effect of COMT per se.

As we predicted, and consistent with our previous work [9], we failed to see a val108/158met genotype effect on intelligence, as assessed by the four-subtest version of the WAIS-R. This result speaks again to a continuing argument in the genetics of cognition literature, whether specific cognitive processes can be parsed genetically independent of their correlation with “g.” While our findings support the contention that COMT val108/158met genotype influences prefrontally-mediated working memory/executive function, with inheritance of two copies of the val allele most detrimental to performance, results from other studies have not been uniformly positive (e.g., [4244]. However, there are a number of methodological differences between these and the current study that may, at least in part, explain some of the discrepancies, including cohort differences, and differences in test design and cognitive manipulation requirements. Evidence from the N-BACK, and to a lesser extent, the ID/ED task, lead us to favor the hypothesis that COMT genotype affects cognitive updating, yet we recognize that other explanations could be supported by these data, including cognitive control hypotheses pertaining to interference suppression, and working memory load.

rs737865 (5’ region)

Overall, findings from this SNP bore similarity to those for val108/158met. For example, significant or near-significant effects of rs737865 genotype were seen across all load levels of the N-BACK, with the G–G genotype group performing most poorly overall. Furthermore, an interaction between rs737865 and diagnosis was seen on both subtests of the CPT, with G–G probands performing most poorly overall. Of note, the observed effects for all of these updating and attentional control tests were in the predicted direction, with probands homozygous for the presumed risk G allele performing most poorly overall.

rs165599 (3’ region) and rs2097603 (P2 promoter region)

Overall, the results from these two SNPs were far less compelling than those for val108/158met and rs737865. Specifically, we failed to see any significant COMT main effects on working memory/executive function for either the rs165599 or rs2097603 SNPs alone. However, there was a near significant interaction between rs165599 and diagnosis for the Distractibility portion of the CPT, with G–G probands performing most poorly overall (similar to that reported by Chan and colleagues [45]).

We also found an interaction effect of rs2097603 and diagnosis on both subtests of the CPT, with the A–A genotype appearing most disadvantageous for probands. Interestingly, the A allele was reported in one study to be (non-significantly) preferentially transmitted in schizophrenia [31], and has a weak effect on COMT enzyme activity in cultured lymphocytes, but less clearly in brain [16].

rs2097603/rs4680 Diplotypes

The complex nature of the findings with the SNPs other than val108/158met may in part be explained by the possibility that combinations of alleles at these various loci modulate the val108/158met effect. This is suggested by the functional analyses in tissue [16, 22] and by an fMRI study of haplotypes in COMT [33]. Because allele frequencies at all these loci in COMT show considerable population variation [23], discrepancies in the literature involving associations with COMT and cognition and psychosis may also be related to some of this haplotypic variation in COMT population genetics [46]. Consistent with models that predict performance on the basis of an inverted u-shaped curve [12, 32, 47], the two diplotypes putatively related to highest and lowest levels of dopaminergic tone also performed worse overall on select tests of attentional control (CPT Vigilance) and, to a lesser extent, cognitive updating (1-BACK). This was especially pronounced in the schizophrenic group, perhaps because they had the least ability to compensate for these more “extremes” of prefrontal physiology. (However, ceiling effects in the performance of the sibling and control groups suggests circumspection regarding this interpretation. Furthermore, different aspects of dopamine transmission at high and low levels of COMT enzyme activity (e.g., tonic versus phasic [48]) may have contributed to these findings). These results add to evidence that COMT genotype per se, even at the val108/158met locus, does not capture the complex biology of interacting functional loci within the gene.

As a final caveat, we note that, despite numerous near significant results, ultimately only two significant main effects of COMT genotype survived our strict Bonferroni correction, and both involved the N-BACK test. Furthermore, medication effects in the schizophrenic group may have influenced our findings. We did not assess these due to the variety of medication regimens, but we note that medication may have produced main effects in some instances. However, the consistency of results across the unmedicated healthy control group somewhat mitigates this.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank the research assistants for testing of subjects.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Financial Disclosures

Dr’s Diaz-Asper, Goldberg, Kolachana, Straub, Egan, & Weinberger reported no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest related directly or indirectly to this work.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Goldberg TE, Green MF. Neurocognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia: an overview. In: Davis D, Coyle J, Nemeroff C, editors. Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress. American College of Neuropsychopharmacology; 2002. pp. 657–669. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Weickert TW GT, Gold JM, Bigelow LB, Egan MF, Weinberger DR. Cognitive impairments in patients with schizophrenia displaying preserved and compromised intellect. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:907–913. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.9.907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Glatt S, Faraone SV, Tsuang MT. Association between a functional catechol-o-methyltransferase gene polymorphism and schizophrenia: meta-analyses of case-control and family-based studies. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:469–476. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.3.469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Harrison P, Weinberger DR. Schizophrenia genes, gene expression, and neuropathology: on the matter of their convergence. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:40–68. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Weinberger D, Egan MF, Bertolino A, Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Lipska BK, et al. Prefrontal neurons and the genetics of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:825–844. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01252-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bilder RM, Volavka J, Czobor P, Malhotra AK, Kennedy JL, Ni XQ, et al. Neurocognitive correlates of the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52:701–707. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01416-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Callicott JH, Mazzanti CM, Straub RE, et al. Effect of COMT Val108/158Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2001;98:6917–6922. doi: 10.1073/pnas.111134598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gallinat J, Bajbouj M, Sander T, Schlattmann P, Xu K, Ferro EF, et al. Association of the G1947A COMT (Val108/158Met) gene polymorphism with prefrontal P300 during information processing. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:40–48. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01973-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Goldberg T, Egan MF, Gscheidle T, Coppola R, Weickert T, Kolachana BS, et al. Executive subprocesses in working memory: relationship to catechol-o-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:889–896. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Joober R, Gauthier J, Lal S, Bloom D, Lalonde P, Rouleau G, et al. Catechol-o-methyltransferase val158met gene variants associated with performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:662–663. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.7.662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Malhotra A, Kestler LJ, Mazzanti C, Bates JA, Goldberg T, Goldman D. A functional polymorphism in the COMT gene and performance on a test of prefrontal cognition. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:652–654. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.4.652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Mattay V, Goldberg TE, Fera F, Hariri AR, Tessitore A, Egan MF, et al. Catechol-o-methyltransferase val158met genotype and individual variation in the brain response to amphetamine. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2003;100:6186–6191. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0931309100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Rosa A, Peralta V, Cuesta MJ, Zarzuela A, Serrano F, Martinez-Larrea A, et al. New evidence of association between COMT gene and prefrontal neurocognitive function in healthy individuals and from sibling pairs discordant for psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1110–1112. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.6.1110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Callicott JH, Bertolino A, Mattay VS, Langheim FJP, Duyn J, Coppola R, et al. Physiological dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia revisited. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:1078–1092. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.11.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bertolino A, Esposito G, Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Van Horn JD, Frank JA, et al. Specific relationship between prefrontal neuronal N-acetylaspartate and activation of the working memory cortical network in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:26–33. doi: 10.1176/ajp.157.1.26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Chen J, Lipska BK, Halim N, Ma QD, Matsumoto M, Melhem S, et al. Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT): Effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am J Hum Genet. 2004;75:807–821. doi: 10.1086/425589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Li T, Sham PC, Vallada H, Xie T, Tang X, Murray RM, et al. Preferential transmission of the high activity allele of COMT in schizophrenia. Psychiatr Genet. 1996;6:131–133. doi: 10.1097/00041444-199623000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Wonodi I, Stine OC, Mitchell BD, Buchanan RW, Thaker GK. Association between val158met polymorphism of the COMT gene and schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet. 2003;120B:47–50. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Shifman S, Bronstein M, Sternfeld M, Pisante-Shalom A, Lev-Lehman E, Weizman A, et al. A highly significant association between a COMT haplotype and schizophrenia. Am J Hum Genet. 2002;71:1296–1302. doi: 10.1086/344514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Fan J-B, Zhang C-S, Gu N-F, Li X-W, Sun W-W, Wang H-Y, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase gene val/met functional polymorphism and risk of schizophrenia: A large-scale association study plus meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:139–144. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Craddock N, Owen MJ, O'Donovan MC. The catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene as a candidate for psychiatric phenotypes: evidence and lessons. Mol Psychiatry. 2006;11:446–458. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bray N, Buckland PR, Williams NM, Williams HJ, Norton N, Owen MJ, et al. A haplotype implicated in schizophrenia susceptibility is associated with reduced COMT expression in human brain. Am J Hum Genet. 2003;73:152–161. doi: 10.1086/376578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.DeMille MMC, Kidd JR, Ruggeri V, Palmatier MA, Goldman D, Odunsi A, et al. Population variation in linkage disequilibrium across the COMT gene considering Promoter region and coding region variation. Hum Genet. 2002;111:521–537. doi: 10.1007/s00439-002-0809-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Palmatier M, Pakstis AJ, Speed W, Paschou P, Goldman D, Odunsi A, et al. COMT haplotypes suggest P2 Promoter region relevance for schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9:859–870. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Blasi G, Mattay VS, Bertolino A, Elvevaag B, Callicott JH, Das S, et al. Effect of catecholo-o-methyltransferase val158met genotype on attentional control. J Neurosci. 2005;25:5038–5045. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0476-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Baunez C, Robbins TW. Effects of dopamine depletion of the dorsal striatum and further interaction with subthalamic nucleus lesions in an attentional task in the rat. Neuroscience. 1999;92:1343–1356. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(99)00065-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Crofts HS, Dalley JW, Collins P, Van Denderen JCM, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW, et al. Differential effects of 6-OHDA lesions of the frontal cortex and caudate nucleus on the ability to acquire an attentional set. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:1015–1026. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.11.1015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Chudasama Y, Robbins TW. Psychopharmacological approaches to modulating attention in the five-choice serial reaction time task: implications for schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology. 2004;174:86–98. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1805-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Nieoullon A. Dopamine and the regulation of cognition and attention. Prog Neurobiol. 2002;67:53–83. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(02)00011-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Servan-Schreiber D, Carter CS, Bruno RM, Cohen JD. Dopamine and the mechanisms of cognition: Part II. D-amphetamine effects in human subjects performing a selective attention task. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43:723–729. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(97)00449-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Li T, Ball D, Zhao J, Murray RM, Liu X, Sham PC, et al. Family-based linkage disequilibrium mapping using SNP marker haplotypes: application to a potential locus for schizophrenia at chromosome 22q11. Mol Psychiatry. 2000;5:77–84. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Goldman-Rakic PS. The cortical dopamine system: role in memory and cognition. Adv Pharmacol. 1998;42:707–711. doi: 10.1016/s1054-3589(08)60846-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Meyer-Lindenberg A, Nichols T, Callicott J, Ding J, Kolachana B, Buckholtz J, et al. Impact of complex genetic variation in COMT on human brain function. Mol Psychiatry. 2006;11:867–877. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Gscheidle T, Weirich M, Bigelow LB, Weinberger DR. Relative risk of attention deficits in siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:1309–1316. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.8.1309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Palmatier M, Kang AM, Kidd KK. Global variation in the frequencies of functionally different catechol-o-methyltransferase alleles. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:557–567. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00098-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. User's Guide for the SCID-I for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders-Research Version. New York: Biometrics Research; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Apud JA, Mattay VS, Chen J, Kolachana BS, Callicott JH, Rasetti R, et al. Tolcapone improves cognition and cortical information processing in normal human subjects Neuropsychopharmacology, 2006. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301227. Oct. 2006 25 [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Chen X, Wang X, O'Neill AF, Walsh D, Kendler KS. Variants on the catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) gene are associated with schizophrenia in Irish high-density families. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9:962–967. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Littell RC, Milliken GA, Stroup WW, Wolfingwer RD. SAS System for Mixed Models (Version 6.12) Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Eisenberg J, Mei-Tal G, Steinberg A, Tartakovsky E, Zohar A, Gritsenko I, et al. Haplotype relative risk study of catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Association of the high-enzyme activity val allele with ADHD impulsive-hyperactive phenotype. Am J Med Genet. 1999;88:497–502. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Jazbec S, Pantelis C, Robbins T, Weickert T, Weinberger DR, Goldberg TE. Intra-dimensional/extradimensional set-shifting performance in schizophrenia: impact of distractors. Schizophr Res. 2007;89:339–349. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Tsai S, Yu YW, Chen TJ, Chen JY, Liou YJ, Chen MC, et al. Association study of a functional catechol-O-methyltransferase-gene polymorphism and cognitive function in healthy females. Neurosci Lett. 2003;338:123–126. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)01396-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ho B, Wassink TH, O'Leary DS, Sheffield VC, Andreasen NC. Catechol-o-methyltransferase val(158)met gene polymorphism in schizophrenia: working memory, frontal lobe MRI morphology and frontal cerebral blood flow. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:287–298. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Stefanis N, van Os J, Avramopoulos D, Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Hantoumi I, et al. Variation in catechol-o-methyltransferase val158met genotype associated with schizotypy but not cognition: a population study in 543 young men. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56:510–515. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Chan RCK, Chen RYL, Chen EYH, Hui TCK, Cheung EFC, Cheung HK, et al. The differential clinical and neurocognitive profiles of COMT SNP rs165599 genotypes in schizophrenia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11:202–204. doi: 10.1017/s1355617705050241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Tunbridge E, Harrison PJ, Weinberger DR. Catechol-o-Methyltransferase, cognition, and psychosis: Val158Met and beyond. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:141–151. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Meyer-Lindenberg A, Kohn PD, Kolachana B, Kippenhan S, McInerney-Leo A, Nussbaum R, et al. Midbrain dopamine and prefrontal function in humans: interaction and modulation by COMT genotype. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:594–596. doi: 10.1038/nn1438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Bilder RM, Volavka J, Lachman HM, Grace AA. The catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism: relations to the tonic-phasic dopamine hypothesis and neurpsychiatric phenotypes. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1943–1961. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtis G. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) Manual-Revised and Expanded. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Gordon M. CPT. Syracuse, NY: Gordon Diagnostic Systems; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Wechsler D. WAIS-R Manual. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Sahakian BJ, Owen AM. Computerized assessment in neuropsychiatry using CANTAB: discussion paper. J R Soc Med. 1992;85:399–402. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES